2005 The Age-Dependent Decline of ADHD - A Meta-Analysis of Follow-Up Studies

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Psychological Medicine, 2006, 36, 159–165.

f 2005 Cambridge University Press


doi:10.1017/S003329170500471X First published online 3 May 2005. Printed in the United Kingdom

REVIEW ARTICLE

The age-dependent decline of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder:


a meta-analysis of follow-up studies
S T E P H E N V. F A R A O N E 1 *, J O S E P H B I E D E R M A N 2, 3 AND E R I C M I C K 2,3
1
Medical Genetics Research Program and Department of Psychiatry, SUNY Upstate Medical University,
Syracuse, NY, USA ; 2 Pediatric Psychopharmacology Unit of the Child Psychiatry Service, Massachusetts
General Hospital, Boston, MA, USA; 3 Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA

ABSTRACT
Background. This study examined the persistence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD) into adulthood.
Method. We analyzed data from published follow-up studies of ADHD. To be included in the
analysis, these additional studies had to meet the following criteria : the study included a control
group and it was clear from the methods if the diagnosis of ADHD included subjects who did not
meet full criteria but showed residual and impairing signs of the disorder. We used a meta-analysis
regression model to separately assess the syndromatic and symptomatic persistence of ADHD.
Results. When we define only those meeting full criteria for ADHD as having ‘ persistent ADHD’,
the rate of persistence is low, y15 % at age 25 years. But when we include cases consistent with
DSM-IV’s definition of ADHD in partial remission, the rate of persistence is much higher, y65 %.
Conclusions. Our results show that estimates of ADHD’s persistence rely heavily on how one defines
persistence. Yet, regardless of definition, our analyses show that evidence for ADHD lessens with
age. More work is needed to determine if this reflects true remission of ADHD symptoms or is due
to the developmental insensitivity of diagnostic criteria for the disorder.

INTRODUCTION should be considered a valid disorder (Barkley,


1997; Faraone, 2000 ; Faraone et al. 2000).
In contrast to most other psychiatric disorders,
The idea that ADHD is rare in adulthood was
investigators and clinicians debate the validity of
given credibility by Hill & Schoener (1996) who
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
fit a mathematical model to the rates of persist-
in adults. Some assert that most cases of ADHD
ence reported by studies that had followed
remit in adulthood ; they conclude that adult
ADHD children from childhood to adolescence
ADHD is very rare (Shaffer, 1994 ; Hill &
Schoener, 1996). If so, most referred cases must or adulthood. Their model, which predicted an
exponential decline in the rate of ADHD, esti-
be due to referral artifacts or missed differential
mated the rate of adult ADHD to range from
diagnoses. Others claim that many cases of
ADHD persist into adulthood and that, even 0.8 % at age 20 to 0.05 % at age 40 years. Given
that long-term outcome studies are the touch-
when its childhood age at onset is established
stone for judging the course of a disorder, these
by retrospective self-reports, ADHD in adults
results appeared to provide strong support for
the idea that ADHD is not a lifelong disorder.
* Address for correspondence: Dr Stephen Faraone, Department Although the current rate of residual ADHD
of Psychiatry, SUNY Upstate Medical University, 750 East Adams
St., Syracuse, NY 13210, USA. was available for some of the studies available
(Email : faraones@upstate.edu) to them, they only analyzed the persistence rates
159
160 S. V. Faraone et al.

