Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

This article was downloaded by: [University of California, San Francisco]

On: 13 December 2014, At: 02:23


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer
House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

International Journal of Psychology


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/pijp20

Environmentalism as a trait: Gauging people's


prosocial personality in terms of environmental
engagement
a b c
Florian G. Kaiser & Katarzyna Byrka
a
Otto-von-Guericke University , Magdeburg, Germany
b
Warsaw School of Social Sciences and Humanities , Wroclaw, Poland
c
Eindhoven University of Technology , Eindhoven, The Netherlands
Published online: 10 Nov 2010.

To cite this article: Florian G. Kaiser & Katarzyna Byrka (2011) Environmentalism as a trait: Gauging people's
prosocial personality in terms of environmental engagement, International Journal of Psychology, 46:1, 71-79, DOI:
10.1080/00207594.2010.516830

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2010.516830

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”)
contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors
make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability
for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions
and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of
the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of
information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands,
costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or
indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or
systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution
in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY, 2011, 46 (1), 71–79

Environmentalism as a trait: Gauging people’s


prosocial personality in terms of
environmental engagement

Florian G. Kaiser1 and Katarzyna Byrka2,3


1
Otto-von-Guericke University, Magdeburg, Germany
2
Warsaw School of Social Sciences and Humanities, Wroclaw, Poland
3
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 02:23 13 December 2014

Eindhoven University of Technology, Eindhoven, The Netherlands

A ccording to Hardin (1968), environmental deterioration stems from self-interest undermining people’s
resource conservation in the collective interest. Not surprisingly, selfless prosocial motives, such as personal
feelings of moral obligation, have often been recognized as a key force behind people’s environmentalism.
In our research, we anticipated that environmentalists—people with an inclination for pro-environmental
engagement—would possess a propensity to generally act prosocially. In an extension of previous work, we
expected that a well-established self-report measure of past conservation behavior would predict people’s active
participation in a psychological experiment. Based on subjects’ degree of environmental engagement, originally
established in 2003, we re-contacted a sample of 502 persons in 2005. Of these 502 (68.5% low, 31.5% high
in environmentalism), 131 showed up for the announced experiment. Among those participants, we found
that environmentalists’ prosocial personalities were additionally reflected in their social value orientations.
Ninety percent of the environmentalists turned out to be prosocials, whereas only 65% of the less
environmentally engaged subjects were prosocials. Overall, our findings lend credit to a notion of environmen-
talism as an indicator of even subtle quantitative differences in a person’s prosocial trait level. By and large,
environmentalists acted more prosocially even in mundane activities unrelated to environmental conservation.
Additional evidence comes from the commons dilemma experiment in which the participants partook.
There, we generally found comparatively more cooperation with others for the collective good from people high
in environmentalism. Our findings represent circumstantial evidence for a prosocial propensity dimension
along which people differ, and which is also reflected in people’s pro-environmental behavioral performance.
If, however, environmentalism has to be regarded as indicative of a prosocial trait rather than a state-like
motive, high hopes for moral norms and other prosocial motives in environmental conservation do not seem
warranted.

Keywords: Environmental attitudes; Personality traits; Social values; Prosocial behavior; Conservation
(ecological behavior).

S elon Hardin (1968), la dégradation environnementale provient de l’intérêt personnel qui mine la protection
des ressources des personnes dans l’intérêt collectif. Sans surprise, des motifs prosociaux désintéressés, tels
que des sentiments personnels d’obligation morale, ont souvent été reconnus comme des forces clef derrière
l’environnementalisme des gens. Dans notre travail de recherche, nous avons anticipé que les environnementa-
listes –personnes ayant une inclination pour un engagement pro-environnemental– auraient une propension a

