Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 26

Collective Violence and Group Solidarity: Evidence from a Feuding Society

Author(s): Roger V. Gould


Source: American Sociological Review, Vol. 64, No. 3 (Jun., 1999), pp. 356-380
Published by: American Sociological Association
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2657491 .
Accessed: 14/11/2014 12:03

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

American Sociological Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
American Sociological Review.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
COLLECTIVE VIOLENCE AND GROUP SOLIDARITY:
EVIDENCE FROM A FEUDING SOCIETY*

Roger V. Gould
University of Chicago

Sociological explanations of group conflict usually presuppose that the vari-


ous factors that breed hostility between collectivities also generate internal
solidarity. Outside of the protest literature, studies of conflict therefore pay
little attention to the collective-action problem facing groups in contention,
and therefore overestimate the likelihood of group conflict: Intergroup
struggle is implicitly regarded as a sufficient condition for group participa-
tion in violent conflict. Examination of nineteenth-century court documents
from Corsica, a society known for its tradition of collectivist feuding, shows
that violent incidents typically did not involve groups. The group character
of violence-in the form of collaborative use of lethal force and inclusion of
disputants' kin-was conditional on collective contention having occurred
before violence began. This and other empirical patterns support the view
that collective violence occurs when group action fails to convince an ad-
versary to back down. The failure to prevent escalation calls the group's
solidarity into question, compelling members to demonstrate that they are
able to overcome their collective-action problem.

O utside the area of political and social distinct social groups see their interests as
protest, sociological research on conflicting in the first place. Research on
group conflict has had little to say about the youth gangs and criminal organizations, for
collective-action dilemma faced by groups in instance, typically tries to explain violent
conflict. Studies of the origins of racial, eth- confrontations among such groups in terms
nic, nationalist, familistic, sectarian, or tribal of ethnic rivalry, competition over turf, or
conflict ordinarily adopt the supraindividual struggles over control of illegal markets
focus characteristic of sociology as a disci- (Black 1983; Janni 1972; Jankowski 1991;
pline: The issue of interest is typically not Reuter 1983; Short and Strodtbeck 1965;
how groups overcome internal obstacles to Suttles 1967). Similarly, studies of ethnic
collective action but rather why members of warfare often trace such conflicts to deep-
* seated hatreds based on past atrocities, cul-
Direct correspondence to Roger V. Gould, tural differences, or competition for territory
Departmentof Sociology, University of Chicago, or resources (Horowitz 1985; LeVine and
1126 East 59th Street, Chicago IL 60637
(rgouldl@midway.uchicago.edu). Research for Campbell 1972; Olzak 1992).
this article was done in part while the author was Because such accounts reason back from
a Fellow at the Center for Advanced Study in the conflict to the factors that are believed to
Behavioral Sciences. I gratefully acknowledge produce it, the transition from group interest
the financial support of the Harry Frank to group action is often treated either implic-
Guggenheim Foundation and the National Sci- itly as unproblematic,or explicitly as a func-
ence Foundation (SBR-9617880). Jim Fearon, tional response to conflict. Classic theoreti-
Roberto Fernandez, Nancy Hewitt, Victor Nee, cal statements (Coser 1956; Simmel [1923]
Gerry Suttles, and participants in the Social Or- 1955) have usually portrayedintergroupten-
ganization of Competition Workshop and the
Workshop on Comparative Politics and Histori-
sion as a basic source of internal solidarity.
cal Sociology at the University of Chicago of- More concretely, writers on youth gangs of-
fered helpful comments, as did the ASR Editor ten argue that fighting between gangs builds
and reviewers. Thanks also go to Tori Lawless internal cohesion (Jankowski 1991:88;
and Genevieve Zubrzycki for research assistance. Suttles 1967). Because group action is not

356 American Sociological Review, 1999, Vol. 64 (June:356-380)

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
COLLECTIVE VIOLENCE AND GROUP SOLIDARITY 357

seen as an anomaly from this perspective, by the tension between collective interest
studies of intergroup conflict concentrate on and individual interest, and because partici-
the sources of tension rather than on how pants in conflict are conscious of this ten-
collections of people manage to mobilize in sion. In the course of a dispute, individuals
response to such tensions (but see Olzak call on allies as a way of demonstrating to
1992).1 Reasoning about group conflict typi- their adversaries that they are not alone. But
cally occurs as follows: a significant con- because people on both sides know that the
frontation is observed-say, a gang war or a other side's solidarity may unravel if the dis-
riot-in which people express and act on pute intensifies, some of the time they will
their hostility in terms of a salient group refuse to back down. The resulting escalation
boundary. Analysts of the event note that pits group against group rather than indi-
members of the two groups compete for ma- vidual against individual, because allies of
terial benefits, residential space, or some the original disputants must now show-to
other valued resource. This preexisting com- themselves, to adversaries, and to wit-
petition (or mere hatred, or cultural misun- nesses-that their display of solidarity was
derstanding, or whatever factor turns out to not hollow. Expressions of group solidarity
be the basis for hostility) therefore "ex- are therefore double-edged: They may suc-
plains" the violent outburst (e.g., see ceed in forestalling escalation, but if they fail
Baldassare 1995). to do so they intensify the violence that oc-
Violent social confrontations attractatten- curs. The general awareness among contend-
tion in proportion to their scale and severity, ers that putatively solidary groups may fail
making it easy to disregard those conflicts to act together contributes to the likelihood
that remain limited to a few persons, or never of escalation to violence, and to the extent of
escalate to violence at all. Yet these lesser the harm that ensues.
conflicts are generally far more common The format of this article is as follows:
than are conflicts that eventuate in signifi- After a general theoretical discussion of
cant mobilization. (Laitin and Fearon [1996] group conflict, I argue that incidents of col-
show, for example, that only a small propor- lective violence are demonstrations of soli-
tion of ethnic disputes lead to war). Thus, darity by groups whose cohesiveness has
extant accounts of group conflict do theirjob been cast into doubt by their failure to per-
too well: They make sense of the cases in suade adversaries to back down. I derive
which preexisting competition coincides empirical implications of this argument and
with violent clashes between rival groups, test them using data on violence in Corsica.
but are unable to explain why most confron- In particular, I analyze the relationship be-
tations do not lead to outbreaksof group vio- tween group participation in violence and
lence. Studying only those instances in the characteristics of those disputes leading
which tension generates group conflict leads to violence. Finally, I show that the empiri-
to an underestimation of the obstacles to cal findings are best understood from a per-
group unity-and, as I shall show, of the im- spective that (1) views group violence as an
portant part these obstacles play in the tra- instance of collective action in which there
jectory of conflict. is a tension between group and individual
I propose and test the argumentthat group interests; and (2) focuses on how this ten-
violence occurs because groups are plagued sion influences the behavior of disputants
and their associates.
1 The usual debates are of two kinds. One is
whether the dimension of difference under study
is fundamental or reducible to some other social ACCOUNTING FOR GROUP
process, as when materialist accounts of conflict CONFLICT
portray racism or ethnic identification as ideo- No social process has attracted more theo-
logical instruments for economic exploitation
(Hechter 1975). The other debate treats group dis-
retical attention than that of conflict among
tinctions as at least socially real (that is, not mere human groups. Nevertheless, attempts to ex-
mystifications), and asks what exogenous factors plain group conflict continually return to
trigger an upsurge in conflict (Olzak 1992; Ross three themes that can be termed interest,
1986). identity, and social organization.

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
358 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Accounts emphasizing interest observe people to organize themselves for mutual de-
that human communities value resources like fense, most often in the form of kinship
territory,capital, and access to markets, and groups. In the typical social-organization ac-
that at times they compete with other com- count of feuding societies, disputes are in-
munities for the use of these resources.2 trinsically collective because the institutional
Group conflict, ranging from exclusion to order obliges members of a kinship group to
war, occurs when migration, subsistence cri- regard offenses against any member as of-
ses, or shifts in relative group strength per- fenses against all. Moreover, it is the
suade one community to encroach on offender's entire group, rather than the indi-
another's resources or to preempt encroach- vidual offender, that bears responsibility.3
ment by the other (Oberschall 1978; Olzak The standardresponse to an injury,therefore,
1983, 1992; Simmel [1923] 1955). Individu- is either a comparableinjury inflicted on any
alist versions of the interest argument assert member of the offender's clan, or a collec-
that the very existence of such communities tive payment to the victim's clan (Black-
results from the benefits their members de- Michaud 1975; Boehm 1987; Campbell
rive from belonging. In other words, the 1964; Evans-Pritchard 1940; Fortes 1945;
Hobbesian problem of conflict between Gluckman 1955; Hasluck 1954; Miller 1990;
groups arises only because it has been solved Peters 1967; Unsal 1990). Group conflict
within groups (Elster 1989; Hardin 1995). thus emerges deterministically from the web
Identity argumentsconcentrate on the idea of relationships governing social life, even
that human beings are sometimes prone to when the triggering event involves just two
see themselves and others as instantiationsof individuals.
categories (e.g., worker, Methodist, Uzbek, It is the literature on traditional feuding
Southerner, woman) rather than as unique societies that most often invokes social or-
and autonomous persons (Hogg and Abrams ganization as the key factor in group conflict,
1988; Sherif 1966; Tajfel 1970; Turner and but variants of the argument also appear in
Giles 1981). In one version of this perspec- research on contemporaryWestern societies,
tive, the need for self-esteem leads people to especially in connection with ethnic conflict,
attach positive value to the categories to organized crime, and urban gang warfare
which they belong and negative value to cat- (Bourgois 1995; Gambetta 1988, 1993;
egories that do not apply to them (Hogg and Jankowski 1991). Although the routinized
Abrams 1988; Tajfel 1974). More generally, mechanisms for conducting feuds that are
group membership may be constitutive of thought to characterize stateless societies
personal identity even though groups are typically do not exist in urban industrial set-
conventionally thought to be aggregations of tings, the relationships that bind people into
persons. With regard to ethnicity and race, groups are nevertheless seen to make most
identity-based explanations attributeconflict conflicts collective, at least in principle.
to deep cultural differences (or beliefs about
such differences) that foster misunderstand-
3 Interest-based accounts make a superficially
ing, hostility, and resentment, often indepen-
similar point in that they concentrate on conflict
dently of any material basis for competition over resources held collectively, such as territory.
(Horowitz 1985; Kaplan 1993; LeVine and The difference is that the social relationships that
Campbell 1972). underlie group loyalty in social-organization ac-
Finally, argumentsthat invoke the concept counts do so independently of shared material in-
of social organization draw attention to the terests. Imagine that the inhabitants of a river-
norms and relationships that bind people into front town collaborate to shore up the levee dur-
groups. This emphasis is prevalent in studies ing a flood. An interest-centered explanation
of societies with weak or nonexistent states, would assert that the effort is collective because
in which feuding is particularlycommon. In the levee will protect the whole town; a social-
organization account, however, would expect col-
such societies, the absence or ineffectiveness laboration even if only a few houses were threat-
of state peacekeeping institutions obliges ened, because the town-dwellers care about each
other's safety. However, both arguments pay in-
2 Although I refer to three kinds of accounts, sufficient attention to the public-goods problem
most writers incorporate elements of each. inherent in such a situation.

