景观叙事提升植物园科普

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

Landscape and Urban Planning 86 (2008) 233–247

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Landscape and Urban Planning


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/landurbplan

Improving educational functions in botanic gardens by employing


landscape narratives
Li-Shin Chang a,∗ , Richard J. Bisgrove b , Ming-Yi Liao c
a
Department of Urban Planning and Landscape Architecture, Chaoyang University of Technology, Taichung County 413, Taiwan, ROC
b
Centre for Horticulture and Landscape, University of Reading, Reading RG6 6AS, UK
c
Graduate Institute of Architecture and Urban Design, Chaoyang University of Technology, Taichung County 413, Taiwan, ROC

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Landscape narrative, combining landscape and narrative, has been employed to create storytelling layouts
Received 23 July 2007 and interpretive information in some famous botanic gardens. In order to assess the educational effec-
Received in revised form 17 February 2008 tiveness of using “landscape narrative” in landscape design, the Heng-Chun Tropical Botanical Garden in
Accepted 18 March 2008
Taiwan was chosen as research target for an empirical study. Based on cognitive theory and the affective
Available online 15 May 2008
responses of environmental psychology, computer simulations and video recordings were used to create
five themed display areas with landscape narrative elements. Two groups of pupils watched simulated
Keywords:
films. The pupils were then given an evaluation test and questionnaire, to determine the effectiveness of
Content retention
Interpretation
the landscape narrative. When the content was well associated and matched with the narrative landscape,
Cognitive psychology the comprehension and retention of content was increased significantly. The results also indicated that
Visual preference visual preference of narrative landscape scenes was increased. This empirical study can be regarded as
a successful model of integrating landscape narrative and interpretation practice that can be applied to
the design of new theme displays in botanic gardens to improve both the effectiveness of interpretation
plans and the visual preference of visitors.
© 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Interpretation involves the narration of thematic information to


visitors by employing appropriate media. It is a major visitor service
To support conservation of biodiversity, more than 600 new on many recreational sites, museums and parks. It is also key to ful-
botanic gardens have been set up worldwide during the past two filling the educational goals of many botanic gardens. Indeed it has
decades. Over 200 million people visit botanic gardens each year, been suggested that without interpretation a botanic garden is little
about one in 33 of all people in the world (Willison, 2006). more than an attractive park (Giffurida, 1996). In botanic gardens,
Botanic gardens therefore play important roles in education typical media used for interpretation include self-guided trails,
as well as in conservation of plant and natural resources and in plant labels and information panels. Guided tours, often regarded
scientific research. They are also regarded as excellent sites for as the most effective interpretation practice, are usually either on
recreation: millions of visitors annually are attracted to botanic a regular basis or by visitor request. High quality interpretation
gardens by their plants, theme gardens and greenhouses (IUCN, should include not only interpretative facilities and interpreters for
1987). If environmental ethics and positive attitudes towards envi- the garden but also themes or landscapes designed for interpreta-
ronmental conservation could be cultivated tacitly by “education tion to assist in the processes of story-telling (Chang, 2001).
in stealth” when visitors come to botanic gardens, attitudes toward Many gardens, for example the Eden Project in the UK, have
environmental conservation could be substantially improved. employed ‘landscape narrative’ as part of their design. Chang (2002)
noted that if the setting of themes in a botanic garden is initially
designed by collaboration between landscape architects and edu-
cators to form a ‘narrative landscape’, this could help the educators
to interpret relevant information on the site. However, to date
∗ Corresponding author at: Department of Urban Planning and Landscape Archi- there has been no investigation of whether “narrative landscape
tecture, Chaoyang University of Technology, No. 168 Jifong E. Road, Wufong design” as a vehicle for interpretation in botanic gardens is bene-
Township, Taichung County 41349, Taiwan, ROC. Tel.: +886 4 23323000x4453;
ficial to the learning process. Neither has there been investigation
fax: +886 4 23742385.
E-mail addresses: lschang@cyut.edu.tw, ahrchagl@yahoo.com.tw (L.S. Chang), of whether or not the narrative setting could increase the attrac-
r.j.bisgrove@reading.ac.uk (R.J. Bisgrove), senao921@yahoo.com.tw (M.Y. Liao). tiveness of the garden and the visual preferences of its visitors,

0169-2046/$ – see front matter © 2008 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2008.03.003
234 L.S. Chang et al. / Landscape and Urban Planning 86 (2008) 233–247

