Sleep Study For Epi

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

American Journal of Epidemiology Vol. 177, No.

4
© The Author 2013. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of DOI: 10.1093/aje/kws422
Public Health. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com. Advance Access publication:
January 16, 2013

Original Contribution

Self-reported Sleep Duration, Sleep Quality, and Breast Cancer Risk in a


Population-based Case-Control Study

Jennifer Girschik*, Jane Heyworth, and Lin Fritschi


* Correspondence to Jennifer Girschik, Western Australian Institute for Medical Research, B Block, Ground Floor, Hospital Avenue, Nedlands,
Western Australia 6009, Australia (e-mail: jennifer.girschik@uwa.edu.au).

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Otago on September 22, 2015


Initially submitted March 8, 2012; accepted for publication April 26, 2012.

Breast cancer is one of the most commonly diagnosed invasive cancers. Established risk factors account for
only a small proportion of cases. Previous studies have found reductions in sleep duration and quality in the
general population over time. There is evidence to suggest a link between poor sleep and an increased risk of
breast cancer. In this study, we investigated the relationship between breast cancer and sleep duration and
quality in Western Australian women. Data were obtained from a population-based case-control study conducted
from 2009 to 2011. Participants completed a self-administered questionnaire that included questions on sleep.
Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals were calculated using unconditional logistic regression. Sensitivity
analysis for potential selection and misclassification bias was also conducted. We found no association between
self-reported sleep duration on workdays and risk of breast cancer (for <6 hours, odds ratio (OR) = 1.05 (95%
CI: 0.82, 1.33); for 6–7 hours, OR = 0.96 (95% CI: 0.80, 1.16); and for >8 hours, OR = 1.10 (95% CI: 0.87, 1.39),
compared with the reference category of 7–8 hours’ sleep). In addition, we found no association between sleep
duration on nonworkdays, subjective sleep quality, or combined duration and quality and risk of breast cancer.
This study does not provide evidence to support an association between self-reported sleep duration or quality
and the risk of breast cancer.

breast cancer; case-control studies; circadian rhythm; sleep; sleep duration; sleep quality

Abbreviation: CI, confidence interval.

Editor’s note: An invited commentary on this article although reductions in sleep duration have not been consis-
appears on page 328 and the authors’ response appears tently found (8, 9). Studies of sleep quality have reported
on page 331. increases in the prevalence of subjective poor-quality sleep
ranging from 5% to almost 40% (3, 4, 10).
While the immediate effects of poor sleep, such as tired-
Breast cancer is the most commonly diagnosed invasive ness, loss of concentration, and injuries, are well recognized
cancer and the leading cause of cancer death among (11), the chronic effects of poor sleep have not been exten-
women, both in Australia and globally (1, 2). While some sively studied. However, there is some evidence to suggest
risk factors, such as primary genetic mutations, reproduc- a link between poor sleep and a range of long-term health
tive history, weight gain, and alcohol consumption, are well effects, including increased risk of cancer (12–18).
established, they do not account for a significant proportion Two plausible biological models have been proposed
of breast cancer cases (2). that would explain how poor sleep can directly influence
Both international and Australian studies have reported the development of cancer: impaired immune function and
changes in sleep over time in developed countries. Some metabolic pathways that lead to obesity (2, 12, 15, 19). An
studies have suggested a reduction in habitual sleep dura- additional indirect mechanism has also been proposed
tion of 13–36 minutes over the last 20–30 years (3–7), which suggests an increased risk of cancer due to altered

316 Am J Epidemiol. 2013;177(4):316–327


Self-reported Sleep and Breast Cancer Risk 317

melatonin release (20, 21). Melatonin release is regulated participants, those who refused to participate were older,
by the light/dark cycle rather than by sleep per se, and pre- while those who did not respond were younger. In addition,
vious studies have used sleep as a proxy for “exposure to women who did not respond were more likely to live in
darkness” (22). All 3 proposed pathways have some evi- very remote areas (4% vs. 2%). There were no differences
dence to support them, but the true process by which sleep in socioeconomic status between breast cancer patients who
might influence cancer risk is unknown. participated and those who refused or did not respond to
Five studies have investigated the relationship between the study invitation.
sleep duration and breast cancer and have shown mixed
results (22–26). Kakizaki et al. (23) reported a statistically Controls
significant trend of decreasing risk with increasing sleep
duration, while McElroy et al. (24) reported a statistically Controls were women aged 18–80 years living in
significant trend of increasing risk with increasing duration. Western Australia between May 2009 and July 2011 who
The 3 remaining studies also found significant trends, but were randomly selected from the electoral roll. They were
only after study populations were restricted or polytomous frequency-matched to the expected distribution of cases by
analysis was conducted (22, 25, 26). Only 1 of those 5-year age group. Being registered to vote is compulsory in
studies also examined subjective sleep quality (22); no as- Australia, and the electoral roll is considered an almost