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
COLLECTIVE VIOLENCE AND GROUP SOLIDARITY 359

All three factors-group interest, group bers. The tension between group welfare
identity, and social organization-underlie and individual welfare is starkest when
the processes that are believed to make hu- group solidarity entails risking one's life:
man existence, and thus the conflicts that All members benefit if the group acts col-
pervade it, collective. When all three factors lectively in defense of its shared interests,
are present, we should expect most disputes but even moderately sensible members
across group boundaries, even those that be- might hesitate before joining a possibly fa-
gin within dyads, to become group conflicts. tal fray. Given that disputants know this, ex-
Thus, from the standpoint of the extant per- pressions of group solidarity in the midst of
spective on social conflict, it is surprising a dispute may avert conflict escalation, but
when individual disputes in collectivist set- only if people are convinced that the expres-
tingsfail to turn into group disputes. More- sions are sincere.
over, if people in such contexts have little To summarize: Individuals benefit from
choice about whether to see any given dis- group life in part because membership of-
pute as collective, the slightest altercation fers protection against predation (along with
between any two persons from distinct more peaceable advantages, including affec-
groups would lead to a potentially devastat- tion and the pooling of economic risk). Pro-
ing group war. This threat would make vio- tection against outsiders, however, is only
lent disputes rare: If group interests domi- as good as the perceived solidarity of the
nated social life, minor dyadic conflict group, and the perception is contingent on
would inevitably lead to mutually disastrous solidary behavior. Expressions of support
collective conflict, leading people to avoid for one member by fellow group members
conflict more assiduously than they do in can convince an outsider not to act aggres-
individualistic societies.4 However, the ex- sively against the threatened member as
isting evidence overwhelmingly suggests long as the support is seen as genuine: If
that there is more rather than less interper- members of a kinship group support each
sonal violence in societies inhabited by self- other in verbal disputes with others but con-
protecting corporate groups (Black 1983; sistently fail to sustain this support when
Blok 1974; Brown 1986; Chagnon 1983; disputes escalate to physical or lethal force,
Given 1977; Otterbein 1968; Otterbein and then their verbal behavior will not dissuade
Otterbein 1965; Ross 1986). Either people adversaries from escalating. Unless groups
in such societies are remarkablyand self-de- have a reputation for following through on
structively imprudent,or collectivism is less solidarity expressed prior to violent conflict,
than complete. such expressions of solidarity constitute
The most straightforwardsolution to this "cheap talk" (Farrell and Rabin 1996; Gib-
theoretical puzzle lies in recognizing that bons 1992), will not convey information
group solidarity is imperfect: Even when about what will happen after escalation, and
members of a lineage, tribe, or ethnic group therefore will have no deterrentvalue.5
have common interests, customs that distin-
guish them symbolically from other groups,
and social ties rooted in kinship or resi- 5Although this argument is about strategic ac-
dence, they also have individual interests tion, it does not presuppose that people are ratio-
that set them apart from fellow group mem- nal egoists: The core concept here is solidarity,
by which I mean individuals' willingness to sac-
4 The threat of mutually destructive conflict rifice narrow self-interest for collective interests.
does in fact appear to push groups into negoti- Even egoists will find it rational to cooperate if
ated settlements, though these are often unsuc- they are likely to meet again and they value the
cessful (Boehm 1987; Evans-Pritchard 1940; future heavily enough. But in cases in which co-
Gluckman 1955; Wilson 1988). But the cultural operating carries a significant risk of death, these
availability of dispute resolution procedures is conditions are difficult to meet. Accordingly, co-
unlikely to be a sufficiently secure escape route operation in high-risk situations probably de-
to eliminate the threatof group war. The most dif- pends on genuine mutual caring: An individual
ficult problem facing competing groups in this deciding to risk death to save another most likely
situation is the fact that neither side wants to ap- views the other's welfare as substitutable for his
pear eager to settle. or her own.

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
360 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Group violence

Violent resolution (P)

contention I
Group/contention
/ Group . 1 One-on-one violence
(1 -p3)

Nonviolent resolution
(1- p1)
Disputes Group violence

Violent resolution (P4)

One-on-one violence
One-on-one ()2

contention (1 4)
Nonviolent resolution
(1 - P2)

Core hypotheses and relevant empirical tests:


(1) Pi < P2 (see Table 2, comparison of assault with lethal violence);
(2) p3 > p4 (see Tables 3-6, association of group violence with prior group contention).

Figure 1. Schematic Representation of Dispute Trajectories


Note: Dots show the endpoints of each trajectory; p represents the probability that the dispute has the
specified character, conditional on reaching the branch in question. For example, Pi represents the probabil-
ity that the dispute will end in violence, conditional on contention having been collective prior to violence.
Similarly, p4 represents the probability of group violence, conditional on the dispute's having involved one-
on-one contention prior to violence and having ended in violence. Note that it is not necessary to have
information on incidents ending in nonviolent resolution to test hypotheses regarding the relative sizes of p3
and p4.

EMPIRICAL IMPLICATIONS portion of disputes falling into the group


contention branch and ending violently that
Two core predictions and two subsidiary include collective violence. Likewise, p4 is
predictions follow from my argument. The the proportion of disputes falling into the
core predictions are illustrated in Figure 1, one-on-one contention branch and ending
which represents the trajectory of conflict as violently that include collective violence.
a tree of outcomes. All conflicts begin as Dots indicate the possible endpoints of the
nonviolent disputes, some of which involve process (i.e., points at which there are no
group contention (upper branch), while oth- furtherbranches).
ers involve only one-on-one contention Suppose there are 100 disputes starting at
(lower branch). For each of these main the root of the tree. If 40 include group con-
branches in the tree, there are two further tention, and 18 of those lead to violence, then
branches: peaceful resolution and violent Pi = 18/40 = .45. If 9 of those 18 violent inci-
resolution. Finally, there are two sub- dents include group violence, thenp3 = 9/18
branches of those disputes that proceed to = .50. This leaves 60 disputes in the one-on-
violent resolution: Some involve one-on-one one contention branch. If 40 of these lead to
violence and some involve collective vio- violence and 20 are resolved peacefully, then
lence. In Figure 1, Pi represents the propor- P2 = 40/60 = .67. If group violence occurs in
tion of disputes in the upper main branch 10 of the 40 incidents that involve one-on-
(those involving group contention) that end one-contention and end in violence, then p4
in violence; P2 represents the proportion of = 10/40 = .25.
disputes in the lower main branch (one-on- The first core prediction is that disputes
one contention) that end in violence. At the involving group contention escalate to lethal
next branching stage, P3 represents the pro- violence less frequently than do disputes in-

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
COLLECTIVE VIOLENCE AND GROUP SOLIDARITY 361

volving one-on-one contention; in terms of force. Consequently, one might predict a


the outcome tree,P1 < P2. This prediction fol- correlation between group contention and
lows directly from the proposition that group group violence even if group contention has
contention discourages escalation: If demon- no value as a deterrent.I therefore provide a
strations of solidarity in a dispute increase furthertest of the prediction that p3 > p4, US-
the likelihood that the adversary will back ing violence that includes nondisputants-
down, then disputes involving only one-on- termed "generalized violence"-as an addi-
one contention should be overrepresented tional indicator of the collective characterof
among those that end in violence. If demon- violence. If group action early in a dispute
strations of solidarity have no deterrent is a promise to act in concert if the situation
value, then disputes involving group conten- worsens, escalation of conflict obliges all
tion will end in violence about as often as available group members-not just those
disputes involving only one-on-one conten- who have already acted-to live up to the
tion, implying Pi = P2. (Because the total promise. Therefore, when group contention
number of disputes that do not escalate to le- makes an issue of reputations for solidarity,
thal violence is unknown, I rely on data con- previously uninvolved members become in-
cerning a subset of such disputes-those that volved in violence at a higher rate than
led only to nonlethal physical assault.) when contention has remained one-on-one.
The second core prediction is that, among In other words, the inclusion of nondispu-
those disputes that escalate to violence, col- tants in violent conflict is not an automatic
lective violence is more likely if group con- consequence of escalation, but is a contin-
tention occurs prior to escalation (i.e., in Fig- gent response to group contention in the
ure 1,p3 > p4). When group members act col- previolence stage. Thus, the proposition that
lectively in the early stages of a dispute, they p3 > p4 implies that group contention prior
communicate to their adversarytheir willing- to escalation is associated not only with a
ness to act collectively at the next, more dan- higher likelihood of collaborative violence,
gerous, stage. If escalation to violence occurs but also with a higher likelihood that vio-
anyway, group action at this stage is more lence will include previously uninvolved as-
likely than in cases in which conflict at the sociates of the disputants.
first stage is one-on-one: The previolence as- The first subsidiary prediction is that the
sertion of group loyalty, combined with the likelihood of both types of collective vio-
failure of this assertion to forestall escala- lence-collaborative and generalized-is
tion, casts doubt on the group's reputation higher in situations in which reputations for
for solidarity. Disputes involving only one- solidarity matter the most. For example, if
on-one contention before escalation likewise kin groups act collectively in conflicts be-
are more likely to remain one-on-one if vio- cause doing so enhances the value of expres-
lence occurs, because associates of the two sions of solidarity in future encounters, col-
disputants have implicitly declined to treat laborative violence should occur most often
the conflict as collective.6 In short, among between disputants with a high likelihood of
disputes that eventually turn violent, group future interaction. Consequently, group vio-
action prior to violence is positively corre- lence is particularlylikely when participants
lated with group participation in violence. on opposing sides of a conflict are already
This prediction requires a careful test be- acquainted and thus have reason to believe
cause there is a simpler explanation if group they will face one another again.
contention prior to escalation is associated The second subsidiary prediction is that
only with a higher likelihood of collabora- group participationin violence is rarerwhen
tion in the use of violence. It is natural to the boundariesbetween opposing sides in the
expect that, if two people join forces in the conflict are unclear. In particular,in societ-
nonviolent stage of a dispute, the same two ies organized principally by kinship, con-
people will act together in employing lethal flicts pitting kin against one another should
lead less often to group violence, net of other
6 A disputant's kin might, for instance, decline factors, than conflicts in which the opposing
to express support because they view his or her sides have no kin connections. When a dis-
claim as trivial, illegitimate, or futile. pute divides two members of a given family

