A Network of Dark Personality Traits

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Research in Personality 73 (2018) 56–62

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Research in Personality


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jrp

Full Length Article

A network of dark personality traits: What lies at the heart of


darkness? q

David K. Marcus a,⇑, Jonathan Preszler a, Virgil Zeigler-Hill b


a
Washington State University, United States
b
Oakland University, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The question of whether there is a common element at the core of the various dark personality traits (e.g.,
Received 14 August 2017 psychopathy, narcissism, Machiavellianism, spitefulness, aggressiveness) has been the subject of debate.
Revised 31 October 2017 Callousness, manipulativeness, and disagreeableness have all been nominated as possibly serving as the
Accepted 7 November 2017
core of these dark traits. Network analysis, which graphically and quantitatively describes the centrality
Available online 10 November 2017
of various related traits, provides a novel technique for examining this issue. We estimated an association
network and an Adaptive Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator network for two large sam-
Keywords:
ples, one college student sample (N = 2831) and one mixed college student and Mechanical Turk sample
Network analysis
Psychopathy
(N = 844). Interpersonal manipulation and callousness were the traits that were central to the networks.
Narcissism Ó 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Machiavellianism
Spitefulness
Dark Triad

1. Introduction Network analysis may provide a novel methodology for examining


this issue.
Paulhus and Williams (2002) coined the term ‘‘Dark Triad” to A meta-analysis of studies that assessed all three Dark Triad
encompass the personality traits of Machiavellianism, narcissism, traits found a large correlation between Machiavellianism and psy-
and psychopathy, because these three traits are socially aversive chopathy and medium-sized correlations between psychopathy
and had received prominent empirical attention. Since the term and narcissism and between Machiavellianism and narcissism
was coined, empirical interest has increased considerably with a (Muris et al., 2017). These correlations suggest that the shared vari-
PsycINFO search of the term ‘‘Dark Triad” yielding over 330 cita- ance among these traits may represent a ‘‘core of darkness” (Jones
tions with most of these articles having been published since & Figueredo, 2013, p. 521). Various traits and characteristics have
2014 (see Muris, Merckelbach, Otgaar, & Meijer, 2017 for a meta- been proposed as the core of the Dark Triad including low
analysis, and Furnham, Richards, & Paulhus, 2013, for a narrative honesty-humility from the HEXACO model of personality (e.g.,
review). The aim of the original Paulhus and Williams (2002) study Book, Visser, & Volk, 2015), low agreeableness (Jakobwitz & Egan,
was to demonstrate that although these three traits are inter- 2006), an exploitive mating strategy (at least in men; Jonason, Li,
correlated, each has distinct characteristics. Although there is con- Webster, & Schmitt, 2009), callousness (Jones & Paulhus, 2011),
sensus that each of these dark traits has unique qualities, the ques- and callousness combined with manipulativeness (Jones &
tion of what is at the core of the Dark Triad remains unresolved. Figueredo, 2013). A variety of statistical methods have been used
in an attempt to identify the core of the Dark Triad including
canonical correlation analysis (Book et al., 2015), various types of
q
The study data are available as an online supplement. This study was not factor or principal components analysis (Jakobwitz & Egan, 2006),
preregistered at an independent institutional registry. All three authors contributed structural equation modeling (Jones & Figueredo, 2013), and con-
to the study conceptualization and report writing. Marcus and Zeigler-Hill were firmatory factor analysis followed by a mediational analysis
responsible for the data collection and preparation. Preszler conducted the data (Jonason et al., 2009). Because network analysis visually depicts
analysis.
⇑ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, Washington State Univer- the associations among traits and provides measures of centrality,
sity, Pullman, WA 99164-4820, United States. it may provide unique insight into the core of the Dark Triad (see
E-mail address: david.marcus@wsu.edu (D.K. Marcus). Costantini et al., 2015, for a review).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2017.11.003
0092-6566/Ó 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
D.K. Marcus et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 73 (2018) 56–62 57