defined by meeting full criteria for the disorder. prognosis, we completed an analysis of ADHD
This method counts as ‘ remitted’ former outcome studies including both studies of
ADHD children who continue to have impair- syndromatic persistence and symptomatic per-
ing symptoms, despite failing to meet all diag- sistence.
nostic criteria (cases that DSM-IV would code
as ADHD in partial remission). Thus, Hill and
Schoener studied what Keck et al. (1998) refer METHOD
to as syndromatic persistence, i.e. the main- Selection of studies
tenance of full diagnostic status. They did not
address symptomatic persistence, i.e. the main- We used a Medline search of the scientific
tenance of partial diagnostic status with im- literature to identify outcome studies of ADHD.
pairment. To be included in the analysis, these studies had
Although studies of syndromatic persistence to meet the following criteria : the study in-
are useful for understanding the natural history cluded a control group and it was clear from the
of a disorder, clinicians also need studies of methods if the diagnosis of ADHD was made
symptomatic persistence to fully understand the based on using full criteria or with modified
prognosis of a disorder. For example, if follow- criteria that required some ADHD symptoms
up studies of schizophrenia had only reported and evidence of residual and impairing signs of
rates of syndromatic persistence, the prognosis the disorder. All the studies reported in Hill &
of schizophrenia would seem much better than Schoener’s (1996) review met these criteria.
we know it to be from studies of symptomatic For each study selected for analysis, Table 1
persistence. Indeed, after their initial psychotic shows the number of subjects in the study, the
episode is stabilized, many schizophrenic pa- mean age of the study group at baseline, the diag-
tients continue to show disabling symptoms nostic system used to define ADHD, the mean
even though they do not meet criteria for the age at follow-up, and the number and per cent
disorder. Although this issue is relevant for most of cases that showed ADHD at follow-up.
psychiatric disorders, it is especially relevant Horizontal lines in the table group together
for ADHD given the ongoing debate over the studies that report data from the same sample at
validity of this diagnosis in adults. different follow-up intervals. An asterisk in the
Our prior research suggest that studies of last column indicates studies that used a residual
syndromatic persistence may give an overly diagnosis of ADHD. These studies did not re-
optimistic view of the long-term outcome of quire subjects to meet full diagnostic criteria for
ADHD (Biederman et al. 2000). We found that, ADHD at the follow-up assessment. Instead,
at the age of 19 years, 38% of children had the they diagnosed ADHD if the subject showed
full ADHD diagnosis, 72 % would show per- impairing signs of the disorder.
sistence of at least one-third of the symptoms
required for the diagnosis and 90 % showed Statistical analysis
evidence of clinically significant impairment. To analyze the persistence rates in Table 1, we
Thus, Hill & Schoener’s (1996) review of used a meta-analysis binomial-logit regression
ADHD follow-up studies may have provided an model, which modeled the persistence rate of
overly optimistic view of the prognosis of each study as a function of age at follow-up
ADHD. Providing an accurate understanding baseline age, and diagnostic approach (full
of the persistence of ADHD has important versus residual). We also included interactions
clinical implications. If, based on Hill & of predictors with follow-up age. The analysis
Schoener’s (1996) review, clinicians assume weighted each study by the number of subjects
ADHD is rare in adulthood, they are likely to recruited at the baseline assessment. The data in
view potential cases with skepticism. In con- Table 1 include multiple reports from several
trast, the view that adult ADHD is a prevalent sites. When there is more than one report for
disorder would alert clinicians that the diagnosis a single site, the data correspond to either
should be considered in adults presenting with different follow-up ages or different diagnostic
histories of ADHD-like symptoms. To clarify approaches (full versus subthreshold diag-
this issue and provide a more complete view of noses). Data points from the same site are not
The age-dependent decline of ADHD 161

Table 1. Prospective follow-up studies of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)


Age range or
mean at Age at ADHD
baseline Diagnostic system follow-up persistence