Correspondence should be addressed to Florian G. Kaiser, Otto-von-Guericke University, Institute of Psychology, PO Box 4120,
D-39016 Magdeburg, Germany. (E-mail: florian.kaiser@ovgu.de).
This research was financially supported by a grant from the J. F. Schouten Graduate School of User–System Interaction
Research at Eindhoven University of Technology, Eindhoven, The Netherlands. Additionally, we were assisted by the
Eindhoven Municipality. We acknowledge the Research & Statistics Department of the Eindhoven Municipality, namely,
Carla Verheijen, Bas Oude Hengel, and Kees van der Hoeven, for their cooperation. We also wish to thank Jane Thompson
and Steven Ralston for their language support, and Caroline Duvier, Jeffrey Joireman, Paul van Lange, three anonymous
reviewers, and the editor, Michèle Robert, for their comments on earlier versions of this paper.
ß 2010 International Union of Psychological Science
http://www.psypress.com/ijp DOI: 10.1080/00207594.2010.516830
72 KAISER AND BYRKA

généralement agir prosocialement. Dans le prolongement d’un travail antérieur, nous nous attendions à ce qu’une
mesure auto-rapportée bien établie d’un comportement passé de protection prédise la participation active de ces
personnes dans une expérience de psychologie. Basé sur le degré d’engagement environnementaliste des
participants, initialement établi en 2003, nous avons contacté à nouveau un échantillon de 502 personnes en 2005.
Finalement, 131 personnes (68.5% bas, 31.5% haut en environnementalisme) ont participé à l’expérience. Parmi
ceux-ci, nous avons trouvé que la personnalité prosociale des environnementalistes se reflétait dans les
orientations de leurs valeurs sociales. 90% des écologistes sont prosociaux tandis que 65% des personnes les
moins engagées l’étaient. Au total nos résultats confèrent du crédit à la notion d’environnementalisme en tant
qu’indicateur de différences quantitatives subtiles au niveau du trait prosocial des personnes. Dans l’ensemble, les
écologistes agissent plus prosocialement même lors d’activités triviales non liées à la protection environnementale.
Des preuves supplémentaires viennent de l’expérience de dilemme des ressources à laquelle les sujets ont participé.
Là, nous avons généralement trouvé plus de coopération avec les autres pour le bien collectif de la part des
personnes fortement impliquées en environnementalisme. Nos résultats présentent des preuves indirectes à
propos de la propension prosociale selon laquelle les gens diffèrent, et qui est aussi reflétée dans leurs
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 02:23 13 December 2014

comportements pro-environnementalistes. Si toutefois l’environnementalisme doit être considéré comme un trait


prosocial plutôt qu’un motif, de grands espoirs pour les normes morales et autres motifs prosociaux dans la
protection environnementale ne sont pas garanties.

S egún Hardin (1968), el deterioro del medio ambiente se deriva del interés propio que socava
la conservación de los recursos de las personas en el interés colectivo. No es de extrañarse que
los motivos prosociales desinteresados, tales como los sentimientos personales de obligación moral, han sido
a menudo reconocidos como una fuerza clave detrás del ambientalismo de las personas. En nuestra
investigación, anticipamos que los ambientalistas –aquellas personas con una inclinación hacia el compromiso
en pro del medioambiente– poseerı́an, en general, una propensión a actuar prosocialmente. En una extensión
de un estudio anterior, supusimos que un instrumento de autoinforme bien establecido de conducta
de conservación administrado previamente predecirı́a la participación activa de los jóvenes en un
experimento psicológico. Basados en el grado de compromiso medioambiental de los sujetos, originalmente
establecido en 2003, se volvió a contactar a una muestra de 502 personas en 2005. De estos 502
(68,5% bajo y 31,5% alto en ambientalismo), 131 se presentaron para el experimento anunciado. Entre los
participantes finales, encontramos que las personalidades prosociales de los ambientalistas se reflejaban,
además, en sus orientaciones hacia los valores sociales. El noventa por ciento de los ambientalistas que se
presentaron para el experimento resultó ser prosocial, mientras que sólo el 65% de los sujetos
ambientalmente menos comprometidos eran prosociales. De modo general, nuestros resultados dan crédito
a la noción de que el ambientalismo es un indicador de diferencias cuantitativas incluso sutiles en el nivel de
prosocialidad de una persona. En general, los ambientalistas actuaron de manera más prosocial, inclusive en
actividades comunes no relacionadas con la conservación del medio ambiente. La evidencia adicional
proviene del experimento de dilemas comunes en el que los participantes estuvieron involucrados.
Comparativamente encontramos más cooperación con los demás por el bien común de parte de las
personas que habı́an puntuado alto en ecologismo. Nuestros resultados representan una evidencia
circunstancial que manifiesta una tendencia prosocial en la que las personas difieren, y que también se
refleja en el desempeño conductual pro-ambiental de las personas. Sin embargo, si el ambientalismo es
considerado como un indicio de un rasgo prosocial en lugar de un estado, las grandes esperanzas en las
normas morales y otros motivos prosociales en relación a la conservación del medio ambiente no parecen
estar justificadas.