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
362 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

or clan, the potential allies of one party are and battery). Assaults are disputes that were
also potential allies of the other, so any dem- "resolved" with a level of contention (e.g.,
onstration of solidarity with one side is off- use of fists) that was sufficiently intense to
set by a commensurate show of distance provoke legal intervention, but was consid-
from the other side. Collective violence in erably less intense than employment of lethal
conflicts among kin therefore conveys no in- force. If the claim that group contention de-
formation about overall group solidarity, and ters escalation to violence holds for disputes
accordingly should occur less often in such in general, it should also hold for the subset
conflicts than in conflicts between nonkin.7 of disputes in which contention includes
physical but nonlethal aggression. Compar-
ing the amount of group action in these inci-
Unobserved Disputes
dents of intermediate intensity with the
The hypothesis that p3 exceeds p4 can be amount of group action in lethal incidents
tested directly by observing only those dis- amounts to a conservative comparison of P2
putes that became violent, because the out- and pi Stated simply, if the deterrence hy-
come of interest (group violence versus one- pothesis is correct, then there must be more
on-one violence) is defined exclusively in group action in disputes that end with low-
terms of this subset of disputes.8 However, level violence than in those that lead to le-
the best test of the hypothesis thatP2 exceeds thal violence. (See Appendix A for a formal
Pi requires comprehensive information about derivation of this prediction.)
all disputes, not just those that turn violent:
Looking only at disputes that end violently,
it is impossible to determine pi and P2 be- DATA SOURCES AND METHODS
cause the denominator (total disputes) has Corsica is a Mediterranean island that has
not been evaluated. The data employed in been part of France almost continuously
this study come primarily from criminal tri- since its cession by Genoa in the late eigh-
als in which the charge was murder or at- teenth century. Throughout the nineteenth
tempted murder; these trials tell us nothing century it was a department(the basic politi-
about the two subbranches in Figure 1 la- cal unit of France since the Revolution of
beled "nonviolent resolution." 1789), with representation in the National
A partial test of this hypothesis can be per- Assembly and the same local political insti-
formed, however, by comparing disputes that tutions as other departments. Linguistically,
escalated to lethal violence (murder and at- Corsica is closer to Italy than to France (the
tempted murder) with disputes involving Corsican language is a dialect of Italian,
only nonlethal physical aggression (assault though most inhabitants also speak French),
and is similar in several respects to other
7 The availability of third parties with kin ties Mediterranean islands like Sicily and
to both disputants should also reduce the overall Sardinia. Corsica's economy has always
likelihood of escalation, as Gluckman (1955) sug- been principally agrarian and pastoral (al-
gests. However, because I do not have detailed
though tourism has become increasingly im-
information about disputes that end peacefully, I
make no predictions about the relative likelihood portant in the second half of the twentieth
of violence for disputes between kin. century), with the land divided between
8 Consider the following analogy. Suppose we sheep-grazing and the cultivation of various
want to know whether drinking coffee at home in crops such as figs, olives, and cereals.
the morning makes workers more productive, and Nineteenth-centuryCorsica closely fits the
we have information on (1) productivity on all profile of what many anthropologists would
days a sample of workers came to work, and (2) call an "honor society" (Abu-Lughod 1986;
whether they drankcoffee on each of those days. Blok 1981; Bourdieu 1966; Campbell 1964;
We could ask whether coffee consumption in- Gilmore 1987; Peristiany 1966; Pitt-Rivers
creased their productivity without knowing any-
thing about those days when they did not come to
1966; Schneider 1971; Wikan 1984; Wyatt-
work. Only if we wanted to know something Brown 1982). Family groups played a major
about the impact of coffee consumption on com- role in all aspects of social life, and group
ing to work at all would we need additional data prestige depended heavily on the chastity of
on nonwork days. female members (a prerequisite for advanta-

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
COLLECTIVE VIOLENCE AND GROUP SOLIDARITY 363

geous marriages) in conjunction with male the incident led to a fatality or injury.
members' reputation for the capacity to de- Twenty-eight cases were dropped because
fend the group's interest through violence the summariesprovided insufficient informa-
(Albitreccia 1942; Busquet [1920] 1990; tion or because there was some doubt about
Marcaggi [1932] 1966; Pomponi 1979; the details of the incident. The resulting data
Versini 1964; Wilson 1988). Although now set includes a total of 221 violent incidents,
used in numerous languages, the word ven- with 174 cases from 1851-1852, 32 cases
detta was originally Corsican. from 1865, and 15 cases from 1893.9
The honor tradition, combined with the The disputes featured in these data are
fact that most Corsican men carried both typical of those leading to homicidal vio-
firearmsand knives, produced an exceptional lence anywhere, although tinged with the
amount of violence throughout the 1800s. character of a pastoral/agrarian society
The annual homicide rate ranged from 17 to where honor plays a key role. Corsicans of
80 per 100,000 inhabitants during this pe- the period killed one another over card
riod, whereas for France as a whole the rate games, insults, property damage, boundary
was consistently less than 1 per 100,000. In disputes, and verbal insults, but also, about
comparison, most contemporaryWestern so- 10 percent of the time, in revenge for past
cieties experience homicide rates under 2 per killings, and another 10 percent of the time
100,000, while the largest U.S. cities have over refusals to marry a "compromised"
rates ranging roughly from 10 to 60 per woman. 10
100,000 (U.S. Bureau of the Census 1996).
Information on violent conflict is drawn
from the records of the Cour d'Assises (court Validityof Trial Documents
of assizes) of Bastia, which handled all cases There are two potential obstacles to the va-
of murder, attempted murder, and man- lidity of court documents as sources of data
slaughter (along with other felonies such as on conflict and violence. First, the docu-
extortion, fraud, and perjury) for the depart- ments could give a misleading picture of ac-
ment of Corsica. Because of confidentiality tual violence through selection of nontypical
restrictions, the most recent year for which cases. If a large number of violent incidents
data are available is 1893. Full transcriptsof failed to lead to a trial (e.g., for lack of evi-
the trials no longer exist, but elaborate sum- dence) and if these incidents differed system-
maries of each case are preserved by the atically from those that appear in the data,
Ministry of Justice. Each summary contains then generalizations from court data to vio-
detailed information on the defendantss, a lent incidents overall might not be justified.
narrativeof the events leading up to the inci- A second difficulty is that facts about the in-
dent, including past conflicts between the in- cidents themselves may have been distorted
volved parties, and the outcome of the trial. in the trials. If accounts given in the trial
Yearly caseloads varied roughly between 30 summaries describe what happened in a sys-
and 100 throughout the nineteenth century. tematically biased way, it would be unrea-
Although most of the cases handled by the sonable to draw inferences about actual vio-
Bastia court pertained to murder or at- lence from these data.
tempted murder, some involved burglary, Available evidence indicates that selection
fraud, perjury,or rape. Only cases involving bias is not a major problem because, for vio-
the use of potentially lethal force (i.e., a fire- lent incidents, attritionfrom the initial inves-
arm, cutting instrument, and in one case a tigation stage to the trial was moderate to
beating with a heavy rock) were included in low-indeed lower in Corsica than in the rest
this study.
In addition to 1893, all cases in 1851-1852 9 The total number for 1893 is smaller partly
and in 1865 were selected from the archival because violent conflict steadily declined during
the nineteenth century, but also because data from
inventory, furnishing data covering a range the last court session in 1893 are not available.
of historical and political contexts. Every 10 Several of the attacks of the latter sort were
case involving lethal or potentially lethal carried out by the women themselves, not by male
violence from these three periods was in- family members-although it was more common
cluded in the sample, regardless of whether for brothers of dishonored women to do so.

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
364 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Table 1. Retention and Attrition of Criminal Investigations in the Department of Corsica and France,
1858 to 1860

Criminal Indictments Indictments


Investigations Recommended Upheld
Disposition of Cases Initiated by Magistrate by Grand Jury

Corsica
Cases remaining 311 230 215
Cases dropped 0 81 15
Percent remaining 100 74 69

France
Cases remaining 24,684 13,256 12,660
Cases dropped 0 11,428 596
Percent remaining 100 54 51

Source: France, Ministere de la Justice (1859, 1860, 1861).

of France for the same period. The French ures cover all felonies in aggregate, they set
criminal justice system, like other European a conservative lower bound on the retention
systems, depended (and still depends) prin- rate for homicides and attemptedhomicides,
cipally on the reportof an investigating mag- which were almost certainly prosecuted more
istrate (juge d'instruction) in determining often than such crimes as burglaryand fraud.
whether a felony complaint should lead to an It seems reasonable to infer that about three-
indictment. Prosecutors sent misdemeanor fourths of investigations of lethal violence
complaints directly to lower courts and re- eventuated in a trial. As a result, the differ-
ferred all felony complaints they deemed ences between selected incidents and those
genuine to a magistrate, whose role re- that do not appear in the data would have to
sembled that of the prosecutor in the Anglo- be large to seriously bias the findings.12
American system but with greater authority. Two factors help to account for the higher
Following an investigation, which typically retention rate in Corsica as compared with
included witness interviews and evidence- the rest of France. In the first place, Corsica
gathering, the magistrate would either dis- was an island with a well-defined, stable, and
miss the case or recommend an indictment small population (roughly 200,000), making
before a grandjury (chambre d'accusation).
The grand jury would in turn either uphold cause suspects sometimes died in custody. (Sus-
or reject the recommendation. The main pects not in custody were tried in absentia, gen-
source of attrition for felonies, then, was the erating a trial document even when suspects had
decision by an investigating magistrate not evaded arrest.) However, the annual figures show
to proceed. Rejection of indictments by the that over 95 percent of indictments led to a trial
within 12 months.
grand jury was a significant but more lim- 12 The most likely candidates for underrepre-
ited source of attrition. sentation in the data are incidents with no victims
Drawing on annual reports of the French and incidents in which all participants in a con-
Ministry of Justice from 1858 through 1860, flict killed one another. A census of crimes
Table 1 gives the numbers of criminal inves- known to the police in Corsica for 1898 included
tigations initiated by magistrates in Corsica no case of the latter type (Archives Nationales,
and France, and shows the percentages re- Series F7 12850). To check whether underrepre-
maining in the courts at each stage of the sentation of victimless incidents affected the find-
process. Total attritionfrom initial investiga- ings, I reran the analyses reported in this paper
after assigning a weight to each case inversely
tion to indictment was a little less than one- proportional to the number of casualties (10 per-
half in France overall, and slightly less than cent of the incidents involved no casualties). The
one-third for Corsica.'" Because these fig- estimates were within one decimal point of those
reported here, indicating at most only minor dis-
I Not all indictments led to a trial, notably be- tortion because of this type of selection bias.

