Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Articles

Long-term effects of migraine on


cognitive function
A population-based study of Danish twins
D. Gaist, MD, PhD; L. Pedersen, MSc; C. Madsen, MD; I. Tsiropoulos, MD; S. Bak, MD, PhD;
S. Sindrup, MD, PhD; M. McGue, PhD; B. Krogh Rasmussen, MD, PhD; and K. Christensen, MD, PhD

Abstract—Objective: To investigate the cognitive functioning of migraineurs vs nonmigraineurs in a large population-


based sample of middle-aged twins where headache diagnoses were established by neurologists. Methods: Twins identified
through the population-based Danish Twin Registry participated in face-to-face structured interviews, which included
cognitive tests and two previously validated questions screening for migraine. Twins who screened positive for migraine
and their co-twins were invited to participate in a telephone-based interview conducted by neurologists, who established
headache diagnoses according to the International Headache Society criteria. Cognitive scores on fluency, digit span,
delayed word recall, and symbol digit substitution test were compared between migraineurs and nonmigraineurs. Com-
parisons within monozygotic and dizygotic same sex twin pairs discordant for migraine were also performed. Results: Of
the 1,789 twins who were eligible for inclusion in the present study, 1,393 (77.8%) were interviewed. A diagnosis of
migraine was established in 536 twins (migraine without aura n ⫽ 347; migraine with aura n ⫽ 157). Average scores on
cognitive tests in twins with migraine or one of the migraine subtypes did not differ from those of nonmigraineurs in any
of the tests. Comparisons within twin pairs discordant for migraine produced highly comparable results. Adjustment for
possible confounders and stratification by cumulated number of lifetime attacks did not influence the results. Conclusions:
A lifetime diagnosis of migraine was not associated with cognitive deficits in middle-aged subjects.
NEUROLOGY 2005;64:600 –607

A recent cross-sectional study found that subjects reported for both migraine without aura (MO) and
with migraine, in particular migraine with aura MA.4-7 Other studies addressing the same issue
(MA), were more prone to subclinical strokes in the found no differences in cognitive skills between mi-
area supplied by the posterial cerebral artery as graineurs and controls.8-12 Potential problems of sam-
evaluated by brain MR images.1 The study also re- ple size, subject selection, the method used to
ported an increased occurrence of deep white matter establish headache diagnoses, and other method-
lesions in female migraineurs.1 Since such lesions ologic issues may, at least in part, explain the con-
accrue with age and are associated with cognitive flicting results reached by these studies. We sought
decline and dementia this finding has led to renewed to overcome several methodologic shortcomings by
speculation on whether migraine is a progressive conducting a large population-based study among
brain disease.2,3 middle-aged Danish twins in which headache diag-
A number of studies have concluded that migraine noses were established by neurologists according to
has a deleterious effect on cognitive skills, a finding the International Headache Society (IHS) criteria.13

Methods. The subjects were identified and included through a


Additional material related to this article can be found on the Neurology
two-step procedure. First, a large cohort of twins was traced
Web site. Go to www.neurology.org and scroll down the Table of Con-
through a population-based register and invited to participate in a
tents for the February 22 issue to find the title link for this article.
structured interview (baseline interview), which included cogni-

See also page 590

From the Danish Twin Register & Epidemiology (Drs. Gaist, Bak, and Christensen), Institute of Public Health, University of Southern Denmark;
Department of Neurology (Drs. Gaist, Madsen, Tsiropoulos, Bak, and Sindrup), Odense University Hospital; Department of Clinical Epidemiology (Dr.
Pedersen), Aarhus & Aalborg University Hospital, Denmark; Department of Psychology (Dr. McGue), University of Minnesota, Minneapolis; and Department
of Neurology (Dr. Krogh Rasmussen), Hillerød Hospital, Denmark.
Supported by the Danish Medical Research Council (grant 22– 00 – 0451) and the Danish Headache Society.
Received May 27, 2004. Accepted in final form October 25, 2004.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. David Gaist, Epidemiology, IST, University of Southern Denmark, Sdr. Boulevard 23A, 5000 Odense C,
Denmark; e-mail: dg@dadlnet.dk