In network analysis, traits can be represented as nodes and the Sample 2. Data for this sample were originally collected for a
associations among pairs of traits are depicted as edges connecting study of spitefulness (Marcus, Zeigler-Hill, Mercer, & Norris,
the nodes, with thicker edges depicting stronger associations. Thus, 2014). This sample included 297 participants who were recruited
a network figure provides a visual representation of a web of through Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) and 547 undergraduate
related traits and shows which traits are the most central. Three students from a public university in the Pacific Northwestern
metrics are commonly used in personality and psychopathology region of the United States. There were originally 853 participants
research to provide quantitative descriptions of the centrality of in this combined sample, but 9 of these participants were missing
the nodes (McNally, 2016). The strength of a node is the sum of one or measures and were exclude from the analyses, leaving 844
the correlations between the node and the other nodes in the net- total participants. The MTurk participants received $1.00 in
work. For most purposes, strength is the most relevant of the three exchange for their participation and the college students received
centrality metrics. Closeness is the inverse of the distance between research credits. The mean age of the MTurk participants was
the node and the other nodes in the network. Betweenness is the 36.4 (SD = 13.1) and 62.3% were women. The racial/ethnic identifi-
number of times that a node is the shortest path between two cation of the MTurk sample was 75.4% Caucasian, 9.8% Black, 6.1%
other nodes. Because there will be increased distance between Hispanic, and 4.7% Asian. The mean age of the student participants
many nodes of the network if a node with high betweenness is was 19.7 years (SD = 2.8) and 79.1% were women. Regarding racial/
removed, betweenness is a measure of a node’s connective value ethnic identity, 71.9% identified as Caucasian, 9.0% as Hispanic,
to a network. Thus, if there is a trait in a network of dark person- 9.0% as Asian, and 4.7%, as Black.
ality traits that is high in strength, closeness, and betweenness, it
is a likely candidate to be at the core. When there were negative 2.2. Materials and procedures
edges (i.e., negative associations) between nodes, we also calcu-
lated a fourth centrality metric, expected influence (Robinaugh, The participants provided informed consent and completed all
Millner, & McNally, 2016). Expected influence is similar to of the study measures online. The measures were administered
strength, but takes into account negative relationships (whereas in random order.
strength is calculated using the absolute value of the edges). There Self-Report Psychopathy Scale (SRP-III; Paulhus, Neumann, &
has been a growing interest in using network analysis in psy- Hare, in press). The SRP-III is a self-report measure of psychopathy
chopathology research to map the relations among sets of psychi- that was modeled on the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R;
atric symptoms (e.g., Borsboom & Cramer, 2013). Hare, 2003). Participants indicated their agreement with each of
There have only been a few applications of network analysis to the 34 statements on scales ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to
personality traits. However, these applications have shown pro- 5 (strongly agree). This version of the SRP-III consists of four sub-
mise in facilitating a novel understanding of personality. For exam- scales: Callousness (8 items; e.g., ‘‘I’m not afraid to step on others
ple, a network of conscientiousness across two samples revealed to get what I want” [aSample 1 = .77; aSample 2 = .80]), Erratic Lifestyle
that the industriousness and promotion focus nodes had the high- (8 items; e.g., ‘‘I enjoy taking risks” [aSample 1 = .77; aSample 2 = .78]),
est betweenness in the networks of both samples, indicating that Interpersonal Manipulation (8 items; e.g., ‘‘I find it easy to manip-
they may be particularly important factors in connecting the differ- ulate people” [aSample 1 = .71; aSample 2 = .74]), and Criminal Tenden-
ent aspects of conscientiousness (Costantini & Perugini, 2016). cies (10 items; e.g., ‘‘Stole money from my parents” [aSample 1 = .82;
Although much of the research on dark personality traits has aSample 2 = .80]).
focused on the Dark Triad, we (Marcus & Zeigler-Hill, 2015; Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Hall, 1981). The
Zeigler-Hill & Marcus, 2016) have advocated for a broader frame- NPI is well-validated and is considered to be the standard measure
work that includes additional antagonistic or dysfunctional traits. of subclinical narcissistic personality features (Paulhus & Williams,
For the current study, we conducted two network analyses exam- 2002). Items on the NPI are in a forced-choice format such that par-
ining the associations among a set of antagonistic dark personality ticipants must choose between a narcissistic and a non-narcissistic
traits (psychopathy, narcissism, Machiavellianism, spitefulness, statement (e.g., ‘‘I like having authority over other people” or ‘‘I
and, in Sample 2, aggressiveness). For psychopathy and narcissism, don’t mind following orders”). Although there has been contro-
which are each multifaceted, we included each facet as a separate versy regarding the underlying factor structure of the 40-item
node. NPI, Ackerman et al. (2010) suggested that the NPI consists of three
factors: Leadership/Authority (11 items; e.g., ‘‘If I ruled the world it
2. Method would be a much better place” [aSample 1 = .77; aSample 2 = .65]),
Grandiose Exhibitionism (10 items; e.g., ‘‘I really like to be the cen-
2.1. Participants ter of attention” [aSample 1 = .75; aSample 2 = .77]), and Exploitative-
ness/Entitlement (4 items; e.g., ‘‘I find it easy to manipulate
Network analysis of psychological data is a relatively new ana- people” [aSample 1 = .45; aSample 2 = .50]).
lytic method that has not yet established standards for sample size Mach-IV (Christie & Geis, 1970). The Mach-IV is a 20-item
or power analysis (Epskamp, Kruis, & Marsman, 2017). However, instrument that was developed to assess Machiavellianism (e.g.,
simulations conducted by Epskamp et al. (2017) indicate that with ‘‘The best way to handle people is to tell them what they want to
10 nodes, LASSO techniques accurately detected sparse networks hear”). Participants rated their level of agreement with the items
with about 500 participants. Based on these initial findings, both using scales that ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly
of our samples were sufficiently large. agree). The Mach-IV has been found to possess adequate psycho-
Sample 1. This sample included 2831 undergraduate students metric properties and is the most widely used measure of Machi-
(663 men, 2165 women, and 3 who did not disclose their gender) avellianism (McHoskey, Worzel, & Szyarto, 1998). The internal
from a public university in the Midwestern region of the United consistency for the Mach-IV was aSample 1 = .73 and aSample 2 = .67.
States. The original sample included 2971 participants, but 140 Spitefulness Scale (Marcus et al., 2014). The Spitefulness Scale is
participants were excluded because they were missing data for a 17-item instrument designed to measure the willingness of a
one or more of the instruments. The mean age of the participants participant to engage in behaviors that would harm another but
was 20.11 years (SD = 3.6). Regarding racial/ethnic identity, 76.6% that would also entail potential harm to oneself. This harm could
identified as Caucasian, 8.7% as Black, 5.3% as Asian, 2.9%, as His- be social, financial, physical, or an inconvenience (e.g., ‘‘I would
panic, and 6.5% other. be willing to take a punch if it meant that someone I did not like
58 D.K. Marcus et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 73 (2018) 56–62

