Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/294693619

Psychology and global climate change: Addressing a multi-faceted


phenomenon and set of challenges. A report by the American Psychological
Association's task force on the interface...

Article in American Psychologist · January 2009

CITATIONS READS

375 6,211

8 authors, including:

Janet Swim Susan Clayton


Pennsylvania State University College of Wooster
137 PUBLICATIONS 12,747 CITATIONS 154 PUBLICATIONS 11,235 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Thomas J Doherty Robert Gifford


Sustainable Self LLC University of Victoria
35 PUBLICATIONS 1,746 CITATIONS 212 PUBLICATIONS 20,308 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Thomas J Doherty on 10 March 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychology’s Contributions to Understanding and
Addressing Global Climate Change
Janet K. Swim Pennsylvania State University
Paul C. Stern National Research Council
Thomas J. Doherty Lewis & Clark Graduate School of Education and
Counseling
Susan Clayton College of Wooster
Joseph P. Reser Griffith University
Elke U. Weber Columbia University
Robert Gifford University of Victoria
George S. Howard University of Notre Dame

Global climate change poses one of the greatest challenges in the climate system, the impacts and human conse-
facing humanity in this century. This article, which intro- quences of climate change will continue for many decades
duces the American Psychologist special issue on global and, in some cases, many centuries (Solomon, Plattner,
climate change, follows from the report of the American Knutti, & Friedlingstein, 2009). Moreover, climate change
Psychological Association Task Force on the Interface will take place in the context of the other sweeping social,
Between Psychology and Global Climate Change. In this technological, and ecological transitions of the 21st century
article, we place psychological dimensions of climate (e.g., increases in population, urbanization, and disparities
change within the broader context of human dimensions of in wealth; Stokols, Misra, Runnerstrom & Hipp, 2009),
climate change by addressing (a) human causes of, conse- making confident anticipation of its effects especially prob-
quences of, and responses (adaptation and mitigation) to lematic. The current state of scientific knowledge on the
climate change and (b) the links between these aspects of causes and consequences of climate change is summarized
climate change and cognitive, affective, motivational, in- in two recent major studies (Karl, Melillo, & Peterson,
terpersonal, and organizational responses and processes. 2009; National Research Council, 2010a).
Characteristics of psychology that cross content domains Climate change is sometimes equated with global
and that make the field well suited for providing an under-
warming, but it involves much more than temperature
standing of climate change and addressing its challenges
change. The human activities that cause temperature
are highlighted. We also consider ethical imperatives for
change set in motion a series of associated phenomena: sea
psychologists’ involvement and provide suggestions for
ways to increase psychologists’ contribution to the science level rise, loss of polar sea ice, melting of continental
of climate change. glaciers, changes in precipitation patterns, progressive
shifting in the habitats of species and the boundaries of
Keywords: climate change, interdisciplinary research, hu- ecosystems, acidification of the oceans, and more (Inter-
man– environment relations, sustainability, psychological governmental Panel on Climate Change, 2007a, 2007b;
dimensions National Research Council, 2010b). These changes and

G
impacts in turn create increasing risks to the planet’s life
lobal climate change poses one of the greatest
challenges facing humanity in this century.
Earth’s climate has changed in many ways over Janet K. Swim, Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State Univer-
geological time, but for the first time, over the past century, sity; Paul C. Stern, Committee on the Human Dimensions of Global
Change, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education,
human activity has become a significant cause of climate National Research Council, Washington, DC; Thomas J. Doherty, Depart-
change. By burning fossil fuels, cutting and burning for- ment of Counseling Psychology, Lewis & Clark Graduate School of
ests, and engaging in other environment-impacting activi- Education and Counseling; Susan Clayton, Department of Psychology,
ties, humans have changed the heat balance of Earth suf- College of Wooster; Joseph P. Reser, School of Psychology, Griffith
University, Gold Coast Campus, Queensland, Australia; Elke U. Weber,
ficiently that the global average temperature has moved Department of Psychology and Center for Research on Environmental
outside the range that has characterized the 10,000 years of Decisions, Graduate School of Business, Columbia University; Robert
recorded human history (Intergovernmental Panel on Cli- Gifford, Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria,
mate Change, 2007a). This climate change “poses signifi- British Columbia, Canada; George S. Howard, Department of Psychol-
ogy, University of Notre Dame.
cant risks for—and in many cases is already affecting—a Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Janet
broad range of human and natural systems” (National Re- K. Swim, Department of Psychology, Pennsylvania State University, 515
search Council, 2010a, p. 2). Because of physical time lags Moore Building, University Park, PA 16802-3106. E-mail: jks4@psu.edu

May–June 2011 ● American Psychologist 241


© 2011 American Psychological Association 0003-066X/11/$12.00
Vol. 66, No. 4, 241–250 DOI: 10.1037/a0023220
support systems and to a myriad of species, including
humankind. Figure 1
The natural sciences have long been engaged in study- Human and Psychological Dimensions of Climate
ing environmental systems, including the physical and Change
chemical processes that change Earth’s heat balance, the
ways in which these processes affect other parts of the
global climate system, and the consequences of all these
changes for physical and biological processes on land and
in the waters. However, a second science of climate change
has been developing for over a quarter of a century: the
science of the “human dimensions” of climate change
(Chen, Boulding, & Schneider 1983; National Research
Council, 1992; Stern, 1993). This field of science seeks to
understand human activities that affect climate change,
consequences of climate change that directly and indirectly
affect people, human responses to anticipated and experi-
enced climate change, and ways to help people respond
effectively. Psychological dimensions are integral to hu-
man dimensions of climate change and have been a part of
broader efforts by psychologists, perhaps most noticeably
environmental psychologists, over the course of several
decades to understand and address a range of environmen-
tal changes and problems (e.g., Gardner & Stern, 2002;
Koger & Winter, 2010; Nickerson, 2003; Schmuck & Note. Adapted from Figure 4-1 (p. 106) in Global Environmental Change:
Understanding the Human Dimensions (by National Research Council, 1992,
Schultz, 2002; Swim, Markowitz, & Bloodhart, in press). A Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences. Copyright 1992 by National
summary of relevant psychological research was provided Academy of Sciences.) and Figure 1 (p. 273) in “Psychological Dimensions of
in the report of the American Psychological Association Global Environmental Change” (by P. C. Stern, 1992, Annual Review of
Psychology, 43, 269 –302. Copyright 1992 by Annual Reviews, Inc.).
Task Force on the Interface Between Psychology and
Global Climate Change (2009). The articles in this special
issue follow from this task force report. The present article
provides an overview of these articles, a model that inte-
grates this literature with the broader literature on human
dimensions of climate change, and some general sugges- cognitive, affective, and motivational processes as well as
tions for psychologists who wish to contribute in this area. human systems at a larger scale. Responses to anticipated
A simple conceptual model distinguishes climate sys- and experienced climate change are mitigation and adap-
tems (which are part of environmental systems) from hu- tation, as depicted in the bottom left and right corners of the
man systems and delineates the connections among them figure. Efforts to mitigate or limit climate change are aimed
(see Figure 1). As noted on the left-hand side of the figure, at directly or indirectly altering the proximate causes of
people affect climate through activities (e.g., burning fossil climate change. Adapting to climate change includes ad-
fuels, clearing forests) that directly alter environmental dressing the psychological and social impacts of both the
conditions that change the climate. These activities, which threat and the unfolding consequences of climate change.
have been called proximate human causes of climate Cognitive, affective, and motivational processes affect mit-
change, are a result of a full range of cultural, economic, igation and adaptation via the influence of psychological
political, and social conditions and processes, depicted as processes on human contributions, systems, and conse-
“human systems” in the figure, and of psychological con- quences. The direct and indirect impacts of these psycho-
siderations noted in the middle of the figure, which include logical processes on many of the elements shown in the
human understanding of climate change, affective re- figure illustrate that human dimensions of climate change
sponses to climate change, and psychological motivations. are inherently psychological and social and that psychology
Psychological considerations are often and appropriately can offer knowledge and concepts that can help explain the
treated as part of human systems. We separate them here to human understanding, causes, and consequences of climate
highlight them for a psychological audience. As depicted change as well as inform responses to it and help make
on the right-hand side of the figure, climate systems affect them more effective.
people through events that directly alter essential aspects of What Does Psychology Have to
the environment that support humans and other living Offer?
things, for example, by changing the frequency of storms
and droughts, the availability of water, the viability of food Over the past three decades, a number of research agendas
crops, and the incidence of disease. Human consequences have been developed for the human dimensions of global
are also both psychological (e.g., distress) and social (e.g., change, including climate change (e.g., Chen et al., 1983;
intergroup relations) and are influenced by intra-individual Kates, Ausubel, & Berberian, 1985; National Research