600 Copyright © 2005 by AAN Enterprises, Inc.


tive tests and questions screening for migraine. In the second socioeconomic status, educational attainment) as well as disor-
step, all twins who screened positive on the migraine questions ders, diseases, and life events (depression, stroke, chronic tension
and their co-twins were invited to be telephone interviewed by type headache, head trauma) have been reported to be linked with
neurologists regarding their headaches (neurologist interview). cognitive skills, and could, if distributed unevenly between mi-
Baseline interview. The Study of Middle Aged Danish Twins graineurs and nonmigraineurs, confound the association between
Survey (MADT) has previously been described.14 In brief, 40 pairs migraine and cognitive functioning. We used information gathered
of each zygosity (monozygotic [MZ], dizygotic same sex [DZss], and during the baseline interview and the neurologist interview to
dizygotic opposite sex [DZos]) were selected at random from the control for these potential confounders.
population-based Danish Twin Registry15 within each of the 22 We used information collected at the time of the baseline inter-
birth cohorts 1931 through 1952. Owing to insufficient numbers of view to classify the subjects’ smoking (current, past, never) and
MZ pairs in the birth cohorts of 1933, 1934, and 1936, we re- alcohol habits (abstainer, 0 to 9, 10 to 19, 20⫹ drinks per week),
trieved an additional 11 MZ pairs from the birth cohorts of 1931 number of years of schooling (⬍9, 9 to 10, 11⫹ years), and voca-
and 1935. Of the 2,640 pairs identified, 5,189 twins were eligible tional training (0, 1 to 2, 3 to 4, 5⫹ years), and self-reported
for participation in the study (91 twins were deceased or had physician lifetime diagnoses of depression, medication treated hy-
moved). The subjects had all previously answered a questionnaire pertension, or other diseases with potential influence on cognitive
that included items on similarity of the twins, based on which we skills (stroke, PD, epilepsy, or hypothyroidism). Depressive symp-
assigned zygosity, a method found to result in misclassification toms at the time of the baseline interview were furthermore as-
rates of less than 5%.16 sessed by an adapted form of the Camdex Depression Scale24 and
Face-to-face interviews in the twins’ homes were conducted by subjects were classified into three groups based on the quartile
a total of 100 interviewers from the Danish National Institute of values of the baseline cohort (lower quartile [⬍22], 2nd and 3rd
Social Research.17 The survey, which lasted an average of 11/2 quartile [22 to 24], upper quartile [25⫹]). Subjects were asked
hours, comprised an extensive structured questionnaire, tests of whether they had ever received a blow to the head resulting in
cognitive and physical functioning, and sampling of DNA. All in- loss of consciousness, and twins answering affirmatively were
terviewers received a detailed training program by a physician identified. At the time of the baseline interview subjects were
(D.G.) and were closely monitored in the 6-month period (October asked to report any use of medication within the prior 14 days. All
1998 through March 1999) during which the interviews were com- reported drugs were subsequently classified by a chemist accord-
pleted. To avoid interviewer bias, which would inflate twin simi- ing to the Anatomic Therapeutic Chemical (ATC) system.25 Sub-
larity, twins from a pair were never interviewed by the same jects reporting use of drugs belonging to ATC codes N05
interviewer. In all, 4,314 (83.1%) twins participated in the base- (anxiolytics and antipsychotic drugs), N02A (strong analgesics), or
line interview. Participation of intact pairs was highest for MZ N06 (antidepressants) were identified. We also identified all sub-
males (77.7%) and lowest for DZos (72.9%). Nonparticipation was jects with a diagnosis of chronic tension-type headache (IHS 2.2)
not highly selected, but showed patterns recognized from other as established through the neurologist interview.
studies: a tendency for lower participation of fraternal twins, per- Finally, we identified monozygotic and same sex dizygotic
sons not married, and urban area dwellers. However, men were pairs where both twins in a pair participated in the present study
more likely to participate than women.14 but only one of the twins was classified as having migraine accord-
Cognitive function measures. A series of cognitive tests18,19 ing to the neurologist interviews, i.e., pairs discordant for mi-
was integrated in the baseline interview and included a test of graine. The comparisons for education and cognitive test scores we
fluency, forward and backward digit span, a modified 12-word carried out within these pairs not only adjust for the effects of
learning test with immediate and delayed word recall, and a genes (100% in MZ twins and 50% in DZ twins), age, and sex, but
symbol-digit substitution test. We assessed the subjects’ cognitive also adjust for socioeconomic status in childhood.
function through their performance on the fluency test, the digit Since migraine is more prevalent in women and several poten-
span (combined sum of scores on forward and backward digit tial confounders vary in their distribution across sexes we chose to
span), the delayed word recall test, and the digit-symbol substitu- present all main results stratified by sex.
tion test. The fluency test requires the examinee to name as many Statistical analysis. Individual-based analyses. We com-
animals as he or she can in 1 minute. The test is useful for pared cognitive scores across strata of sex, age, and educational
detecting frontal lobe damage and early mental decline.20,21 The attainment in participants of the baseline interview. To assess the
digit span test and the delayed word recall test are widely used influence of nonparticipation in the present study, we compared
tests of working memory and long-term memory. The digit-symbol means and distributions of cognitive measures and potential con-
substitution test is a timed test that involves the pairing of num- founders in neurology interview participants and nonparticipants.
bers with corresponding symbols according to a code that is visible These comparisons were also performed between participants
to the participant. This test has frequently been employed in with migraine including the major subtypes (MO and MA) and
neuropsychologic and epidemiologic contexts to assess sustained nonmigraineurs. The differences in cognitive test scores between
attention and psychomotor speed.22 Furthermore, similar versions migraineurs and nonmigraineurs were furthermore adjusted for
of the digit span, delayed word recall, and the symbol substitution the effect of potential confounders in an analysis of variance
test have been used in previous studies of migraine and cognitive (ANOVA) model. We had a priori selected age, sex, educational
function.5,6,9,11 attainment, and depressive symptom score as variables to be in-
Neurologist interview. The following two questions were used cluded in the model. Since we cannot overrule that educational
to screen for migraine in the questionnaire part of the baseline attainment and depression lie on the causal pathway of a poten-
interview: “Have you ever had migraine?” and “Have you ever had tial association between migraine and cognitive performance, we
visual disturbances lasting from 5 to 60 minutes, which were also fitted models where these variables were not included. Other
followed by a headache?” Twins answering positively to either of variables were only included in the model if bivariate analyses
these questions were considered screening positive for migraine. showed that their distribution varied significantly (p ⱕ 0.05) be-
These questions were previously employed in a study on Danish tween migraineurs and nonmigraineurs. However, the effect of
twins.23 To assess the validity of our screening instrument we certain variables that only involved few subjects, e.g., chronic
identified all pairs where both twins had screened negative for tension-type headache, was gauged by comparing the main analy-
migraine and randomly selected 100 subjects from 100 different ses run with and without the involved subjects.
twin pairs. We repeated the analyses after stratifying the subjects into
Twins identified for participation in the telephone interview three age groups (⬍50, 50 to 59, 60⫹). Furthermore, to assess the
were sent a letter of invitation and a brief questionnaire. Nonre- effect of the cumulative number of attacks of migraine on cogni-
sponders were approached up to three times by mail. Responders tive function we compared the mean cognitive scores of subjects
were telephone-interviewed by a neurologist, who used a struc- with MO and MA across three strata of reported lifetime number
tured interview and established the participants’ headaches ac- of attacks: ⬍50, 50 to 99, and 100⫹. We also performed these
cording to the IHS classification, 1st version.13 Four neurologists analyses for strata of duration of migraine attacks (⬍20, 20 to 29,
carried out the interviews and twins from a pair were never inter- and 30⫹ years), and age at migraine debut (⬍20, 20 to 29, 30⫹
viewed by the same neurologist. years old).
Potential confounders. Several lifestyle (smoking, alcohol, Our choice of controls could be questioned due to the inclusion
and medication consumption) and demographic factors (childhood of co-twins to the migraineurs and subjects with self-reported
February (2 of 2) 2005 NEUROLOGY 64 601
Table 1 Mean cognitive scores (SD) of 4,311 Danish twins participating in the baseline interview

Female* Male*

Symbol digit Symbol digit


Fluency, Digit span,† Delayed recall, substitution, Fluency, Digit span,† Delayed recall, substitution,
n ⫽ 2,113 n ⫽ 2,114 n ⫽ 2,108 n ⫽ 2,040 n ⫽ 2,191 n ⫽ 2,195 n ⫽ 2,187 n ⫽ 2,119