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
COLLECTIVE VIOLENCE AND GROUP SOLIDARITY 365

it easier to identify suspects there than, for try to hide the truth, they did so in ways that
instance, in large cities on the mainland. In- served their competing purposes. Accounts
deed, only 1 in 12 discontinued investiga- given by defendants, complainants, and third
tions were dropped because no suspects parties therefore diverged often enough (e.g.,
could be found, compared with 1 in 9 for regarding who drew a weapon first) to dispel
France as a whole. Second, investigations any illusions about collective defiance of the
were unusually aggressive in Corsica be- court. Neither magistrates nor lawyers ex-
cause the central authoritieswere determined pected to hear completely honest statements
to suppress violence in a region they re- from principals or from witnesses who may
garded as troublesome and backward (Bindi have been their associates. The trial process
1991; Culioli 1990; France, Ministere de la therefore incorporatedthe testimony of up to
Justice 1860, 1882; Marcaggi [1932] 1966; three dozen witnesses (averaging 20 for in-
Wilson 1988). In their quarterly reports to vestigations and 12 for trials).
the Minister of Justice in Paris, magistrates Second, the court usually had recourse to
in Corsica continually referred to the effect forensic evidence concerning the size and
they hoped their activities would have on the shape of knife wounds, the angle of entry and
"mores"of this ratherun-French society. exit for gunshot wounds, the position of vic-
This tension between the legal-rational tims' bodies, and gunpowder traces on re-
customs of the French state and Corsican tra- cently used weapons. This evidence was par-
ditions for dealing with conflict is central to ticularly important when, for instance, a de-
the second validity issue-the accuracy of fendant claimed that only his brother had
trial summaries' accounts of what happened. used his weapon. Finally, prosecutors took
Given disagreementbetween state authorities pains to demonstrate to witnesses and to
and the inhabitants about what constituted Corsicans in general that they did not take
acceptable behavior, defendants and wit- false testimony lightly-between 1851 and
nesses sometimes tried to mislead the court 1860, nearly 50 perjurycases appearedin the
about the circumstances surroundingviolent docket of the Bastia assize court (France,
conflict. For example, after shooting and Ministere de la Justice 1882).
wounding the local priest at Bastiano in No court is infallible, particularlyin cases
1852, Michele Taddei testified that he had with few witnesses, but it seems likely that
been angry because the priest was having an for the majority of the trials used in this pa-
affair with his wife. Prosecutors maintained per the variables of interest correspond to
that he had simply been trying to rob the what actually happened. Local customs ex-
priest and that the story of the affair was a erted influence on the judicial process not so
ruse to gain sympathy from the jury. He was much in establishing facts as in deciding the
convicted, but the jury granted the excuse of verdict and sentence-Corsican juries ac-
'provocation.' quitted defendants over 50 percent more of-
Clearly, some testimony was false, and ten than did juries in the rest of France. Even
some Corsicans saw the courts as an alien after convictions, magistrates frequently la-
justice system to be manipulated when pos- mented what they viewed as unwarranted
sible with the aid of deception and indig- jury indulgence, including grants of extenu-
enous juries. It would be wise, however, not ating circumstances and recognition of mur-
to exaggerate the ease with which such non- ders as "excusable."14 The clash between the
cooperation could divert the court from dis- traditional and modern justice systems ap-
covering who did what. In the first place, pears to have played itself out in the applica-
prosecutors relied on testimony from numer-
14 One of the most common such acts was to
ous eyewitnesses and from opposing sides in
each incident.13 When interested parties did admit the existence of "provocation" for a mur-
der defendant who had been insulted verbally.
Judges interpreted"provocation"to mean that the
13 A reviewer suggested that police and pros- victim had physically assaulted the defendant or
ecutors might have faced general noncooperation somehow made a physical confrontation unavoid-
from witnesses. In perusing over 20 years of court able. Extension of the term to include insults
documents, I have uncovered no complaints to clearly reflected the importance of honor to
this effect on the part of court officers. Corsican jurors.

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
366 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

tion of penalties ratherthan in the establish- acted preemptively if they anticipated that a
ment of factual details. new person would join in violence. This vari-
Despite these considerations, it is still pos-
able permits a more stringent test of my ar-
sible that the trial documents systematically gument because the only relevance a previ-
distort some of the relevant measures. I re- ously uninvolved person has is that he or she
is tied in some way to one of the disputants;
visit this issue later, and discuss its implica-
tions for interpretingthe results (see the sec-by definition, a nondisputant has taken no
tion entitled "Competing Interpretations" action that might invite violence or indicate
following the results section). an intention to participate in it. Inclusion of
nondisputants in violence is a stark indica-
tion that what may have begun as a one-on-
Outcome Measures one dispute has become a group conflict in
The outcome of interest-whether violence the eyes of at least one person.16 Naturally,it
has a group character or one-on-one charac- is logically possible, given the two defini-
ter-has two dimensions, termed collabora- tions, for generalization to occur indepen-
tive violence and generalized violence. Any dently of whether collaborative violence oc-
incident in which more than one person on curs. Empirically, however, collaborative
one side of a dispute employed lethal force violence and generalized violence are posi-
(fired a weapon, lunged at someone with a tively associated, reinforcing their interpre-
knife or sharpinstrument,or wielded a heavy tation as alternate measures of the collective
object at a person's head) was coded as an nature of violence. Like collaborative vio-
instance of collaborative violence. Violence lence, generalized violence is a dichotomous
was also considered collaborative when one measure, with 1 corresponding to inclusion
person audibly encouraged another to use a of a new person in violence.
weapon, furnished someone with a weapon
that the latter then used, or arranged for a
thirdparty to attack an adversary.(Five cases Independent Measures
involving the latter sort of activity-essen- The relevant independent measures for the
tially contract killing-appear in the data.) analysis are (1) whether group contention
The rationale for including these more indi- occurred prior to the first use of lethal force;
rect forms of group action is that individuals (2) whether disputants knew one another
who participatein these ways materially con- prior to the dispute; (3) whether there was a
tribute to violence, and in so doing bear kin connection between opposing sides in the
some of the risk both of counter-violence and incident; and (4) whether disputants be-
prosecution.15 The measure for collaborative longed to groups with a history of conflict or
violence is dichotomous; a value of 1 indi- competition.
cates collaborative violence, and 0 indicates Group contention. I noted any behavior
one-on-one violence. mentioned in the trial summarythat occurred
Generalized violence reflects whether the prior to the outbreak of violence and could
incident included at least one person-either clearly be construed as taking sides in a dis-
as a user of violence or as a target of vio-
lence-who had not previously taken any ac- 16 There are two cases in the data of an injury
tion, verbal or physical, in the dispute. New to someone completely unconnected to any of the
targets are treated the same way as new ac- disputants, and a third case of a person missing
tors in coding because disputantsmight have his target and injuring his own brother.In a fourth
incident, the assailant intended to kill his adver-
sary as he slept, but instead killed the man's in-
15 The choice to treat verbal encouragement as fant son in the bed next to him. The first three
equivalent to actual use of lethal force was moti- cases were coded as if the target was a disputant
vated mainly by the fact that formal legal systems because the trial clearly established in each case
have consistently treated such behavior as com- that the gunshot was aimed at one of the dispu-
plicity in homicide. Verbal encouragement in- tants ratherthan at the bystander. The fourth case
creases the likelihood that violence will occur, in was coded as generalized violence because it was
part because it persuades the assailant that he or obvious to the killer that the child was in the line
she has an ally. of fire.

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
COLLECTIVE VIOLENCE AND GROUP SOLIDARITY 367

pute.17 If more than one person on one side of any two individuals on opposite sides of a
a dispute had engaged in such behavior at any dispute sufficed to qualify the dispute as oc-
point before the first use of violence, the dis- curring between acquaintances.
pute was recorded as involving group con- Kin tie between disputants. Disputants
tention. For example, in one case from 1852, were coded as connected by kinship only if
two youths from the town of Bastelica were specific reference to a kin tie appearedin the
arguing over a cap one had taken from the trial summary. Shared surname was not suf-
other. The elder brothers of the two inter- ficient to establish kinship, given the consid-
vened on behalf of their respective relatives, erable number of villages in Corsica with
leading ultimately to a stabbing of one by the only one or two family names.19 Initially,
other. This event was coded as involving consanguineous relationships were distin-
group contention (multiple disputantson both guished from relationships by marriage
sides) but one-on-one violence (at most one (Daly and Wilson 1988), but these were com-
person on each side employing lethal force). bined when investigation failed to reveal a
Disputes were exhaustively classified as difference in their associations with the de-
involving (1) purely one-on-one contention pendent variables. By construction, when op-
prior to violence or (2) group contention posing sides were coded as tied by kinship,
prior to violence (multiple persons on at least they could not be coded as unacquainted.
one side against one or more persons on the Diffuse group conflict. In Corsican com-
other).18 Actions that qualified as taking munities, as in many other face-to-face set-
sides include voicing an opinion in favor of tings, recurrent conflict between the same
one party, insulting someone, issuing a de- sets of people was common, and such con-
mand, threatening some future action, strik- flict was often institutionalized in the form of
ing someone with a hand or stick, seizing political factions (Albitreccia 1942; Pomponi
contested property, and filing a legal com- 1979; Versini 1964; Wilson 1988). Trial sum-
plaint. Actions not coded as taking a side in- maries contain frequent references to a his-
cluded attempting to calm someone down, tory of conflictual (though not necessarily
restraining someone engaged in a fistfight, violent) relations between kin groups, or be-
informing one person about the actions of tween a particular family and an individual
another, and arranging for a meeting to re- with whom they were on bad terms. Because
solve differences. new disputes occurring against the back-
Prior tie. Disputants were coded as know- ground of group conflict would be more
ing one another either if the trial summary likely to lead to group violence, diffuse group
made it clear that this was so or if they lived conflict was included as an indicatorvariable.
in the same village. Because most communi- Associations between group contention and
ties had populations of only a few hundred,it the two outcome variables are thus estimated
would have been nearly impossible for resi- net of a background situation favoring col-
dents of the same village not to be acquainted. laborative or generalized violence.
For residents of different communities, the
threshold for inferring a social tie was low:
Controls
Disputants were coded as knowing one an-
other if they had had business dealings, had Vendetta. Twenty-six of the 221 incidents
been drinking or gambling together at the were vendettas-murders or attempted mur-
time of the incident, if they were engaged in ders in retaliation for past killings. The
a sexual rivalry,or if the documentmentioned analyses include an indicator variable for
past interactions of any sort. Any tie between such incidents because of the high likelihood
that these incidents would exhibit a different
17 Given this criterion, it was not necessary for
two people to be members of the same kinship
group for group action to occur. In all but a few 19 As in many small and isolated societies,
cases, however, only relatives acted together in Corsicans could in principle establish some kin
the previolence phase of disputes. The same was connection with almost any other inhabitant of
true of violence itself. the island. This meant that only third-degree and
18 No disputes involving more than two sides closer relationships were customarily recognized
appeared in the data. (Lenclud 1979; Wilson 1988).

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
368 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

pattern from that of disputes in which vio- variable is included that reflects whether any
lence occurred for the first time. In particu- third party intervened prior to the first use of
lar, use of violence by a previously un- violence.
involved individual was common in vendet- Sample year. The data come from three
tas (39 percent of vendettas, as contrasted distinct time points rather than from a ran-
with 22 percent of other incidents).20 dom sample of incidents across the whole
Presence of other group members. Col- period. Two indicator variables (one for 1865
laborative violence and generalized violence and one for 1893) are included in the multi-
were more likely when two or more mem- variate analysis to allow for possible differ-
bers of a kin group were physically present ences over time in the base rate of collabora-
at the moment violence began, simply be- tive violence and generalized violence and
cause multiple group members were avail- for empirical associations between time
able either as actors or as targets. The regres- point and the other independent variables.
sion models initially included a term for the
presence of multiple persons on one side of
the dispute and another for group presence Coding Reliability
on both sides. Various specifications showed The documents employed in this study are
that group presence was associated with a narrativesconcerning a diverse set of events
higher likelihood of collaborative violence whose details varied from one case to the
and generalized violence whether group next. Classifying each incident therefore re-
presence was mutual or asymmetric, but quires interpretation by a coder, creating a
principally in the former case. Hence, the risk of interratervariation in coding even if
models reportedhere control for group pres- the concepts underlying each variable are
ence on both sides. clear. To determine whether the data and
Female disputant. In light of the central- classification scheme afforded an acceptable
ity to Corsican society of male dominance level of reliability, I provided an assistant
within families and of female sexual purity, with a sample of 37 court summaries (every
disputes involving only men and disputes in- sixth case) and a description of the concepts
volving at least one woman might have dif- (group contention, involvement of non-
fered in the degree to which group solidarity disputants, and so on) used in the study. The
was activated. In particular,spousal violence percent agreement between my coding and
(of which there are several cases in the data) that of the assistant (from 85 to 97 percent
is a strong a priori candidate for a more dy- for the key variables) suggests that the trial
adic outcome. Therefore, I include an indi- summaries provide sufficiently clear infor-
cator variable reflecting involvement of a mation for substantial agreement regarding
woman as disputant, user of violence, or tar- values on the core variables.
get of violence.
Intervention by third party. Many inci-
dents of violence occurred after bystanders RESULTS
tried to separate disputants or, in the case of Table 2 tests the prediction that group con-
more drawn-out conflicts, after local no- tention deters escalation (pi < P2) by com-
tables attempted to work out a compromise. paring the amount of collaboration in lethal
Intervention of this kind could reduce the violence and in nonlethal violence in Corsica
likelihood of collaborative violence or gen- at two points in the mid-to-late nineteenth
eralized violence, if only because occasion- century. Data on France as a whole (includ-
ally it delayed escalation and gave people an ing Corsica) are included as a reference. The
opportunity to disperse. Thus, an indicator number of participants per incident can be
interpreted as an index of the level of col-
20 Why some violent confrontations led to ven- laboration in the two kinds of conflict: If
dettas but most did not will be the subject of fu-
all incidents involved a single assailant, the
ture investigation. Contrary to conventional no- value of this index would be unity. Note first
tions, the data for the nineteenth century suggest that Corsica, with less than 1 percent of the
that fewer than 10 percent of homicides resulted population of France, accounted for one-
in subsequent revenge killings. tenth of the homicides in the nation for the