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Otago on September 22, 2015


sociation with breast cancer was found. complete list of Australian citizens. Controls were ineligible
Our aim in this study was to investigate the relationship if they had had a previous diagnosis of invasive breast
between breast cancer and 3 domains of sleep (sleep dura- cancer or did not speak adequate English to complete the
tion on workdays, sleep duration on nonworkdays, and sub- questionnaire. From a total of 4,358 eligible controls, 1,789
jective sleep quality) in a population of Western Australian (41%) consented to participate. Of the eligible controls
women. who did not participate, 939 (22%) refused, 1,628 (37%)
did not respond, and 2 had died. Compared with partici-
MATERIALS AND METHODS pants, those who refused were older, while those who did
not respond were younger. There were no differences in res-
The Breast Cancer Environment and Employment Study idential remoteness or socioeconomic status between
was a case-control study of occupational and environmental control women who participated and those who refused or
risk factors for breast cancer conducted in Western Australia. did not respond to the study invitation.
A sample size of 1,000 cases and 2,000 age-matched con-
trols was calculated a priori based on the expected number Data collection
and recruitment rate of incident breast cancer patients in
Western Australia, the prevalence of exposure to shift work We asked participants to complete a self-administered
(the main study hypothesis), and the increased risk of postal questionnaire that contained questions on demo-
breast cancer we expected to detect. The study was ap- graphic, reproductive, and lifestyle factors as well as sleep.
proved by the human research ethics committees of the The domains of sleep investigated were: usual duration of
Western Australian Department of Health and The Universi- sleep on workdays; usual duration of sleep on nonwork-
ty of Western Australia. days; and subjective sleep quality. Specifically, the ques-
tions were: “Ignoring the last year, how many hours of
Cases sleep on [nonwork/work] days do you usually get?”; “Ig-
noring the last year, do you generally consider yourself to
Cases were female residents of Western Australia aged be a good sleeper; that is, do you fall asleep easily and
18–80 years with a first incident invasive breast cancer (In- sleep soundly?” We prefaced the questions with the words
ternational Classification of Diseases, Tenth Revision, “Ignoring the last year…” in order to avoid identifying
code C50) that was diagnosed between May 1, 2009, and recent sleep habits that might be associated with underlying
January 31, 2011, and reported to the Western Australian disease in cases.
Cancer Registry before July 31, 2011. The Western Australian Answer categories for the sleep duration questions were
Cancer Registry is a population-based cancer registry that <5 hours, 5–6 hours, 6–7 hours, 7–8 hours, 8–9 hours, and
was established in 1981. Notification of cancer cases (ex- >9 hours. There is a widely held but scientifically unsup-
cluding skin-primary basal cell carcinoma and squamous ported view that the normal/optimal sleep duration is 8
cell carcinoma) is mandatory for pathologists, hematolo- hours per night (27). These nonexclusive answer categories
gists, and radiation oncologists. Cases were excluded if the were used to try to discourage participants from reflexively
breast cancer was not invasive (i.e., ductal carcinoma in giving a “normative” answer (e.g., 8 hours) and to allow
situ); if diagnosis had occurred more than 6 months prior to participants to consider the variability in their sleep and the
the date on which the Western Australian Cancer Registry direction in which this variability was more likely to occur.
received the notification; or if the patient had inadequate Answer categories such as “7–7.9 hours” have recently
comprehension of English to complete the questionnaire. been criticized in the context of sleep research for forcing
Of the 2,089 eligible breast cancer cases identified, 1,205 participants into answering with a level of precision that is
women (58%) consented to participate. Of the eligible inconsistent with a quantitative estimate of a highly vari-
cases who did not participate, 334 (16%) refused, 545 able trait (28). Answer categories for the sleep quality ques-
(26%) did not respond, and 5 had died. Compared with tions were: very good sleeper; fairly good sleeper; fairly

Am J Epidemiol. 2013;177(4):316–327
318 Girschik et al.

bad sleeper; and very bad sleeper. In addition, we created a rigidity scale). Lower scores on the flexibility/rigidity scale
categorical variable that combined sleep duration on work- are indicative of more rigid habits, conceptualized as an
days with 2 categories of sleep quality (good and bad) to affinity for routine sleeping and eating times, while higher
investigate any joint effect of sleep length and quality. scores indicate increasing flexibility (32, 33). The Circadian
Type Inventory sets the cutpoints for both scales at the 25th
Statistical analysis and 75th percentiles, with the middle 50% of each scale
defined as “neither.” Differing sleep need and responses to
Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals were calculated sleep disturbance have been reported within the subtypes of
using unconditional logistic regression, with the frequency- all 3 characteristics (33, 36).
matched variable age included in all models. Variables Selection bias. Information on age (in 5-year groups),
thought to be associated with both the outcome and the ex- socioeconomic status (as defined by the Australian Bureau
posure were considered “probable confounders” and were of Statistics Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas quintiles of
included in multivariable models regardless of any univari- advantage-disadvantage) (37), and residential remoteness (as
ate association with the outcome. Variables thought to be defined by the Accessibility/Remoteness Index of Australia)
associated with either the outcome or the exposure (but not (38) was available for nonparticipants. To investigate selec-
both) were considered “possible confounders” and were in- tion bias, we recalculated the age-adjusted odds ratios after

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Otago on September 22, 2015


cluded in the multivariate models if a χ2 test of their univar- making two assumptions about sleep duration and quality
iate association with the outcome resulted in a P value of in nonparticipants. The first assumption was that nonpartic-
0.25 or less. ipants might be more likely to be “better” sleepers than par-
The “possible confounders” were: country of birth, edu- ticipants (sleep duration 7–8 hours; sleep quality very good
cation, family history of breast cancer, previous breast con- or fairly good). The second assumption was that people
ditions, use of oral contraceptives in the last 5 years, and who did not consent might be “worse” sleepers (sleep dura-
circadian amplitude. The possible confounders were then tion <6 hours; sleep quality very bad or fairly bad), while
assessed for their contribution to the fully adjusted multi- those who did not respond might be “better” sleepers (sleep
variable effect size by means of a user-written Stata duration >8 hours or 7–8 hours; sleep quality very good or
program (StataCorp LP, College Station, Texas) (29). None fairly good), in comparison with people who participated.
of the possible confounders contributed to a change in the This second assumption was based on the age differences
odds ratio of more than 5%, and as such they were exclud- between participants, nonconsenters, and nonresponders in
ed from the final multivariable model. “Probable confound- this study and the association between increasing age and
ers” included in the final multivariable model were: age, in decreasing sleep duration and quality (39). Under these two
5-year groups; number of children; age at first birth; assumptions, 7 scenarios for nonparticipant sleep were de-
whether children were breastfed; menopausal status; use of veloped and assessed.
physician-prescribed hormone replacement therapy; dura- Misclassification bias. Self-reported data are potentially
tion of use of hormone replacement therapy; usual alcohol subject to misclassification, and because our exposure vari-
consumption (glasses per week); comparative weight at age ables were categorical, even random misclassification could
30 years (compared with others of the same age and have resulted in bias in any direction (40). Studies of expo-
height); ever use of melatonin; and total physical activity in sure misclassification have found that most of the error
metabolic equivalents per week (quartiles). occurs within adjacent categories (41). For sleep quality,
Polytomous regression models were used to determine we investigated the impact of various levels of 1-category
odds ratios according to the menopausal status ( pre- or nondifferential misclassification on our unadjusted odds
post-) of cases at the time of diagnosis relative to controls ratios using the method described by Marshall et al. (42).
and for estrogen receptor–positive/estrogen receptor–negative For the analysis of misclassification of sleep duration,
cases relative to controls (30, 31). we used a modified version of the method described by
Stratified analysis was also conducted for 3 characteris- Marshall et al. (42). A validity study of the sleep question-
tics of the circadian rhythm ( phase, amplitude, and stabil- naire used in this study (43) and supported by Patel et al. (44)
ity) (32, 33). The phase of the circadian rhythm is found that the bias in self-reported duration was pointing
commonly recognized as a preference for being active in away from the median. We recalculated odds ratios using
either the morning or the evening and was measured using varying levels of misclassification, where misclassified par-
the Horne-Ostberg Morningness/Evening questionnaire ticipants could only move towards the reference group (7–8
(34), with cutpoints developed by Taillard et al. (35). The hours).
amplitude and stability of the rhythm are increasingly being All statistical analysis was conducted using Stata 12 or
recognized as potential factors in adjustment to shift work Microsoft Excel 2010 (Microsoft, Redmond, Washington),
(32, 33). The Circadian Type Inventory reports the ampli- and all statistical tests were 2-sided.
tude of the rhythm as languidness or vigorousness (lan-
guidness/vigorousness scale). Lower scores on the RESULTS
languidness/vigorousness scale indicate a tendency toward
vigorousness, representing the ability to more easily over- Of the 2,994 participants who completed the study, ap-
come drowsiness, while higher scores indicate a tendency proximately 5% had missing data, leaving 2,828 for sleep
toward languidness. The stability of the rhythm is reported duration analysis and 2,827 for sleep quality analysis. For
as the flexibility or rigidity of sleeping habits (flexibility/ the polytomous model, information on estrogen receptor