All 24.0 (7.3) 10.9 (3.3) 5.5 (2.4) 47.4 (13.2) 24.5 (7.4) 11.1 (3.6) 4.7 (2.2) 46.2 (12.2)
Age, y
⬍50 25.4 (7.1) 11.5 (3.4) 6.4 (2.4) 53.4 (11.7) 25.2 (7.6) 11.4 (3.8) 5.5 (2.3) 50.8 (10.1)
50–54 24.3 (7.3) 10.9 (3.3) 5.8 (2.5) 49.2 (11.2) 25.0 (7.6) 11.5 (3.6) 4.9 (2.2) 48.8 (11.9)
55–59 24.0 (7.4) 10.8 (3.6) 5.3 (2.3) 47.2 (13.5) 24.8 (7.1) 11.1 (3.3) 4.5 (2.2) 45.9 (11.8)
60⫹ 23.1 (7.1) 10.8 (3.2) 4.9 (2.3) 43.1 (13.5) 23.6 (7.2) 10.6 (3.5) 4.4 (2.0) 42.3 (12.4)
Schooling, y
⬍9 22.4 (6.7) 10.1 (2.9) 4.9 (2.3) 42.9 (13.2) 22.9 (7.2) 10.0 (3.8) 4.2 (2.0) 42.1 (12.1)
9–10 25.4 (7.3) 11.6 (3.6) 6.0 (2.3) 51.7 (11.5) 25.8 (6.6) 12.1 (3.6) 5.2 (2.1) 50.8 (9.9)
11⫹ 28.2 (7.4) 12.9 (3.5) 7.1 (2.3) 55.3 (9.4) 29.1 (7.9) 13.7 (3.7) 5.9 (2.4) 54.3 (9.3)
Vocational training, y
0 21.7 (6.7) 9.8 (3.0) 4.7 (2.3) 41.6 (13.1) 21.6 (7.5) 9.4 (3.2) 3.9 (2.2) 39.0 (12.6)
1–2 24.3 (7.1) 11.2 (3.3) 5.7 (2.3) 49.1 (12.3) 24.3 (6.8) 10.9 (3.3) 4.7 (2.1) 46.1 (11.2)
3–4 27.1 (7.3) 12.0 (3.4) 6.4 (2.6) 52.2 (12.8) 26.8 (7.9) 12.7 (3.6) 5.3 (2.3) 52.3 (12.0)
5⫹ 28.2 (7.0) 13.5 (3.1) 7.1 (2.2) 54.4 (7.8) 29.0 (7.0) 13.6 (3.7) 5.8 (2.4) 53.2 (9.4)

* No cognitive tests completed by one woman and six men.


† Sum of scores on digit forward span and digit backward span.