Study n Mean Recruitment Follow-up Mean n %

Mendelson et al. (1971) 83 9.9 DSM-II a


DSM-II a
13.4 42 50
Borland & Heckman (1976) 20 7.5 DSM-IIa DSM-IIa 30.4 10 50*

Mannuzza & Gittelman (1984) 36 7.9 DSM-II DSM-III 17.4 12 33


Mannuzza & Gittelman (1984) 36 7.9 DSM-II DSM-III 17.4 13 36*
Gittelman et al. (1985) 101 9.3 DSM-II DSM-III 18.3 31 31
Gittelman et al. (1985) 101 9.3 DSM-II DSM-III 18.3 40 40*
Mannuzza et al. (1991) 94 7.3 DSM-II DSM-III 18.5 21 22
Mannuzza et al. (1991) 94 7.3 DSM-II DSM-III 18.5 41 43*
Mannuzza et al. (1993) 91 9.3 DSM-II DSM-III, IIIR 25.5 7 8
Mannuzza et al. (1993) 91 9.3 DSM-II DSM-III, IIIR 25.5 10 11*
Mannuzza et al. (1998) 85 7.3 DSM-II DSM-IIIR 24.1 3 4
Mannuzza et al. (1998) 85 7.3 DSM-II DSM-IIIR 24.1 3 4*

Lambert et al. (1987) 59 7.7 DSM-II DSM-III 14.3 25 43


Lambert (1988) 59 9.3 DSM-II DSM-III 18.3 47 80*

Barkley et al. (1990) 123 4–12 DSM-IIIRb DSM-IIIR 14.9 88 72


Barkley et al. (1990) 123 4–12 DSM-IIIRb DSM-IIIR 14.9 102 83*
Barkley et al. (2002) 147 4–12 DSM-IIIRb DSM-IV 21.1 78 58
Barkley et al. (2002) 147 4–12 DSM-IIIRb DSM-IV 21.1 89 66*

Feldman et al. (1979) 81 10.0 DSM-IIa DSM-IIa 15.5 35 43


August et al. (1983) 22 10.7 DSM-IIa DSM-III 14.2 19 86*
Weiss et al. (1985) 63 6–12 DSM-IIa DSM-III 25.1 42 66*
Cantwell & Baker (1989) 35 5.5 DSM-III DSM-III 9.7 28 80
Offord et al. (1992) 48 4–12 DSM-III DSM-III 8–16 16 34

Hart et al. (1995) 106 9.4 DSM-IIIR DSM-IIIR 10.4 89 77


Hart et al. (1995) 106 9.4 DSM-IIIR DSM-IIIR 11.4 90 85
Hart et al. (1995) 106 9.4 DSM-IIIR DSM-IIIR 12.4 92 84

Claude & Firestone (1995) 52 7.3 DSM-III DSM-IIIR 19.7 26 50

Biederman et al. (1996) 128 10.5 DSM-IIIR DSM-IIIR 14.5 109 85*
Biederman et al. (1996) 128 10.5 DSM-IIIR DSM-IIIR 14.5 78 61

Rasmussen & Gillberg (2000) 50 7 DSM-IIIc DSM-IV 22 28 56*


Rasmussen & Gillberg (2000) 50 7 DSM-IIIc DSM-IV 22 24 48

Yan (1996) 197 10.0 DSM-IIa DSM-IIIRd 25.5 140 70*

Studies grouped within horizontal lines report data from the same sample at different follow-up intervals.
* Residual ADHD diagnosis.
a
Diagnostic system not stated but completed in DSM-II era.
b
Diagnoses shown to be equivalent to DSM-III-R.
c
Diagnoses shown to be equivalent to DSM-III.
d
Diagnostic system not stated but completed in DSM-III-R era.

statistically independent of one another. If not to distributional assumptions. All computations


addressed, non-independence leads to inaccu- used STATA 8.0 (Stata Corporation, 1992).
rate estimates of statistical significance. To deal
with this issue, the variance estimates were
adjusted using Huber’s formula (Huber, 1967), RESULTS
a ‘theoretical bootstrap ’ that produces robust We first evaluated the estimates of persistence
statistical tests. The method works by entering from Table 1 for outliers. Data were stratified
the site scores (i.e. sum of scores within sites) by diagnostic criterion used at follow-up
into the formula for the estimate of variance. (residual or full diagnostic status) and age at
The resulting F statistics and p values are robust follow-up (child/adolescent, <18 years of age ;
162 S. V. Faraone et al.