242 May–June 2011 ● American Psychologist


Council, 1992, 1999). Many efforts, including that of the Implicit and explicit individual consumption decisions
American Psychological Association’s task force on cli- provide a critical link between contextual influences on
mate change, have focused on the possible contributions of decisions and the proximate behavioral causes of climate
psychology (e.g., American Psychological Association change (Swim et al., 2011). Individual-level predictors of
Task Force on the Interface Between Psychology and consumption decisions include personal capabilities (e.g.,
Global Climate Change, 2009; Center for Research on income, skills), motivations (e.g., connection to nature,
Environmental Decisions, 2009; Clayton & Brook, 2005; perceptions of needs vs. luxuries, basic psychological
Clayton & Myers, 2009; Cvetkovich & Werner, 1994; needs), and core values and beliefs. By attending to a
Fischhoff & Furby, 1983; Gifford, 2008; Kazdin, 2009; variety of individual predictors, researchers can help ex-
Sjöberg, 1989; Spence, Pidgeon, & Uzzell, 2009; Stern, plain instances in which individual and household behavior
1992; Uzzell & Räthzel, 2009; Vlek & Steg, 2007). Envi- does not follow simple models of economic benefit maxi-
ronmental psychology provides the most obvious input. mization, such as when individuals and households fail to
Gifford (2007) described several themes in environmental make energy-saving investments that would yield individ-
psychology that have emerged over the last 50 years that ual benefits at no cost or very attractive rates of financial
are relevant to climate change: (a) an interest in informing return (Creyts, Granade, & Ostrowski, 2010; Stern, 1986).
and aiding public policy; (b) attention to technology both as In addition, individual decisions are influenced by and
a contributor to environmental problems and as a means to operate through the immediate and distal physical and
improve sustainability; (c) a tendency to value and benefit social contexts in which they are embedded (Bin & Dow-
from multidisciplinary collaborations and theories from latabadi, 2005; Black, Stern, & Elworth, 1985; Gifford,
other fields; and (d) expansion of interest to include mul- 2006). Contextual-level predictors include aspects of the
tiple levels of analysis from small-scale studies of individ- physical infrastructure (e.g., the structure of human settle-
uals and small groups to larger scale issues of sustainabil- ments, which influence the demand for motorized travel),
ity. Yet, as past reviews have indicated, many other available technology (e.g., machines that enable more rapid
subfields, such as cognitive, human factors, social, com- harvesting of resources such as trees and fish), geophysical
munity, clinical, and counseling psychology, to name a events (e.g., drought, disasters, and storms), interpersonal
few, have also provided valuable insights. Drawing on contexts (e.g., social and cultural norms and comparisons),
and cultural, economic, and political conditions (e.g., con-
research from environmental psychology and other sub-
sumerism, a culture’s orientation toward time and nature,
fields, we elaborate on the model illustrated in Figure 1 by
energy prices, government policies). By attending to the
describing ways that psychological research has contrib-
interface between individual decisions and contextual pre-
uted to the understanding and addressing of (a) human
dictors, psychologists can potentially help explain behavior
contributions to climate change, (b) psychological and in-
in situations where individual behaviors appear constrained
terpersonal consequences of climate change, (c) mitigation
by their cultural contexts and also when households ac-
and adaptation responses to climate change, and (d) human tively attempt to overcome these contexts, such as when
cognitive, affective, and motivational responses to climate they join counterconsumer movements (Bekin, Carrigan, &
change. Then we consider some overarching characteristics Szmigin, 2005; Craig-Lees & Hill, 2002).
of psychological research and practice that are common to Second, psychology can help describe and explain the
these four specific topical contributions. human consequences of climate change. These conse-
First, psychology can help describe and explain the quences obviously include biological impacts and hazards
human causes of climate change. Climate change is a to physical health and to human settlements (Intergovern-
quintessential commons problem: It involves collective mental Panel on Climate Change, 2007b; World Health
action driven by individuals’ short-term benefits that de- Organization, 2010). As Doherty and Clayton (2011, this
grades a long-term common good (National Research issue) note, they may also include direct and indirect psy-
Council, 2002; for reviews of the psychological literature, chological and interpersonal impacts. Although all of these
see Gifford, 2008, and Kopelman, Weber, & Messick, potential impacts cannot be described with certainty and
2002). Human behavioral contributions to climate change full clarity, the cumulative and interacting psychosocial
occur through the use of goods and services that directly effects of climate change may well be profound. Heat,
and indirectly result in fossil fuel consumption and the extreme weather events, and increased competition for
other biophysical changes that alter the climate. These scarce environmental resources, compounded by preexist-
activities, sometimes referred to as environmental consump- ing inequalities and disproportionate impacts among
tion (Stern, 1997), are linked to consumer spending (economic groups and nations, affect interpersonal and intergroup
consumption), although the two are not the same. As Swim, behavior and can result in increasing stress and anxiety.
Clayton, and Howard (2011, this issue) and Stern (2011, this Even in the absence of direct impacts, anticipation and
issue) show, psychologists can help us conceptualize and concern about the threat of climate change may erode
better understand the predictors of environmental and eco- quality of life and threaten mental health. Individual and
nomic consumption by providing psychological analyses of contextual features can influence the extent to which indi-
types of consumption behaviors and directing attention to viduals and communities experience different impacts.
behaviors that contribute the most to climate-driving emis- Those who have the fewest social and economic resources
sions. are likely to be the most vulnerable to physical and psy-