migraine that was not verified at the neurologist interview. We (figure). Participation rate did not vary by sex (78.0% in
therefore repeated the analyses using another control group com- women vs 77.7% in men). We stratified eligible subjects by
prising twins from pairs where both twins had screened negative
for migraine.
sex and migraine screening status and found that in these
Within-pair analyses. We also performed within-pair compar- strata participants were generally younger (mean differ-
isons of the cognitive scores of twins with migraine, MO or MA, ence 0.6 to 1.1 years) than nonparticipants, with the excep-
with that of their nonmigraineur co-twins. Within-pair differences tion of screening positive men (mean difference ⫺0.3
in mean cognitive scores were adjusted for potential confounders years) (table 2). Participants had completed more years of
through an ANOVA model that a priori included educational at-
tainment and depressive symptom score. Other variables were schooling and vocational training, and achieved higher
chosen for inclusion as described above. Within-pair comparisons scores on all cognitive tests vs nonparticipants (see table
for cognitive scores were also performed stratified by age (⬍55, 2). However, with the exception of schooling, the magni-
55⫹ years), cumulative number of attacks (⬍50, 50⫹), and zygos- tude of these differences did not vary substantially by screen-
ity (monozygotic vs same sex dizygotic).
To account for the non-independence of the observations on
ing status (see table 2). In a separate analysis comparing the
twins, subjects from pairs where both twins participated (intact combined group of migraineurs and screening-positive non-
pairs) were analyzed as clusters of two in all multivariate models. participants vs the combined group of non-migraineurs and
Our primary goal was to study whether migraine through its screening-negative nonparticipants (worse case scenario
attacks had a detrimental effect on cognitive skills. We viewed analysis), the two groups achieved highly comparable cogni-
educational attainment as a potential confounder and adjusted for
its effect in the multivariate analyses. However, some mi- tive scores (data not presented).
graineurs have attacks at an age where they might interfere with A lifetime diagnosis of migraine (IHS 1) was established
their schooling or vocational training. Furthermore, according to in 536 subjects henceforth referred to as migraineurs. Mi-
some researchers migraine may be a neurodevelopmental disor- graineurs comprised 347 subjects with MO (IHS 1.1), 157
der26,27 in which case poor educational attainment may be linked
with migraine. We therefore also present data on educational
subjects with MA (IHS 1.2), and 85 twins with migrainous
attainment for both the entire dataset and the intrapair disorder (IHS 1.7). Twins with migrainous disorder ful-
comparisons. filled all but one of the criteria for a diagnosis of MO (83
In the within-pair comparisons we also estimated ORs for edu- twins) or MA (2 twins). A diagnosis of chronic tension-type
cational attainment using conditional logistic regression. In order headache (IHS 2.2) was established in 68 subjects, and a
to provide more robust estimates we collapsed the data into two
groups for this analysis (schooling: ⬍9, 10⫹ years; vocational further 5 subjects had episodic cluster headache (IHS 3.1).
training: 0 to 2, 3⫹ years). In all, 64 subjects were diagnosed with secondary head-
aches. Although 17 of these subjects also had migraine, the
Results. The cognitive scores of participants in the base- debut of the primary and secondary headaches was sepa-
line interview varied across strata of sex, age, and educa- rated by more than a year in all cases. Finally, 51 mi-
tional attainment in a predictable manner (table 1). Of the graineurs and 5 nonmigraineurs had had a headache that
4,314 twins participating in the baseline survey, 1,789 they could not recall in sufficient detail to permit a diagno-
were eligible for inclusion in the neurologist interview, and sis to be established and were therefore classified under
1,393 (77.9%) subjects aged 45 to 67 years participated IHS 13, ”Headache not classifiable.”
602 NEUROLOGY 64 February (2 of 2) 2005
migraine (see table 3). Comparison of schooling and voca-
tional training between twins with migraine or one of the
migraine subtypes and nonmigraineurs did not reveal any
consistent patterns for migraine and MO (table 4). How-
ever, subjects with MA of both sexes had higher schooling
than nonmigraineurs, and female twins with MA had re-
ceived more vocational training than female nonmi-
graineurs, although none of these differences were
significant. Sex-stratified cognitive scores of migraineurs
and nonmigraineurs were highly comparable, although
subjects with migraine, MO or MA, consistently scored
marginally better than nonmigraineurs (see table 4).
Stratifying by age produced similar results (data not pre-
sented) as did adjusting for potential confounders (see ta-
ble E-1 on the Neurology Web site at www.neurology.org).
Consecutive inclusion of variables in the multivariate mod-
els as described in Methods had little impact on the results
(data not presented). Repeating the analyses with the
2,599 screening-negative twins as a control group produced
highly comparable results (data not presented).
Cognitive scores showed little variation when stratified
by the cumulative number of attacks of MO a subject had
had. Although not significantly so, subjects who reported
having had more than 100 attacks of MA had lower scores
on the digit span (mean 11.1 [SD: 3.4]), delayed word recall
(5.4 [2.4]), and symbol digit substitution test (47.7 [11.6])
than both subjects with 50 to 100 lifetime attacks (12.7
[4.9]; 6.0 [1.9]]; 48.2 [9.7]) and less than 50 lifetime attacks
(11.8 [3.9]; 5.9 [2.0]; 49.3 [12.1]) of MA. These differences
were less marked in women and were attenuated after
adjustment for potential confounders (data not presented).
No association was found between the age at migraine
Figure. Flow chart of study participation. *Baseline in-
debut or the number of years with migraine and cognitive
terview comprised 4,314 twins. However, in all, 22 twins
skills (data not presented).
emigrated (3), died (16), or were lost to follow-up due to
Within–pair comparisons. We identified a total of 139
secret address (3) after completion of baseline interview,
twin pairs (61 monozygotic and 78 same sex dizygotic) that
but before selection for present study. A further three twins
were discordant for migraine, MO or MA. Comparisons of
did not respond to migraine screening questions and were
mean cognitive scores revealed only minor differences of
also excluded. One twin died after random selection but
similar magnitude and direction as the individual-based
before contact was established.
analyses described above, in spite of the fact that the twins
with migraine had fewer years of schooling and vocational
A total of 75 twins participated from the random sample training than their co-twins (table 5). Adjusted within-pair
of 100 pairs where both twins were screening negative. A comparisons and analyses stratified by age and sex, and
diagnosis of MO was made in one twin, MA in none, and cumulative number of lifetime attacks produced similar
migrainous disorder in two twins. The negative predictive results (data not presented).
value of the screening questions for migraine including Twins with migraine had completed fewer years of
migrainous disorder (IHS 1) was 96.0% (95% CI [88.8% to schooling (10⫹ years OR: 0.31 [0.14 to 0.74]), and this
99.2%]), for MO it was 98.7% (95% CI [92.8% to 99.9%]), difference was also apparent in twin pairs discordant for
and for MA 100% (one sided 97.5% CI: 95.2%). MO and MA (see table 5). Twins with migraine, MO, or
Compared with nonmigraineurs of the same sex, mi- MA were not significantly different with regard to voca-
graineurs were of similar age, but less frequently single, tional training compared with their co-twins (see table 5).
current smokers, or heavy alcohol drinkers, and these dif-
ferences were more marked in men (table 3). Male mi-
Discussion. We found that 536 middle-aged Danes
graineurs also had a higher current depressive symptom
score compared with nonmigraineurs (p ⫽ 0.08). A neurol-
with a lifetime diagnosis of migraine or one of its
ogist diagnosis of chronic tension-type headache was 1.7 main subtypes achieved cognitive scores that were
times more common in women (p ⫽ 0.05) and 4.5 times highly comparable with those of 857 nonmigraineurs
more common in male migraineurs (p ⫽ 0.001) vs nonmi- in the same age group. Our results were not influ-
graineurs of the same sex. Female migraineurs were more enced by several potential confounders. Stratifica-
frequently current users of medications with a potential tion by number of lifetime attacks of MO or MA had
negative influence on cognition than nonmigraineurs no major impact on our results. Our study suggests
(13.6% vs 9.4%; p ⫽ 0.07) (see table 3). Less marked differ- that a lifetime history of migraine or one of its prin-
ences were found for a number of other potential confound- cipal subtypes has little impact on cognitive function.
ers of the association between cognitive skills and A number of studies provide evidence of cognitive
February (2 of 2) 2005 NEUROLOGY 64 603
Table 2 Eligible subjects stratified by sex, migraine screening status, and participation in present study (n ⫽ 1,789)

Female Male

Screening positive Screening negative Screening positive Screening negative

Participants, Nonparticipants, Participants, Nonparticipants, Participants, Nonparticipants, Participants, Nonparticipants,


n ⫽ 570 n ⫽ 148 n ⫽ 240 n ⫽ 81 n ⫽ 302 n ⫽ 82 n ⫽ 281 n ⫽ 85

Age, y, mean (SD) 56.2 (6.1) 58.3 (5.9) 56.2 (6.2) 58.2 (6.0) 56.5 (6.3) 56.2 (6.5) 56.9 (6.3) 57.5 (6.9)
Schooling, y, n (%)
⬍9 288 (50.5) 101 (68.2) 130 (54.2) 57 (70.4) 156 (51.7) 51 (62.2) 156 (55.5) 57 (67.1)
9–10 219 (38.4) 42 (28.4) 91 (37.9) 18 (22.2) 109 (36.1) 26 (31.7) 90 (32.0) 17 (20.0)
11⫹ 63 (11.1) 4 (2.7) 19 (7.9) 6 (7.4) 37 (12.3) 5 (6.1) 35 (12.5) 11 (12.9)
Vocational training, y,
n (%)
0 148 (26.0) 73 (49.3) 76 (31.7) 42 (51.9) 51 (16.9) 26 (31.7) 44 (15.7) 28 (32.9)
1–2 314 (55.1) 59 (39.9) 124 (51.7) 26 (32.1) 177 (58.6) 47 (57.3) 167 (59.4) 46 (54.1)
3–4 89 (15.6) 14 (9.5) 33 (13.8) 13 (16.0) 41 (13.6) 5 (6.1) 43 (15.3) 7 (8.2)
5⫹ 18 (3.2) 2 (1.4) 7 (2.9) 0 (0.0) 33 (10.9) 4 (4.9) 27 (9.6) 4 (4.7)
Cognitive test scores,
mean (SD)
Fluency* 24.4 (7.0) 21.6 (8.0) 24.4 (6.4) 21.9 (7.5) 24.9 (7.1) 22.0 (8.0) 25.3 (7.1) 23.2 (6.6)
Digit span† 11.0 (3.5) 9.8 (3.5) 11.2 (3.2) 10.0 (3.3) 11.3 (3.6) 10.5 (3.7) 11.4 (3.4) 10.4 (3.2)
Delayed word 5.8 (2.4) 4.8 (2.6) 5.7 (2.2) 5.0 (2.5) 4.8 (2.3) 4.5 (2.2) 4.8 (2.2) 3.8 (2.0)
recall‡
Symbol digit 48.9 (12.5) 43.6 (13.9) 48.2 (13.7) 42.7 (13.1) 46.9 (12.9) 44.3 (13.7) 45.7 (11.0) 42.9 (13.9)
substitution§