factors which would influence the desirability of implementing the audiences as possible. To employ “landscape narrative” in design
idea. practice might be a key to effectively provide implicit education for
The definition of narrative is usually about words, written and visitors.
spoken but narrative is not necessarily in the form of words.
Traditional English gardens often embody messages in statuary, 2.2. Landscape narrative and information conveyance
inscription and eloquent architecture (Hunt and Willis, 1988). For
instance, the garden Stourhead, UK, presents a multilayered set According to the Oxford Concise Dictionary, “Narrative is a spo-
of narratives in its landscape layout, architecture and sculptures ken or written account of connected events in order of happening”,
(Potteiger and Purinton, 1998). or “the practice or art of narration”. Ricoeur (1981) states that
In order to understand the influence of landscape narrative on narrative combines two dimensions, one a temporal sequence of
interpretation, various narrative elements associated with targeted events and the other a non-chronological configuration, that orga-
thematic displays in Heng-Chun Tropical Botanical Garden were nize narrative into spatial patterns. Narrative or story telling is a
simulated in order to investigate whether they improved the learn- linguistic form which people use to understand the world about
ing efficacy of pupils. The results could be valuable for the design of them (Polkinghorne, 1988, 1995). For these reasons, narrative is
display areas in other botanic gardens to improve their educational widely used in education and has proved to work effectively in lan-
functions. guage learning and school education. For school learning students
are exposed to literature in the form of the storyline in novels that
2. Literature review deal with topics related to course content and this provides a use-
ful method of instruction (Kostelecky and Hoskinson, 2005). New
2.1. Plant display and interpretation in botanic gardens heuristic device application of narratives in rural study are also pro-
posed by Soliva (2007), by whom the four ideal types of narrative,
In recent decades, the roles of botanic gardens have been wilderness narrative, modernization narrative, subsistence narra-
changing to reflect the increasing importance of conservation tive and endogenous development narrative, were constructed and
of biodiversity and environmental education as well as to meet used for rural study. Those types of narrative also are viewed to
increased demands for recreation. Botanic gardens should be reveal the diverse views, values and assumption of stakeholders
regarded not only as recreational sites but should also take special regarding changes in landscape, land use and biodiversity (Soliva,
responsibility for environmental education for sustainable devel- 2007).
opment (Chang, 2001; Oikawa, 2000; Willison, 2006). As Potteiger and Purinton (1998) state, the term ‘landscape
In botanic gardens, the plant display area usually seen as theme narrative’ designates the interplay and mutual relationship that
gardens or thematic displays is often near the entrance to the gar- develops between landscape elements and narrative. Landscape
dens to attract visitors and is also the main “channel” by which not only serves as the background setting for stories, but is itself
interpretation may occur. According to interviews with educators in a changing, eventful figure and process that engenders its own sto-
UK botanic gardens, the most popular features in their gardens were ries. There are two ways to turn landscape narrative into real design
greenhouses and theme gardens. Similar results were also found in practices. The narrative can use explicit storytelling landscapes to
an investigation of botanic gardens in Taiwan (Chang, 2001). These convey messages and to create or constitute the landscape embod-
popular sites therefore offer the most opportunities for successful iment of collective human memory, or it can be already implicit in
education. landscapes as inscribed by natural processes and cultural practices
The setting of display areas in botanic gardens, whether indoors (Potteiger and Purinton, 1998; Rakatansky, 1992).
or outdoors, is usually designed around the themes of plant Landscape narratives are classified into several types such as
related subjects, e.g. taxonomy, botany, biotechnology, ethno- ‘narrative experiences’, ‘association and references’ and ‘memory
botany, evolution, geographical distribution, ecology, utilization, landscapes’ (e.g. interpretive landscapes and storytelling land-
or a combination of these (Chang, 2003; Lai, 1993). The choice scapes) (Potteiger and Purinton, 1998). Association and reference
of medium for plant display depends on what kinds of messages need not necessarily be memory landscapes, but some elements
the garden staff intend to pass on. In other words, the plant dis- in a landscape should become connected with experiences, events,
plays must embed information that educators want to disseminate. history or other form of narrative. However, interpretive landscape
If education is one of the important missions of botanic gardens, is a landscape that has elements and programs that tell what hap-
interpretation will be the core of educational practices. pened in a certain place. Storytelling landscapes are usually places
Tilden (1977) gave the excellent definition of interpretation as designed to tell specific stories with explicit references to plot,
“an educational activity which aims to reveal meanings and rela- scenes, events characters, etc. (Potteiger and Purinton, 1998). In
tionship through the use of original objects”. Interpretation in a landscape narrative, metaphor, metonymy, synecdoche, and irony
botanic garden requires professional knowledge and skill in order to are regarded as the four major tropes (Potteiger and Purinton, 1998).
help people to appreciate special plants and the stories associated In terms of environmental psychology, landscape narrative
with them. Through interpretation, visitors obtain and understand would relate to several affective and cognitive responses, includ-
the knowledge or messages that educators are trying to deliver or ing “identification” and “encoding” regarding the four major tropes.
disseminate. “Identification” is to recognize an object and structure that recog-
According to visitor surveys for the Royal Botanic Gardens, Kew nition according to existing knowledge. “Encoding” refers to the
by Burbidge (1990) and for the Taipei Botanic Garden by Chiou storage of the perceived environment in human memory (Gaerling
and Lin (1992), most visitors came to the garden for recreational and Golledge, 1993; Gifford, 1996; Ulrich, 1986). A similar mecha-
purposes. In addition, Oikawa (2000) also pointed out that only nism is also present for reference and associative types of landscape
a small fraction of the total number of visitors to botanic gar- narrative, assisting observers to comprehend them. In this research,
dens engage consciously in some form of education program. In encoding could reflect on the memory of messages that narrative
order for interpretation to be conducted successfully, therefore, it landscapes help to deliver to viewers.
needs to be pleasurable or it will not hold the attention of those One specific example of a storytelling landscape is the landscape
non-captive visitors. Innovative strategies need to be offered in garden Stourhead in Wiltshire, UK. The 18th-century scenic gar-
botanic gardens to attract and educate as wide a spectrum of den was progressively developed by Hoare family (Siciliano, 2005).
L.S. Chang et al. / Landscape and Urban Planning 86 (2008) 233–247 235