May–June 2011 ● American Psychologist 243


chological impacts. It may also be useful to attend to intergroup relations and processes, and perceived equity
possible positive consequences, such as people taking col- and procedural justice, may be useful when addressing
lective responsibility for solving a shared problem, that conflicts that may emerge over environmental justice issues
may aid individual coping and community responses. or among social groups as an indirect result of climate
Third, psychology can help describe, explain, and also change and when developing and framing policy responses
inform our responses to climate change. The consequences to climate change (Clayton & Brook, 2005; Spence et al.,
of climate change both affect and are affected by the ways 2009; Swim & Clayton, 2010).
that individuals and communities adapt. Adaptation ex- Psychology can also make important contributions by
tends beyond making physical and structural adjustments to informing efforts to mitigate or limit climate change, al-
environmental changes. As Reser and Swim (2011, this though, as with other responses to climate change, re-
issue) note, adaptation also includes a range of coping sponses are influenced by both individual and contextual
actions that individuals and communities may take, as well factors. Much policy attention has been given to structural
as psychological processes (e.g., threat and response ap- barriers to behavioral change, but Gifford (2011, this issue)
praisals, emotion management, and cognitive reframing argues that removing these barriers is not likely to be
responses) that both precede and follow behavioral re- sufficient because of other resistances to change. Some of
sponses. Adapting to, and coping with, climate change is these may relate to a lack of understanding of climate
dynamic; it involves many intrapsychic processes that in- change, whereas others relate to habitual behavioral pat-
fluence reactions to (and preparations for) adverse impacts terns, bounded rationality, affective processes, personal
of climate change, including chronic environmental condi- and social motivations, and interpersonal processes (Gif-
tions and extreme events. Some relevant psychological ford, 2011; Shogren, Parkhurst, & Banerjee, 2010; Weber
processes include sense-making; causal and responsibility & Stern, 2011, this issue). For instance, a few of the many
attributions for adverse climate change impacts; appraisals psychological and social barriers include hyperbolic dis-
of impacts, resources, and possible coping responses; af- counting, which gives current outcomes more weight than
fective responses; and motivational processes related to temporally distant outcomes even when the latter are of
needs for security, stability, coherence, and control. These greater value; reactance against policies perceived to re-
processes are influenced by media representations of cli- duce individual control; and perceived social norms that
mate change and by formal and informal social discourse encourage energy use.
that involves social construction, representation, amplifica- Yet, as Stern (2011) notes, psychologists have helped
tion, and attenuation of climate change risk and its impacts. to design interventions to encourage actions that limit hu-
These processes reflect and motivate both intrapsychic man contributions to climate change at individual, organi-
responses (e.g., emotion management, cognitive adapta- zational, cultural, and policy levels and have broadened our
tion, problem solving) and individual and community re- understanding of why people do or do not respond to
sponses. Individual and cultural variation influence all as- different types of interventions. Psychologists also provide
pects of the process, providing context, worldviews, values, and apply behavioral models that describe human and
concerns, resilience, and vulnerability (Bloodhart & Swim, household behavioral contributions and have helped design
2010; Heath & Gifford, 2006; Weber, 2010). effective interventions and evaluations to further develop
Psychologists are well positioned to design, imple- and improve them. Some of these interventions provide
ment, and assess interventions to ameliorate psychosocial people with better information so they know which actions
impacts of climate change (see Doherty & Clayton, 2011). have beneficial outcomes for themselves and for climate
Climate change will be accompanied by and itself epito- mitigation and adaptation. Psychologists design and test
mizes both natural and technological disasters of unprece- systems that make certain environmental choices more
dented consequence, and climate change has been framed noticeable (e.g., energy-use feedback) or attractive (e.g.,
as a truly global disaster and profound challenge for hu- financial incentives that lower initial costs of energy-
manity (e.g. Spratt & Sutton, 2008). It is important to efficient equipment or move the financial benefits of the
acknowledge that some of the possible disasters associated equipment temporally closer) or that make environmentally
with global climate change are not under conventional beneficial actions more convenient, combining economic
national jurisdiction and that responses to them may not be and noneconomic inducements to action in highly effective
covered by conventional agencies, policies, or procedures. ways (Vandenbergh, Stern, Gardner, Dietz, & Gilligan,
However, much of the existing and extensive body of 2010). Psychologists also suggest strategies for engaging
psychological, social, and health science research and prac- values that encourage contributions to the public good
tice on mitigation, preparedness, response, and recovery in beyond those in an individual’s self-interest. They help
the context of natural and technological disasters can still design informative messages and engage informal net-
be applied and utilized (Elrod, Hamblen, & Norris, 2006; works (e.g., in communities, on the Internet) that promote
Haskett, Scott, Nears, & Grimmett, 2008; Reyes & Jacobs, and activate social norms for participating in collective
2006) The disaster research literature, with its transdisci- efforts to limit greenhouse gas emissions. They are begin-
plinary orientation, has developed methodologies, mea- ning to examine behavioral factors in organizations that
sures, and many models and tools particularly apposite to enable or inhibit reductions in organizations’ contributions
the domain of psychology and climate change (e.g., Stokols to climate change. Finally, they can help design and adapt
et al., 2009). Psychological research in other areas, such as formal and informal institutions at local to global levels