* One missing value.


† Sum of score for digit forward and digit backward span.
‡ Three missing values.
§ Fifty-nine missing values (female screening positive: 21 participants, 7 nonparticipants; female screening negative: 3 participants, 4 nonparticipants,
male screening positive: 12 participants, 3 nonparticipants; male screening negative: 4 participants, 5 nonparticipants).

deficits in migraineurs, in particular in subjects with left-handed and had lower performances at age 3 to
MA,5-7,28 involving psychomotor skills,4,6,7 memory,4,5,7 13 on tests of verbal ability, but not on other tests of
and dysfunctions in the early stages of visual pro- IQ.27 Migraineurs in that study had also performed
cessing.29 However, in other studies of this issue no less well on school examinations at the age of 15 to
evidence of interictal cortical dysfunction was found in 17 and had not achieved bachelor degrees as fre-
subjects with migraine or MA.8-12 Several authors have quently as headache-free subjects.27 Also, a number
suggested that these inconsistencies in the literature of cross-sectional studies, in particular from the
may be due to methodologic issues such as sample size, United States, have reported an inverse relationship
highly unrepresentative samples, and lack of adequate between migraine prevalence and socioeconomic sta-
comparison groups.6,9,27 Interestingly, our results are in tus as measured by education or income,30-33 al-
line with the only other study of the subject that was though this association was less pronounced in a
carried out in a large population-based setting. This more recent study.34 In our study, we found no evi-
study identified 99 middle-aged subjects with self- dence of a link between educational attainment and
reported current migraine within the cohort of the migraine in the overall analyses. However, in the
Maastricht Aging Study and found that they fared within-pair comparisons, which are particularly in-
equally well on the letter-digit substitution test and teresting since these analyses control the large num-
delayed recall tests compared with 1,753 nonmi- ber of factors the twins share including childhood
graineurs from the same cohort.11 socioeconomic status, a tendency toward lower num-
Some researchers believe that migraine may be a ber of years of schooling in subjects with migraine,
neurodevelopmental disorder, in which case cogni- and in particular MA, was found. However, as the
tive function would be expected to be affected even confidence limits indicate, our results concerning ed-
before the onset of migraine attacks.26,27 A cohort of ucational attainment in the within-pair analysis
980 3-year-old children from New Zealand were fol- should be interpreted with caution.
lowed up for 23 years, serially subjected to neuropsy- Our study has a number of strengths. The sample
chological tests, and had migraine diagnoses is large and population-based, and the participation
established according to the IHS criteria at age 26. rate was high. The cognitive tests we used are sensi-
Compared with headache-free individuals, subjects tive to even mild cognitive impairment as demon-
who later developed migraine were more frequently strated by the analyses of the entire baseline sample
604 NEUROLOGY 64 February (2 of 2) 2005
Table 3 Characteristics of study participants stratified by sex and migraine diagnosis (n ⫽ 1,393)

Female Male

Migraineurs, Nonmigraineurs, p Migraineurs, Nonmigraineurs, p


n ⫽ 376 n ⫽ 434 Value* n ⫽ 160 n ⫽ 423 Value*

Age, y, mean (SD) 56.4 (6.2) 56.1 (6.1) 0.48 56.3 (6.1) 56.8 (6.4) 0.31
Married or cohabiting 301 (80.1) 338 (77.9) 0.45 147 (91.9) 347 (82.0) 0.003
Alcohol,† drinks per week
Abstainers 46 (12.2) 48 (11.1) 0.57 4 (2.5) 21 (5.0) 0.09
0–9 274 (72.9) 312 (71.9) 94 (58.8) 211 (50.0)
10–19 42 (11.2) 57 (13.1) 42 (26.3) 114 (27.0)
20⫹ 10 (2.7) 13 (3.0) 20 (12.5) 76 (18.0)
Smoker
Current 140 (37.2) 178 (41.0) 0.32 52 (32.5) 183 (43.3) 0.05
Past 87 (23.1) 83 (19.1) 61 (38.1) 146 (34.5)
Never 149 (39.6) 173 (39.9) 47 (29.4) 94 (22.2)
Current depressive symptom score†‡
⬍22 103 (27.4) 98 (22.6) 0.30 53 (33.1) 179 (42.3) 0.08
22–24 131 (34.8) 161 (37.2) 54 (33.8) 137 (32.4)
25⫹ 142 (37.8) 174 (40.1) 53 (33.1) 107 (25.3)
Ever self-reported physician diagnosed
Depression 80 (21.3) 74 (17.1) 0.13 17 (10.6) 45 (10.6) 0.99
Hypertension (drug treated) 75 (19.9) 74 (17.1) 0.29 24 (15.0) 62 (14.7) 0.92
Other disease with potential influence 26 (6.9) 41 (9.4) 0.19 8 (5.0) 17 (4.0) 0.60
on cognition§
Ever head-blow with loss of 76 (20.2) 83 (19.1) 0.71 53 (33.1) 126 (29.8) 0.45
consciousness†
Current drug use with potential cognitive 51 (13.6) 41 (9.4) 0.07 9 (5.6) 32 (7.6) 0.41
influence
Chronic tension-type headache§ 31 (8.2) 21 (4.8) 0.05 10 (6.3) 6 (1.4) 0.001

Numbers (%) unless otherwise stated.