If environmental deterioration stems from self- have also been recognized as key forces behind
interest, which undermines people’s resource environmental conservation (e.g., Stern, 2000).
conservation in the common interest, many Based on self-reported data and with up to 80–
currently existing environmental problems can 90% explained behavior variance, Kaiser (2006)
be viewed as commons dilemmas (Hardin, 1968). accordingly concluded that self-interest (i.e., utility
Concomitantly, cooperation in commons considerations) was irrelevant for people’s envir-
dilemmas has been regarded as a comparatively onmental engagement (cf. Milfont & Duckitt,
selfless, prosocial behavior (Kramer, McClintock, 2004; Schultz, 2001). Due to this and some similar
& Messick, 1986), and a person’s prosocial findings of a strong prosocial basis for people’s
disposition has consistently predicted coopera- environmental engagement, we suspect a trait-like
tion in commons dilemmas (Van Lange, 1999). (i.e., an enduring and comparatively unalterable)
In conservation psychology, selfless prosocial disposition to be at work rather than a context-
motives, such as a person’s felt moral obligation, specific, situation-dependent ad-hoc motivational
ENVIRONMENTALISM AS A TRAIT 73

state. In other words, we have come to believe that PROSOCIAL CONSIDERATIONS IN


environmental engagement essentially reflects a ENVIRONMENTAL CONSERVATION
person’s prosocial, moral nature.
In this paper, we argue that environmentalists In conservation psychology, the accumulated
(i.e., persons high in environmental engagement) evidence indicates that people’s conservation
are the ‘‘better’’ people. Regardless of the specifics performance is strongly determined by normative
of any particular prosocial engagement, we prosocial influences. In particular, a person’s
expected environmentalism to predict people’s moral obligation seems to be the most promising
propensity to act prosocially even outside the predictor (e.g., Bamberg & Möser, 2007). The
environmental domain. Also, we extended existing effect becomes even more prominent when
work by exploring the consistency between peo- concepts are assessed in a more trait-like fashion
ple’s self-reported environmental engagement and and with technically advantageous—in terms of
some specific overt but mundane prosocial accuracy—aggregated, multi-item measures.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 02:23 13 December 2014