Am J Epidemiol. 2013;177(4):316–327
Self-reported Sleep and Breast Cancer Risk 319

Table 1. Distribution of Demographic and Risk Factors Among Table 1. Continued


Women With and Without Breast Cancer, Breast Cancer
Environment and Employment Study, 2009–2011 Controls Cases
(n = 1,695) (n = 1,133)
Controls Cases No. % No. %
(n = 1,695) (n = 1,133)
Menopausal status
No. % No. %
Postmenopausal 1,279 75.5 782 69.0
Age group, years
Premenopausal 416 24.5 351 31.0
<45 179 10.6 157 13.9
Use of hormone replacement
45–49 201 11.9 171 15.1 therapy
50–54 229 13.5 149 13.2 None 999 58.9 711 62.8
55–59 288 17.0 175 15.5 Estrogen only 254 15.0 118 10.4
60–64 303 17.9 180 15.9 Progesterone only 72 4.3 27 2.4
65–69 250 14.8 135 11.9 Estrogen + progesterone 62 3.7 68 6.0
≥70 245 14.5 166 14.7 Mixture of treatments 150 8.9 110 9.7

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Otago on September 22, 2015


Country of birth Nonhormonal treatment 32 1.9 33 2.9
Australia/New Zealand 1,139 67.2 725 64.0 Unknown treatment 126 7.4 66 5.8
United Kingdom/Ireland 362 21.4 244 21.5 Highest level of education
completed
Europe 75 4.4 64 5.7
Year 9, 10, or 11 592 34.9 401 35.4
Asia 62 3.7 56 4.9
Other 57 3.4 44 3.9 Year 12 390 23.0 233 20.6
Trade/apprenticeship 414 24.4 247 21.8
No. of children
University degree 299 17.6 252 22.2
0 183 10.8 148 13.1
1 123 7.3 109 9.6 Usual no. of alcoholic drinks per
week
2 663 39.1 437 38.6
0 271 16.0 185 16.3
3 460 27.1 284 25.1 <1 211 12.5 129 11.4
4 184 10.9 98 8.7
1–3 395 23.3 265 23.4
≥5 82 4.8 57 5.0
4–6 227 13.4 142 12.5
Age at first childbirth, yearsa 7–9 138 8.1 114 10.1
<20 164 10.9 106 10.8
≥10 453 26.7 298 26.3
20–24 605 40.0 404 41.0
Weight status at age 30 years,
25–29 498 32.9 274 27.8 compared with others of the
same age and height
≥30 245 16.2 201 20.4
Weighed a lot less 80 4.7 82 7.2
Breastfed childrena
Weighed a little less 458 27.0 312 27.5
No 145 9.6 116 11.8
Weighed the same 793 46.8 528 46.6
Yes 1,367 90.4 869 88.2
Weighed a little more 296 17.5 169 14.9
Family history of breast cancer
Weighed a lot more 68 4.0 42 3.7
No family history 1,210 71.4 688 60.7
a
Unknown family history 4 0.2 4 0.4 Limited to participants who had 1 or more children.
Some family history 357 21.1 283 25.0
Increased-risk family history 124 7.32 158 14.0
Table continues
disorders (<1%) in this sample were consistent with those
reported in the general population (19). Compared with
controls, cases were slightly younger and were more
status was not available for 169 women with breast cancer, likely to have been born outside of Australia/New
and they were excluded from this analysis. Zealand, to have obtained a university degree, to have a
After the distribution of sleep durations was reviewed, family history of breast cancer, to have a later age at first
fewer than 5% of participants fell into the first and last cat- birth, to have taken estrogen and progesterone combina-
egories, and thus the results were condensed into 4 catego- tion hormone replacement therapy, and to have weighed
ries for analysis: <6 hours, 6–7 hours, 7–8 hours (reference less than their peers at age 30 years (Table 1). In addition,
category), and >8 hours. cases were less likely than controls to have had 1 or more
The prevalences of diagnosed sleep apnea (2.5%), in- children, to have breastfed their children, and to be
somnia (2.9%), restless legs (2.6%), and other sleep postmenopausal.

Am J Epidemiol. 2013;177(4):316–327
320 Girschik et al.

Table 2. Age-adjusted and Multivariate-adjusted Odds Ratios for the Association Between Sleep and Breast Cancer Risk, Breast Cancer
Environment and Employment Study, 2009–2011

Controls Cases
OR 95% CI Adjusted ORa 95% CI
No. % No. %

Duration of sleep on workdays, hours (n = 2,828)


<6 238 14.0 163 14.4 1.04 0.83, 1.32 1.05 0.82, 1.33
6–7 544 32.1 341 30.1 0.95 0.79, 1.14 0.96 0.80, 1.16
7–8 681 40.2 461 40.7 1.00 1.00
>8 232 13.7 168 14.8 1.05 0.83, 1.33 1.10 0.87, 1.39
Duration of sleep on nonworkdays, hours (n = 2,828)
<6 166 9.8 118 10.4 1.07 0.82, 1.39 1.06 0.80, 1.39
6–7 369 21.8 235 20.7 0.96 0.78, 1.17 0.96 0.78, 1.18
7–8 670 39.5 461 40.7 1.00 1.00
>8 490 28.9 319 28.2 0.92 0.76, 1.11 0.94 0.78, 1.13