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
COLLECTIVE VIOLENCE AND GROUP SOLIDARITY 369

Table 2. Level of Collaboration in Violence by Type of Violence (Nonlethal/Lethal) and Year: Corsica
and France, 1858 to 1860 and 1882 to 1885

Total Total Participants


Type of Violence and Date Incidentsa Participants per Incident
Assault and Battery
Corsica
1858-1860 1,111 1,728 1.56
1882-1885 2,065 3,211 1.55
France
1858-1860 40,092 55,036 1.37
1882-1885 84,196 112,232 1.33

Murder and Attempted Murder'


Corsica
1858-1860 87 117 1.34
1882-1885 167 210 1.26
France
1858-1860 826 971 1.18
1882-1885 1,673 2,044 1.22
Source: France,Ministerede la Justice (1859, 1860, 1861, 1882, 1883, 1884, 1886).
a
Refers to those incidentsclearedwith an arrest(i.e., those incidentsfor which at least one assailantwas
identified).
b Combines
premeditatedand unpremeditatedmurder(assassinat and meurtre).Negligent homicides are
excluded.

years covered in the table. Similarly, nonle- pattern if group contention constitutes a
thal forms of violent conflict appear to have show of solidarity-and hence a threat of
been more common in Corsica than in France group violence-in relatively collectivist so-
as a whole, though the gap was smaller: cieties accustomed to violent conflict.
Corsica's annual assault rate for this period Tables 3 and 4 present percentage distri-
was 1.8 per 1,000, as contrasted with .4 per butions of the two group violence measures
thousand for France as a whole. (collaborative violence and generalized vio-
My central claim is that group action dis- lence), disaggregated by the pattern of
courages escalation to violence by convey- previolence contention. The marginal totals
ing a message that violence will be collec- in Table 3 show that of the 221 incidents, 24
tive. For this assertion to be correct, group percent (53 incidents) involved collaborative
action must be more common in disputes that violence. (Recall that collaboration included
do not escalate to lethal force than in those verbal encouragementor furnishing someone
that do. Taking data for assault and battery with a weapon.) Thus three out of four inci-
as an indicator of the characterof contention dents involved at most one assailant on each
prior to lethal violence, Table 2 suggests that side of the conflict. Similarly, the marginal
the rate of group involvement in these inci- totals in Table 4 reveal that generalization of
dents- 1.55 actors per assault-was indeed violence to nondisputants was considerably
considerably higher than for homicides, for rarer than incidents in which violence was
which the rate was 1.26 actors per incident. restricted to disputants. This was especially
Moreover, the difference is greater,both pro- true of violence directed against individuals
portionally and absolutely, for Corsica than who had not actively taken sides in the con-
for France as a whole.21 This is the expected flict-30 cases (16 if vendettas are excluded)

21 Using the data for Corsican homicides tried 1852, 1865, and 1893), the comparable figure is
during the years covered in this study (i.e., 185 1- 1.31.

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
370 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Table 3. Percentage Distribution of Types of Table 4. Percentage Distribution of Types of


Violence (One-on-One/Collaborative) Violence (Disputants Only/Generalized)
by Type of Prior Contention: Corsica, by Type of Prior Contention: Corsica,
1851-1852, 1865, and 1893 1851-1852,1865, and 1893

Type of Prior Type of Prior


Contention Contention
Type of Violence One-on-One Group Total Type of Violence One-on-One Group Total

One-on-oneviolence 90.2 63.9 76.0 Violence involving 82.4 60.5 70.6


only disputants
Collaborativeviolence 9.8 36.1 24.0
Generalizedviolence 17.6 39.5 29.4
Total percent 100.0 100.0 100.0 Total percent 100.0 100.0 100.1

Numberof cases 102 119 221 Numberof cases 102 119 221

Note: X2 = 20.89, d.f. = 1; p < .001. Note: X2 - 12.63, d.f. = 1; p < .001.

ended with someone using lethal force esis of no relationship were true, the ob-
against at least one nondisputant, and only served association between group contention
22 cases (10 not counting vendettas) began and generalized violence would be negative.
this way. Fifty cases involved a nondisputant
as a user of violence, and in 43 of those cases
the violence was initiated by the non- Multivariate Analyses
disputant. Thus, generalization of the latter The logistic regression results in Tables 5
type was not a response to escalation, but an and 6 test whether the bivariate correlation
inherent element of escalation. between group contention and the two indi-
The bivariate analysis demonstrates that cators of group violence (collaborative vio-
both collaborative violence and generalized lence and generalized violence) persists af-
violence were significantly more likely (p < ter controlling for a variety of other factors.
.001 in each case) when at least one side had These regression models also include coeffi-
acted collectively in the previolence stage of cient estimates for the subsidiary hypothesis
the dispute.22Moreover, the difference was of an association between group violence and
substantively large: Collaborative violence preexisting social connections between dis-
was nearly four times more common follow- putants.
ing group contention than following one-on- Table 5 shows, across a variety of specifi-
one contention, and generalized violence was cations, that group contention prior to vio-
more than twice as likely following group lence was associated with a significantly
contention as compared with one-on-one higher rate of collaborative violence. Trans-
contention. The result in Table 4 is especially forming the point estimate from Model 6 into
stark because the definition of the variable an odds ratio (e1 36= 3.90) reproducesthe bi-
and the coding procedures were biased variate result that, net of other factors, the
against this finding-the more people de- odds of collaborative violence (conditional
fined as disputants prior to violence, the on violence occurring) were close to four
fewer candidates there are for generalized times greater when group contention oc-
violence. Consequently, if the null hypoth- curred prior to escalation. The presence of
two groups and diffuse group conflict were
22 The association appears to be additive in the
also significantly associated with collabora-
sense that group violence was more likely when tive violence at similar levels. After these
groups had been active on both sides than when
only one side had acted collectively; but the num-
background conditions are controlled, group
ber of cases is too limited for this result to be sta- contention prior to violence continues to
tistically reliable. To preserve statistical power, show a strong association with the dependent
the analyses combine these two conditions into a variable. Thus, even in situations where dis-
single category that reflects group contention on putants were already predisposed to see con-
one or both sides. flicts as collective, expressions of solidarity

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
COLLECTIVE VIOLENCE AND GROUP SOLIDARITY 371

Table 5. Coefficients from the Logistic Regression of Collaborative Violence on Selected Indepen-
dent Variables: Corsica, 1851-1852, 1865, and 1893

Independent Variable Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6

Group contention 1.65** 1.66** 1.75** 1.57** 1.34** 1.36


prior to violence (.38) (.38) (.40) (.40) (.42) (.42)
Kin tie linking -.26 -.18 -.18 -.15 -.14
disputants (.42) (.43) (.45) (.46) (.47)
No prior tie between -.49 -.55 -.24 -.44 -.41
disputants (.51) (.52) (.53) (.55) (.55)
Female disputant -.43 -.38 -.39 -.47
(.38) (.39) (.40) (.41)
Diffuse group conflict 1.20* 1.03* 1.00*
prior to dispute (.48) (.50) (.51)
Vendetta -.21 .03 -.07
(.61) (.62) (.64)
Two groups - 1.15** 1.26**
physically present (.44) (.46)
Third-party - - -.50
intervention (.44)
1865 - - .01
(.51)
1893 - - .20
(.79)

Constant -2.22 -2.10 -2.05 -2.26 -2.31 -2.17


-2 log-likelihood 221.13 219.95 218.61 210.45 203.57 202.08
Improvement x2 22.36** 23.54** 24.87** 33.03 39.92** 41.40
Degrees of freedom 1 3 4 6 7 10

Note: N = 221. Numbers in parentheses are standard errors.


<.05 ** < .01 (two-tailed tests)

prior to violence made a difference in the an association may reflect a confounding of


outcome. several phenomena. Several cases involving
The estimates for preexisting kin ties be- women were attacks on wives by their hus-
tween disputants are consistently negative, bands, who invariably committed such acts
alone; others were attacks on men who had
as expected, but the coefficients are not sig-
nificant. (This is also true for absence of refused to marrywomen with whom they had
any prior tie.) Thus, these data show no evi-had sexual relationships. Because the latter
dence of a different rate of collaborative incidents often involved violence by more
violence for conflicts between kin compared than one family member, the two incident
with conflicts between nonkin. The result is types correlate with collaborative violence in
unchanged when consanguineous relation- opposite directions.
ships are distinguished from ties through Results for generalized violence, presented
marriage. in Table 6, largely parallel those for collabo-
There were no observable differences in rative violence. The odds that a nondisputant
odds of collaborative violence between ven- would become involved as either a user or a
dettas and other conflicts, across sample target of violence were between two and
years, or between incidents in which a third three times greater when group contention
party intervened and those in which none occurred prior to the first use of lethal force.
did. Nor is there an association between the As with collaborative violence, the presence
odds of collaborative violence and involve- of two groups and diffuse group conflict
ment of a woman in disputes. The absence of prior to the dispute were associated with

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
372 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Table 6. Coefficients from the Logistic Regression of Generalized Violence on Selected Independent
Variables: Corsica, 1851-1852, 1865, and 1893

Independent Variable Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6

Group contention 1.11 ** 1.13** 1.17** 1.12** .86* .87*


prior to violence (.32) (.33) (.34) (.40) (.43) (.43)
Kin tie linking -.87* -.83 -.63 -.63 -.60
disputants (.42) (1.15) (.49) (.50) (.51)
No prior tie between -.93 -.95 -.46 -.66 -.67
disputants (.50) (.50) (.57) (.59) (.58)
Female disputant -.19 -.06 -.22 -.20
(.36) (.41) (.42) (.43)
Diffuse group conflict 1.59** 1.43** 1.43**
prior to dispute (.48) (.50) (.50)
Vendetta 2- 2.43** 2.52** 2.517*
(.74) (.75) (.76)
Two groups 1.42** 1.41**
physically present (.51) (.51)
Third-party -.95* -.91*
intervention (.45) (.46)
1865 -.28
(.55)
1893 .21
(.81)

Constant -1.54 -1.27 -1.24 -2.02 -1.76 -1.76


-2 log-likelihood 254.74 247.53 247.25 194.60 183.85 183.48
Improvement x2 13.02* 20.24** 20.51* 73.16* 83.92** 84.28**
Degrees of freedom 1 3 4 6 8 10

Note: N = 221. Numbers in parentheses are standard errors.