would receive two punches”). Participants rated their level of the small-world property (Humphries & Gurney, 2008). We used
agreement with the items of the Spitefulness Scale using scales the R package qgraph (Epskamp, Cramer, Waldorp, Schmittmann,
that ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The & Borsboom, 2012) to perform the network analytics.
internal consistency for the Spitefulness Scale was aSample 1 = .91
and aSample 2 = .90.
Forms and Functions of Aggression Scale (AS; Little, Henrich, 3. Results
Jones, & Hawley, 2003). This measure is based on the idea that
aggressive behavior can be understood as the combination of the Descriptive statistics for the variables measured for both sam-
function of aggressive behavior (i.e., pure, reactive, or instrumen- ples are provided in Table 1. The edge weights (partial correlations
tal) and the behavioral form of aggression (i.e., overt or relational). among the variables) are provided in Table 2. The raw bivariate
Participants rated their level of agreement with each of the 36 correlations among the variables are provided in supplementary
items using scales ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (completely true). Table S1. Across the two samples, the correlation of the edge
The AS was only administered to Sample 2. For the current analy- weights for the adaptive LASSO networks was .86 and the correla-
sis, we used the total score on the scale (a = .96). tion of the raw bivariate correlations for the association networks
was .93, indicating that the results from the two samples were gen-
2.3. Analytic strategy erally consistent.