244 May–June 2011 ● American Psychologist


that will provide assurance to individual contributors that mitigation may also increase psychological adaptation to
others will also contribute, thereby promoting coordinated climate change.
and equitable responses. Not only do psychologists help Psychologists also examine individuals within natural
improve public understanding of climate change and help and constructed physical and social contexts. Interpersonal,
inform personal and societal choices about proposed tech- social-structural, and technological contexts affect behav-
nological solutions, they also address (a) how public un- iors that have important impacts on environmental systems
derstanding of the nature and impacts of climate change (Clayton & Brook, 2005; Gardner & Stern, 2002; Wapner
differs in important ways from scientific accounts of cli- & Demick, 2002). Attention to transactions and relation-
mate change (Weber & Stern, 2011) and (b) how such ships between individuals and their natural and human-
individual and societal understandings also serve important designed environments has long been a defining feature of
psychological and cultural needs, with important implica- environmental psychology (Gifford, 2007). Investigations
tions for adaptation and behavior change (e.g., Reser, in at the intersection of psychology and the natural sciences,
press). health sciences, humanities, and other social sciences can
Fourth, psychology can make further important con- be particularly useful for addressing global climate change
tributions to the understanding of how people think and feel (Kazdin, 2009).
about climate change, which in turn influences their moti- Psychologists have also uncovered individual, inter-
vations and behavioral responses to perceived and objec- personal, and social forces capable of explaining and
tive causes and consequences of climate change. As Gif- changing human behavior in ways that others may fail to
ford (2011) notes, individuals perceive climate change consider. Although people are, for the most part, able to
differently depending on their awareness of the problem, articulate their opinions, beliefs, and preferences accu-
their knowledge and certainty of the facts, and their trust in rately, they are notoriously poor at recognizing the causes
experts, among other barriers. Furthermore, as Reser and of their behavior (Li, Johnson, & Zaval, 2010; Nisbett &
Swim (2011) and Doherty and Clayton (2011, this issue) Wilson, 1977). This finding has been well demonstrated,
note, the threat and unfolding impacts of climate change for example, with regard to the consumption of energy
may be experienced directly or indirectly. Direct encoun- (Nolan, Schultz, Cialdini, Goldstein, & Griskevicius,
ters can range from the experience and distress of chronic 2008). Behavioral investigations that employ experimental
stressors, such as drought and landscape change, to acute methodologies can identify psychological determinants of
and cataclysmic weather events such as hurricanes, heat trade-offs and choices made by people (Hardisty, Johnson,
waves, and floods. Indirect experiences result from contin- & Weber, 2010; Weber et al., 2007) that collectively in-
uous exposure to multimedia coverage and representations, fluence human contributions to climate change, including
educational sources, and interpersonal interactions and ex- energy-consuming behaviors. These psychological deter-
change. Social constructions and representations of climate minants can be utilized in communication campaigns or in
change can both reflect and influence sense-making and the design of decision environments to encourage people to
whether or not events are attributed to climate change. reduce their energy consumption and to prevent such cam-
Weber and Stern (2011) show that gaining a scientifically paigns from being ineffective or, worse, inadvertently dis-
appropriate understanding of climate change is difficult couraging behavior change (e.g., Center for Research on
because of (a) the difficulties inherent in understanding the Environmental Decisions, 2009). Psychological methodol-
physical phenomena involved and the state of relevant ogies and measures can also aid in ongoing monitoring of
scientific knowledge; (b) psychological tendencies to rely behavioral changes, psychological impacts, and psycholog-
on personal experience and simple mental models (both of ical processes that might otherwise be overlooked and that
which are often misleading; see also Gifford, 2011); and (c) influence individuals’ and communities’ willingness and
a well-organized and ideologically motivated campaign to capacity to engage with adaptive change and the nature and
promote models of climate change that are at substantial forms of adaptations they select.
variance with scientific evidence and the broad scientific
consensus. Why Should Psychology Help
Aside from psychological contributions related to el- Address Climate Change?
ements of the model illustrated in Figure 1, there are a
number of characteristics of psychology as a discipline that Psychologists should help, first of all, because we can. On
make it useful, if not crucial, for adequately addressing the basis of past research efforts and the knowledge base in
global climate change and its impacts. The most obvious the discipline, we believe that further efforts to improve
contributions of psychology are at the individual level, understanding of the psychological processes related to
where, as noted above, it can provide both a theoretical and climate change and effective incorporation and utilization
an empirical understanding of human causes of climate of psychological knowledge in collaborative research and
change, public risk perceptions and understandings of cli- policy initiatives can help humanity effectively mitigate
mate change, experienced impacts of climate change, and and adapt to climate change.
human responses to climate change. This individual level Second, the magnitude and potential irreversibility of
of analysis also illustrates links between psychologically global climate change and its likely psychological impacts
and environmentally significant behaviors and between ad- and effects on quality of life and the environment prompt a
aptation and mitigation. For instance, individual efforts at consideration of ethical imperatives for psychologists’ in-

May–June 2011 ● American Psychologist 245


volvement. Climate change has the potential to have sig- the competence of their work and to protect those they
nificant negative effects on global mental health, and these work with and others from harm (APA, 2010). This also
effects will likely be unevenly distributed (Costello et al., means that work needs to be done to broaden our compe-
2009; Fritze, Blashki, Burke, & Wiseman, 2008; Page & tencies in the domain of climate change and to continually
Howard, 2009). In the near term, climate change probably develop new competencies as our understanding and the
will have a disproportionately direct impact on those less impacts of climate change unfold.
economically privileged or with lower social status
(Agyeman, Bullard, & Evans, 2003; McMichael, Friel, How Can Psychologists Increase Their
Nyong, & Corvalan, 2008), and as with other environmen- Contribution to the Science of Climate
tal health issues (Bullard & Johnson, 2000), social justice Change?
implications will demand consideration (e.g., Brown et al.,
2006). Poverty, reduced individual preparedness, and lack Psychologists can be dramatically more effective if they
of access to community-level resources for disaster relief relate and connect psychological work to constructs and
are central to vulnerability to environmental risks (Brou- perspectives developed in the broader climate research
wer, Akter, Bander, & Haque, 2007). Natural disasters community and collaborate with scientists from other
expose the inadequacies of mental health systems at the fields. Although psychologists have been investigating cli-
moment those systems are most needed (Sontag, 2010). mate change and related subjects for decades and the dis-
Provision of mental health services in many low- and cipline has a unique perspective and body of knowledge to
middle-income countries is already inadequate (Jacob et contribute, the value of psychological contributions is not
al., 2007; Page & Howard, 2010), and improvement in yet widely accepted, nor are psychological insights and
these services is challenged by economic crises and envi- findings widely applied. The following principles can help
ronmental disasters. maximize the value and use of psychological concepts,
The American Psychological Association’s (APA’s) research, and perspectives for understanding the causes and
Ethics Code (APA, 2010), mission statement (APA, 2008), impacts of climate change and for informing effective
and vision statement (APA, 2009) have direct implications responses to climate change:
in these local and global climate change contexts. They all 1. Become Conversant With Language and
indicate that psychologists are committed to creating, com- Research Used in Other Social, Engineering,
municating, and applying psychological knowledge in or- and Natural Science Fields That Address
der to benefit individuals and society and facilitate the Climate Change.
resolution of global challenges. Because global climate
change presents both direct and indirect threats to individ- Anthropogenic climate change is an interdisciplinary issue
ual and community mental health, climate change can be with an emerging interdisciplinary vocabulary and lan-
considered an appropriate arena of ethical obligation for guage. Scientists from many disciplines readily understand
psychologists (Doherty & Clayton, 2011). As with other human interactions with climate change in terms of human
social issues on which psychologists have taken a stand contributions or drivers, impacts or consequences, and re-
(e.g., poverty, discrimination), climate change is an ex- sponses. However, some terms have particular meanings or
tremely consequential concern because it can harm psycho- connotations in certain disciplines or discourses. Being
logical functioning, interpersonal and community relations, aware of the language and concepts used by others and
and human and nonhuman life and because changes in using them when appropriate can aid communication. Dif-
thinking, motivation, and behavior are required for success- ferences between fields in the usage of certain terms and
ful climate change mitigation and adaptation. APA’s ethi- constructs may also reveal fundamental differences in per-
cal principles provide a basis for addressing social justice spective. When use of the language of one field might
issues, such as disparities inherent in climate change im- result in confusion or a loss of meaning or clarity for a
pacts and threats to the rights and welfare of persons who cross-disciplinary audience, it is important to be explicit
or communities which may be most vulnerable to climate about differences in usage. Adaptation, for example, has
change impacts (APA, 2007, 2010). Increased efforts to varying usages across disciplines that reflect differing fa-
highlight, study, and address the psychological impacts of vored levels of analysis and different conceptualizations of
climate change do not pose obvious counter risks (Hansen, human responses. Clarifying such differences is one way
von Krauss, & Tickner, 2008). Indeed, awareness of psy- psychologists can help improve overall understanding of
chological impacts may promote engagement in the issue climate change, communication between disciplines, and
and subsequent behavioral change. our own approaches to climate change.
APA ethical principles and standards also stress that Research from other fields can be important for inter-
psychologists must exercise reasonable judgment and rec- preting the meaning or relevance of psychological vari-
ognize the boundaries of their competence (APA, 2010). ables, because their effects sometimes depend on factors
This recognition is essential for dealing with novel and and parameters that are more thoroughly understood in
complex interdisciplinary issues such as global climate other fields. The reverse is also true. For example, the
change. The ethical standards also state that, in emerging effectiveness of financial incentives for household energy
areas in which recognized standards for training do not yet efficiency depends on how people understand the available
exist, psychologists should take reasonable steps to ensure information and on the level of behavioral effort needed to