*p < .05 **p < .01 (two-tailed tests)

fourfold increases in the odds of generalized definition distinct from the original victim.
violence. There is some indication that gen- Second, incidents in which third parties in-
eralized violence was more likely when dis- tervened prior to the first use of lethal force
putantsknew one anotherthan when they did generalized to new group members signifi-
not, and less likely when they were related cantly less often than did incidents in which
by kinship. Again, however, these relation- no intervention occurred. Evidently prior in-
ships are statistically weak-particularly af- tervention did not prevent collaborative vio-
ter controlling for diffuse group conflict, lence conditional on escalation to violence
which is correlated negatively with kin con- (though it may well have prevented escala-
nection and positively (by construction) with tion itself), but it did shield nondisputants
prior acquaintance. from being drawn in.
Table 6 echoes the previous finding for
disputes involving women in that such dis-
putes were comparableto others in frequency SUMMARY
of generalized violence. Two other results Analysis of violent incidents in nineteenth-
distinguish the findings for generalized vio- century Corsica reveals that two types of
lence from those for collaborative violence. group violence-collaborative use of lethal
First, vendettas were far more likely than force and generalization of violence to
other conflicts to include nondisputants ei- nondisputants-was systematically related to
ther as users or targets of violence, primarily prior patterns of contention and to the type
because the assailant in a vendetta was by of social connection between adversaries.

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
COLLECTIVE VIOLENCE AND GROUP SOLIDARITY 373

Specifically, if contention in the previolence collaborative violence between cousins.


stage was collective on one or both sides of When pairs of individuals within nuclear
a dispute, then it was considerably more families fought, the fact that nondisputants
likely that use of lethal force also would be were closely tied to both sides may have ef-
either collaborative or generalized to non- fectively disqualified them from inclusion,
disputants (or both). This relationship ob- even though this pattern did not hold for ex-
tained even after other relevant circum- tended families.
stances, such as the physical presence of po-
tential allies or a history of contention be-
tween disputants' families, were taken into Competing Interpretations
account. Evidence suggests that violence ex- One alternativeinterpretationis that the find-
tended to nondisputantssomewhat less often ings are artifacts of the data-collection pro-
when disputants were strangers or were tied cedure, principally because the data are fil-
by kinship, but the association of these fac- tered through the judicial process. Trial
tors with generalized violence is hard to dis- documents may fail to accurately reflect
tinguish from the impact of background what happened. Deception, manipulation,
group conflict, with which both are corre- and simple lack of evidence may have made
lated. This result implies that kin ties be- it difficult for prosecutors,juries, and judges
tween disputants may discourage collective to ascertain the details of violent incidents.
violence over the long term by preventing This is a significant issue in contexts in
disputes from solidifying into permanent which the formal justice system is perceived
hostility, but may have less influence on as external to indigenous norms, which was
short-termpatterns of escalation. the case for some Corsicans during the pe-
Several other considerations may shed riod studied. Occasionally, for instance, pros-
light on the unpredictedfinding that kin con- ecutors uncovered evidence of secret peace
nections between disputants did not, net of treaties in which a killer's family agreed to
other factors, reduce the odds of collabora- banish him and in return the victim's family
tive or generalized violence.23 First, kin con- agreed not to testify-though our knowledge
nections may have prevented disputes from of such agreements shows that they did not
becoming violent, even if they did not affect necessarily succeed (e.g., see Archives
the form violence took in those instances that Nationales, BB20 146/1, trials of Lorenzo
did escalate. Second, feuding families in Moreschi and Matteo Versini).
Corsica occasionally tried to make peace by The danger in using court documents to re-
contractinga marriage.Marriageas a method construct actual events is not that they are
of dispute resolution is institutionalized in not always accurate-measurement error is
many societies (and, of course, was a well- always present-but ratherthat they may be
known tool of foreign policy among Euro- systematically inaccurate in ways that bias
pean monarchies). As a result, kinship some- the relevant coefficient estimates. This
times paralleled underlying bases for con- would be the case, for example, if the com-
flict, making it harder to observe its damp- ponent of the error term resulting from un-
ening effect on group violence. Third, it measuredevidence distortion were correlated
seems likely that some differences might be with the variable measuring group conten-
observed if the analysis distinguished tion. There are no external validity checks to
nuclear family connections from second-de- establish whether this occurred, but an inter-
gree and more distant relationships. For ex- nal check is available. The interval between
ample, the data include no cases of two the date of the incident and the trial date re-
brothers attacking a third brother, although flects in part the difficulty police and pros-
there are cases of lethal violence between ecutors faced in gathering evidence and ap-
two brothers and cases of generalized and prehending suspects. Trials occurred later if
suspects evaded arrestand were usually post-
23 In one instance of intrafamily violence, a pair poned if a key witness failed to appear or if
of brothers attacked their sister's husband for try- the prosecutor uncovered evidence of private
ing to evict their father from his room in the mar- efforts to influence testimony. These kinds of
ried couple's home. behavior would also interfere with fact-gath-

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
374 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

ering, so factual distortion in trial summaries matic assumption that delineates what kinds
should be associated with this time interval. of arguments qualify as sociological, rather
To test whether such distortion was system- than a proposition subject to modification in
atically related to the variables of interest, I the face of empirical evidence. Individualist
compared the mean intervals in months accounts of behavior (i.e., explanations of
across the values of the two dependent vari- social phenomena as aggregations of autono-
ables and the key independent variable; I mous individual decisions) are seen either as
found no statistically significant differences. the preserve of other disciplines (usually
For collaborative violence, the average dif- psychology or economics) or as heretical if
ference was 2.1 months (12.8 versus 14.9; t proffered within sociology. All three of the
= .57); for generalized violence, the differ- theoretical descent groups in sociology-the
ence in intervals was 3.1 months (12.4 ver- functionalist, conflict, and institutional ap-
sus 15.5; t = .90); and for group contention, proaches-favor society-level or group-level
the difference was 2.4 months (12.0 versus accounts over individualist accounts, inas-
14.4; t = .45). Because there is no evidence much as they couch explanations in terms of
that the key measures are systematically re- value systems, class structures, organiza-
lated to the proxy for factual distortion, I in- tional practices and forms, and so forth.
fer that the interpretationof the results as ar- Against this disciplinary grain, recent stud-
tifacts is less persuasive than the substantive ies of collective action have concentrated on
interpretation. the social dilemma inherent in cooperative
A second competing interpretationis that social behavior-namely, the incentive for
the dependent and independent variables are each individual to let others make an effort
measuring the same thing, making the deter- from which all will benefit (Chong 1991;
rence account superfluous. If two people Hardin 1982; Macy 1991, 1993; Marwell,
have acted together prior to violence, one Oliver, and Prahl 1988; Oliver and Marwell
might reasonably predict their joint partici- 1993). Observing that people nonetheless act
pation in violence. But this interpretationcan collectively, even in situations where doing
explain only the finding for collaborative so incurs significant risks, sociologists have
violence; even if it were accepted, the fact painstakingly documented the processes by
that contention prior to escalation was also which groups manage to solve their free-
correlated with generalization to nondis- rider problems. Chief among these are two
putants would require a separate account. In mechanisms that generate solidarity and
fact, if the reason for the first result were that hence voluntary subordination of individual
persons engaged at one stage were also en- interests to group interests: social influence,
gaged at the next stage, group contention mediated by strong personal relationships,
would be negatively rather than positively through which people influence one another
associated with generalized violence-each to act cooperatively (Fernandez and
additional person involved in conflict prior McAdam 1988; Gould 1991, 1993; McAdam
to violence would mean one less nondis- 1986; McAdam and Paulsen 1993), and the
putant available for inclusion in violence. construction of interpretive frameworks, es-
Viewing collaborative violence and general- pecially those involving collective identities,
ized violence as demonstrations of group which enable people to see themselves as oc-
solidarity accounts for both observed rela- cupying similar social positions and sharing
tionships and for their persistence after con- a common fate (Ansell 1997; Calhoun 1995;
trolling for backgroundconditions that might Fantasia 1988; Gould 1995; McAdam 1982;
account for the effect of group contention. Melucci 1989; Taylor and Whittier 1992).24

DISCUSSION AND EXTENSIONS 24 Even those who have most vehemently criti-
cized the rationalist acr unt of collective action
The idea of the group as an entity indepen- have typically done so by outlining how people
dent of its individual members is founda- overcome the free-rider problem. In so doing,
tional to sociology. For many sociologists, they help answer a question that would not have
the priority of the social group relative to the been posed but for the "overly" rationalistic for-
individual is an article of faith-a paradig- mulation of the issue.

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
COLLECTIVE VIOLENCE AND GROUP SOLIDARITY 375

These views on collective action have been dividual survival, some people will select
most thoroughly developed and explored in inaction.
the area of social protest, but the problem Because everyone involved in a dispute
they address seems to be common to all knows all of this, the knowledge that an ad-
forms of group conflict. Any confrontation versary has potential allies, some of whom
between collections of people-whether the may be present, cannot always prevent esca-
boundary dividing them is one of class, lation. Expressions of mutual support by kin
ethnicity, neighborhood, clan, race, gender, are a way of raising the stakes: Making soli-
or religion-involves the possibility that darity explicit before violence occurs (in the
group action will not occur because behavior hope that this will avert violence) commits
is dominated by individualistic motives. Yet group members to act collectively if the
rarely do social scientists writing outside the conflict does escalate. Group action before
protest literature discuss the significance of escalation, in short, reduces the likelihood
this obstacle to group violence. The purpose of escalation at the price of intensified vio-
of this study has been to document the way lence for those disputes that do escalate.
the collective-action problem influences the And because collective contention high-
outcome of group conflict. I have tried to lights group-level reputations for solidar-
show that groups use demonstrationsof soli- ity-not just the aggregate of individual
darity to forestall escalation, but that such reputations for bravery-even nondisputants
demonstrations make violence worse when are implicated in such cases. Thus, family
escalation does occur-that is, when adver- members who have not engaged in conten-
saries are unconvinced. tion may nonetheless feel compelled to join
In contexts in which the state's role in in violence-or become targets because
dispute resolution is weak, people are more they are expected to join-if group action
likely to protect their interests with deadly has preceded escalation. They will not al-
force, either aggressively, defensively, or ways do so, but they do so more often when
preemptively. This pattern has been widely the solidarity of their kin group is explicitly
documented in traditional societies and in at stake, both in their own eyes and in the
marginalized urban communities in the con- eyes of others.
temporaryindustrialized world. In such situ- The Corsican data exhibit several of the
ations, group membership has enormous patternspredicted by this account. First, even
value in that it constitutes a mutual-defense in a society notable for its collectivist social
pact. It is surely more than twice as risky to organization, the transition from individual
attack two solidary individuals than a single to group conflict was hardly automatic. In
person, even for multiple assailants.25 Un- fact, one-on-one violence was the norm. Sec-
fortunately, the promise of mutual defense, ond, group action was more common in con-
even if expressed in the course of a dispute, flicts that did not escalate to lethal violence
is not ironclad and thus does not always (as measured by prosecutions for assault and
convince the other side to back down. The battery) than in those that did. Third, among
reason is that someone who joins in vio- those that did escalate, violence was much
lence may enhance a family's reputation for more likely to be collective when at least one
solidarity, but may not live to reap any of side in a dispute had acted as a group prior
the reward. Faced with the choice between to escalation, thereby making their cohesive-
action that benefits the group but bears ness a salient issue: Group contention prior
deadly risk, and inaction that fails to help to escalation was associated with higher rates
the group but affords a better chance for in- of collaborative violence and with higher
rates of violence involving nondisputants.
25 From this standpoint, vendettas are simply
Finally, both types of collective violence
an extension of the mutual-defense pact. The (collaboration and generalized violence) oc-
threat of vengeance tells potential assailants that
adversaries are not socially alone, even if they are
curred more frequently in conflicts in which
physically alone. In practice, however, the threat the adversaries had a history (and thus a
of retaliation is not entirely credible, despite ro- probable future) of group contention, a situ-
mantic images to the contrary:Most homicides in ation that enhanced the importance of group-
Corsica were not avenged. level reputations for solidarity.