The estate owner Henry Hoare II hired an architect, Henry Flitcroft, plays of the Eden Project in the UK (Fig. 1), landscape narrative
to plot the spatial configuration of the garden as the story line of has been incorporated within the design to form interpretive land-
the Aeneas’s wanderings around the Mediterranean basin. Flitcroft scapes. In the Humid Tropics Biome at Eden there are narrative
transformed the stream and small pond in the valley into a grand elements such as the huge bow of a cargo ship standing on the
lake. Around the lake, a footpath was laid to connect a series of clas- route to Crop and Cultivation Display, indicating the prosperous
sical buildings including the Temple of Flora, a grotto, the Pantheon, trade for economically important plants and products (Fig. 1A). The
a rock arch, the Temple of Apollo and five-arched Palladian bridge. Asian house and rubber tires relate to the plants alongside illustrat-
It has been suggested that the design was intended to reveal the ing the contribution of cultivated plants to human welfare (Fig. 1B
story of Aeneas’ journey and the founding of Rome in order to retell and C). In the Mediterranean Basin Display, a sculpture of Diony-
in the landscape with high degree authority and control over the sus, the Greek god of the vines, depicted as a bull surrounded by
means of presentation (Potteiger and Purinton, 1998). The design maenads, immediately catches the eye of visitors (Fig. 1D). In the
also represents aspects of Hoare’s life experience of love and con- outside landscape, a giant model of a bee is a focal point illustrating
frontations with death (Olin, 1999; Siciliano, 2005; Woodbridge, its ecological role and allowing more comprehensive observation of
2001). The landscape at Stourhead is profound through name, ref- its morphology (Fig. 1E). Such landscape narratives seem to attract
erences, association and symbol which were engraved and carried visitors’ attention and arouse their curiosity (Chang, 2003).
by architecture, statues and setting of the garden (Potteiger and Theoretically, students should be more motivated to learn if they
Purinton, 1998). become curious about the issues and are placed in an environment
Such metaphoric themes are very attractive as they were made which is rich for learning. In addition, curiosity-arousing scenes
to tell stories or to impart knowledge that the estate owner had that connect with visitors through experience, events, history, or
learned and associated with his prestige and the tragic events religious allegory could be referred to as association and reference
occurring in his life. The landscape elements, lay-out and design type of landscape narrative. They also enhance the visitors’ percep-
vocabulary were employed to present an image of a mythic world. tion, cognition and experiences. It would be beneficial for landscape
Consequently, for visitors approaching the ambience of these designers and educators, in creating thematic displays, if a positive
objects, the meaning of the narrative landscape is conveyed subtly, influence of narrative landscape on learning could be scientifically
particularly obviously when adding with oral interpretation. demonstrated.
In botanic gardens, narration may or may not be implanted This study attempts to examine the learning effectiveness of
when the landscape is formed, although various plants always have pupils in a simulated guided tour of a narrative landscape setting
the potential to embody stories. The creation of a narrative setting and tries to deduce principles for the design of thematic displays
where interpretation should take place would allow that interpre- in botanic gardens in order to improve their educational and recre-
tation to be conducted with maximum fluency. Such an approach ational effectiveness.
may also be referred to as landscape narrative or interpretive land-
scape (Chang, 2002). Themed landscapes shape and reconstitute 2.3. Landscape perception and preferences
memory into clear, controlled narrative tableaux. In some thematic
display gardens, for example the Evolution House and the Japanese The perception of landscape and preference judgments were
Garden of the Royal Botanic Garden, Kew or several thematic dis- reviewed for this research as they are relevant to the questions

Fig. 1. Application of landscape narrative through landscape design in display areas of the Eden Project in the UK. Landscape narrative elements: (A) cargo ship; (B) Asian
cottage; (C) rubber tires along the foothill for rubber trees; (D) sculpture of Dionysus and maenads; (E) giant bee model (quote Chang, 2003).
236 L.S. Chang et al. / Landscape and Urban Planning 86 (2008) 233–247

“can interpretative landscape influence the judgment of landscape


quality?” and “in terms of theoretical base, to what extent does the
quality of the landscape encourage revisits”.
It has been proposed that landscape perception as a part of
environmental perception can be considered as a function of the
interaction between humans and the landscape (Zube et al., 1982).
The model revealed the relationships between human, landscape,
interaction and outcome. In this model the ‘human’ component is
influenced by previous experiences, knowledge, expectations and
socio-cultural context of individuals and groups. The ‘landscape’
component includes both individual tangible elements and intan-
gibles such as composition, form and complexity but the landscape
is always viewed from the perspective of the perceptual interaction. Fig. 2. Conceptual framework of this study. Participant responses were examined
As a result, a series of interactions such as aesthetic and preference after watching simulated films with or without landscape narrative.
judgments is generated. This interaction between environment and
human results in outcomes which in turn could feed back to affect
both human and landscape inputs, even to affect environment man- to tests, it would be beneficial to know how landscape preference is
agement. ranked.
In the processes of landscape perception, visitors as respondents On the other hand, experiences gained on a visit made purely
receive stimuli from the environment and emotional stimulation, for pleasure could trigger visitors’ revisits. Bell (1999) revealed that
the instructive recognition of an aesthetic quality and visual pref- a sense of pleasure is a part of the aesthetic experience. Emo-
erence judged through the interaction (Bell, 1999, 2001; Kaplan, tional stimulation, pleasure and displeasure are gained through our
1985; Kaplan and Kaplan, 1989; Zube et al., 1982). In the meantime perception. Bell recognized that different landscapes evoke these
the ability of the mind to connect sensory information to knowl- responses in viewers to different degrees. A sense of pleasure, as
edge is used to develop opinions about what has been perceived a part of the aesthetic experiences, can be generated through high
(Bell, 2001). aesthetic quality of the landscape and environment. Viewers, as
For landscape perception assessment, Zube and his colleagues tourists, could seek visual interests as satisfying entertainment,
identified four paradigms of research directions, the expert because they are oriented by aesthetic landscape and move to an
paradigm, psychophysical paradigm, cognitive paradigm and area with ongoing activities (Jakle, 1987).
experiential paradigm, based on human–landscape-interaction- Pleasurable visiting experiences encourage further visits. Such
outcome model (Zube et al., 1982). In the cognitive paradigm, also positive feedback can lead to more educational opportunities being
labeled as the psychological model (Daniel and Vining, 1983), land- generated by return visits. On the other hand, how people perceive
scape quality is usually evaluated and ranked by showing color the environment according to their personal background, or the
slides or photos to respondents. The dimensions of size, openness, purposes of visiting, could be related to what action will be taken
colorfulness, information, value, orderliness, fearfulness, beauty, and how people will use the site.
stress and preference matrix (complexity, legibility, mystery and “If we wish to communicate with people about the landscape
coherence) are usually scored. Preference ratings and ratings on the we need to understand how the patterns are perceived in all the
a priori dimensions are usually obtained. In these researches, how meanings of visual language” (Bell, 2001). As the previous section
the meaning or content carried by the landscape could influence revealed, elements in the landscape could communicate in several
preference judgment was seldom directly discussed, however. ways but how people perceive the landscape is associated with
It has been also addressed that the preference regarding the nat- viewers’ past experiences. To improve the educational functions of
uralistic character of landscape varied between children and adult botanic gardens, visitor surveys need to be done to assess visitors’
respondents (Bernáldez et al., 1984; Zube et al., 1983). Bernáldez et responses towards the landscape, plant displays, signage, and other
al. (1987) applied multivariate analysis of the preference responses interpretation media. Evaluation is especially important in ensur-
of children and young adults to study three independent pref- ing that educational and recreational facilities and services meet
erence dimensions, the results showed that both shadowed, less both visitor needs and management objectives, as well as help-
illuminated scenes and harsh, rough scenes with aggressive forms ing botanic garden staff to prioritize visitor needs by investigating
were less preferred by young children (11 years old) than by older information such as aesthetic quality of landscape or themes, trends
children (16 years old). For younger children, darkness and deep and visit patterns. Feedback from visitors could enable the design,
shadow, like ‘mystery’ conceal a part of the scene’s information interpretation and management to be adjusted appropriately to
but have a strong risk and uncertainty connotation, related to encourage the visitors to learn pleasurably in botanic gardens.
fears typical of children. Conversely, darkness and shadow, like
Kaplan and Kaplan’s mystery, can also stimulate curiosity in some 2.4. Framework for this study
observers. These observations demonstrated the change from nega-
tive influences on preference when the characteristics are perceived Narrative landscape is a landscape composed of explicit or
as frightening, to positive effect on preference when they are per- implicit elements or settings that can help the narration of specific
ceived as exciting and stimulating. The results indicated that the stories or information. It may or may not have a particular theme.
meaning or content of landscape perceived and subsequent pref- In this study to test the efficacy of employing landscape narrative
erence judgment was significantly affected by the experience and in thematic display design, video recordings of existing landscapes
background of different viewers. Consequently, it is difficult to clar- were combined with specific computer simulations to develop a
ify how respondents perceive interpretive landscape and its effects 2 min filmed walk through five thematic displays.
on preference judgment, as the preference judgment is mixed The simulated parts of these treatments were made as convinc-
with visual quality of landscape and the meaning or content of ing as possible by using real existing background and objects to
landscape perceived by respondents. Therefore, if the meaning of compensate for the inability of the computer technique to replicate
landscape has been sent to respondent through interpretation prior a 3-D scene.
L.S. Chang et al. / Landscape and Urban Planning 86 (2008) 233–247 237