0 F F RR
R
0·8 F
F
F
R R
Log of persistence

–1 0·6 F R F R
F F F R
F F R
0·4 R
R
F F F
–2
0·2 F

F
–3

Rate of persistence
R
0·0
Full Residual Full Residual 10 15 20 25 30
Child/Adolescent Adult Age at follow-up
FIG. 1. Identification of outliers in extant data.
F F RR
R
0·8 F
F
F
or adult, o18 years of age). Fig. 1 shows evi- R R
dence for statistical outliers in two samples 0·6 F R F R
reporting residual diagnoses. The points in F F F R
Fig. 1 that fall far outside the range covered 0·4 F F R
R
R
by the standard deviation correspond to the F F F
rate of residual diagnoses at follow-up in the 0·2 F
Mannuzza et al. (1993, 1998) publications.
Thus, our subsequent analyses were conducted R
F

both with and without these data. 0·0


Using all studies (including the two outliers) 10 15 20 25 30
we found a statistically significant association Age at follow-up
between age at assessment and the rate of per- FIG. 2. Persistence of full (F) and residual diagnosis (R) of ADHD.
sistence for the full [odds ratio (OR) 0.83, 95% (a) All studies ; (b) excluding outliers. Predicted rate of persistence for
full diagnoses (–––) and residual diagnoses (– – –).
confidence interval (CI) 0.74–0.93; Wald’s
x2=10.3, df=1, p=0.001) but not the residual
(OR 0.89, 95 % CI 0.78–1.03; Wald’s x2=2.5, persistence of ADHD symptoms associated
df=1, p=0.1) diagnostic criterion. We did not with a 1-year change in age. Thus, the prob-
find a statistically significant interaction be- ability of an individual with ADHD meeting full
tween follow-up age and diagnostic approach criteria for ADHD 1 year later is 83 % while the
(Wald’s x2=0.8, df=1, p=0.4). Fig. 2 shows the probability that that individual would continue
nature of symptom decline stratified by follow- to have residual symptoms of the disorder 1 year
up diagnostic criterion. Actual data points are later is 96 %.
plotted with the letter ‘ F ’ for studies using full The fit of our models improved when strat-
diagnoses and ‘R ’ for studies using residual ifying by outliers. The correlation between actual
diagnoses. The two fitted curves graph the persistence rates and the persistence rates pre-
predicted values from the regression models for dicted by the model was 0.62 when we included
full and residual diagnoses. As Fig. 2 a shows, outliers (p<0.001) and 0.76 when outliers were
studies using a full threshold diagnosis of accounted for (p<0.001). We found no signifi-
ADHD reported a greater remission at each cant effects for either baseline age (z=1.3,
age than studies using a residual diagnosis. p=0.2) or its interaction with follow-up age
When the statistical outliers were removed from (z=0.15, p=0.9).
the estimate of age-dependent persistence of the
residual diagnosis (Fig. 2 b) the slope of the age
DISCUSSION
decline is reduced (OR 0.96, 95% CI 0.87–1.05 ;
Wald’s x2=0.7, df=1, p=0.4). The odds ratios Our meta-analysis of ADHD follow-up studies
from these models represent the probability of has produced some intriguing results but these
The age-dependent decline of ADHD 163