We estimated an association network and an Adaptive Least


Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator (LASSO) network for 3.1. Network structures
each sample. Association networks are graphical representations
of zero-order correlations of data and usually result in fully con- 3.1.1. Sample 1 adaptive LASSO network
nected networks. Because data sets typically include correlations The adaptive LASSO network for Sample 1 is displayed in Fig. 1.
that are spurious or not theoretically meaningful, estimating net- Sample 1 included 9 traits in the network, making the total possi-
works using partial correlations (i.e., the association that is left ble number of edges 36 (9  8  0.5). Of these 36 possible edges,
between any two variables after conditioning on all other vari- 25 (69%) were present in the adaptive LASSO estimation. The four
ables) is useful for understanding the genuine and meaningful rela- psychopathic traits connected to make a strong loop, but with no
tionships among the variables. Estimation using adaptive LASSO association between ‘‘Callousness” and ‘‘Erratic Lifestyle,” and only
induces a sparse network from the data, shrinking small correla- a small positive association between ‘‘Interpersonal Manipulation”
tions to zero by using a LASSO penalty during estimation and ‘‘Criminal Tendencies.” The ‘‘Spitefulness” and ‘‘Machiavellia
(Costantini et al., 2015). Consequently, the edges are more likely nism” nodes clustered strongly with each other and the psycho-
to reflect direct relationships. Quantitative indices, such as central- pathic nodes. Regarding narcissism, whereas ‘‘Grandiose Exhibi-
ity measures, can then be used to interpret sparse networks fur- tionism” and ‘‘Leadership/Authority” were strongly connected
ther. Therefore, we provide adaptive LASSO networks in the with each other, ‘‘Exploitativeness/Entitlement” was only moder-
primary results (the results from the association networks of the ately connected with any other trait, including the other two nar-
raw correlations are provided in an online supplement). We calcu- cissism traits. In addition, the narcissism nodes were only
lated four centrality measures (expected influence, strength, close- moderately connected with other nodes in the network, and occu-
ness, and betweenness) and one clustering coefficient for each pied peripheral positions on the graph. The narcissism traits also
adaptive LASSO network to identify nodes that are important to included the strongest negative connection on the graph, that
the network structure. All of these centrality measures were calcu- between ‘‘Leadership/Authority” and ‘‘Machiavellianism.” The
lated using the partial correlations. Thus, for example, the strength small-worldness value of the Sample 1 network was 1.03, indicat-
index for a node was the sum of the partial correlations between ing it does not have the small-world property.
the node and the other nodes in the network. Similarly, the close-
ness values are a measure of the inverse sum of the distances of a
given node to all other nodes in the partial correlation network 3.1.2. Sample 1 centrality
(even nodes with which the given node only has an indirect Centrality measures index the role and relationships a given
connection). node has in a network. Table 3 presents three standardized central-
Clustering coefficients measure ‘‘the number of connections ity measures (i.e., strength, expected influence, and closeness) as
among the neighbors of a focal node over the maximum possible well as the raw scores for betweenness. ‘‘Interpersonal Manipula-
number of such connections” (Costantini et al., 2015, p. 18). The tion” and ‘‘Callousness” had the highest centrality measures across
clustering coefficient is high when the nodes that a node is con- all four indices. The other two psychopathic traits (‘‘Erratic Life-
nected to are also strongly connected to each other. A high cluster- style” and ‘‘Criminal Tendencies”) were average, except for ‘‘Crim-
ing coefficient often indicates redundancy (i.e., if the node were inal Tendencies” having moderate expected influence and ‘‘Erratic
removed, the general network structure would remain). If a node Lifestyle” having low betweenness. In contrast, the narcissism
has elevated centrality measures, then an elevated clustering coef- nodes were relatively non-central. Specifically, ‘‘Grandiose Exhibi-
ficient might indicate that the centrality is artificially inflated. To tionism” and ‘‘Exploitativeness/Entitlement” had the lowest cen-
assess node clustering, we used the Zhang clustering coefficient trality scores across all four indices, and ‘‘Leadership/Authority”
(Zhang & Horvath, 2005) because of its applicability to correla- had above average betweenness (4 connections) but below average
tional data. We used the ‘‘signed” version of the Zhang coefficient, strength and closeness. ‘‘Machiavellianism” had just below average
which takes into account negative edge weights. We also calcu- centrality values except for expected influence, which was lower
lated the small-worldness value for each adaptive LASSO network. than the others, likely because of its negative connection with ‘‘Lea
Small-worldness is a measure of the tendency for any two nodes dership/Authority.” Lastly, ‘‘Spitefulness” had approximately aver-
within a network to be easily connected through other nodes. A age values across all centrality indices. Spitefulness did, however,
small-world network has high transitivity, indicating that each yield the highest Zhang clustering coefficient values in both this
node quickly affects many of the nodes in the network (i.e., change sample and Sample 2, suggesting that this scale may be redundant
to a single node would quickly result in change to the whole net- and capturing information that is already measured by the other
work). A small-worldness value above three is considered to have scales.
D.K. Marcus et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 73 (2018) 56–62 59

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics.

Symptom Sample 1 (N = 2831) Sample 2 (N = 845)


Skewness Kurtosis Mean (SD) Skewness Kurtosis Mean (SD)
Psychopathy
Interpersonal manipulation 0.47 0.01 2.15 (0.65) 0.32 0.06 2.29 (0.69)
Callousness 0.95 0.38 1.75 (0.59) 0.57 0.53 1.91 (0.62)
Criminal tendencies 1.30 1.33 1.64 (0.64) 0.71 0.12 1.90 (0.70)
Erratic lifestyle 0.25 0.54 2.44 (0.74) 0.01 0.48 2.61 (0.75)
Narcissism
Exploitativeness/entitlement 1.06 0.55 0.84 (0.97) 1.11 0.50 0.88 (1.04)
Grandiose exhibitionism 0.57 0.46 3.25 (2.47) 0.52 0.63 3.26 (2.59)
Leadership/authority 0.12 0.92 5.26 (2.92) 0.00 .096 5.39 (2.53)
Machiavellianism, spitefulness, & aggression
Machiavellianism 0.27 0.22 2.62 (0.42) 0.49 2.47 2.81 (0.38)
Spitefulness 0.84 0.12 1.82 (0.67) 0.55 0.00 1.94 (0.61)
Aggression – – – 1.78 3.09 1.39 (0.42)

Note. Aggression items were not used in Sample 1.

Table 2
Edge weights (partial correlations) of each network.