This follows Stamp’s (1990, 1999) finding that using meta-

creel built from wood and


Association and reference

+Narrative (a house and a


analysis to show photographs is a good surrogate for direct

−Narrative, no house and


(story telling landscape)
experience in the evaluation of scenic preference. It is also con-

Taiwan Ethno-botany
sidered that using photographs to show public preference reaction
is a highly effective procedure for deriving salient perceptual cat-

creel present
egories of the view (Amir and Sobol, 1990; Cerny, 1974; Misgav,

Nos. 18, 19
Nos. 17, 20
2000). Fig. 2 shows the conceptual framework developed for this

Display

House

vines)
study. The control and experimental scenes of the thematic displays
were designed as “landscape” and “landscape with narratives”,
respectively. Based on results of interviews with targeted botanic
garden staff five themes were selected. Research themes presented
to respondents shown in oral information for these two treat-

−Narrative (wooden canoe


+Narrative scene (wooden
Association and reference
ments are identical. The highlighted central box shows the inner

Lanyu Islet Ethno-botany

(story telling landscape)


process for identification, encoding and aesthetic evaluation of
respondents. The core variables (shown in the right of Fig. 2 in two
boxes), comprehension and retention of interpretation content and

Wooden canoe

canoe present)
attractiveness and preference of landscape, refer to an evaluating

Nos. 13, 14
Nos.15, 16
judgment of the efficacy of message sending and the visual quality

absent)
Display
of the narrative landscape created. According to literature review in
Section 2.3, preference judgment scored was mixed with the influ-
ence of visual preference and meaning of landscape. However in
this research the focus is on the extent to which high visual pref-

−Narrative (−flower and fruits


+Narrative scene (flower and
fruits of Indian Barringtonia)
erence triggers revisits rather than on discussion of their influence

Association and reference


Indian Barrington Display
on preference judgment.

(story telling landscape)

of Indian Barringtonia)
Artwork installation
3. Research methods

Nos. 10, 11
3.1. Site selection—the targeted botanic garden and thematic

Nos. 9, 12
display

Heng-Chun Tropical Botanic Garden, established in 1901, was


selected as the target garden in this research. In the ‘National

Natural element with narrative


Botanic Garden Network Plan’ proposed by the government of Tai-

−Narrative scene (− buttress

+Narrative (+buttress roots)


wan in 2002, the garden was selected to be remodeled in order to
Looking glass tree Display

fulfill the mission statement and management objectives set up by


Interpretive landscape

the management authority.


The theme gardens for this study were selected after interviews
with volunteer guides and staff. According to their recommenda-
characters

tion, the five most representative theme garden displays were:

Nos. 5, 8
Nos. 6, 7
roots)

(1) White Ficus (Ficus benjamina L.).


(2) Looking glass tree (Heritiera littoralis Dryand).
(3) Indian Barringtonia (Barringtonia asiatica L.).
Natural element with narrative

(4) Lanyu Islet Ethno-botany.


−Narrative (−twisted roots)
+a Narrative scene (obvious

(5) Taiwan Ethno-botany.


Selected theme areas to simulate narrative landscape and variables

Interpretive landscape
White Ficus Display

These five themes were selected as the target displays of this


study.
twisted roots)

Nos. 1, 2, 3
characters

3.2. Experimental design


(+) Narrative scenes, (−) non-narrative scene.
No. 4

To evaluate the influence of narrative landscape on the learn-


ing efficiency and visual preference of pupils, the scenery of each
of the five selected theme displays was recorded by video camera
Questions disassociated with scenes
Experimental and control treatment

Questions associated with scenes

(JVC GR-D230 digital camcorder). Narrative elements were selected


or simulated and integrated into a scene shown in a photo for each
Types of landscape narrative

theme that was finally arranged at the end of the experimental


version of each film (Tables 1 and 2A–E). The control treatment of
each theme was the existing scenery, sharing the same composition
Narrative elements
Display area (item)

of background with the experimental treatment. According to the


classification by Potteiger and Purinton (1998), in this research the
five selected theme areas can be classified into ‘Interpretative land-
Film A