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Otago on September 22, 2015


Sleep quality (n = 2,827)b
Very good 420 24.8 281 24.8 1.00 1.00
Fairly good 852 50.3 580 51.2 1.03 0.85, 1.24 1.06 0.88, 1.28
Fairly bad 331 19.6 208 18.3 0.97 0.77, 1.22 0.98 0.77, 1.24
Very bad 90 5.3 65 5.7 1.14 0.80, 1.63 1.11 0.77, 1.61
Duration on workdays (hours)/quality (n = 2,826)c
<6/good 96 5.7 69 6.1 1.10 0.79, 1.55 1.12 0.80, 1.58
6–7/good 378 22.3 244 21.5 0.99 0.80, 1.21 1.02 0.82, 1.26
7–8/good 591 34.9 394 34.8 1.00 1.00
>8/good 207 12.2 154 13.6 1.10 0.86, 1.40 1.14 0.89, 1.47
<6/bad 142 8.4 94 8.3 1.03 0.77, 1.38 1.03 0.67, 1.39
6–7/bad 164 9.7 97 8.6 0.93 0.70, 1.24 0.90 0.67, 1.21
7–8/bad 90 5.3 67 5.9 1.13 0.80, 1.59 1.15 0.81, 1.63
>8/bad 25 1.5 14 1.2 0.82 0.42, 1.60 0.93 0.47, 1.83

Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; OR, odds ratio.


a
Adjusted for age, number of children, age at first birth, breastfeeding, menopausal status, use of hormone replacement therapy, duration of
use of hormone replacement therapy, alcohol consumption, comparative weight at age 30 years, ever use of melatonin, and physical activity.
b
One person was missing data for sleep quality.
c
Two people were missing data for creating a combined sleep duration-and-quality variable.

There was no evidence of any associations between sleep among duration of sleep on workdays (Table 5), sleep
duration (on either workdays or nonworkdays), sleep quality, quality (Table 6), and the risk of breast cancer also showed
or combined duration/quality and risk of breast cancer in no relationships. There was weak evidence to support the ap-
either the age-adjusted models or the fully adjusted models pearance of a U-shaped relationship between sleep duration
(Table 2). The fully adjusted odds ratios for the association and breast cancer risk when the analysis was restricted to
between sleep duration on workdays and breast cancer were women who were scored as vigorous types on the languid-
1.05 (95% confidence interval (CI): 0.82, 1.33) for <6 hours, ness/vigorousness scale. There was also weak evidence to
0.96 (95% CI: 0.80, 1.16) for 6–7 hours, and 1.10 (95% CI: suggest a trend of increasing risk with decreasing sleep
0.87, 1.39) for >8 hours, as compared with the reference cat- quality when the analysis was restricted to women who were
egory of 7–8 hours. Retaining the <5 and >9 hours-of-sleep scored as languid types on the languidness/vigorousness
groups in the logistic regression analysis did not appreciably scale.
alter the odds ratios or provide any evidence of trends. Age-stratified analysis did not provide any evidence of
Polytomous analysis examining differential risks for pre- differential risk by age (data not shown).
and postmenopausal cancer (Table 3) and estrogen receptor–
positive and –negative tumors (Table 4) also showed no Sensitivity analyses
association with sleep duration, sleep quality, or risk of
breast cancer subtype. Under the sensitivity analyses, 3 of the 7 scenarios pro-
Stratified analysis investigating the impact of circadian duced strong, statistically significant results. First, if we
rhythm (phase, amplitude, and stability) on the relationships assumed that all nonparticipants slept for 7–8 hours per

Am J Epidemiol. 2013;177(4):316–327
Self-reported Sleep and Breast Cancer Risk 321

Table 3. Adjusted Odds Ratios for the Associations Between Sleep Duration and Subjective Sleep Quality and the Risks of Pre- and
Postmenopausal Breast Cancer, Breast Cancer Environment and Employment Study, 2009–2011

Premenopausal Cases Postmenopausal Cases


Controls, % P Valueb
% AORa 95% CI % AORa 95% CI

Duration of sleep on workdays, hours (n = 2,828) 0.73


<6 14.0 11.4 1.00 0.62, 1.60 15.7 1.07 0.81, 1.40
6–7 32.1 28.2 1.11 0.79, 1.57 31.0 0.91 0.74, 1.13
7–8 40.2 43.0 1.00 39.6 1.00
>8 13.7 17.4 1.01 0.68, 1.51 13.7 1.17 0.88, 1.55
Duration of sleep on nonworkdays, hours (n = 2,828) 0.92
<6 9.8 7.7 1.02 0.58, 1.81 11.6 1.09 0.80, 1.48
6–7 21.8 15.1 0.90 0.59, 1.38 23.3 0.99 0.78, 1.25
7–8 39.5 42.2 1.00 40.0 1.00
>8 28.9 35.0 0.82 0.60, 1.13 25.1 1.00 0.80, 1.26

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Otago on September 22, 2015


Sleep quality (n = 2,827)c 0.80
Very good 24.8 27.1 1.00 23.8 1.00
Fairly good 50.3 51.3 1.04 0.75, 1.44 51.1 1.09 0.87, 1.36
Fairly bad 19.6 18.2 1.18 0.77, 1.82 18.4 0.91 0.69, 1.21
Very bad 5.3 3.4 1.04 0.46, 2.35 6.8 1.12 0.75, 1.68

Abbreviations: AOR, adjusted odds ratio; CI, confidence interval.


a
Adjusted for age, number of children, age at first birth, breastfeeding, use of hormone replacement therapy, duration of use of hormone
replacement therapy, alcohol consumption, comparative weight at age 30 years, ever use of melatonin, and physical activity.
b
P value for the difference between pre- and postmenopausal cancer risk.
c
One person was missing data for sleep quality.