M L/A GE E/E C EL IM CT S
Machiavellianism (M) .09 – .08 .12 .01 .15 – .28
Leadership/authority (L/A) .09 .36 .12 – .05 .12 – .01
Grandiose exhibitionism (GE) .05 .45 .13 – .08 – – –
Exploitativeness/entitlement (E/E) – .04 .12 .12 .02 .09 – .04
Callousness (C) .01 – – .08 – .28 .32 .22
Erratic lifestyle (EL) .13 .07 .12 – – .25 .35 –
Interpersonal manipulation (IM) .15 .20 – .11 .30 .24 .05 .08
Criminal tendencies (CT) – – – – .23 .36 – .19
Spitefulness (S) .08 .01 – – .35 – .15 .11
Aggression .06 .05 – .20 .23 .07 – .17 .16

Note. Values above the diagonal are for Sample 1 (N = 2831) and those below the diagonal are for Sample 2 (N = 845). ‘‘–” indicates the LASSO estimation shrunk the
correlation to ‘‘0”. There is no column for ‘‘Aggression” because Aggression was not measured in Sample 1.

3.1.3. Sample 2 adaptive LASSO network 3.1.4. Sample 2 centrality


The adaptive LASSO network for Sample 2 is displayed in Fig. 1. The centrality measures for Sample 2 are displayed in Table 4.
Sample 2 included 10 traits in the network, making the total pos- Consistent with their central locations on the graphical display,
sible number of edges 45 (10  9  0.5). Of these 45 possible edges, ‘‘Interpersonal Manipulation” and ‘‘Callousness” had the highest
29 (64.4%) were estimated by the adaptive LASSO procedure. The centrality index scores across all four indices. No other nodes were
psychopathic traits made a loop with each other (i.e., ‘‘Criminal noticeably above average on strength or expected influence. ‘‘Ma
Tendencies” – ‘‘Callousness” – ‘‘Interpersonal Manipulation” – chiavellianism,” ‘‘Grandiose Exhibitionism,” and ‘‘Exploitative
‘‘Erratic Lifestyle” – ‘‘Criminal Tendencies”), with no connections ness/Entitlement” had well below average centrality scores across
between ‘‘Callousness” and ‘‘Erratic Lifestyle” or ‘‘Criminal Tenden- the indices, congruent with their peripheral position on the graph.
cies” and ‘‘Interpersonal Manipulation” (the apparent negative Both the ‘‘Erratic Lifestyle” and ‘‘Criminal Tendencies” nodes hov-
connection is actually between ‘‘Erratic Lifestyle” and ‘‘Aggres- ered around average values across all four indices.
sion”). Of these psychopathic traits, ‘‘Interpersonal Manipulation”
and ‘‘Callousness” seem to occupy a central role within the graph, 3.2. Supplementary analyses
where they both have numerous moderate-to-strong connections
with other nodes. Specifically, ‘‘Callousness” is connected strongly 3.2.1. Association networks
with ‘‘Spitefulness” and ‘‘Aggression,” whereas ‘‘Interpersonal Consistent with the assertion that ‘‘the correlations between
Manipulation” is connected with ‘‘Machiavellianism” and ‘‘Leader pairs of symptoms that remain when all other symptoms are con-
ship/Authority.” This formation almost mimicked the loop found trolled for. . .may be considered to provide clues about the causal
in Sample 1, but without the weak connection between ‘‘Interper- skeleton of a network” (Borsboom & Cramer, 2013, p. 105), our pri-
sonal Manipulation” and ‘‘Criminal Tendencies.” Of note is that the mary results focused on the LASSO networks of partial correlations.
narcissistic traits are on the periphery of the graph, with a strong However, it may be worthwhile to compare these findings to the
connection between ‘‘Grandiose Exhibitionism” and ‘‘Leadership/ results from association networks based on raw bivariate correla-
Authority,” but with few strong connections besides these two tions. These networks are displayed in Supplementary Fig. S1 and
nodes and the rest of the network. Three relatively weak negative the centrality measures for these association networks are pro-
connections were present: between ‘‘Machiavellianism” and ‘‘Lea vided in Supplementary Tables S2 and S3. Comparisons of Table 3
dership/Authority,” between ‘‘Leadership/Authority” and ‘‘Aggres- with Table S2 and Table 4 with Table S3 indicate that the few dif-
sion,” and between ‘‘Erratic Lifestyle” and Aggression.” The small- ferences in strength and closeness between the LASSO and associ-
worldness value of the Sample 2 network was 0.99, indicating it ation networks in either sample were in magnitude and not in the
does not have the small-world property. ordering of the traits. Interpersonal Manipulation and Callousness
60 D.K. Marcus et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 73 (2018) 56–62