246 May–June 2011 ● American Psychologist


take advantage of the incentive (Stern, 1986). Conversely, plasticity), or amount of behavioral change measured as
the effectiveness of information and persuasion depends on frequency or duration. However, what matters in the con-
the economic incentives surrounding the behavior. A thor- text of global climate change is the impact of causes or
ough understanding of the potential of psychological con- effects in environmental terms. For example, a good indi-
tributions to research on and responses to climate change cator of the impact of psychological variables in under-
should be informed by knowledge from other social sci- standing human contributions to climate change is the
ences, such as anthropology, geography, sociology, politi- amount of greenhouse gas emissions those variables can
cal science, communications, and economics, as well as explain. The impact (I) of a behavioral change on climate
knowledge from engineering, business, and other fields. change depends on the combined effects of the number of
For example, individuals’ understandings of climate individuals (n) who might change their behavior, multi-
change are affected by the psychological processes that plied by the technical potential (t) of the behavior to alter
influence seeking out and making sense of information as emissions and the plasticity (p) of the behavior (the I ! tpn
well as the activities of social movements and the judg- equation, see Stern, 2011). This is not to diminish the
ments and decisions of journalists, editors, and corporate psychological significance and multiple benefits of taking
media organizations. Thus, individual understandings of personally meaningful actions in the context of climate
climate change are best understood by combining insights change, and indeed such psychological significance medi-
from psychology, sociology, communications, and other ates and enhances environmental significance. To demon-
social science fields (Weber & Stern, 2011). This injunc- strate the importance of psychological variables for under-
tion to be interdisciplinary applies, of course, to both psy- standing the human consequences of climate change, it is
chologists and nonpsychologists. helpful to show how these variables affect the anticipation
or experience of specific aspects of climate change and how
2. Attend to Psychological Contributions That these reactions affect major or widespread human conse-
Address Issues Recognized as Important to quences that are generally considered important.
Climate Science.
3. Explain Psychological Contributions That
Psychologists must prioritize issues and behaviors recog-
Are Missing From Others’ Analyses and That
nized as important in terms of climate change causes,
May Be Misunderstood by Others, but Be
consequences, or responses. For example, in developing
Aware of the Limitations of This Research.
and describing psychological contributions to efforts to
mitigate climate change, greater emphasis should be placed Psychologists can provide climate researchers from other
on changes that have large potential effects on emissions disciplines with psychological constructs that are relevant
(e.g., using more fuel-efficient means of transportation) to understanding problems that other disciplines already
than on changes that have smaller potential effects in terms recognize, and they can correct misunderstandings and
of their technical potential (e.g., recycling household misuse of psychological constructs when these are encoun-
waste) and behavioral plasticity (e.g., traveling by carpool) tered. For instance, psychologists can describe how the
(see Stern, 2011). The general public is not always aware of psychological processes of risk perception and stress man-
such differences in effectiveness (Attari, DeKay, Davidson, agement may alter people’s willingness to make anticipa-
& Bruine de Bruin, 2010). If findings about lower impact tory adaptations to climate risks (Weber, 2006). As another
behaviors are deemed important, that importance should be example, disciplines vary in their tendency to focus on
described in terms of the implications for climate change different levels of analysis. Psychologists can provide par-
overall, perhaps by making the case that a principle estab- ticular insight into the usefulness and importance of indi-
lished in studies of low-impact behaviors is generalizable vidual-level and experience-informed analyses and can
to higher impact behaviors. Similarly, when studying psy- make connections between this level and more macro-level
chological consequences of climate change, psychologists analyses (Winkel, Saegert, & Evans, 2009).
should be prepared to indicate the broader importance and However, we must be cognizant of the possibility that
relevance of these consequences. For instance, the impor- psychological phenomena are context dependent. Psycho-
tance of affective or “risk-as-feelings” responses (Loewen- logical principles often are established in narrowly defined
stein, Weber, Hsee, & Welch, 2001) may need to be ex- contexts: laboratory experiments, small-scale field experi-
plained to nonpsychologists. Such an explanation could ments, and surveys of particular populations. To apply
potentially be made in terms of how affective responses these principles to climate change, one must consider their
influence risk perceptions and subsequent willingness to external validity: That is, one must consider whether the
change behaviors or support policies; how debilitating principles are applicable in other cultures or economies, in
mental health outcomes influence preparation for, or re- places with very different physical infrastructures or gov-
sponses to, the climate change impacts; or how the mag- ernment regulations, or in vastly changed technological
nitude of these outcomes compares with the magnitude of contexts that might appear a generation in the future. For
other social phenomena (Ferguson & Branscombe, 2010; example, the effectiveness of an intervention to change
Fritze et al., 2008; Weber & Stern, 2011). commuting behavior (and therefore energy use) among
In psychological research, findings typically are pre- college students has been demonstrated (Heath & Gifford,
sented in terms of statistical significance, effect size, pro- 2002). However, without further research, the effectiveness
portion of people whose behavior changed (behavioral of the intervention beyond the population and time period