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
376 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Although the evidence I present comes testers turnout to be willing to confront a vol-
from a single setting, the argumentis stated ley of bullets. Much of the time one side (usu-
in terms sufficiently abstractto invite empiri- ally the protesters)retreatsbecause the threat
cal verification in other domains. Indeed, the of escalation is persuasive, but violence
ways in which Corsica was special (and to sometimes occurs because someone gambles
some degree remains so) enhance, ratherthan that the other side's solidarity will falter. In a
undermine, the broader relevance of my ar- less contentious setting, leaders of firms or
gument about the communicative role of public agencies occasionally find that dis-
group contention. My analysis is a conserva- missal of a critic hurts their own legitimacy
tive test of the argumentbecause Corsica, like when, contraryto expectation, othermembers
other feuding societies, was a classical locus of the organizationresign in protest.
for group conflict. Thus, the finding that vio- As a counterpointto this dynamic, authori-
lence was only contingently collective carries tarian regimes never miss an opportunity to
special significance. If group action in high- publicize the betrayals they induce among
risk situations was problematic in a clan so- dissidents through bribes, threats, or other
ciety, then it must be at least as difficult for means corrosive of solidarity. Even when
groups in individualistic societies to credibly these betrayals are invented, the possibility
assert and act on their solidarity. that they are not weakens the mutual trust on
I propose that the mechanism operating in which dissident groups depend. The House
Corsican conflicts is a specific instance of a Un-American Activities Committee operated
fairly general process. There are numerous in this way in its efforts to undermine not
situations in which the social dilemma inher- only the public image, but also the internal
ent in collective action-especially risky col- cohesion, of American Communism.
lective action-weakens the shield of mutual The principal insight in this account is that
protection that groups purport to offer their it is the fragility of group solidarity, not its
members. Because solidarity-by which I strength, that leads to the intensification of
mean the willingness of individuals to act as conflict. People try to prevent escalation by
if the interests of fellow group members are asserting, implicitly or explicitly, that they
indistinguishable from their own-is observ- will act collectively if escalation occurs. The
able only in its results, people engaged in credibility of this assertion lies in the fact
conflict can never be certain that the solidar- that groups, like individuals, have to live up
ity their groups have exhibited can be sus- to their claims if these claims are to have any
tained at a more intense level of struggle. value in the future. But because some uncer-
Urban gangs, for instance, judge their rela- tainty always remains ex ante, escalation
tive strength not only in numbers but also on nonetheless takes place some of the time. At
the more elusive dimension of cohesiveness this point, group members either must act as
(Jankowski 1991; Shakur 1993; Short and they promised they would or face even more
Strodtbeck 1965; Suttles 1967). Thus, youth challenges in the future and more insecurity
gang members and professional gangsters about their ability to meet those challenges.
talk almost obsessively about the intense loy- Delivering on the promise of mutual support
alty they feel for one anotherand persistently may well be what people engaged in con-
apply the rhetoric of kinship to what are in flicts are talking about when they say they
fact voluntary relationships (Gambetta 1993; are defending their group's honor.
lanni 1972; Jankowski 1991; Maas 1968;
Salerno and Rivele 1990; Thompson 1967). Roger V. Gould is Associate Professor of Sociol-
It is likely that, if cohesion in such groups ogy at the University of Chicago, and Editor of
were as strong as members say, they would the American Journal of Sociology. He does re-
not need to talk about it quite so often. search on social networks and social conflict in
both historical and contemporarysettings. Recent
The same issue faces protesters and gov-
publications include two articles on the Whiskey
ernment forces during civil disturbances: Rebellion of 1794 and a book (Insurgent Identi-
Neither troops nor civilians know whetherci- ties, University of Chicago Press, 1995) on ur-
vilians will risk injury or death for one an- ban uprisings in France. He is currently complet-
other if the troops open fire, and neither side ing a book manuscript on violence, revenge, and
knows if troops will supporteach other if pro- group conflict.

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
COLLECTIVE VIOLENCE AND GROUP SOLIDARITY 377

Appendix A. Derivation of the Prediction that Disputes Ending in Low-Level Violence Are Charac-
terized by Higher Levels of Group Action Than Are Disputes Ending in Lethal Violence

The proposition that nonviolent group contention laboratory,m, as in nonviolent contention,the total
deters escalation to violence (in terms of Figure 1, numberof participantsin violence, a,, is given by
the hypothesis that Pi < P2) logically implies that a P n
there is less group action in lethal conflict than in = + 2
nonlethal contention. For simplicity, assume that r n
group action occurs only on one side of a dispute, +[P1 P(P3) + P2 1P4)] 2
whether for group contention prior to violence or Rearranginggives
collaboration in violence itself. (The argument is a = P [1 + p3 (m i)f-+ p2[1 + p4 (nl 1)]--
easily extended to cases in which group contention 2 2
can occur on both sides.) The phrase"numberof ac- Dividing by the total numberof disputes that es-
tors"thereforerefers to the numberon only one side calate to violence, given in expression A-2, yields
of each dispute. the following expression for the average numberof
Assume there are n disputes, half of which in- participantsper violent incident:
volve one-on-one contention and half of which in-
clude group contention (the assumption of equal P1[ + p3(m - )] + P2[1 + p4(n - 1)]
numberscan be relaxed without changing the out-
come). Assume furtherthat the expected numberof +P2
actorscollaboratingin an instance of group conten- As Pi approachesP2 from below, the upper bound
tion is m, where m is an integergreaterthanor equal for this expressionis
to 2. It follows that the numberof actorsparticipat-
ing in contention(call this numbera,) is given by P2[2 + (p3 + p4)(m - 1)]

a ,=(m+1)n (A-1) 2P2


2 which simplifies to
In other words, there is one actor participatingin
each of the n12 one-on-one disputes, and m actors (p3 + p4)m + [2 - (p3 + P4)] (A-3)
(on average)in each of the n12 disputesthatinvolve 2
groupcontention.
Now, by definition of Pi and P2. the numberof From equationA-1, the average numberof partici-
disputesthatescalate to violence is equal to pants in nonviolentcontentionis given by

(P1 +P2)2
as ml(A-4)
(A-2)
2' n 2
and, by definition of p3 and p4, the total numberof Because by definitionin is at least 2, the value of
disputesthatescalate and involve collaborativevio-expression A-3 is less than (m + 1)/2 as long as
lence is p3 + p4 is less than 1. Thus, as long as collaborative
violence is not more probableoverall than one-on-
(P1P3 + P2P4>- one violence, Pi < P2 implies thatthe numberof par-
2 ticipants per incident of nonlethal contention ex-
Assumingthateach incident involving collabora- ceeds the numberper incident of lethal contention.
tive violence has the same expected numberof col- Q.E.D.