Table 4. Adjusted Odds Ratiosa for the Associations Between Sleep Duration and Subjective Sleep Quality and the Risks of Estrogen
Receptor–Positive and Estrogen Receptor–Negative Breast Cancer, Breast Cancer Environment and Employment Study, 2009–2011

Estrogen Receptor– Estrogen Receptor–


Controls, % Positive Casesa Negative Casesa P Valueb
c c
% AOR 95% CI % AOR 95% CI

Duration of sleep on workdays, hours (n = 2,659)


<6 14.0 14.0 1.08 0.82, 1.41 13.8 0.99 0.59, 1.68 0.86
6–7 32.1 32.2 1.08 0.88, 1.32 28.3 0.94 0.62, 1.41
7–8 40.2 38.8 1.00 40.3 1.00
>8 13.7 15.0 1.17 0.90, 1.52 17.6 1.26 0.78, 2.04
Duration of sleep on nonworkdays, hours (n = 2,659)
<6 9.8 9.9 1.00 0.73, 1.36 12.0 1.30 0.74, 2.29 0.73
6–7 21.8 22.2 1.01 0.81, 1.27 17.6 0.90 0.56, 1.46
7–8 39.5 41.0 1.00 37.7 1.00
>8 28.9 26.8 0.88 0.71, 1.09 32.7 1.11 0.74, 1.66
Sleep quality (n = 2,658)
Very good 24.8 24.0 1.00 28.3 1.00 0.52
Fairly good 50.3 50.9 1.10 0.89, 1.37 48.8 0.88 0.59, 1.31
Fairly bad 19.6 19.7 1.10 0.84, 1.43 15.1 0.73 0.43, 1.25
Very bad 5.3 5.5 1.11 0.73, 1.68 8.2 1.49 0.75, 2.97

Abbreviations: AOR, adjusted odds ratio; CI, confidence interval.


a
A total of 169 women with breast cancer did not have estrogen receptor status recorded on their Western Australian Cancer Registry
records.
b
P value for the difference between estrogen receptor–positive and estrogen receptor–negative cancer risk.
c
Adjusted for age, number of children, age at first birth, breastfeeding, menopausal status, use of hormone replacement therapy, duration of
use of hormone replacement therapy, alcohol consumption, comparative weight at age 30 years, ever use of melatonin, and physical activity.

Am J Epidemiol. 2013;177(4):316–327
322 Girschik et al.

Table 5. Adjusted Odds Ratios for the Association Between Duration of Sleep on Workdays and Breast Cancer
Risk According to 3 Characteristics of Circadian Rhythm,a Breast Cancer Environment and Employment Study,
2009–2011

Circadian Type Controls Cases


Adjusted 95% Confidence
and Duration of
No. % No. % Odds Ratiob Interval
Sleep, hours

Phase (Morningness/Eveningness) (n = 2,811) c


Neither type
<6 111 12.7 79 14.1 1.26 0.89, 1.79
6–7 283 32.3 176 31.4 1.08 0.83, 1.40
7–8 373 42.5 233 41.6 1.00
>8 110 12.5 72 12.9 1.15 0.80, 1.63
Evening type
<6 57 16.6 35 15.0 0.66 0.38, 1.15
6–7 123 35.9 71 30.3 0.66 0.43, 1.02

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Otago on September 22, 2015


7–8 125 36.4 98 41.9 1.00
>8 38 11.1 30 12.8 0.87 0.48, 1.59
Morning type
<6 70 15.0 47 14.2 0.99 0.63, 1.57
6–7 135 29.0 92 27.8 0.98 0.68, 1.42
7–8 180 38.6 127 38.4 1.00
>8 81 17.4 65 19.6 1.16 0.76, 1.78
Amplitude (Languidness/Vigorousness) (n = 2,808) c
Neither type
<6 121 14.9 76 13.1 0.83 0.58, 1.18
6–7 299 36.7 184 31.8 0.82 0.63, 1.06
7–8 301 37.0 240 41.5 1.00
>8 93 11.4 79 13.6 1.19 0.83, 1.70
Vigorous type
<6 55 17.2 47 21.6 1.78 1.02, 3.09
6–7 108 33.9 72 33.0 1.21 0.76, 1.93
7–8 120 37.6 67 30.7 1.00
>8 36 11.3 32 14.7 1.70 0.90, 3.21
Languid type
<6 58 10.6 38 11.6 1.12 0.69, 1.83
6–7 133 24.2 82 25.0 1.02 0.71, 1.46
7–8 256 46.6 151 46.0 1.00
>8 103 18.7 57 17.4 0.90 0.60, 1.35
Table continues

night, the age-adjusted odds ratio for breast cancer among sleepers. Third, if we assumed that nonconsenters slept for
women who slept less than 6 hours per night was 1.74 less than 6 hours while nonresponders slept 7–8 hours, the
(95% CI: 1.42, 2.12); the odds ratio for those who slept 6– age-adjusted odds ratio for less than 6 hours was 1.03 (95%
7 hours was 1.55 (95% CI: 1.34, 1.79); and the odds ratio CI: 0.90, 1.17); the odds ratio for 6–7 hours was 1.48 (95%
for those who slept more than 8 hours was 1.78 (95% CI: CI: 1.27, 1.72); and the odds ratio for more than 8 hours
1.45, 2.18), as compared with the reference category of 7–8 was 1.70 (95% CI: 1.38, 2.09), compared with the refer-
hours. Second, if we assumed that all nonparticipants were ence category of 7–8 hours. None of the other sensitivity
very good sleepers, the age-adjusted odds ratio for fairly scenarios were statistically significant.
good sleepers was 1.76 (95% CI: 1.54, 1.97); the odds ratio Misclassification of sleep duration on workdays and the
for fairly bad sleepers was 1.63 (95% CI: 1.36, 1.96); and sleep quality of 10%, 20%, and 50% of participants were
the odds ratio for very bad sleepers was 1.90 (95% CI: investigated, but the odds ratios for risk of breast cancer
1.38, 2.62), compared with reference group of very good were not appreciably altered (data not shown).

Am J Epidemiol. 2013;177(4):316–327
Self-reported Sleep and Breast Cancer Risk 323

Table 5. Continued

Circadian Type Controls Cases


Adjusted 95% Confidence
and Duration of
No. % No. % Odds Ratiob Interval
Sleep, hours

Stability (Flexibility/Rigidity) (n = 2,807) c


Neither type
<6 109 12.1 75 13.0 1.04 0.73, 1.49
6–7 292 32.5 155 26.9 0.83 0.64, 1.08
7–8 374 41.7 257 44.5 1.00
>8 123 13.7 90 15.6 1.07 0.77, 1.49
Rigid type
<6 31 8.6 28 10.6 1.48 0.79, 2.77
6–7 87 24.0 80 30.3 1.31 0.86, 1.99
7–8 166 45.9 112 42.4 1.00

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Otago on September 22, 2015


>8 78 21.5 44 16.7 0.74 0.46, 1.20
Flexible type
<6 94 22.2 58 20.6 0.87 0.55, 1.38
6–7 162 38.2 104 36.9 0.93 0.63, 1.36
7–8 137 32.3 90 31.9 1.00
>8 31 7.3 30 10.6 1.44 0.78, 2.65
a
Phase (morningness/eveningness), amplitude (languidness or vigorousness), and stability (the flexibility or
rigidity of sleeping habits).
b
Adjusted for age, number of children, age at first birth, breastfeeding, menopausal status, use of hormone
replacement therapy, duration of use of hormone replacement therapy, alcohol consumption, comparative weight at
age 30 years, ever use of melatonin, physical activity, country of birth, highest level of education, family history of
breast cancer, previous noncancerous breast conditions, use of oral contraceptives in the last 5 years, and ever
smoking tobacco.
c
Seventeen participants were missing data on circadian phase, 20 were missing data on amplitude, and 21
were missing data on stability.