Film B
Table 1

scape’ and ‘Association and Reference’ of landscape narrative. The


a

‘Interpretative landscapes’ selected for this research were a natural


238 L.S. Chang et al. / Landscape and Urban Planning 86 (2008) 233–247

Table 2
Scenes of narrative and non- narrative landscape of theme areas

type of landscape narrative, which referred to elements expressing control treatment were carefully chosen from the existing natural
significant phenomena of plant natural adaptation. These themes environment of the targeted botanic gardens.
included the twisting roots of the White Ficus and the buttress The types “association and references” indicate that the ele-
roots of the Looking glass tree, both classified as a kind of implicit ments added in the display area were intended to connect to
natural interpretative landscape. The scene of experimental and visitors’ experience in some ways. The narrative elements, includ-
L.S. Chang et al. / Landscape and Urban Planning 86 (2008) 233–247 239

ing a marble sculpture of Indian Barringtonia flower and fruits, Table 3


Respondent profiles
wooden canoes and aboriginal houses, were all integrated into the
end of the respective theme displays in the experimental film. The A. Distribution of gender and number of students
five experimental narrative landscape treatments were randomly Gender 2 -test
assigned into film A and film B (Table 1), giving a total of a ten
Male Female Total
minute “walk” through the five designed routes of each thematic
display. A group 111 98 209 2 = 0.614
B group 105 108 213 P = 0.438
The same carefully designed interpretative information (see
Appendix A, A–E interpretive information) was orally presented B. Previous visit experience to Heng-Chun Botanic Garden
along with the control and the experimental film to resemble a Previous visit experience 2 -test
guided tour of the scene as if pupils were visiting the theme display
Yes No Total
with interpretation by a guide.
A group 27 182 209 2 = 0.980
B group 21 192 213 P = 0.322
3.3. Selection of respondents and questionnaire survey
C. Preference of respondents on plant related knowledge

Eight of the 68 elementary schools in Taichung city were ran- Degree of interest 2 -test
domly sampled. One class of students from both the third and fourth Not interested Neutral Interested Total
grade of the sampled school was chosen following a random sam-
A group 63 51 98 209 2 = 2.935
pling process. The total number of pupils in the sample represented B group 53 66 94 213 P = 0.569
2% of their age population in Taichung. Third and fourth grade pupils
(age 9–10 years) are generally curious and able to respond to com-
plicated texts. To reduce bias, it was confirmed with their teachers 4. Results
that interpretative information for each theme delivered in this
research had not been learned before the test. The total number of questionnaires distributed and returned
The experiment was conducted in September and October, 2005. was 425, of which 422 were valid. This high percentage validity
The respondents from each class of pupils were randomly assigned was expected because the experiment was conducted on a class
into two groups to watch film A or film B. All the films were played basis and the questionnaires were collected immediately after the
in the audio room for these two groups of pupils. A structured ques- students had answered all the questions. Of all the students, 209
tionnaire was designed to compare the interpretative information watched film A and 213 watched film B.
gained by respondents after watching the experiment and control
film. All pupils answered the same questionnaire. The overall time 4.1. Respondent profiles
needed for all procedures including the questionnaire survey was
approximately 30 min. The analysis found that there was no difference (P = 0.438)
detected on respondents’ gender distribution (Table 3A). The pro-
3.4. Questionnaire design portion of respondents in the two groups who had been to
Heng-Chun Botanical Garden was similar (P = 0.322, Table 3B). No
To fulfill the research objectives, the questionnaire was divided difference was observed on the degree of preference on plant-
into three parts: the context of the respondents, evaluation of learn- related knowledge (P = 0.569, Table 3C). These results demonstrated
ing, and visual evaluation of the scenes. The respondent profiles that the two groups of students are satisfactorily homogenous to
included gender, Likert scale of their interest in plants or related justify subsequent comparisons.
knowledge, and a question about their visit experience to the Heng-
Chun Tropical Botanical Garden. These were included in order to 4.2. Effects of landscape narrative on learning efficiency
recheck the homogeneity of these two randomly assigned student
The results presented in Table 4 show the average scores of
groups.
the experimental and control groups on questions associated with
In the learning evaluation part of questionnaire, four multiple-
landscape narrative. The scores for the five narrative landscape
choice questions for each theme were designed according to the
experiments were all significantly higher than those of the controls.
information provided in the film (see Appendix A(A–E)) in order to
Apart from the Indian Barringtonia Display, in which the probabil-
assess how much information was retained by respondents. Of the
ity was significant at P = 0.012 < 0.05, the effects of the remaining
four questions, two were associated with the narrative landscape
landscape narrative treatments on learning performance were all
and the other two questions, designed as the control, were not. In
the White Ficus Display, only one question was associated with the
landscape narrative. Table 4
Effects of landscape narrative on learning efficiency—questions associated with
In order to assess the landscape quality of simulated landscape landscape narrative
with narrative elements and the control, pupils were asked to
evaluate the variables, attractiveness and visual preference, on a Theme display Average scorea t-Test

five-point Likert scale. For example, ‘Strongly like’ was given a score Narrative (N)b Non-narrative (N) t-Valuec P-value
of 5, and ‘strongly dislike’ a score of 1. Scores could then be averaged White Ficus 79.9 (209) 67.3 (213) 2.95 **
0.003
across the population of respondents. Looking glass tree 47.6 (213) 37.5 (209) 2.71** 0.003
Indian Barringtonia 58.8 (209) 49.7 (213) 2.51* 0.012
Lanyu Islet Ethno-botany 47.3 (209) 39.2 (213) 3.36** 0.001
3.5. Statistical analysis Taiwan Ethno-botany 50.0 (213) 40.9 (209) 2.78** 0.006
a
Average score of 2 associated questions, 1 correct answer was scored 50, 2 correct
The Statistical Package for Social Science (SPSS v. 10) was used answers were scored 100.
to analyze the collected data. The 2 -test and t-test and were used b
N, total number of students.
to test the hypotheses at 5% significance level. c
*, **: Significant at 5% and 1% level.
240 L.S. Chang et al. / Landscape and Urban Planning 86 (2008) 233–247