* Comparison between migraineurs and nonmigraineurs, ␹2 test for proportions and t-test for means.
† Information was missing for nine twins on alcohol, one on depressive symptom score, two on head-blow with LOC.
‡ Assessed by an adapted form of the Camdex Depression Scale (see ref. Johnson 2003).
§ According to International Headache Society criteria (2.2) and established through neurologist interview.

of 4,311 twins where poorer cognitive scores were were screening negative. Our validation study indi-
achieved with increasing age, even within the rela- cated that this is not a major concern, since the
tively narrow age-bands employed. Headache diag- negative predictive value of the screening instru-
noses were established by experienced neurologists ment was very high. Nonparticipants clearly had
according to the IHS criteria. The cognitive tests and lower cognitive scores and educational attainment
the questions screening for migraine were adminis- than participants, which could have biased our re-
tered as part of the baseline interview, at which time sults. However, the participation rate was high and
both the trained interviewers and the subjects were most of the observed differences were equally pro-
unaware of our research hypothesis. The extensive nounced among screening positive and screening
baseline interview information allowed us to gauge negative eligible subjects. Furthermore, cognitive
the effect of, and if necessary control for a multitude scores of migraineurs and nonmigraineurs remained
of confounders. Finally, the twin design enabled us highly comparable even after we included nonpartic-
to perform within-pair comparisons where we were ipants according to screening status (worse case
also able to adjust for any potential confounding ef- scenario analysis). We therefore believe that nonpar-
fects of childhood socioeconomic environment. ticipation had no major impact on our results. Our
Our study also has potential limitations. We used choice of control group could be questioned, mostly
a screening instrument to identify potential mi- owing to the high proportion of unconfirmed self-
graineurs. This approach could have resulted in false reported migraine. However, repeating the analyses
negative cases, i.e., subjects who had migraine but with the use of a large control group unrelated to the
February (2 of 2) 2005 NEUROLOGY 64 605
Table 4 Education and cognitive test scores of 1,393 middle-aged Danish twins by sex and migraine diagnosis

Migraine Migraine without aura Migraine with aura Nonmigraineurs

Female, Male, Female, Male, Female, Male, Female, Male,


n ⫽ 376 n ⫽ 160 n ⫽ 255 n ⫽ 92 n ⫽ 100 n ⫽ 57 n ⫽ 434 n ⫽ 423

Cognitive tests,* mean (SD)


Fluency 24.5 (6.3) 25.2 (7.0) 24.3 (6.9) 25.6 (6.9) 24.9 (5.5) 25.9 (7.1) 24.2 (7.2) 25.0 (7.0)
Digit span 11.2 (3.6) 11.4 (3.9) 11.1 (3.5) 11.5 (3.9) 11.6 (3.9) 12.1 (4.2) 11.0 (3.2) 11.3 (3.4)
Delayed word recall† 5.9 (2.2) 5.0 (2.2) 5.8 (2.3) 4.8 (2.4) 5.9 (2.3) 5.4 (1.8) 5.7 (2.4) 4.7 (2.3)
Symbol digit substitution‡ 49.0 (12.6) 47.1 (11.7) 48.9 (12.4) 47.2 (11.6) 49.3 (11.9) 47.1 (10.5) 48.4 (13.1) 46.0 (12.2)
Schooling,* y, n (%)
⬍9 197 (52.4) 87 (54.4) 146 (57.3) 48 (52.2) 43 (43.0) 25 (43.9) 221 (50.9) 225 (53.2)
9–10 137 (36.4) 53 (33.1) 83 (32.5) 35 (38.0) 41 (41.0) 22 (38.6) 173 (39.9) 146 (34.5)
11⫹ 42 (11.2) 20 (12.5) 26 (10.2) 9 (9.8) 16 (16.0) 10 (17.5) 40 (9.2) 52 (12.3)
Vocational training,* y, n (%)
0 102 (27.1) 29 (18.1) 74 (29.0) 15 (16.3) 20 (20.0) 10 (17.5) 122 (28.1) 66 (15.6)
1–2 200 (53.2) 93 (58.1) 135 (52.9) 54 (58.7) 57 (57.0) 31 (54.4) 238 (54.8) 251 (59.3)
3–4 63 (16.8) 21 (13.1) 39 (15.3) 11 (12.0) 21 (21.0) 10 (17.5) 59 (13.6) 63 (14.9)
5⫹ 10 (2.7) 17 (10.6) 6 (2.4) 12 (13.0) 2 (2.0) 6 (10.5) 15 (3.5) 43 (10.2)

* Migraineurs (migraine, MO, or MA) compared with nonmigraineurs of same sex; t-test for means and ␹2 test for proportions. Men
with MA had better score on delayed recall (p ⫽ 0.03) compared with nonmigraineurs. All other tests produced p values greater than 0.10.
† Test not performed by one female and one male nonmigraineur.
‡ Test not performed by 11 female and 3 male migraineurs and 13 female and 13 male nonmigraineurs.

migraineurs and consisting entirely of subjects from some subjects who had problems recalling lifetime
pairs where both twins screened negative for mi- headache characteristics. We cannot overrule that
graine did not influence our results. It could be ar- this is the case for some migraineurs, in particular
gued that the age group studied may have included those with milder or less frequent headache attacks.

Table 5 Cognitive scores and educational attainment of 139 middle-aged Danish twin pairs discordant for a lifetime diagnosis of
migraine

Migraine, n ⫽ 139 pairs Migraine without aura, n ⫽ 94 pairs Migraine with aura, n ⫽ 30 pairs

Adjusted Adjusted Adjusted


difference in difference in difference in
cognitive score cognitive score cognitive score
Case Co-twin (95% CI)* Case Co-twin (95% CI)* Case Co-twin (95% CI)*

Cognitive tests,
mean (SD)
Fluency 24.4 (6.8) 24.5 (7.0) 0.3 (⫺1.2 to 1.8) 24.7 (6.9) 24.3 (7.3) 0.9 (⫺1.1 to 2.9) 24.1 (8.1) 24.9 (7.0) ⫺0.4 (⫺5.7 to 4.9)
Digit span 11.2 (3.5) 10.6 (3.0) 0.9 (0.2 to 1.6) 11.0 (3.5) 10.4 (2.9) 0.9 (⫺0.1 to 1.8) 11.7 (3.5) 11.5 (3.0) 0.4 (⫺1.2 to 2.0)
Delayed word 5.6 (2.4) 5.5 (2.3) 0.3 (⫺0.2 to 0.7) 5.7 (2.4) 5.4 (2.3) 0.5 (⫺0.1 to 1.1) 5.6 (2.4) 5.5 (2.2) 0.4 (⫺0.9 to 1.7)
recall
Symbol digit 48.2 (11.2) 46.3 (13.0) 3.0 (0.6 to 5.4) 47.7 (10.1) 45.0 (13.0) 3.5 (0.5 to 6.5) 48.4 (11.3) 47.8 (13.8) 2.9 (⫺3.9 to 9.6)
substitution†
Schooling, y, n (%) OR OR OR
(95% CI)‡ (95% CI)‡ (95% CI)‡