Machiavellianism

Grandiose
Exhibitionism

Leadership/
Authority

Grandiose
Exhibitionism

Erratic

Exploitativeness/
Entitlement
Leadership/
Authority

Machiavellianism
Interpersonal

Criminal

Interpersonal

Callous
Spite Callous
Erratic

Exploitativeness/
Entitlement

Spite

Criminal

Machiavellianism

Leadership/
Authority

Grandiose
Exhibitionism

Erratic

Interpersonal

Criminal

Exploitativeness/
Entitlement

Callous

Aggression

Spite

Fig. 1. Adaptive LASSO network graphs. Top left graph = Sample 1 (N = 2831). Top right Graph = Sample 2 (N = 845) without aggression included. Bottom graph = Sample 2
with aggression included. Blue = Psychopathic traits; Gray = Narcissistic traits; Green = Spite; Red = Machiavellianism; Yellow = Aggression. The nodes represent personality
traits and the edges represent the adaptive LASSO partial correlations between the items. Thicker edges represent stronger associations, with green edges representing
positive associations and red edges representing negative associations. The Fruchterman-Reingold (specified as ‘‘spring” in the qgraph package) layout is used to display the
results. (For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

remained most central on both of these metrics and they both in these networks, suggesting that when raw correlations are
appear to be central to the networks in Fig. S1. Because association examined, Spitefulness may provide some unique information.
networks involve many more direct connections between nodes,
there was less betweenness (i.e., fewer nodes could serve as the 3.2.2. Dark Triad
shortest path between pairs of nodes) and Interpersonal Manipula- In an attempt to broaden the network of dark personality traits,
tion accounted for all of the betweenness (7) in Sample 1 and the primary analyses included spitefulness (Samples 1 and 2) and
almost all of the betweenness (6) in Sample 2. Thus, whether aggression (Sample 2). However, given past interest in identifying
examining association (raw correlations) or LASSO (partial correla- the core of the Dark Triad, we also ran the analyses with just the
tions) networks, Interpersonal Manipulation and Callousness Dark Triad traits. These LASSO networks are depicted in Supple-
appear to be at the core of these dark traits. Interestingly, Spiteful- mentary Fig. S2 and the centrality measures for these networks
ness no longer yielded especially high Zhang clustering coefficients are provided in Supplementary Tables S4 and S5. Interpersonal
D.K. Marcus et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 73 (2018) 56–62 61

Table 3
Sample 1 (N = 2831) centrality and clustering measures for adaptive LASSO network.

Trait Strength EI Closeness Betweenness Zhang


Psychopathy
Interpersonal manipulation 1.29 1.30 1.49 5 0.38
Callousness 1.55 1.52 1.39 6 0.61
Criminal tendencies 0.60 0.72 0.43 3 0.16
Erratic lifestyle 0.30 0.21 0.20 1 1.56
Narcissism
Exploitativeness/entitlement 1.14 0.90 0.85 0 0.39
Grandiose exhibitionism 1.38 0.92 1.47 0 0.13
Leadership/authority 0.35 0.90 0.83 4 1.47
Machiavellianism & spitefulness
Machiavellianism 0.38 0.93 0.19 2 0.21
Spitefulness 0.12 0.32 0.16 2 1.91

Note. Betweenness values are raw scores. All other values are z-scores. EI = Step1 Expected Influence. The Zhang clustering coefficient is used because of its usability with
correlational data.

Table 4
Sample 2 (N = 845) centrality and clustering measures for adaptive LASSO network.

Trait Strength EI Closeness Betweenness Zhang


Psychopathy
Interpersonal manipulation 1.25 1.43 1.82 11 0.27
Callousness 1.47 1.62 1.38 8 0.82
Criminal tendencies 0.07 0.29 0.32 3 0.02
Erratic lifestyle 0.52 0.21 0.33 4 0.95
Narcissism
Exploitativeness/entitlement 1.50 0.95 0.97 1 0.45
Grandiose exhibitionism 0.61 0.17 0.69 1 0.67
Leadership/authority 0.17 0.67 0.39 4 0.98
Machiavellianism, spitefulness, & aggression
Machiavellianism 1.49 1.64 1.38 0 0.92
Spitefulness 0.10 0.18 0.32 0 2.20
Aggression 0.37 0.30 0.10 3 0.16

Note. Betweenness values are raw scores. All other values are z-scores. EI = Step1 Expected Influence. The Zhang clustering coefficient is used because of its usability with
correlational data.