Table 5 landscape narrative can help not only to convey information to


Effects of landscape narrative on learning efficiency – questions not associated with
visitors, but may also increase the aesthetic quality of the landscape.
landscape narrative

Theme display Average scorea t-Test 5. Discussions


a
Narrative (N) Non-narrative (N) t-Value P-value

White Ficus 65.2 (209) 65.2 (213) 0.006 0.996


In this study, two types of narrative landscape elements, ‘Inter-
Looking glass tree 78.2 (213) 78.5 (209) 0.108 0.914 pretive’ and ‘Association and Reference’, were incorporated by
Indian Barringtonia 41.1 (209) 45.5 (213) 1.204 0.229 computer simulation into videos of actual landscapes and both
Lanyu Islet Ethno-botany 65.3 (209) 63.1 (213) 0.624 0.533 showed substantial improvement in learning by 9–10 years old chil-
Taiwan Ethno-botany 73.7 (213) 73.4 (209) 0.077 0.939
dren. The attractiveness and preference of the theme displays were
Pooled un-associated data 62.3 (209) 63.3 (213) 0.551 0.582 also increased.
a
Average score of 2 non-associated questions, 1 correct answer was scored 50, 2 In the natural-type interpretive landscape White Ficus and Look-
correct answers were scored 100. ing glass tree displays, the twisting roots and buttress roots were
shown in their early development stage and in the developed stage
Table 6 in the theme display site. The scenes of developed entwisted roots
The effects of landscape narrative on attraction and visual preferencea of the scene and buttress roots in the experimental film clearly helped the pupils
with landscape narrative to understand these natural phenomena through oral interpreta-
Theme display Landscape Attractiveness Preference tion and the pupils gained better performance (Table 4, P = 0.003
narrative (N) score score for both theme displays) in subsequent evaluation.
In the Lanyu Islet Ethno-botany Display, colorful wooden canoes
Indian Barringtonia Yes (209) 3.98 3.9
No (213) 3.7 3.74 were incorporated since they are recognized as one of the most
t 2.255 1.303 representative features of that island off Taiwan. The inclusion of
P 0.025 0.193 these characteristic canoes in the scenery showed the most signifi-
Significanceb *
NS
cant effects on both learning (Table 4, P = 0.001) and attractiveness
Lanyu Islet Ethno-botany Yes (209) 4.16 4.02 (Table 6, P < 0.001). Similar results were demonstrated by the aver-
No (213) 3.65 3.62 age score in the Taiwan Ethno-botany Display (Table 4, P = 0.006).
t 4.045 3.908
There was one more difficult question each in both the Lanyu Islet
P 0.000 0.002
Significance *** ** and Taiwan Ethno-botany Displays, that asked the name of the plant
used to build the canoe and to construct the walls of thatch cot-
Taiwan Ethno-botany Yes (213) 3.99 3.9
No (209) 3.68 3.59
tages. The percentage of respondents who gave correct answers
t 2.660 2.568 was 11% and 32%, respectively (data not shown), lower than the
P 0.008 0.011 scores of other questions. This lower percentage of correct answers
** *
Significance indicates that botanical names of plant are generally more difficult
a
The score given is from 1 to 5. to remember. However, the percentage of correct answers was still
b
NS, *, **, ***, denote non-significant, 5%, 1%, and 0.1% level of significance. higher than when there was no narrative element present (data not
shown).
In the Indian Barringtonia Display, designed as an association
highly significant (P < 0.01). (P < 0.01 means significant at the 99% and reference type of narrative landscape, the narrative elements
confidence level.) included were large-scale marble sculptures of the flowers and
In contrast, on questions not associated with landscape narra- fruits of the Indian Barringtonia. The fact that the sculpture was
tive, the performance of the two groups of students showed no less realistic than other introduced elements may be the reason for
significant difference (Table 5). These results indicate that their lower, but still significant, effects on improving learning.
background knowledge was not related to the narrative scene of It has been pointed out that in the landscape perception pro-
each theme, confirming that the designed narrative landscape ben- cess, the senses are used to perceive the surroundings in order to
efits the learning efficiency in this study. accumulate humans’ experiences and compile their understanding
of the environment. All the sensations together are interconnected
4.3. Preference of the narrative landscape to form a complete picture when viewers perceive the environ-
ment (Bell, 1999). When presented with themes related to our daily
In addition to comparing the effects of narrative landscape on life, respondents tend to feel familiar and provide better responses.
learning efficiency, it is important to know whether or not such However, plant names or abstract concepts seem to be more dif-
narrative elements added into a landscape would influence visual ficult since information has flowed away before the recognition
preferences. Such knowledge would be useful in evaluating the and encoding process can occur. In contrast, visual scenes such
merits of applying landscape narrative in the design of thematic as canoes and thatched cottage, where clear patterns are shown,
display to improve educational functions in botanic gardens. In the are easier to encode more-or-less instantly through the pattern
previous experiment, the White Ficus and Looking glass tree Dis- recognition process of brain and associated information was taken
plays used two different natural scenes to deliver the information, naturally, so there were more improvements on learning.
so there was no basis for comparing the influence of narrative ele- Visual impressions are most important for perception and
ments added in the landscape. Therefore the influence of narrative the corresponding aesthetic response. As a result, to make the
elements on preference was compared only for the three themes scenery visually attractive is the first step to attract visitors and
shown with the same background. thus increase the possibility of conveying the intended message.
For the visual preference, it is shown that when narrative Furthermore aesthetic experiences could be pleasurable for the
elements were added into the specific site, the degrees of attrac- observers. As Edwards (2000) suggests, gaining such pleasure could
tiveness and preference were almost all significantly enhanced motivate repeated visits to a botanic garden.
(Table 6) except that the increase in preference for Indian Bar- It was also demonstrated generally that, if elements for the cre-
ringtonia was not significant (P = 0.193). These results show that ation of a narrative environment were integrated in a landscape
L.S. Chang et al. / Landscape and Urban Planning 86 (2008) 233–247 241