⬍9 78 (56.1) 63 (45.3) 1 (reference) 55 (58.5) 43 (45.7) 1 (reference) 15 (50.0) 10 (33.3) 1 (reference)


10⫹ 61 (43.9) 76 (54.7) 0.31 (0.14–0.74) 39 (41.5) 51 (54.3) 0.25 (0.08–0.74) 15 (50.0) 20 (66.7) 0.17 (0.02–1.38)
Vocational training,§
y, n (%)
0–2 110 (79.7) 102 (73.9) 1 (reference) 75 (80.7) 70 (75.3) 1 (reference) 21 (70.0) 20 (66.7) 1 (reference)
3⫹ 28 (20.3) 36 (26.1) 0.53 (0.24–1.19) 18 (19.4) 23 (24.7) 0.55 (0.20–1.47) 9 (30.0) 10 (33.3) 0.67 (0.11–3.98)

* Difference in cognitive score (95% CI) adjusted for schooling, vocational education, depressive symptom score, and smoking.
† Missing information on four migraineurs and three nonmigraineurs (seven migraine pairs and three migraine without aura pairs).
‡ OR (95% CI) estimated through conditional logistic regression.
§ One pair not included due to missing information on one subject with migraine.

606 NEUROLOGY 64 February (2 of 2) 2005


Several lines of evidence in our data suggest that 10. Palmer JE, Chronicle EP. Cognitive processing in migraine: a failure to
find facilitation in patients with aura. Cephalalgia 1998;18:125–132.
this is not a major problem. Our finding of highly 11. Jelicic M, van Boxtel MPJ, Houx PJ, Jolles J. Does migraine headache
comparable cognitive scores between migraineurs affect cognitive function in the elderly? Report from the Maastricht
Aging Study (MAAS). Headache 2000;40:715–719.
and nonmigraineurs was not affected by age stratifi- 12. Haverkamp F, Hönscheid A, Müller-Sinik K. Cognitive development in
cation. The youngest age stratum included subjects children with migraine and their healthy unaffected siblings. Headache
who were 46 to 49 years old, an age group where we 2002;42:776 –779.
13. Headache Classification Committee of the International Headache Soci-
would not expect major problems in recollecting mi- ety. Classification and diagnostic criteria for headache disorders, cra-
graine attacks. Furthermore, the neurologists re- nial neuralgias, and facial pain. Cephalalgia 1988;8(suppl 7):1–96.
14. Gaist D, Bathum L, Skytthe A, et al. Strength and anthropometric
ported that they could not classify a headache due to measures in identical and fraternal twins: no evidence of masculiniza-
lack of detail on characteristics in only five nonmi- tion of females with male co-twins. Epidemiology 2000;11:340 –343.
15. Skytthe A, Kyvik K, Holm NV, Vaupel JW, Christensen K. The Danish
graineurs. Finally, using the method described in a Twin Registry: 127 birth cohorts of twins. Twin Res 2002;5:352–357.
previous Danish twin study,35 we estimated the ad- 16. Hauge M. The Danish Twin Register. In: Mednick SA, Baert AE, Back-
man BP, eds. Prospective longitudinal research: an empirical basis for
justed lifetime prevalence of migraine, MO, and MA the primary prevention of psychosocial disorders. London: Oxford Uni-
in the entire baseline cohort to be 20.0% (95% CI: versity Press, 1981;218 –221.
18.9% to 21.2%), 12.4% (11.5% to 13.5%), and 5.6% 17. Christensen K, Holm NV, McGue M, Corder L, Vaupel JW. A Danish
population-based twin study on general health in the elderly. J Aging
(5.0% to 6.4%). These estimates are highly compara- Health 1999;11:49 – 64.
ble with previous epidemiologic studies of the preva- 18. McGue M, Christensen K. The heritability of cognitive functioning in
the very old: evidence from Danish twins aged 75 years and older.
lence of migraine in Denmark.36,37 Psychol Aging 2001;16:272–280.
In older studies twins achieved lower mean cogni- 19. Andersen K, Nybo H, Gaist D, et al. Cognitive impairment and mortal-
ity among nonagenarians: The Danish 1905 cohort survey. Dement
tive test scores compared with singletons.38,39 How- Geriatr Cogn Disord 2002;13:156 –163.
ever, more recent research that overcame the 20. Benton AL, Eslinger PJ, Damasio AR. Normative observations on neu-
methodologic problems of previous studies found no ropsychological test performances in old age. J Clin Neuropsychol 1981;
3:33– 42.
difference in cognitive skills between twins and sin- 21. Duff Canning SJ, Leach L, Stuss D, Ngo L, Black SE. Diagnostic utility
gletons.40 We therefore believe that the use of twins of abbreviated fluency measures in Alzheimer disease and vascular
dementia. Neurology 2004;62:556 –562.
does not influence the generalizability of our results. 22. Pavlik VN, de Moraes SA, Szklo M, Knopman DS, Mosley TH, Hyman
A recent population-based neuroimaging study re- DJ. Relation between cognitive function and mortality in middle-aged
adults. The atherosclerosis risk in communities study. Am J Epidemiol
ported that migraineurs were at increased risk of 2003;157:327–334.
subclinical strokes in the area supplied by the poste- 23. Gervil M, Ulrich V, Olesen J, Russell MB. Screening for migraine in the
general population: validation of a simple questionnaire. Cephalalgia
rial cerebral artery compared with age- and sex- 1998;18:342–348.
matched controls.1 Our study was not able to address 24. Johnson W, McGue M, Gaist D, Vaupel JW, Christensen K. Frequency
this issue. However, in the same neuroimaging study and heritability of depression symptomatology in the second half of life:
evidence from Danish twins over 45. Psychol Med 2002;32:1175–1185.
it was reported that female subjects with MO or MA 25. WHO Collaborating Centre for Drug Statistics Methodology. Anatomi-
had a higher deep white matter lesion load, a link cal therapeutic chemical (ATC) classification index including defined
daily doses (DDD) for plain substances. Oslo: WHO, 1995.
that was more pronounced in subjects with frequent 26. Geschwind N, Behan P. Left-handedness: associations with immune