DISCUSSION trend of increasing risk with increasing sleep duration.


While the trend reported by McElroy et al. was statistically
This study found no association between self-reported significant, it may not be clinically significant; the odds
sleep duration on workdays, sleep duration on nonworkdays, ratio was 1.01 (95% CI: 0.84, 1.23) for the longest duration
subjective sleep quality, or combined duration and quality compared with the reference group (24).
and risk of breast cancer. Polytomous analysis examining Among the 5 studies described above, 4 found stronger
differential risks for breast cancer by menopause and the es- results in subanalyses (22, 24–26). When analyses were re-
trogen receptor status of cases also showed no association of stricted to stable sleepers, Verkasalo et al. (22) reported a
sleep duration or quality with risk of breast cancer. In addi- statistically significant decreasing trend between sleep dura-
tion, stratification by circadian rhythm showed no association tion and breast cancer risk, while Pinheiro et al. (25) reported
between sleep duration and breast cancer, although there was a statistically significant increasing trend. Wu et al. (26)
weak evidence for a possible association between sleep dura- found a statistically significant trend of decreasing breast
tion and breast cancer in women scored as more vigorous cancer risk with increasing sleep duration in postmenopaus-
types on the languidness/vigorousness scale and between al women, but this trend did not differ significantly from
sleep quality and breast cancer in women scored as more that in premenopausal cases. The very small increasing risk
languid types on the scale. reported by McElroy et al. became stronger in cases with
These results are consistent with those of a number of more advanced disease in polytomous models, but the com-
other studies. In a study of subjective sleep quality and parison between localized and regional/distant cases was
breast cancer risk, Verkasalo et al. (22) also found no asso- not statistically significant (24).
ciation. Three of 5 studies that investigated the relationship Previous studies have not assessed the role of circadian
between sleep duration and breast cancer (22, 25, 26) rhythm in breast cancer risk. If the associations found in
found no relationship, while the 2 remaining studies pro- this study are true, they are somewhat contradictory. On
duced conflicting results (23, 24). Kakizaki et al. (23) re- the languidness/vigorousness scale, languidness is associated
ported a statistically significant trend of decreasing risk, with higher levels of impairment following disruption of
while McElroy et al. (24) reported a statistically significant sleep in comparison with vigorousness (32, 33). It could

Am J Epidemiol. 2013;177(4):316–327
324 Girschik et al.

Table 6. Adjusted Odds Ratios for the Association Between Quality of Sleep and Breast Cancer Risk According
to 3 Characteristics of Circadian Rhythm,a Breast Cancer Environment and Employment Study, 2009–2011

Circadian Type and Controls Cases Adjusted Odds 95% Confidence


Quality of Sleep No. % No. % Ratiob Interval

Phase (Morningness/Eveningness) (n = 2,810) c


Neither type
Very good 196 22.4 125 22.3 1.00
Fairly good 470 53.7 305 54.5 1.12 0.85, 1.49
Fairly bad 163 18.6 97 17.3 1.01 0.71, 1.45
Very bad 46 5.3 33 5.9 1.37 0.81, 2.32
Evening type
Very good 73 21.3 54 23.0 1.00
Fairly good 161 46.9 100 42.5 0.89 0.55, 1.42
Fairly bad 83 24.2 63 26.8 1.13 0.66, 1.92

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Otago on September 22, 2015


Very bad 26 7.6 18 7.7 0.87 0.40, 1.88
Morning type
Very good 148 31.8 102 30.8 1.00
Fairly good 218 46.8 168 50.8 1.12 0.80, 1.59
Fairly bad 82 17.6 47 14.2 0.83 0.52, 1.32
Very bad 18 3.9 14 4.2 0.98 0.44, 2.16
Amplitude (Languidness/Vigorousness) (n = 2,807) c
Neither type
Very good 185 22.8 149 25.7 1.00
Fairly good 418 51.5 306 52.8 0.96 0.73, 1.26
Fairly bad 160 19.7 95 16.4 0.75 0.53, 1.07
Very bad 49 6.03 30 5.2 0.78 0.46, 1.33
Vigorous type
Very good 90 28.2 56 25.7 1.00
Fairly good 151 47.3 107 49.1 1.07 0.67, 1.70
Fairly bad 60 18.8 36 16.5 1.00 0.55, 1.82
Very bad 18 5.6 19 8.7 1.74 0.73, 4.18
Languid type
Very good 143 26.0 75 22.9 1.00
Fairly good 278 50.6 162 49.4 1.22 0.85, 1.75
Fairly bad 107 19.5 75 22.9 1.39 0.90, 2.16
Very bad 22 4.0 16 4.9 1.46 0.67, 3.17
Table continues

be hypothesized that these impairments in languid types of as compared with “usual” sleep duration) and whether the
persons may extend to physiological processes associated analysis was restricted by participant trait.
with the development of cancer. We saw a weak associa- The strengths of this large population-based case-control
tion between sleep quality and breast cancer risk in study include distinguishing between sleep durations on
languid types which appears to support this. However, workdays and nonworkdays and including subjective sleep
under this hypothesis, one would not expect our finding of quality, which has been investigated in only 1 other study. In
a (weak) increased risk with duration of sleep for vigorous addition, our study is the first (to our knowledge) to have
types. investigated the role of circadian rhythm in breast cancer risk.
It is unclear why studies of sleep duration and breast One weakness of this study was that sleep duration and
cancer have found such conflicting results. Differing data sleep quality were self-reported, and misclassification was
collection and analysis methods may go some way toward possible. While the questionnaire we used has been shown to
explaining this. In particular, the studies differed in terms be reliable (45), it showed only poor validity in comparison
of the information they collected (“current” sleep duration with wrist actigraphy in a small validation study (43). The