through the design processes, the degree of attractiveness and the narrative landscape is effective in terms of improving learning
visual preference was greatly enhanced (Table 6). In this study it efficiency.
was found that, where landscape narrative was used to create an In order to control variables of the research effectively, pupils
educational landscape, the preference was also increased. In this were chosen as respondents to take a simulated guided tour with
research all simulated scenes are well designed to carry positive visual and oral interpretation, and to answer the questionnaires
message. Thus the advantages of applying landscape narrative in afterwards. In this research, the pupils can therefore be regarded
design practice are further increased. In future investigations it as captive learners, and atypical of most botanic garden visitors.
would be worth adding negative elements to interpretive landscape Oikawa (2000) states that only small portion of visitors to botanic
to test whether preference is still enhanced. gardens take an educational program or guided tour. Thus, inherent
From the results of this experiment, it is clear that using nar- public education is an important task for designers and educators.
rative landscape for the design of display areas or theme gardens, It has been suggested that visitors to public sites, whether they
in conjunction with appropriate interpretation, can increase the are facing an information panel or a live interpreter, may pick up
efficacy of learning. It was reported that of the sources of human only messages that they are interested in. This is sometimes called
experience, 40% is gained through vision and 25% is through lis- “selective attention” by psychologists (Ham, 1992). Consequently,
tening, while by combining the two sources, the percentage can be how to arouse public interests and let visitors obtain knowledge
increased to 70% (Li, 1990). It has been proposed that landscape from narrative landscape is worthy of future study.
perception as a part of environment perception can be consid- In the long run to use a virtual environment to better under-
ered as a function of the interaction of humans and the landscape stand human perception and behaviour is not just useful to support
(Zube et al., 1982). During the processes of landscape perception, planning decisions, but also to create sensory realism (Bishop and
visitors as respondents receive stimuli from the landscape and Rohrmann, 2003). As more mature virtual simulation techniques
generate a series of interactions such as aesthetic and preference are developed, there will be more possibility to evaluate the effec-
judgments. The understanding and memory of interpreted infor- tiveness of narrative landscape elements before installing them in
mation is strengthened through pattern recognition and attention. the real landscape.
Presently, there is little research on landscape narrative and its con- The importance of environmental education has gradually
nections with cognitive psychology and learning. The theoretical drawn government attention, so in addition to botanic gardens,
basis for the improvement of learning by landscape narrative is school grounds and some parks also have great potential to combine
worthy of further investigation, particularly since the cultivation of environmental information with pleasure. There is a need for more
an environmental ethic and environmental education are regarded studies to investigate landscape narrative and assess its educational
as the primary key to sustainable development of the Earth. benefits.

6. Conclusions Acknowledgements

Scientific research into landscape narrative is still in its early The authors wish to thank the National Science Council for the
stages, and there are few studies on it. However, there is evidence generous grant (project code: NSC93-2313-B-324-003). We would
for the great potential of landscape narrative on forming an ‘edu- like to express our gratitude to Dr. Hsiang-hua Wang and Ms. Shu-
cational landscape’. The strong support of landscape narrative to hui Wu for their suggestions and assistance over the development
the educational function of botanic gardens shown in this study of this study.
can be used in the design and management of such gardens. It
is suggested that botanic garden managers could screen the spe- Appendix A. Interpretive information and content of
cial features and scenes from the resources it has, set up thematic questions of theme areas
landscapes with effective content and interpretation, in order to
deliver messages most effectively. Narrative landscapes could be A. White Ficus Display.
designed and integrated into the environment appropriate to the B. Looking glass tree Display.
display topic coordinated by educators and landscape designers. C. Indian Barringtonia Display.
In the future, narrative landscapes could be simulated and evalu- D. Lanyu Islet Ethno-botany Display.
ated following the procedures reported in this study to ensure that E. Taiwan Ethno-botany Display.
242 L.S. Chang et al. / Landscape and Urban Planning 86 (2008) 233–247
L.S. Chang et al. / Landscape and Urban Planning 86 (2008) 233–247 243
244 L.S. Chang et al. / Landscape and Urban Planning 86 (2008) 233–247
L.S. Chang et al. / Landscape and Urban Planning 86 (2008) 233–247 245
246 L.S. Chang et al. / Landscape and Urban Planning 86 (2008) 233–247