attacks. This finding raised concern as to whether disease, migraine and developmental learning disorders. Proc Natl
Acad Sci USA 1982;79:5097–5100.
such brain lesions, if truly associated with migraine, 27. Waldie KE, Hausmann M, Milne BJ, Poulton R. Migraine and cognitive
may lead to cognitive impairment.3 Lack of imaging function: a life-course study. Neurology 2002;59:904 –908.
data in our study and other design issues do not 28. Chronicle EP, Wilkins AJ, Coleston DM. Thresholds for detection of a
target against background grating suggest visual dysfunction in migraine
permit straightforward comparisons with the results with aura but not migraine without aura. Cephalalgia 1995;15:117–122.
of the neuroimaging study.1 In spite of these limita- 29. Wray SH, Mijovic-Prelec D, Kosslyn SM. Visual processing in mi-
graineurs. Brain 1995;118:25–35.
tions we believe that migraineurs should find it reas- 30. Breslau N, Davis GC, Andreski P. Migraine, psychiatric disorders, and
suring that the present study found no evidence of a suicide attempts: an epidemiologic study of young adults. Psychiatry
Res 1991;37:11–23.
link between migraine and cognitive skills. 31. Stewart WF, Lipton R, Celentano DD, Reed JL. Prevalence of migraine
headache in the United States. Relation to age, income, race, and other
References sociodemographic factors. JAMA 1992;267:64 – 69.
32. Stang PE, Osterhaus JT. Impact of migraine in the United States: data
1. Kruit MC, van Buchem MA, Hofman PAM, et al. Migraine as a risk from the National Health Interview Survey. Headache 1993;33:29 –35.
factor for subclinical brain lesions. JAMA 2004;291:427– 434. 33. Lipton RB, Stewart WF, Diamond S, Diamond ML, Reed M. Prevalence
2. Lipton RB, Pan J. Is migraine a progressive brain disease? JAMA and burden of migraine in the United States: data from the American
2004;291:493– 494. Migraine Study II. Headache 2001;41:646 – 657.
3. Tietjen GE. Stroke and migraine linked by silent strokes. Lancet Neu- 34. Lipton RB, Scher AI, Kolodner K, Liberman J, Steiner TJ, Stewart WF.
rol 2004;3:267. Migraine in the United States: epidemiology and patterns of health
4. Zeitlin C, Oddy M. Cognitive impairment in patients with severe mi- care use. Neurology 2002;58:885– 894.
graine. Br J Clin Psychol 1984;23:27–35. 35. Gervil M, Ulrich V, Kaprio J, Olesen J, Russell MB. The relative role of
5. HookerWD, Raskin NH. Neuropsychologic alterations in classic and genetic and environmental factors in migraine without aura. Neurology
common migraine. Arch Neurol 1986;43:709 –712. 1999;53:995–999.
6. Mulder EJCM, Linssen WHJP, Passchier J, Orlebeke JF, de Geus EJC. 36. Rasmussen BK, Jensen R, Schroll M, Olesen J. Epidemiology of head-
Interictal and postictal cognitive changes in migraine. Cephalalgia ache in a general population—a prevalence study. J Clin Epidemiol
1999;19:557–565. 1991;44:1147–1157.
7. Calandre EP, Bembibre J, Arnedo ML, Becerra D. Cognitive distur- 37. Russell MB, Rasmussen BK, Thorvaldsen P, Olesen J. Prevalence and
bances and regional cerebral blood flow abnormalities in migraine pa- sex-ratio of the subtypes of migraine. Int J Epidemiol 1995;24:612– 618.
tients: their relationship with the clinical manifestations of the illness. 38. Byrns R, Healy J. The intelligence of twins. J Genet Psychol 1936;49:
Cephalalgia 2002;22:291–302. 474 – 478.
8. Sinforiani E, Farina S, Mancuso A, Manzoni GC, Bono G, Mazzucchi A. 39. Record RG, McKeown T, Edwards JH. An investigation of the difference
Analysis of higher nervous functions in migraine and cluster headache. in measured intelligence between twins and single births. Ann Hum
Funct Neurol 1987;2:69 –77. Genet 1970;34:11–20.
9. Leijdekkers ML, Passchier J, Goudswaard P, Menges LJ, Orlebeke JF. 40. Posthuma D, de Geus EJC, Bleichrodt N, Boomsma DI. Twin-singleton
Migraine patients cognitively impaired? Headache 1990;30:352–358. differences in intelligence? Twin Res 2000;3:83– 87.

February (2 of 2) 2005 NEUROLOGY 64 607


Long-term effects of migraine on cognitive function: A population-based study of
Danish twins
D. Gaist, L. Pedersen, C. Madsen, et al.
Neurology 2005;64;600-607
DOI 10.1212/01.WNL.0000151858.15482.66

This information is current as of February 22, 2005

Updated Information & including high resolution figures, can be found at:
Services http://www.neurology.org/content/64/4/600.full.html

Supplementary Material Supplementary material can be found at:


http://www.neurology.org/content/suppl/2005/02/20/64.4.600.DC1.htm
l
References This article cites 36 articles, 13 of which you can access for free at:
http://www.neurology.org/content/64/4/600.full.html##ref-list-1
Citations This article has been cited by 14 HighWire-hosted articles:
http://www.neurology.org/content/64/4/600.full.html##otherarticles
Subspecialty Collections This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in the
following collection(s):
All Genetics
http://www.neurology.org//cgi/collection/all_genetics
All Neuropsychology/Behavior
http://www.neurology.org//cgi/collection/all_neuropsychology_behavio
r
Migraine
http://www.neurology.org//cgi/collection/migraine
Permissions & Licensing Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures,tables) or in
its entirety can be found online at:
http://www.neurology.org/misc/about.xhtml#permissions
Reprints Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
http://www.neurology.org/misc/addir.xhtml#reprintsus

Neurology ® is the official journal of the American Academy of Neurology. Published continuously since
1951, it is now a weekly with 48 issues per year. Copyright . All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0028-3878.
Online ISSN: 1526-632X.

You might also like