Am J Epidemiol. 2013;177(4):316–327
Self-reported Sleep and Breast Cancer Risk 325

Table 6. Continued

Circadian Type and Controls Cases Adjusted Odds 95% Confidence


Quality of Sleep No. % No. % Ratiob Interval

Stability (Flexibility/Rigidity) (n = 2,806) c


Neither type
Very good 214 23.9 140 24.3 1.00
Fairly good 461 51.5 314 54.4 1.08 0.82, 1.41
Fairly bad 185 20.7 98 17.0 0.84 0.60, 1.19
Very bad 36 4.0 25 4.3 1.14 0.64, 2.04
Rigid type
Very good 95 26.2 68 25.8 1.00
Fairly good 188 51.9 133 50.4 1.08 0.72, 1.63
Fairly bad 60 16.6 53 20.1 1.39 0.82, 2.33

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Otago on September 22, 2015


Very bad 19 5.3 10 3.8 1.04 0.41, 2.62
Flexible type
Very good 110 25.9 69 24.4 1.00
Fairly good 195 46.0 129 45.8 1.07 0.72, 1.60
Fairly bad 85 20.1 57 20.1 1.13 0.69, 1.83
Very bad 34 8.0 28 9.9 1.37 0.72, 2.59
a
Phase (morningness/eveningness), amplitude (languidness or vigorousness), and stability (the flexibility or
rigidity of sleeping habits).
b
Adjusted for age, number of children, age at first birth, breastfeeding, menopausal status, use of hormone
replacement therapy, duration of use of hormone replacement therapy, alcohol consumption, comparative weight at
age 30 years, ever use of melatonin, and physical activity.
c
Seventeen participants were missing data on circadian phase, 20 were missing data on amplitude, and 21
were missing data on stability.

lack of association between subjectively and objectively mea- the assumptions on which these 3 scenarios were based were
sured sleep has been recognized as a problem and is an the most conservative.
ongoing issue, particularly for epidemiologists, for whom Recall bias is less likely to have affected the results, since
subjectively measured sleep is the only practical option in sleep is not widely recognized by the public as a possible
large studies. While it is possible that a true association in risk factor for cancer, and in order for recall bias to have pro-
this study might have been masked by misclassification, our duced the results found in this study, cases would have had
analyses of the potential effect of exposure misclassification to be underreporting and/or controls overreporting exposure
showed that the odds ratios were not appreciably altered. to short/long sleep duration and poor sleep quality.
The low response fraction, particularly for controls, had In summary, this study does not provide evidence to
the potential to introduce selection bias into the study. Selec- support an association between self-reported sleep duration
tion bias could account for these results if nonparticipants or quality and the risk of breast cancer. The ongoing issue
experienced different sleep duration and quality than partici- of how best to measure sleep in epidemiologic studies
pants. Sensitivity analysis found 3 scenarios under which the needs to be addressed.
results would provide evidence for an association. The age-
adjusted odds ratios under the assumption that all nonpar-
ticipants slept for 7–8 hours on workdays showed a strong
U-shaped distribution. The assumption that nonconsenters ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
all slept for less than 6 hours and that nonresponders all
slept 7–8 hours on workdays also showed results that were Author affiliations: Western Australian Institute for
statistically significant, although the U-shaped distribution Medical Research, The University of Western Australia,
was not as strong. The age-adjusted odds ratios under the Nedlands, Western Australia, Australia (Jennifer Girschik,
assumption that all nonparticipants were very good sleepers Lin Fritschi); and School of Population Health, The Uni-
showed an increasing trend with decreasing quality. versity of Western Australia, Crawley, Western Australia,
However, this relationship was not present under the assump- Australia (Jennifer Girschik, Jane Heyworth).
tion that all nonparticipants were only fairly good sleepers. This work was supported by the Australian National
While this analysis highlights the potential for selection bias, Health and Medical Research Council (project grant 572530).

Am J Epidemiol. 2013;177(4):316–327
326 Girschik et al.

Jennifer Girschik was supported by The University of Western 15. Knutson KL, Spiegel K, Penev P, et al. The metabolic
Australia Hackett PhD Scholarship and a Cancer Council consequences of sleep deprivation. Sleep Med Rev. 2007;
Western Australia PhD top-up scholarship. Prof. Lin Fritschi 11(3):163–178.
was supported by a National Health and Medical Research 16. Stone KL, Ewing SK, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Self-reported
sleep and nap habits and risk of mortality in a large cohort of
Council Fellowship.
older women. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2009;57(4):604–611.
We thank Dr. Allyson Thomson, Pierra Rogers, Dr. 17. Kojima M, Wakai K, Kawamura T, et al. Sleep patterns and
Christobel Saunders, Dr. Deborah Glass, Dr. Thomas total mortality: a 12-year follow-up study in Japan.
Erren, Terry Slevin, Dr. Kristan Aronson, Terry Boyle, J Epidemiol. 2000;10(2):87–93.
Sonia el Zaemey, and Ann d’Orsogna for their valuable 18. Hublin C, Partinen M, Koskenvuo M, et al. Sleep and
contribution to the Breast Cancer Environment and Em- mortality: a population-based 22-year follow-up study. Sleep.
ployment Study. 2007;30(10):1245–1253.
Conflict of interest: none declared. 19. Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, eds. Principles and
Practice of Sleep Medicine. 4th ed. Philadelphia, PA:
Saunders/Elsevier; 2005.
20. Stevens RG. Electric power use and breast cancer: a
hypothesis. Am J Epidemiol. 1987;125(4):556–561.
REFERENCES 21. Stevens RG, Blask DE, Brainard GC, et al. Meeting report:

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Otago on September 22, 2015


the role of environmental lighting and circadian disruption in
1. Australian Institute of Health and Welfare. Cancer in cancer and other diseases. Environ Health Perspect. 2007;
Australia: actual incidence and mortality data from 1982 to 115(9):1357–1362.
2007 and projections to 2010. Asia Pac J Clin Oncol. 2011; 22. Verkasalo PK, Lillberg K, Stevens RG, et al. Sleep duration
7(4):325–338. and breast cancer: a prospective cohort study. Cancer Res.
2. Colditz G, Baer I, Tamimi R. Breast cancer. In: 2005;65(20):9595–9600.
Schottenfeld D, Fraumeni J, eds. Cancer Epidemiology and 23. Kakizaki M, Kuriyama S, Sone T, et al. Sleep duration and
Prevention. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; the risk of breast cancer: the Ohsaki Cohort Study. Br
2006:995–1012. J Cancer. 2008;99(9):1502–1505.
3. Kronholm E, Partonen T, Laatikainen T, et al. Trends in self- 24. McElroy J, Newcomb P, Titus-Ernstoff L, et al. Duration of
reported sleep duration and insomnia-related symptoms in sleep and breast cancer risk in a large population-based case-
Finland from 1972 to 2005: a comparative review and re- control study. J Sleep Res. 2006;15(3):241–249.
analysis of Finnish population samples. J Sleep Res. 2008; 25. Pinheiro SP, Schernhammer ES, Tworoger SS, et al. A
17(1):54–62. prospective study on habitual duration of sleep and incidence
4. Pires M, Benedito-Silva A, Mello M, et al. Sleep habits of breast cancer in a large cohort of women. Cancer Res.
and complaints of adults in the city of Sao Paulo, Brazil, in 2006;66(10):5521–5525.
1987 and 1995. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2007;40(11): 26. Wu AH, Wang R, Koh W-P, et al. Sleep duration, melatonin
1505–1515. and breast cancer among Chinese women in Singapore.
5. Knutson K, Van Cauter E, Rathouz PJ, et al. Trends in the Carcinogenesis. 2008;29(6):1244–1248.
prevalence of short sleepers in the USA: 1975–2006. Sleep. 27. Lichstein KL. Epidemiology of Sleep: Age, Gender, and
2010;33(1):37–45. Ethnicity. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2004.
6. Dollman J, Ridley K, Olds T, et al. Trends in the duration of 28. Gehrman P, Matt GE, Turingan M, et al. Towards an
school-day sleep among 10- to 15-year-old South Australians understanding of self-reports of sleep. J Sleep Res. 2002;
between 1985 and 2004. Acta Paediatr. 2007;96(7):1011–1014. 11(3):229–236.
7. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. QuickStats: 29. Wang Z. Two postestimation commands for assessing
percent of adults who reported an average of <6 hrs sleep, confounding effect in epidemiological studies. Stata J. 2007;
1985 and 2006. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2008; 7(2):183–196.
57(8):209. 30. Srinivasan V, Spence DW, Pandi-Perumal SR, et al.
8. Bin YS, Marshall NS, Glozier N. Secular trends in adult Melatonin, environmental light, and breast cancer. Breast
sleep duration: a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2012; Cancer Res Treat. 2008;108(3):339–350.
16(3):223–230. 31. Hill S, Blask D, Xiang S, et al. Melatonin and associated
9. Bin YS, Marshall NS, Glozier NS. Secular changes in sleep signaling pathways that control normal breast epithelium and
duration among Australian adults, 1992–2006. Med J Aust. breast cancer. J Mammary Gland Biol Neoplasia. 2011;
2011;195(11-12):670–672. 16(3):235–245.
10. Byles JE, Mishra GD, Harris MA, et al. The problems of 32. Di Milia L, Smith PA, Folkard S. Refining the psychometric
sleep for older women: changes in health outcomes. Age properties of the Circadian Type Inventory. Pers Individ Dif.
Ageing. 2003;32(2):154–163. 2004;36(8):1953–1964.
11. Knutsson A. Health disorders of shift workers. Occup Med. 33. Di Milia L, Smith PA, Folkard S. A validation of the revised
2003;53(2):103–108. Circadian Type Inventory in a working sample. Pers Individ
12. Marshall NS, Glozier N, Grunstein RR. Is sleep duration Dif. 2005;39(7):1293–1305.
related to obesity? A critical review of the epidemiological 34. Horne J, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to
evidence. Sleep Med Rev. 2008;12(4):289–298. determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian
13. Wingard DL, Berkman LF. Mortality risk associated with rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. 1976;4(2):97–110.
sleeping patterns among adults. Sleep. 1983;6(2):102–107. 35. Taillard J, Philip P, Chastang J, et al. Validation of Horne and
14. Kakizaki M, Inoue K, Kuriyama S, et al. Sleep duration Östberg morningness-eveningness questionnaire in a middle-
and the risk of prostate cancer: the Ohsaki Cohort Study. aged population of French workers. J Biol Rhythms. 2004;
Br J Cancer. 2008;99(1):176–178. 19(1):76–86.

Am J Epidemiol. 2013;177(4):316–327
Self-reported Sleep and Breast Cancer Risk 327

36. Volk S, Dyroff J, Georgi K, et al. Subjective sleepiness 40. Rothman KJ, Greenland S, Lash TL. Modern
and physiological sleep tendency in healthy young Epidemiology. 3rd ed. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott-Raven;
morning and evening subjects. J Sleep Res. 1994;3(3): 1998.
138–143. 41. Marshall J, Priore R, Haughey B, et al. Spouse-subject
37. Australian Bureau of Statistics. SEIFA: Socio-Economic interviews and the reliability of diet studies. Am J Epidemiol.
Indexes for Areas. Canberra, Australia: Australian Bureau of 1980;112(5):675–683.
Statistics; 2006. (http://www.abs.gov.au/websitedbs/ 42. Marshall JR, Priore R, Graham S, et al. On the distortion of
D3310114.nsf/home/Seifa_entry_page). (Accessed September risk estimates in multiple exposure level case-control studies.
26, 2011). Am J Epidemiol. 1981;113(4):464–473.
38. Australian Government Department of Health and Ageing. 43. Girschik J, Fritschi L, Heyworth J, et al. Validation of self-
Measuring Remoteness: Accessibility/Remoteness Index of reported sleep against actigraphy. J Epidemiol. 2012;22(5):
Australia. Canberra, Australia: Australian Department of 462–468.
Health and Ageing; 2001. 44. Patel SR, Ayas NT, Malhotra MR, et al. A prospective study
39. Ohayon MM, Carskadon MA, Guilleminault C, et al. Meta- of sleep duration and mortality risk in women. Sleep. 2004;
analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to 27(3):440–444.
old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep 45. Girschik J, Heyworth J, Fritschi L. Reliability of a sleep
values across the human lifespan. Sleep. 2004;27(7): quality questionnaire for use in epidemiologic studies.
1255–1273. J Epidemiol. 2012;22(3):244–250.

Downloaded from http://aje.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Otago on September 22, 2015

Am J Epidemiol. 2013;177(4):316–327

You might also like