May–June 2011 ● American Psychologist 247


studied may be questionable. Psychologists must be careful responses can vary by race, ethnicity, age, disabilities,
not to claim that findings from any specific group have religion, and so forth.
general applicability without evidence or strong theory to For both climate change adaptation and mitigation,
support such claims. Another example is the “foot-in-the- cultural contexts and differences are likely to be important
door effect”—the process by which inducing a small be- elements of the human dimensions of global climate
havioral change can set in motion psychological changes change. Acknowledging different cultural insights, per-
that lead, over time, to larger behavioral changes. Efforts to spectives, and experiences with disasters and adaptation
change environmentally relevant behaviors with small en- can advance our understanding of the human causes of
vironmental impacts (e.g., recycling) could possibly lead to climate change, its impacts, and the means of responding to
change in more environmentally consequential behaviors it. Cultural considerations will also be critical in the pro-
(e.g., travel mode choice). Yet the available evidence raises vision of suitable interventions and resources for commu-
questions about whether this effect operates with behaviors nities that are experiencing dramatic upheavals as a result
that affect climate (Thøgersen & Crompton, 2009). of global climate change. Different cultural meanings and
In speaking outside the discipline, we must be explicit social justice concerns can limit the applicability of exist-
about the extent to which psychological phenomena have ing research and may require attention when speaking with
been shown to operate in climate-relevant contexts, and if different populations. Finally, in attending to social and
they have not been shown to do so, we must be cautious cultural differences, psychologists can further research by
about extrapolation from the contexts in which the phe- suggesting new ways of thinking about basic psychological
nomena were established. Psychologists’ expertise about processes and by advancing interventions through the for-
preparing for and responding to natural disasters is likely mation of sensitive cross-cultural and collaborative initia-
applicable to climate change disasters. Yet some inferences tives.
may be required if one considers climate change disasters
to be both natural and technological (see Doherty & Clay- Conclusion
ton, 2011). When considering psychological consequences Global climate change provides an opportunity for greater
of climate change, extrapolation is usually necessary be- integration of approaches within psychology as well as
cause, except for the consequences of warnings about cli- transdisciplinary cooperation with the other social sciences
mate change, the most significant consequences lie in the and the physical sciences. Such integration and collabora-
future. Thus, in their extrapolations, psychologists should tion typically most often occur around a common goal or
be explicit about their evidence base and its likely appli- problem. Global climate change presents a shared problem
cability to projected future events. and daunting challenges, but these alone do not ensure
4. Be Mindful of Social Disparities and Ethical cooperation among disciplines. This article and this special
and Justice Issues That Interface With issue are intended to help psychologists become more
Climate Change Appraisals, Responses, and knowledgeable about how their disciplinary knowledge
Impacts. and expertise can inform collaborative discussions, re-
search, and policy initiatives related to climate change
Much of the psychological research related to climate adaptation and mitigation. More intensive engagement with
change has been conducted in the United States and Eu- nonpsychologists will likely highlight the urgency, chal-
rope. Other populations may have different understandings lenges, and importance of global climate change as a prob-
of climate change and of the choices they face, which can lem for psychological research; encourage us to teach our
in turn influence the social and psychological impacts of students about the psychological aspects of climate change
climate change. Understandings of, and responses to, cli- and human– environment transactions; and most important,
mate change will be influenced by worldviews, cultures, help us find ways to make our contributions more useful.
and social identities (Bloodhart & Swim, 2010; Clayton & Psychology has already made notable contributions in ad-
Myers, 2009; Heath & Gifford, 2006; Swim & Becker, dressing climate change, but it can do much more, partic-
2010; Weber, 2010). Much climate science research in ularly in collaboration with others from diverse fields of
other disciplines has taken place in regions of the world interest and other countries.
such as parts of Africa, Asia, the Andes, Australia, and
Alaska, where climate change impacts are far more evident REFERENCES
and salient. Local populations in these areas have cultural
vantage points, economies, and lifestyles far removed from Agyeman, J., Bullard, R., & Evans, B. (2003). Just sustainabilities:
those found in the urban-based, highly industrialized, set- Development in an unequal world. London, England: Earthscan/MIT
Press.
tings of much of North America and Europe. The influence American Psychological Association. (2007). The road to resilience.
of the mass media and contemporary information technol- Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/helpcenter/road-resilience.aspx
ogies also varies considerably across regions of the world. American Psychological Association. (2008). Mission statement. Re-
Other relevant differences within all countries reflect vari- trieved from http://www.apa.org/about/governance/council/policy/
ation in demographic group membership (such as age, chapter-6.aspx#mission-statement
American Psychological Association. (2009). Vision statement. Retrieved
gender, and education), social identities, and the combina- from http://www.apa.org/about/governance/council/policy/chapter-6.
tions of these factors. For example, gender differences in aspx#mission-statement
experiences with climate change and in climate change American Psychological Association. (2010). Ethical principles of psy-