should be interpreted in the context of several Any theory of ADHD must explain the
limitations. Although meta-analysis regression apparent waning of symptoms over time, which
provides a method for pooling data across in Fig. 1 is evident from both full and residual
studies, it cannot correct for the deficiencies of diagnoses. Although on the face of it, these re-
the individual reports. As Fig. 2 b shows, only sults suggest that ADHD attenuates over time,
one data-point followed subjects into their the finding of symptom reduction with age may
thirties and only eight followed subjects into reflect the developmental insensitivity of the
their twenties. This limits our ability to draw DSM-IV, not the natural history of ADHD. As
conclusions about ADHD in older adults. This Barkley (1997) and Faraone (2000) have argued,
problem is highlighted by the large impact that developmental change makes it difficult for
outlying observations had on our estimates of ADHD children to meet criteria for ADHD as
age-dependent symptom decline (Mannuzza they age.
et al. 1993, 1998). Why these data were such For ADHD, DSM-IV addresses development
extreme outliers is not clear, but it may have in several ways. It cautions diagnosticians that
been because that study excluded hyperactive maturation mitigates symptoms ; they become
children whose primary reason for referral was less conspicuous. Older children may be restless
aggressive behavior. Since hyperactive children and fidgety, but not overly hyperactive. With
with aggressive behavior are at greatest risk for age, inattention predominates as tasks at school
continued ADHD in adulthood (Hechtman, or work tax attentional capacity. Also, DSM-IV
1992), the low rate of ADHD adulthood notes that symptoms should be considered
reported by that study is likely to be an under- present only if they are not consistent with
estimate. developmental level.
Despite these limitations, our pooled analysis Despite these cautions, it is possible that the
of ADHD outcome studies confirms the prior DSM is not sufficiently sensitive to develop-
work of Barkley et al. (2002) and Biederman mental variations in symptom expression. Some
et al. (2000) who concluded that the apparent items that are relevant in childhood may simply
prognosis of ADHD depends on what definition not apply in adulthood. For example, one
of persistence one uses. When we define only DSM-IV symptom of ADHD is ‘often leaves
those meeting full criteria for ADHD as having seat in classroom or in other situations in which
‘persistent ADHD’, the rate of persistence is remaining seated is expected ’. This is devel-
low, y15 % at age 25 years. But when we in- opmentally insensitive for two reasons. With
clude cases consistent with DSM-IV’s definition development, children mature and conquer
of ADHD in partial remission, the rate of developmental challenges. Although remaining
persistence is much higher, y40–60 %, with seated is difficult at age 5 years, it becomes easier
the higher estimate excluding the outlying ob- with age as socialization and brain development
servations. improve the child’s capacity for inhibiting
Given that the prevalence of ADHD in impulses. Also, development leads to changes in
childhood is y8 % (Faraone et al. 2003), these the environment. For example, unlike school-
data suggest the prevalence of adult ADHD at children, many adults are not required to sit for
age 25 years should range from 1.2 % for the several hours each day. Thus, they have fewer
narrow full-threshold diagnosis of ADHD and opportunities to leave their seat.
3.2% when including cases in partial remission. The results of this study are limited by several
This range is consistent with Murphy & factors. Because this is a meta-analysis, the data
Barkley’s (1996) survey of 720 adults applying available from published reports over the past
for or renewing their drivers licenses in the three decades is often lacking. We could not
state of Massachusetts. In this sample, 4.7 % evaluate the impact of other psychiatric co-
of adults met DSM-IV criteria for ADHD morbidity, study design (e.g. employing dif-
based on a rating scale assessment. Similarly, ferential diagnosis, blindness, time referent)
Heiligenstein et al. (1998) examined 448 college consistently across all studies. We could also not
students not selected for any psychiatric determine from this study whether the residual
diagnosis. Four per cent of the students met symptoms : (a) occur in more than one setting
DSM-IV criteria for ADHD. or (b) were symptoms of another co-morbid
164 S. V. Faraone et al.