Manipulation and Callousness remain the most central nodes on ness and callousness form the ‘‘dark core” (p. 528) of a malevolent
each of the centrality metrics in both samples, and visually Inter- personality. The current study was similar to Jones and Figueredo
personal Manipulation was at the center of both networks. Overall, (2013) in that both studies used the same instruments to measure
the results from these supplementary analyses confirm the central- the Dark Triad personality traits, and both used a combination of
ity of Interpersonal Manipulation and Callousness to both the Dark college student and MTurk participants. Still, the consistency in
Triad and to a slightly broader network of dark traits. results across these two studies is remarkable because Jones and
Figueredo used a very different analytic strategy (i.e., multi-
4. Discussion sample structural equation modeling). Furthermore, the findings
were consistent even though the current study added spitefulness
Two components of psychopathy, interpersonal manipulation and (in Sample 2) aggressiveness to the network of dark personal-
and callousness, occupied central positions in both sets of network ity traits, whereas Jones and Figueredo added social dominance
analyses. In fact, interpersonal manipulation and callousness were orientation to their analyses for their second study. Thus, these
the only two traits to yield above average values on all four central- consistent findings across samples and diverse analytic methods
ity metrics across both samples. Furthermore, as depicted in Fig. 1, may be seen as an example of ‘‘critical multiplism” (Cook, 1985)
interpersonal manipulation and callousness are the only traits that that enhances our confidence that interpersonal manipulation
are located centrally in the network and not along the periphery. It and callousness are at the dark core of antagonistic traits. Given
is noteworthy that these two traits were often the ones that linked recent concerns about the reproducibility of psychological findings
the other dark personality traits, accounting for roughly half of the and the need for replication in research (Open Science
betweenness values in the adaptive LASSO networks (11 of 23 in Collaboration, 2015), finding such consistency is both reassuring
Sample 1 and 19 of 35 in Sample 2). When only the Dark Triad and valuable.
was analyzed, they accounted for more than half of the between- Jones and Figueredo (2013) made a compelling argument why
ness values in each sample (11 of 17 in Sample 1, 11 of 18 in Sam- the combination of both interpersonal manipulation and callous-
ple 2). Removing interpersonal manipulation and callousness from ness is necessary for a malevolent or antagonistic personality.
these dark networks would leave the other nodes much less den- Essentially, an individual who is callous or unemotional but not
sely connected. Overall, these results suggest that interpersonal manipulative may simply be socially disconnected (e.g., schizoid
manipulation and callousness are the dark traits that hold the Dark or autistic). Many manipulative individuals, who are not callous,
Triad (or an expanded set of dark traits) together. may direct their manipulation toward benign pursuits. In contrast,
These results are strikingly consistent with the conclusion of individuals who are both callous and manipulative are positioned
Jones and Figueredo (2013) that the combination of manipulative- to force others into harmful outcomes. In the current networks,
62 D.K. Marcus et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 73 (2018) 56–62