248 May–June 2011 ● American Psychologist


chologists and code of conduct (2002, Amended June 1, 2010). Re- the effect of beliefs about global warming on willingness to engage in
trieved from http://www.apa.org/ethics/code/index.aspx mitigation behavior. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 30, 135–
American Psychological Association Task Force on the Interface Between 142. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.11.010
Psychology and Global Climate Change. (2009). Psychology and global Fischhoff, B., & Furby, L. (1983). Psychological dimensions of climatic
climate change: Addressing a multi-faceted phenomenon and set of chal- change. In R. S. Chen, E. Boulding, & S. H. Schneider (Eds.), Social
lenges. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/science/about/publications/ science research and climate change: An interdiscı̀plinary appraisal
climate-change.aspx (pp. 180 –203). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Reidel.
Attari, S. Z., DeKay, M. L., Davidson, C. I., & Bruine de Bruin, W. (2010). Fritze, J. G., Blashki, G. A., Burke, S., & Wiseman, J. (2008). Hope,
Public perceptions of energy consumption and savings. Proceedings of the despair and transformation: Climate change and the promotion of
National Academy of Sciences, USA, 107, 16054 –16059. doi:10.1073/ mental health and wellbeing. International Journal of Mental Health
pnas.1001509107 Systems, 2(13). doi:10.1186/1752-4458-2-13
Bekin, C., Carrigan, M., & Szmigin, I. (2005). Defying marketing sover- Gardner, G. T., & Stern, P. C. (2002). Environmental problems and
eignty: Voluntary simplicity at new consumption communities. Quali- human behavior (2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson Custom.
tative Market Research: An International Journal, 8(4), 413– 429. Gifford, R. (2006). A general model of social dilemmas. International
doi:10.1108/13522750510619779 Journal of Ecological Economics & Statistics, 5, 23– 40.
Bin, S., & Dowlatabadi, H. (2005). Consumer lifestyle approach to US Gifford, R. (2007). Environmental psychology and sustainable develop-
energy use and the related CO2 emissions. Energy Policy, 33(2), ment: Expansion, maturation, and challenges. Journal of Social Issues,
197–208. doi:10.1016/S0301-4215(03)00210-6 63(1), 199 –212. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2007.00503.x
Black, J. S., Stern, P. C., & Elworth, J. T. (1985). Personal and contextual Gifford, R. (2008). Psychology’s essential role in alleviating the impacts
influences on household energy adaptations. Journal of Applied Psy- of climate change. Canadian Psychology, 49(4), 273–280. doi:10.1037/
chology, 70, 3–21. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.70.1.3 a0013234
Bloodhart, B., & Swim, J. K. (2010). Equality, harmony, and the envi- Gifford, R. (2011). The dragons of inaction: Psychological barriers that
ronment: An ecofeminist approach to understanding the role of cultural limit climate change mitigation and adaptation. American Psychologist,
values on the treatment of women and nature. Ecopsychology, 2, 66, 290 –302. doi:10.1037/a0023566
187–194. doi:10.1089/eco.2010.0057 Hansen, S. F., von Krauss, M. K., & Tickner, J. A. (2008). The precau-
Brouwer, R., Akter, S., Brander, L., & Haque, E. (2007). Socioeconomic tionary principle and risk–risk tradeoffs. Journal of Risk Research, 11,
vulnerability and adaptation to environmental risk: A case study of 423– 464. doi:10.1080/13669870801967192
climate change and flooding in Bangladesh. Risk Analysis, 27, 313–326. Hardisty, D. J., Johnson, E. J., & Weber, E. U. (2010). A dirty word or a
doi:10.1111/j.1539-6924.2007.00884.x dirty world? Attribute framing, political affiliation, and query theory.
Brown, D., Tuana, N., Averill, M., Baer, P., Born, R., Brandão, C. E. L., Psychological Science, 21, 86 –92. doi:10.1177/0956797609355572
. . . Westra, L. (2006, November 8). White paper on the ethical dimen- Haskett, M. E., Scott, S. S., Nears, K., & Grimmett, M. A. (2008). Lessons
sions of climate change. Report presented at the United Nations Climate from Katrina: Disaster mental health service in the Gulf Coast region.
Change Conference, Nairobi, Kenya. Retrieved from The Pennsylvania Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 39, 93–99. doi:
State University Rock Ethics Institute website: http://rockethics.psu. 10.1037/0735-7028.39.1.93
edu/climate/whitepaper/edcc-whitepaper.pdf Heath, Y., & Gifford, R. (2002). Extending the theory of planned behavior:
Bullard, R., & Johnson, G. S. (2000). Environmental justice: Grassroots Predicting the use of public transportation. Journal of Applied Social
activism and its impact on public policy decision-making. Journal of Psychology, 32, 2154 –2189. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb02068.x
Social Issues, 56, 555–578. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00184 Heath, Y., & Gifford, R. (2006). Free-market ideology and environmental
Center for Research on Environmental Decisions. (2009). The psychology degradation: The case of belief in global climate change. Environment
of climate change communication: A guide for scientists, journalists, and Behavior, 38, 48 –71. doi:10.1177/0013916505277998
educators, political aides, and the interested public. Retrieved from Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. (2007a). Climate change
http://www.cred.columbia.edu/guide/ 2007: The physical science basis. Contribution of Working Group I to
Chen, R. S., Boulding, E., & Schneider, S. H. (Eds.). (1983). Social the Fourth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Cli-
science research and climate change: An interdiscı̀plinary appraisal. mate Change (S. Solomon, D. Qin, M. Manning, Z. Chen, M. Marquis,
Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Reidel. K. B. Averyt, M. Tignor, & H. L. Miller, Eds.). New York, NY:
Clayton, S., & Brook, A. (2005). Can psychology help save the world? A Cambridge University Press.
model for conservation psychology. Analyses of Social Issues and Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. (2007b). Climate change
Public Policy, 5(1), 87–102. doi:10.1111/j.1530-2415.2005.00057.x 2007: Impacts, adaptation, and vulnerability. Contribution of Working
Clayton, S., & Myers, G. (2009). Conservation psychology. Oxford, Group II to the Fourth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental
England: Wiley-Blackwell. Panel on Climate Change (M. L. Parry, O. F. Canziani, J. P. Palutikof,
Costello, A., Abbas, M., Allen, A., Ball, S., Bell, S., Bellamy, R., . . . P. J. van der Linden, & C. E. Hanson, Eds.). New York, NY: Cam-
Patterson, C. (2009). Managing the health effects of climate change: Lancet bridge University Press.
and University College London Institute for Global Health Commission. Jacob, K. S., Sharan, P., Mirza, I., Garrido-Cumbrera, M., Seedat, S.,
Lancet, 373, 1693–1733. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60935-1 Mari, J., . . . Saxena, S. (2007). Mental health systems in countries:
Craig-Lees, M., & Hill, C. (2002). Understanding voluntary simplifiers. Where are we now? Lancet, 370, 1061–1077. doi:10.1016/S0140-
Psychology & Marketing, 19(2), 187–210. doi:10.1002/mar.10009 6736(07)61241-0
Creyts, J., Granade, H. C., & Ostrowski, K. J. (2010, January). U.S. Karl, T., Melillo, J. M., & Peterson, T. C. (Eds.). (2009). Global climate
energy savings: Opportunities and challenges. McKinsey Quarterly. change impacts in the United States: A state of knowledge report from
Retrieved from http://www.mckinseyquarterly.com/US_energy_ the U.S. Global Change Research Program. New York, NY: Cam-
savings_Opportunities_and_challenges_2511 bridge University Press.
Cvetkovich, G. T., & Werner, R. (1994). How can psychology help save Kates, R. W., Ausubel, J., & Berberian, M. (Eds.). (1985). Climate impact
the planet? A research agenda on environmental problems [Statement assessment: Studies of the interaction of climate and society. New
distributed by the APA Taskforce on Psychology and Environmental York, NY: Wiley.
Problems]. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Kazdin, A. E. (2009). Psychological science’s contributions to a sustain-
Doherty, T. J., & Clayton, S. D. (2011). The psychological impacts of able environment: Extending our reach to a grand challenge of society.
global climate change. American Psychologist, 66, 265–276. doi: American Psychologist, 64, 339 –356. doi:10.1037/a0015685
10.1037/a0023141 Koger, S. M., & Winter, D. D. (2010). The psychology of environmental
Elrod, C. L., Hamblen, J. L., & Norris, F. H. (2006). Challenges in problems: Psychology for sustainability (3rd ed.). New York, NY:
implementing disaster mental health programs. Annals of the American Psychology Press, Taylor & Francis.
Academy of Political and Social Science, 604, 152–170. doi:10.1177/ Kopelman, S., Weber, J. M., & Messick, D. M. (2002). Factors influenc-
0002716205285186 ing cooperation in commons dilemmas: A review of experimental
Ferguson, M. A., & Branscombe, N. R. (2010). Collective guilt mediates psychological research. In National Research Council (E. Ostrom, T.