References
Chiou, W.L., Lin, C.C., 1992. The survey of visitor information in Taipei botanical
Amir, S., Sobol, E., 1990. The use of geomorphological elements for evaluation of garden. Bull. Taiwan For. Res. Inst. New Series 7 (1), 15–21 (in Chinese).
Visual Quality of Israeli Coast. Geol. J. 21 (3), 233–240. Daniel, T.C., Vining, J., 1983. Methodological issues in the assessment of landscape
Bell, S., 1999. Landscape: Pattern, Perception and Process. E & FN Spon, London. quality. In: Altman, I., Wohlwill, J.F. (Eds.), Behavior and the Natural Environ-
Bell, S., 2001. Landscape pattern, perception and visualization in the visual manage- ment. Plenum Press, New York, pp. 39–84.
ment of forests. Landsc. Urban Plann. 54, 201–211. Edwards, I.D., 2000. Education by stealth: the subtle art of educating people who
Bernáldez, F.G., Ruiz, J.P., Ruiz, M., 1984. Landscape perception and appraisal: ethic, didn’t come to learn. Roots 20, 37–40.
aesthetics and utility. In: Proceeding of the 8th International Conference on Gaerling, T., Golledge, R.G., 1993. Environmental perception and cognition. In: Behav-
Environment and Human Action, Berlin, IAPS 8. ior and Environment: Psychological and Geographical Approaches. Elsevier,
Bernáldez, F.G., Gallardo, D., Abello, R.P., 1987. Children’s landscape preferences: Amsterdam (Chapter 7).
from rejection to attraction. J. Environ. Psychol. 7 (1), 169–179. Gifford, R., 1996. Environmental Psychology: Principles and Practice. Allyn & Bacon.
Bishop, I.D., Rohrmann, B., 2003. Subjective responses to simulated and real envi-
Giffurida, L., 1996. Planning an interpretation programme. In: Gonzáles, N. (Ed.),
ronments: a comparison. Landsc. Urban Plann. 65 (4), 261–277.
Cultivating Green Awareness. Proceeding of the Second International Congress
Burbidge, R.B., 1990. Interpretation in botanic gardens. In: He-Shan-An, et al. (Eds.),
on Education in Botanic Gardens. Jardı́n Botánico Canario “Vieray Clavijo” Las
Proceedings of the International Symposium on Botanical Gardens. Jiangsu Sci-
Palmas de Gran Canaria, Spain, pp. 189–193.
ence and Technology Publishing House, Nanjing, China, pp. 269–277.
Ham, S.H., 1992. Environmental Interpretation: A Practical Guide for People with Big
Cerny, J.W., 1974. Scenic Analysis and Assessment, CRC-Critical Reviews in Environ-
Ideas and Small Budgets. North American Press, USA.
mental Control. University of New Hampshire, Durham, pp. 231–250.
Chang, L.S., 2001. The role of landscape design in improving the educational func- Hunt, J.D., Willis, P., 1988. The Genius of the Places. The English Landscape Garden
tions of botanic gardens in Taiwan. Ph.D. Thesis. The University of Reading, 1620–1820. MIT press, London.
UK. IUCN, 1987. The IUCN botanic gardens conservation strategy: a summary. In:
Chang, L.S., 2002. Introduction and analysis on the ways of interpreting living plant Bramwell, D., Hamann, O., Heywood, V., Synge, H. (Eds.), Botanic Gardens and
collections at site of Eden Project. UK. Landsc. Architec. 43, 75–84, in Chinese. the World Conservation Strategy. Academic Press, London.
Chang, L.S., 2003. Studies on the ways of plant display in botanic gardens from Jakle, J.A., 1987. The Visual Elements of Landscape. University of Massachusetts Press,
historical aspects. Landsc. Architec. 46, 31–38 (in Chinese). Amherst.
L.S. Chang et al. / Landscape and Urban Planning 86 (2008) 233–247 247

Kaplan, R., 1985. The analysis of perception via preference: a strategy for studying Stamps, A.E., 1990. Use of photographs to simulate environments: a meta-analysis.
how the environment is experienced. Landsc. Plann. 12, 161–176. Percep. Motor skills 71 (1), 907–913.
Kaplan, R., Kaplan, S., 1989. The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective. Stamps, A.E., 1999. Demographic effects in environmental aesthetics: a meta-
Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. analysis. J. Plann. Lit. 14 (2), 155–175.
Kostelecky, K.L., Hoskinson, M.J., 2005. A “novel” approach to motivating students. Tilden, F., 1977. Interpreting Our Heritage, 3rd ed. The University of North Carolina
Education 125 (3), 438–443. Press, Chapel Hill, USA.
Lai, M.C., 1993. An introduction to botanic gardens. Landsc. Architec. 14, 22–27 (in Ulrich, R.S., 1986. Human responses to vegetation and landscapes. Landsc. Urban
Chinese). Plann. 13, 29–44.
Li, J.W., 1990. Educational Media and Educational Technology. Shi Da Shu Yuan Press, Willison, J., 2006. Education for Sustainable Development: Guidelines for Action in
Taipei. Botanic Gardens. Botanic Gardens Conservation International, UK.
Misgav, A., 2000. Visual preference of the public for vegetation groups in Israel. Woodbridge, K., 2001. The Stourhead Landscape. The National Trust, UK.
Landsc. Urban Plann. 48, 143–159. Zube, L.H., Sell, J.L., Taylor, J.G., 1982. Landscape perception: research, application
Oikawa, J., 2000. Future role of living plant collections in gardens for biodiversity and theory. Landsc. Plann. 9, 1–33.
conservation. Ph.D. Thesis. The University of Reading, UK. Zube, L.H., Pitt, D.G., Evans, G.W., 1983. A lifespan developmental study of landscape
Olin, L., 1999. Across the Open Field: Essays Drawn from English Landscapes. Uni- assessment. J. Environ. Psychol. 3, 115–128.
versity of Pennsylvania Press.
Potteiger, M., Purinton, J., 1998. Landscape Narratives Design Practices for Telling Li-Shin Chang is an assistant professor in Urban Planning and Landscape
Stories. J. Wiley & Sons, New York. Architecture at Chaoyang University of Technology, Taiwan. She holds a
Polkinghorne, D.E., 1988. Narrative Knowing and the Human Science. State Univer- PhD from the Centre for Horticulture and Landscape, University of Reading,
sity of New York Press, Albany. UK and a BSc in horticultural science from the National Taiwan Univer-
Polkinghorne, D.E., 1995. Narrative configuration in qualitative analysis. In: Hatch, sity, Taiwan. Research interests include Interpretive design for environmental
J.A., Risniewski (Eds.), Life History and Narrative. The Falmer Press, London. education, Landscape perception and behavior, Landscape ecology and urban
Rakatansky, M., 1992. Spatial narratives. In: Whiteman, J., Kipnis, J., Burdett, R. greenery.
(Eds.), Strategies in Architectural Thinking. Chicago Institute for Architecture
and Urbanism. MIT Press, Chicago. R.J. Bisgrove is Senior Lecturer in the Centre for Horticulture and Landscape,
Ricoeur, P., 1981. Narrative time. In: Mitchell, W.J.T. (Ed.), On Narrative. University of University of Reading, UK and Course Director for the BSc degree in Landscape Man-
Chicago Press, Chicago. agement. He has a BSc in horticultural science from the University of Reading, UK,
Siciliano, P.C., 2005. Landscape expression: the early-eighteenth-century English and a MLA from the University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, USA. His research interests
landscape. In: Landscape Interpretations, History, Techniques and Design Inspi-
include History of garden design, Amenity turf management and the creation of
ration. Thomson Delmar Learning, Canada.
species-rich meadows and Planting design.
Soliva, R., 2007. Landscape stories: using ideal type narratives as a heuristic device
in rural studies. J. Rural Stud. 23 (1), 62–74.

You might also like