disorder. This study also does not address the DECLARATION OF INTEREST
putative functional impairments associated with
None.
symptomatic persistence of ADHD. Thus, the
limited goal of this study was to document the
level of symptom expression in follow-up
samples of different ages, rather than to attempt REFERENCES
to understand the underlying causes or conse- August, G. J., Stewart, M. A. & Holmes, C. S. (1983). A four-year
follow-up of hyperactive boys with and without conduct disorder.
quences of residual ADHD symptoms. British Journal of Psychiatry 143, 192–198.
Accurate knowledge about the prevalence of Barkley, R. (1997). Age dependent decline in ADHD : True recovery
disorders is essential for effective screening and or statistical illusion ? The ADHD Report 5, 1–5.
Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Edelbrock, C. S. & Smallish, L. (1990).
diagnosis. The belief that a disorder is rare The adolescent outcome of hyperactive children diagnosed by re-
would be expected to make clinicians wary of search criteria : I. An 8-year prospective follow-up study. Journal
patients expressing ADHD symptoms. In con- of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 29,
546–557.
trast, the higher prevalence suggested by this Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Smallish, L. & Fletcher, K. (2002). The
report and by the two community studies persistence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into young
adulthood as a function of reporting source and definition of
(Murphy & Barkley, 1996 ; Heiligenstein et al. disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 111, 279–289.
1998) should motivate clinicians to be less sus- Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Milberger, S., Curtis, S., Chen, L.,
picious that apparent cases of ADHD in adults Marrs, A., Ouellette, C., Moore, P. & Spencer, T. (1996).
Predictors of persistence and remission of ADHD : results from a
are false positives. Such a change would be four-year prospective follow-up study of ADHD children. Journal
especially important in primary-care settings. of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 35,
Faraone et al. (2004) showed that, although 343–351.
Biederman, J., Mick, E. & Faraone, S. V. (2000). Age-dependent
adult ADHD is a substantial source of morbidity decline of symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder :
in such settings, primary-care practitioners impact of remission definition and symptom type. American
Journal of Psychiatry 157, 816–818.
(PCPs) are hesitant to diagnose ADHD in Borland, B. L. & Heckman, H. K. (1976). Hyperactive boys and their
adults in patients who had not been previously brothers: a 25-year follow-up study. Archives of General Psy-
diagnosed in childhood. The idea that ADHD is chiatry 33, 669–675.
Cantwell, D. P. & Baker, L. (1989). Stability and natural history of
rare in adulthood would be expected to worsen DSM-III childhood diagnoses. Journal of the American Academy
the conservative use of the diagnosis in primary of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 28, 691–700.
care. In contrast, psychiatrists were more likely Claude, D. & Firestone, P. (1995). The development of ADHD boys :
a 12-year follow-up. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science 27,
to diagnose such patients by establishing a 226–249.
retrospectively reported onset in childhood. The Faraone, S. V. (2000). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in
adults : Implications for theories of diagnosis. Current Directions in
idea that PCPs are more conservative with the Psychological Science 9, 33–36.
diagnosis of ADHD was further supported by Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Spencer, T., Wilens, T., Seidman, L. J.,
data showing that PCPs took a longer time Mick, E. & Doyle, A. (2000). Attention deficit hyperactivity dis-
order in adults : an overview. Biological Psychiatry 48, 9–20.
to make the diagnosis than did psychiatrists. Faraone, S. V., Sergeant, J., Gillberg, C. & Biederman, J. (2003). The
Identifying cases of adult ADHD requires care- worldwide prevalence of ADHD : is it an American condition?
ful interviewing about symptoms of inattention, World Psychiatry 2, 104–113.
Faraone, S. V., Spencer, T., Montano, C. B. & Biederman, J. (2004).
restlessness, impulsivity and disorganization. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults : a survey of
More research is needed of adults meeting current practice in psychiatry and primary care. Archives of
Internal Medicine 164, 1221–1226.
residual diagnostic criteria in order to better Feldman, S., Denhoff, E. & Denhoff, J. (1979). The attention dis-
understand the clinical correlates and associated orders and related syndromes : Outcome in adolescent and young
dysfunction in reference to both non-ADHD adult life. In Minimal Brain Dysfunction: A Developmental
Approach (ed. E. Denhoff and L. Stern), pp. 133–148. Masson
adults and those that continue to meet full Publishing Inc. : New York.
diagnostic criteria. Gittelman, R., Mannuzza, S., Shenker, R. & Bonagura, N. (1985).
Hyperactive boys almost grown up : I. Psychiatric status. Archives
of General Psychiatry 42, 937–947.
Hart, E., Lahey, B., Loeber, R., Applegate, B. & Frick, P. (1995).
Developmental change in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS in boys : a four-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child
Psychology 23, 729–749.
This work was supported in part by grants Hechtman, L. (1992). Long-term outcome in attention-deficit
R01MH57934, R01HD37694, R13MH59126 to hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America 1,
553–565.
Dr Faraone from the National Institutes of Heiligenstein, E., Conyers, L. M., Berns, A. R., Miller, M. A. &
Health. Smith, M. A. (1998). Preliminary normative data on DSM-IV
The age-dependent decline of ADHD 165

attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in college students [pub- Mannuzza, S., Klein, R. G., Bessler, A., Malloy, P. & LaPadula, M.
lished erratum appears in Journal of the American College of (1993). Adult outcome of hyperactive boys : Educational
Health (1998), 46, 213]. Journal of the American College of Health achievement, occupational rank and psychiatric status. Archives of
46, 185–188. General Psychiatry 50, 565–576.
Hill, J. & Schoener, E. (1996). Age-dependent decline of attention Mendelson, W., Johnson, N. & Stewart, M. (1971). Hyperactive
deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 153, children as teenagers : a follow-up study. Journal of Nervous and
1143–1146. Mental Diseases 153, 273–279.
Huber, P. J. (1967). The behavior of maximum likelihood estimates Murphy, K. & Barkley, R. (1996). Prevalence of DSM-IV symptoms
under non-standard conditions. Proceedings of the Fifth Berkeley of ADHD in adult licensed drivers : Implications for clinical
Symposium on Mathematical Statistics and Probability 1, 221–233. diagnosis. Journal of Attention Disorders 1, 147–161.
Keck, P., McElroy, S., Strakowski, S., West, S., Sax, K., Hawkins, J., Offord, D. R., Boyle, M. H., Racine, Y. A., Fleming, J. E., Cadman,
Bourne, M. & Haggard, P. (1998). 12-month outcome of patients D. T., Blum, H. M., Byrne, C., Links, P. S., Lipman, E. L. &
with bipolar disorder following hospitalization for a manic or Macmillan, H. L. (1992). Outcome, prognosis and risk in a longi-
mixed episode. American Journal of Psychiatry 155, 646–652. tudinal follow-up study. Journal of the American Academy of Child
Lambert, N., Hartsough, C., Sassone, D. & Sandoval, J. and Adolescent Psychiatry 31, 916–923.
(1987). Persistence of hyperactivity symptoms from childhood to Rasmussen, P. & Gillberg, C. (2000). Natural outcome of ADHD
adolesence and associated outcomes. American Journal of Ortho- with developmental coordination disorder at age 22 years : a
psychiatry 57, 22–32. controlled, longitudinal, community-based study. Journal of the
Lambert, N. M. (1988). Adolescent outcomes for hyperactive chil- American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 39, 1424–
dren : Perspectives on general and specific patterns of childhood 1431.
risk for adolescent educational, social and mental health problems. Shaffer, D. (1994). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults.
American Psychologist 43, 786–799. American Journal of Psychiatry 151, 633–638.
Mannuzza, S. & Gittelman, R. (1984). The adolescent outcome of Stata Corporation (1992). Stata Reference Manual : Release 3.1 (6th
hyperactive girls. Psychiatry Research 13, 19–29. edn). Stata Corporation : College Station, TX.
Mannuzza, S., Gittelman, R., Klein, R., Bonagura, N., Malloy, P., Weiss, G., Hechtman, L., Milroy, T. & Perlman, T. (1985).
Giampino, T. L. & Addalli, K. A. (1991). Hyperactive boys almost Psychiatric status of hyperactives as adults : a controlled prospec-
grown up: V. Replication of psychiatric status. Archives of General tive 15-year follow-up of 63 hyperactive children. Journal of the
Psychiatry 48, 77–83. American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 24, 211–
Mannuzza, S., Klein, R., Bessler, A., Malloy, P. & LaPadula, M. 220.
(1998). Adult psychiatric status of hyperactive boys grown up. Yan, W. (1996). An investigation of adult outcome of hyperactive
American Journal of Psychiatry 155, 493–498. children in Shanghai. Chinese Medical Journal 109, 877–880.

You might also like