interpersonal manipulation and callousness had strong connec- network analysis of personality data in R. Journal of Research in Personality, 54,
13–29.
tions with one another, and each was not only connected to the
Costantini, G., & Perugini, M. (2016). The network of conscientiousness. Journal of
other psychopathy nodes, but to nodes from each of the other dark Research in Personality, 65, 68–88.
traits. Epskamp, S., Cramer, A. O., Waldorp, L. J., Schmittmann, V. D., & Borsboom, D.
One limitation of the current study is that the networks did not (2012). Qgraph: Network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data.
Journal of Statistical Software, 48, 1–18.
include other dark personality traits, most notably sadism, which Epskamp, S., Kruis, J., & Marsman, M. (2017). Estimating psychopathological
has been identified as being a component of the ‘‘Dark Tetrad” networks: Be careful what you wish for. PLoS ONE, 12(6), e0179891.
(Chabrol, Van Leeuwen, Rodgers, & Séjourné, 2009). Future studies Furnham, A., Richards, S. C., & Paulhus, D. L. (2013). The dark triad of personality: A
10 year review. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7, 199–216.
may also consider including additional malevolent traits such as Hare, R. D. (2003). Manual for the revised psychopathy checklist (2nd ed.). Toronto,
greed (Seuntjens, Zeelenberg, van de Ven, & Breugelmans, 2015) ON: Multi-Health Systems.
and authoritarianism (Ludeke & Krueger, 2013). Furthermore, the Humphries, M. D., & Gurney, K. (2008). Network ‘small-world-ness’: A quantitative
method for determining canonical network equivalence. PLoS ONE, 3(4),
current study focused on the relations among specific dark traits e0002051.
and did not include broadband personality traits such as agreeable- Jakobwitz, S., & Egan, V. (2006). The dark triad and normal personality traits.
ness from the Big Five or honesty-humility from the HEXACO per- Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 331–339.
Jonason, P. K., Li, N. P., Webster, G. D., & Schmitt, D. P. (2009). The dark triad:
sonality model. Network analysis studies that include these Facilitating a short-term mating strategy in men. European Journal of Personality,
broader traits may further clarify the nature of the dark core. Dark 23, 5–18.
Triad research has focused on ‘‘subclinical” manifestations of the- Jones, D. N., & Figueredo, A. J. (2013). The core of darkness: Uncovering the heart of
the dark triad. European Journal of Personality, 27, 521–531.
ses antagonistic personality traits and the current study had the
Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2011). Differentiating the dark triad within the
advantage of including both college and national (MTurk) samples. interpersonal circumplex. In L. M. Horowitz & S. Strack (Eds.), Handbook of
Still, it would be worthwhile to examine whether these network interpersonal psychology: Theory, research, assessment, and therapeutic
structures replicate in samples with higher rates of antisocial or interventions (pp. 249–268). New York, NY: Wiley.
Little, T. D., Henrich, C. C., Jones, S. M., & Hawley, P. H. (2003). Disentangling the
malevolent behavior, such as criminal offenders or other forensic ‘whys’ from the ‘whats’ of aggressive behaviour. International Journal of
samples. It would also be beneficial to examine the consistency Behavioral Research, 27, 122–133.
of these network structures in specific contexts such as highly Ludeke, S. G., & Krueger, R. F. (2013). Authoritarianism as a personality trait:
Evidence from a longitudinal behavior genetic study. Personality and Individual
competitive or stressful environments. Differences, 55, 480–484.
Marcus, D. K., & Zeigler-Hill, V. (2015). A big tent of dark personality traits. Social
Acknowledgement and Personality Psychology Compass, 9, 434–446.
Marcus, D. K., Zeigler-Hill, V., Mercer, S., & Norris, A. L. (2014). The psychology of
spite and the measurement of spitefulness. Psychological Assessment, 26,
Jonathan Preszler’s work was supported by the Anthony Mar- 563–574.
chionne Foundation for the Scientific Study of Human Relations McHoskey, J. W., Worzel, W., & Szyarto, C. (1998). Machiavellianism and
psychopathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 192–210.
and Psychological Processes Endowed Graduate Fellowship for McNally, R. J. (2016). Can network analysis transform psychopathology? Behaviour
Research at Washington State University. Research and Therapy, 86, 95–104.
Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Otgaar, H., & Meijer, E. (2017). The malevolent side of
human nature: A meta-analysis and critical review of the literature on the dark
Appendix A. Supplementary material triad (narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy). Perspectives on
Psychological Science, 12, 183–204.
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in Open Science Collaboration (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological
science. Science, 349, aac4716.
the online version, at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2017.11.003. Paulhus, D. L., Neumann, C. S., & Hare, R. D. (in press). Manual for the Self-Report
Psychopathy Scale (4th ed.). Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems.
References Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The dark triad of personality: Narcissism,
Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36,
556–563.
Ackerman, R. A., Witt, E. A., Donnellan, B., Trzesniewski, K. H., Robins, R. W., & Kashy,
Raskin, R. N., & Hall, C. S. (1981). The narcissistic personality inventory: Alternate
D. A. (2010). What does the narcissistic personality inventory really measure?
form reliability and further evidence of construct validity. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 18, 67–87.
Assessment, 45, 159–162.
Book, A., Visser, B. A., & Volk, A. A. (2015). Unpacking ‘‘evil”: Claiming the core of the
Robinaugh, D. J., Millner, A. J., & McNally, R. J. (2016). Identifying highly influential
dark triad. Personality and Individual Differences, 73, 29–38.
nodes in the complicated grief network. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125,
Borsboom, D., & Cramer, A. O. (2013). Network analysis: An integrative approach to
747–757.
the structure of psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9,
Seuntjens, T. G., Zeelenberg, M., van de Ven, N., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2015).
91–121.
Dispositional greed. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 917–933.
Chabrol, H., Van Leeuwen, N., Rodgers, R., & Séjourné, N. (2009). Contributions of
Zeigler-Hill, V., & Marcus, D. K. (2016). The dark side of personality: Science and
psychopathic, narcissistic, Machiavellian, and sadistic personality traits to
practice in social, personality, and clinical psychology. Washington, DC: American
juvenile delinquency. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 734–739.
Psychological Association.
Christie, R., & Geis, R. L. (1970). Studies in machiavellianism. New York, NY: Academic
Zhang, B., & Horvath, S. (2005). A general framework for weighted gene co-
Press.
expression network analysis. Statistical Applications in Genetics and Molecular
Cook, T. D. (1985). Postpositivist critical multiplism. In L. Shotland & M. M. Mark
Biology, 4(1), 17.
(Eds.), Social science and social policy (pp. 21–62). Beverly Hills, CA: SAGE.
Costantini, G., Epskamp, S., Borsboom, D., Perugini, M., Mottus, R., Waldorp, L. J., &
Cramer, A. O. J. (2015). State of the aRt personality research: A tutorial on

You might also like