REFERENCES
Abu-Lughod, Lila. 1986. Veiled Sentiments: Black, Donald. 1983. "Crime as Social Control."
Honor and Poetry in a Bedouin Society. Ber- American Sociological Review 48:34-45.
keley, CA: University of California Press. Black-Michaud, Jacob. 1975. Cohesive Force:
Albitreccia, Antoine. 1942. La Corse (Corsica). Feud in the Mediterranean and the Middle
Paris, France:Presses Universitaires de France. East. New York: St. Martin's Press.
Ansell, Christopher K. 1997. "Symbolic Net- Blok, Anton. 1974. The Mafia of a Sicilian Vil-
works: The Realignment of the French Work- lage 1860-1960: A Study of Violent Peasant
ing Class, 1887-1894." American Journal of Entrepreneurs. New York: Harper.
Sociology 103:359-90. . 1981. "Rams and Billy-Goats: A Key to
Baldassare, Mark, ed. 1995. The Los Angeles the MediterraneanCode of Honour." Man 16:
Riot. Boulder, CO: Westview. 427-40.
Bindi, Ange-Laurent. 1991. Inventorla Corse: di- Boehm, Christopher. 1987. Blood Revenge. The
mensions de l'autonomisme politique (Invent- Enactment and Management of Revenge in
ing Corsica: Dimensions of Political Montenegro and Other Tribal Societies. Phila-
Autonomism). Paris, France: L'Harmattan. delphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
378 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Bourdieu, Pierre. 1966. "The Sentiment of Honor tion de la justice criminelle en France pendant
in Kabyle Society." Pp. 191-242 in Honor and 1'anne'e1859 (General Report on the Adminis-
Shame: The Values of Mediterranean Society, tration of Criminal Justice in France in 1859).
edited by J. G. Peristiany. Chicago, IL: Univer- Paris, France: Imprimerie Nationale.
sity of Chicago Press. . 1861. Compte general de l'administra-
Bourgois, Philippe. 1995. In Search of Respect: tion de la justice criminelle en France pendant
Selling Crack in El Barrio. New York: Cam- l'anne'e 1860 (General Report on the Adminis-
bridge University Press. tration of Criminal Justice in France in 1860).
Brown, Keith M. 1986. Bloodfeud in Scotland, Paris, France: Imprimerie Nationale.
1573-1625. Edinburgh,Scotland: John Donald. . 1882. Compte general de 1'administra-
Busquet, Jacques. [1920] 1990. Le Droit de la tion de la justice criminelle en France pendant
vendetta et les paci corses (The Law of Ven- l'anne'e 1880 et rapport relatif aux annees
detta and Peace Treaties in Corsica). Paris, 1826 a 1880 (General Report on the Adminis-
France: Lafitte. tration of Criminal Justice in France in 1880
Calhoun, Craig. 1995. "'New Social Movements' and Retrospective Report for 1826-1880).
of the Early Nineteenth Century."Pp. 173-215 Paris, France: Imprimerie Nationale.
in Repertoires and Cycles of Collective Action, . 1883. Compte general de l'administra-
edited by M. Traugott. Durham, NC: Duke tion de la justice criminelle en France pendant
University Press. V'anne'e1881 (General Report on the Adminis-
Campbell, John King. 1964. Honor, Family, and tration of Criminal Justice in France in 1881).
Patronage: A Study of Institutions and Moral Paris, France: Imprimerie Nationale.
Values in a Greek Mountain Community.Ox- . 1884. Compte general de 1'administra-
ford, England: Clarendon Press. tion de la justice criminelle en France pendant
Chagnon, Napoleon. 1983. Yanomamo: The l'anne'e 1882 (General Report on the Adminis-
Fierce People. New York: Holt, Rinehart and tration of Criminal Justice in France in 1882).
Winston. Paris, France: Imprimerie Nationale.
Chong, Dennis. 1991. Collective Action and the . 1886. Compte general de l'administra-
Civil Rights Movement. Princeton, NJ: tion de la justice criminelle en France pendant
Princeton University Press. l'anne'e 1885 (General Report on the Adminis-
Coser, Lewis. 1956. The Functions of Social Con- tration of Criminal Justice in France in 1885).
flict. New York: Free Press. Paris, France: Imprimerie Nationale.
Culioli, Gabriel Xavier. 1990. Le Complexe corse Gambetta,Diego. 1988. "Mafia:The Price of Dis-
(The Corsican Complex). Paris, France: trust." Pp. 158-75 in Trust: The Making and
Gallimard. Breaking of Cooperative Relations, edited by
Daly, Martin and Margo Wilson. 1988. Homicide. D. Gambetta. New York: Basil Blackwell.
New York: Aldine de Gruyter. . 1993. The Sicilian Mafia: The Business
Elster, Jon. 1989. The Cement of Society. Cam- of Private Protection. Cambridge, MA:
bridge, England: Cambridge University Press. HarvardUniversity Press.
Evans-Pritchard, E. E. 1940. The Nuer. Oxford, Gibbons, Robert. 1992. Game Theoryfor Applied
England: Oxford University Press. Economists. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univer-
Fantasia, Rick. 1988. Cultures of Solidarity. Ber- sity Press.
keley, CA: University of California Press. Gilmore, David D. 1987. "Honor, Honesty,
Farrell, Joseph and Matthew Rabin. 1996. "Cheap Shame: Male Status in Contemporary
Talk." Journal of Economic Perspectives Andalusia." Pp. 90-103 in Honor and Shame
10:103-18. and the Unity of the Mediterranean, edited by
Fernandez, Roberto M. and Doug McAdam. D. D. Gilmore. Washington, DC: American
1988. "Social Networks and Social Move- Anthropological Association.
ments: Multiorganizational Fields and Recruit- Given, James B. 1977. Society and Homicide in
ment to Freedom Summer." Sociological Fo- Thirteenth-Century England. Stanford, CA:
rum 3:441-60. Stanford University Press.
Fortes, Meyer. 1945. The Dynamics of Clanship Gluckman, Max. 1955. Custom and Conflict in
among the Tallensi. London, England: Oxford Africa. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
University Press. Gould, Roger V. 1991. "Multiple Networks and
France, Ministere de la Justice. 1859. Compte Mobilization in the Paris Commune, 1871."
general de administration de la justice American Sociological Review 56:716-29.
criminelle en France pendant l'annee 1858 . 1993. "TradeCohesion, Class Unity, and
(General Report on the Administration of Urban Insurrection: Artisanal Activism in the
Criminal Justice in France in 1858). Paris, Paris Commune." American Journal of Sociol-
France: ImprimerieNationale. ogy 98:721-54.
. 1860. Compte general de l'administra- . 1995. Insurgent Identities: Class, Com-

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
COLLECTIVE VIOLENCE AND GROUP SOLIDARITY 379

munity, and Protest in Paris from 1848 to the . 1986. "Recruitmentto High-Risk Activ-
Commune.Chicago, IL: University of Chicago ism: The Case of Freedom Summer." Ameri-
Press. can Journal of Sociology 92:64-90.
Hardin, Russell. 1982. Collective Action. Balti- McAdam, Doug and Ronnelle Paulsen. 1993.
more, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. "Specifying the Relationship between Social
1995. One for All: The Logic of Group Ties and Activism." American Journal of Soci-
Conflict. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University ology 99:640-67.
Press. Melucci, Alberto. 1989. Nomads of the Present.
Hasluck, Margaret. 1954. The Unwritten Law in London, England: Hutchinson Radius.
Albania. Cambridge,England: CambridgeUni- Miller, William Ian. 1990. Bloodtaking and
versity Press. Peacemaking: Feud, Law, and Society in Saga
Hechter, Michael. 1975. Internal Colonialism. Iceland. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago
Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Press.
Hogg, Michael and Dominic Abrams. 1988. So- Oberschall, Anthony. 1978. "Theories of Social
cial Identifications: A Social Psychology of In- Conflict." Annual Review of Sociology 4:291-
tergroup Relations and Group Processes. Lon- 315.
don, England: Routledge. Oliver, Pamela E. and Gerald Marwell. 1993. The
Horowitz, Donald L. 1985. Ethnic Groups in Free-Rider Problem in Collective Action. New
Conflict. Berkeley, CA: University of Califor- York: Cambridge University Press.
nia Press. Olzak, Susan. 1983. "ContemporaryEthnic Mo-
Ianni, Francis A. J. 1972. A Family Business: bilization." Annual Review of Sociology 9:355-
Kinship and Social Control in Organized 74.
Crime. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. 1992. The Dynamics of Ethnic Competi-
Jankowski, Martin Sanchez. 1991. Islands in the tion and Conflict. Stanford: Stanford Univer-
Street: Gangs and American Urban Society. sity Press.
Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Otterbein, Keith F. 1968. "InternalWar: A Cross-
Kaplan, Robert D. 1993. Balkan Ghosts: A Jour- Cultural Comparison." American Anthropolo-
ney through History. New York: St. Martin's gist 70:277-89.
Press. Otterbein, Keith F. and Charlotte Swanson
Laitin, David and James D. Fearon. 1996. "Ex- Otterbein. 1965. "An Eye for an Eye, a Tooth
plaining Interethnic Cooperation." American for a Tooth: A Cross-Cultural Study of Feud-
Political Science Review 90:715-35. ing." American Anthropologist 67:1470-82.
Lenclud, Gerard. 1979. "Des feux introuvables: Peristiany, Jean G., ed. 1966. Honour and Shame:
l'organisation familiale dans un village de la The Values of Mediterranean Society. Chicago,
Corse traditionnelle"(Missing Hearths:Family IL: University of Chicago Press.
Organization in a Traditional Corsican Vil- Peristiany, Jean G. and Julian Pitt-Rivers, eds.
lage). Etudes rurales 76:7-50. 1992. Honour and Grace in Anthropology.
LeVine, Robert A. and Donald T. Campbell. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
1972. Ethnocentrism: Theories of Conflict, Press.
Ethnic Attitudes, and Group Behavior. New Peters, Emrys L. 1967. "Some StructuralAspects
York: Wiley. of the Feud among the Camel-HerdingBedouin
Maas, Peter. 1968. The Valachi Papers. New of Cyrenaica."Africa 37:261-82.
York: Putnam. Pitt-Rivers, Julian. 1966. "Honor and Social Sta-
Macy, Michael W. 1991. "Chains of Cooperation: tus." Pp. 19-78 in Honor and Shame: The Val-
Threshold Effects in Collective Action." ues of Mediterranean Society, edited by J. G.
American Sociological Review 56:730-47. Peristiany. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago
. 1993. "Backward-Looking Social Con- Press.
trol." American Sociological Review 58:819- Pomponi, Francis. 1979. Histoire de la Corse
36. (History of Corsica). Paris, France: Hachette.
Marcaggi, Jean-Baptiste. [1932] 1966. Les Ban- Reuter, Peter. 1983. Disorganized Crime. Cam-
dits horses: histoire de la vendetta (The Ban- bridge, MA: MIT Press.
dits of Corsica: History of a Vendetta). Paris, Ross, Marc H. 1986. "The Limits to Social Struc-
France: Desroches. ture: Social Structural and Psychocultural Ex-
Marwell, Gerald, Pamela E. Oliver, and Ralph planations for Political Conflict and Violence."
Prahl. 1988. "Social Networks and Collective Anthropological Quarterly 59:171-76.
Action: A Theory of the Critical Mass. Ill." Salerno, Joseph and Stephen Rivele. 1990. The
American Journal of Sociology 94:502-534. Plumber: The True Story of How One Good
McAdam, Doug. 1982. Political Process and the Man Helped Destroy the Entire Philadelphia
Development of Black Insurgency 1930-1970. Mafia. New York: Knightsbridge.
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Schneider, Jane. 1971. "Of Vigilance and Vir-

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
380 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

gins: Honor, Shame, and Access to Resources Theory, edited by A. Morris and C. Mueller.
in Mediterranean Societies." Ethnology 10:1- New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
24. Thompson, Hunter S. 1967. Hell's Angels: A
Shakur, Sanyika. 1993. Monster. New York: Pen- Strange and Terrible Saga. New York:
guin Books. Ballantine.
Sherif, Muzafer. 1966. Group Conflict and Co- Turner,John and Howard Giles. 1981. Intergroup
operation. New York: St. Martin's Press. Behavior. Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell.
Short, James F. and Fred L. Strodbeck. 1965. Unsal, Artun. 1990. Tuer pour survivre: La Ven-
Group Process and Gang Delinquency. Chi- detta (Killing to Survive: The Vendetta). Paris,
cago, IL: University of Chicago Press. France: Editions L'Harmattan.
Simmel, Georg. [1923] 1955. Conflict and the U.S. Bureau of the Census. 1996. Statistical Ab-
Web of Group Affiliations. Glencoe, IL: Free stract of the United States. U.S. Bureau of the
Press. Census, Washington, DC.
Suttles, Gerald R. 1967. The Social Order of the Versini, Xavier. 1964. Un Siecle de banditisme
Slum. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago en Corse (1814-1914) (A Century of Banditry
Press. in Corsica [1814-1914]). Paris, France:
Tajfel, Henri. 1970. "Experiments in Intergroup Albatros.
Discrimination." Scientific American, vol. 223, Wikan, Unni. 1984. "Shame and Honor: A Con-
no. 5, November, pp. 96-102. testable Pair." Man 19:635-52.
. 1974. "Social Identity and IntergroupBe- Wilson, Stephen K. 1988. Feuding, Conflict, and
havior." Social Science Information 13:65-93. Banditry in Nineteenth-Century Corsica. Cam-
Taylor, Verta and Nancy E. Whittier. 1992. "Col- bridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
lective Identity in Social Movement Commu- Wyatt-Brown, Bertram. 1982. Southern Honor:
nities: Lesbian Feminist Mobilization." Pp. Ethics and Behavior in the Old South. New
104-29 in Frontiers of Social Movement York: Oxford University Press.

This content downloaded from 74.93.143.69 on Fri, 14 Nov 2014 12:03:40 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like