May–June 2011 ● American Psychologist 249


Dietz, N. Dolsak, P. C. Stern, S. Stonich, & E. U. Weber, Eds.), The doesn’t say about energy use. Journal of Policy Analysis and Manage-
drama of the commons (pp. 113–156). Washington, DC: National ment, 5, 200 –227. doi:10.2307/3323541
Academy Press. Stern, P. C. (1992). Psychological dimensions of global environmental
Li, Y., Johnson, E. J., & Zaval, L. (2011). Local warming: Daily temper- change. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 269 –302. doi:10.1146/
ature deviation affects beliefs and concern about climate change. annurev.ps.43.020192.001413
Manuscript submitted for publication. Stern, P. C. (1993, June 25). A second environmental science: Human–
Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, E. (2001). Risk environment interactions. Science, 260, 1897–1899. doi:10.1126/
as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267–286. doi:10.1037/0033- science.260.5116.1897
2909.127.2.267 Stern, P. C. (1997). Toward a working definition of consumption for
McMichael, A. J., Friel, S., Nyong, A., & Corvalan, C. (2008). Global environ- environmental research and policy. In National Research Council (P. C.
mental change and health: Impact, inequalities, and the health sector. Stern, T. Dietz, V. R. Ruttan, R. H. Socolow, & J. L. Sweeney, Eds.),
British Medical Journal, 336, 191–194. doi:10.1136/bmj.39392.473727.AD Environmentally significant consumption: Research directions (pp. 12–
National Research Council. (1992). Global environmental change: 35). Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Understanding the human dimensions (P. C. Stern, O. R. Young, & Stern, P. C. (2011). Contributions of psychology to limiting climate
D. Druckman, Eds.). Washington, DC: National Academy of Sci- change. American Psychologist, 66, 303–314. doi:10.1037/a0023235
ences. Stokols, D., Misra, S., Runnerstrom, M. G., & Hipp, A. (2009). Psychol-
National Research Council. (1999). Human dimensions of global environ- ogy in an age of ecological crisis. American Psychologist, 64, 181–193.
mental change: Pathways for the next decade. Washington, DC: Na- doi:10.1037/a0014717
tional Academy Press. Swim, J. K., & Becker, J. (2010). Individuals’ efforts to mitigate global
National Research Council. (2002). The drama of the commons (E. Ostrom, climate change: A comparison of U.S. versus German residents’ en-
T. Dietz, N. Dolsak, P. C. Stern, S. Stonich, & E. U. Weber, Eds.). gagement in direct and indirect energy reduction behaviors. Manu-
Washington, DC: National Academy Press. script submitted for publication.
National Research Council. (2010a). Advancing the science of climate Swim, J. K., & Clayton, S. (2010). SPSSI policy statement: SPSSI and
change. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. global climate change. Retrieved from http://www.spssi.org/index.
National Research Council. (2010b). Adapting to the impacts of climate cfm?fuseaction!page.viewpage&pageid!1455
change. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. Swim, J. K., Clayton, S., & Howard, G. S. (2011). Human behavioral
Nickerson, R. S. (2003). Psychology and environmental change. Mahwah, contributions to climate change: Psychological and contextual drivers.
NJ: Erlbaum. American Psychologist, 66, xxx–xxx. doi:10.1037/a0023472
Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Swim, J. K., Markowitz, E. M., & Bloodhart, B. (in press). Psychology
Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231– and climate change: Beliefs, impacts, and human contributions. In S.
259. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.84.3.231 Clayton (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of environmental and conserva-
tion psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Nolan, J. M., Schultz, P. W., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J., & Griskevi-
Thøgersen, J., & Crompton, T. (2009). Simple and painless? The limita-
cius, V. (2008). Normative social influence is underdetected. Person-
tions of spillover in environmental campaigning. Journal of Consumer
ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 913–923. doi:10.1177/
Policy, 32, 141–163. doi:10.1007/s10603-009-9101-1
0146167208316691
Uzzell, D., & Räthzel, N. (2009). Transforming environmental psychol-
Page, L., & Howard, L. (2010). The impact of climate change on mental
ogy. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29(3), 340 –350. doi:
health (but will mental health be discussed at Copenhagen?) Psycho-
10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.11.005
logical Medicine: A Journal of Research in Psychiatry and the Allied
Vandenbergh, M. P., Stern, P. C., Gardner, G. T., Dietz, T., & Gilligan, J.
Sciences, 40, 177–180. doi:10.1017/S0033291709992169
(2010). Implementing the behavioral wedge: Designing and adopting
Reser, J. P. (in press). Public understandings of climate change: Reflec- effective carbon emissions reduction programs. Environmental Law
tions and reframing. Risk Analysis. Reporter, 40, 10545–10552.
Reser, J. P., & Swim, J. K. (2011). Adapting to and coping with the threat Vlek, C., & Steg, L. (2007). Human behavior and environmental sustain-
and impacts of climate change. American Psychologist, 66, 277–289. ability: Problems, driving forces, and research topics. Journal of Social
doi:10.1037/a0023412 Issues, 63(1), 1–19. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.2007.00493.x
Reyes, G., & Jacobs, G. A. (Eds.). (2006). Handbook of international Wapner, S., & Demick, J. (2002). The increasing contexts of context in the
disaster psychology (Vols. 1– 4). Westport, CT: Praeger. study of environment behavior relations. In R. B. Bechtel & A. Church-
Schmuck, P., & Schultz, W. P. (Eds.). (2002). The psychology of sustain- man (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology (pp. 3–14). Hobo-
able development. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic. ken, NJ: Wiley.
Shogren, J. R., Parkhurst, G., & Banerjee, P. (2010). Two cheers and a Weber, E. U. (2006). Experience-based and description-based perceptions
qualm for behavioral environmental economics. Environmental and of long-term risk: Why global warming does not scare us (yet). Climatic
Resource Economics, 46(2), 235–247. doi:10.1007/s10640-010-9376-3 Change, 77, 103–120. doi:10.1007/s10584-006-9060-3
Sjöberg, L. (1989). Global change and human action: Psychological Weber, E. U. (2010). What shapes perceptions of climate change? Wiley
perspectives. International Social Science Journal, 41, 413– 432. Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change, 1(3). doi:10.1002/wcc.41
Solomon, S., Plattner, G., Knutti, R., & Friedlingstein, P. (2009). Irre- Weber, E. U., Johnson, E. J., Milch, K., Chang, H., Brodscholl, J., &
versible climate change due to carbon dioxide emissions. Proceedings Goldstein, D. (2007). Asymmetric discounting in intertemporal choice:
of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 106(6), 1704 –1709. doi: A query theory account. Psychological Science, 18, 516 –523. doi:
10.1073/pnas.0812721106 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01932.x
Sontag, D. (2010, March 20). In Haiti, mental health system is in collapse. Weber, E. U., & Stern, P. C. (2011). Public understanding of climate
The New York Times, p. A1. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/ change in the United States. American Psychologist, 66, 315–328.
2010/03/20/world/americas/20haiti.html?hp doi:10.1037/a0023253
Spence, A., Pidgeon, N., & Uzzell, D. (2009). Climate change: Psychol- Winkel, G., Saegert, S., & Evans, G. W. (2009). An ecological perspective
ogy’s contribution. The Psychologist, 21(2), 108 –111. on theory, methods, and analysis in environmental psychology: Ad-
Spratt, D., & Sutton, P. (2008). Climate code red: The case for a vances and challenges. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29, 318 –
sustainable emergency. Fitzroy, Melbourne, Australia: Friends of the 328. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.02.005
Earth. World Health Organization. (2010). Climate change and human health.
Stern, P. C. (1986). Blind spots in policy analysis: What economics Retrieved from http://www.who.int/globalchange/en/

250 May–June 2011 ● American Psychologist

View publication stats

You might also like