behavior. In three systematic but atypical (as scales were


employed) explorations of a moral norms-
extended planned behavior model (Ajzen, 1991),
a person’s moral obligation did not improve
SOCIAL VALUE ORIENTATION IN explanatory power (e.g., Kaiser, Hübner, &
COMMONS DILEMMAS Bogner, 2005). Instead, the evidence showed a
substantial overlap between environmental atti-
Social dilemmas (including public good or tude and moral norms, which resulted in suppres-
resource dilemmas with open access to the sor effects in two of three studies by Kaiser and his
resource; see e.g., Komorita & Parks, 1995) are colleagues. It seemed as though people’s moral
situations in which people are confronted with a norms were already represented in their environ-
choice between personal and collective benefits. mental attitudes. In other words, with a more trait-
Psychologically, we assume that people are con- like, scale-based measurement of concepts, which
fronted with a genuine conflict between their already reflects a prosocial disposition, people’s
prosocial propensity to cooperate with others for environmental engagement can be predicted with
the greater collective good and their inclination to accuracies up to 80–90% (Kaiser, 2006). Thus, we
maximize their personal gains. By and large, the believe that these findings speak largely of a trait-
pursuit of self-interest (i.e., maximizing personal like, durable disposition to rather selflessly engage
benefits) is commonly found to be the prevailing in pro-environmental actions.
strategy (Gifford & Hine, 1997).
Nevertheless, people differ in their tendencies to
cooperate with one another in social dilemmas INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN
(Kramer et al., 1986). While multiple explanations ENVIRONMENTAL ENGAGEMENT
have been recognized for these individual differ-
ences, most of them seem to be linked to people’s If a person aims to achieve a certain goal
prosocial makeup: their conformity, altruism, (e.g., environmental conservation), we can expect
righteousness, moral integrity (Gifford & Hine, this person to engage in various respective
1997; Komorita & Parks, 1995), or social value activities, such as bike riding or recycling. Such
orientation (Van Lange, 1999). According to a composite of behaviors implies that people can
theory on social value orientation, one can select from various alternatives to realize their
expect prosocials to be dispositionally inclined to individual aspiration level, which leaves them with
maximize collective outcomes and, thus, to be choices. Instead of running energy-efficient wash-
more likely to cooperate with others. ing programs, people can abstain from using a
Research on social value orientation has addi- dryer or focus more on which detergents they use.
tionally confirmed its ecological validity by corro- A person’s pro-environmental aspiration becomes
borating its ability to predict cooperative obvious in the face of increasingly demanding
behaviors, such as volunteering time and helping activities or progressively intolerable sacrifices
the poor or ill with donations, even outside the (cf. Schultz & Oskamp, 1996). Necessarily, the
laboratory (e.g., McClintock & Allison, 1989). more obstacles someone overcomes and the more
Concomitantly, social value orientation has effort a person expends along the way to realizing
repeatedly also been found to be related to his or her goal, the more evident that person’s
environmental conservation (e.g., Joireman, van engagement is. Why would someone bother to ride
Lange, Kuhlman, van Vugt, & Shelley, 1997). a bike every morning through rain and snow,
74 KAISER AND BYRKA

switch to a vegetarian diet, and struggle with his prosocially, regardless of the specifics of the
or her kids to save warm water if one was not particular behavior. Logically, we anticipated a
committed to environmental conservation? well-established environmental engagement mea-
Likewise, when the tiniest inconvenience is enough sure to validly reflect people’s prosocial disposi-
to stop a person from engaging in a behavior, the tions rather well. In other words, people high in
devotion to the particular goal must be rather low. environmental engagement should act more pro-
Simultaneously, any behavior involves costs as it socially even when asked for rather mundane
requires personal resources such as time, money, favors, such as participating in a psychological
and effort. If one’s engagement can be manifested experiment. We aimed to extend previous research
by a variety of different acts, we can anticipate as we explored the relationship of a self-report
a prudent selection of these behaviors. Naturally, measure of overall environmental engagement
people commonly prefer the more convenient, with a specific overt prosocial behavior.
more socially sanctioned behaviors over the more
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 02:23 13 December 2014

complicated, strenuous, or pricey ones. Thus, we


can assume that people favor less demanding METHOD
actions over more demanding ones. This becomes
tangible in the relative popularity of various Participants and procedures
conservation behaviors. This popularity of a
behavior is, in turn, anticipated to be a function In 2003, we randomly sampled 4,445 residents
of two components: (a) people’s overall environ- from the municipal register for Eindhoven, The
mental engagement, and (b) the composition of all Netherlands. Of these, 1,746 persons returned
the figurative costs of a particular behavior. Kaiser usable questionnaires (response rate: 39.28%),
and colleagues corroborated that the Rasch model either spontaneously or after one reminder. Their
can be used to describe this functional relationship average age was 46.38 (SD ¼ 18.16, range: 15–84).
(e.g., Kaiser & Wilson, 2004). The classical Rasch Of these participants, 868 were female (49.71%)
model and, thus, the functional link between a and 878 were male (50.29%). This sample was not
person’s overall engagement and his or her representative of Eindhoven but gave no indica-
probability to act in a certain way is depicted tion of a severe restriction of range in its
with the following formula (for more details see, environmental engagement.
e.g., Bond & Fox, 2001): In 2005, based on their degree of environmental
engagement, we re-contacted 502 (28.75%)
 
pki persons from the original 1,746 participants first
ln ¼ k  i by mail and subsequently by telephone. The
1  pki
participants were invited to partake in a study
where the natural logarithm of the ratio of the about decision making without any reference to
probability (pki) of person k to take on a specific the particular content of the decision task. Of the
behavior i relative to the probability of non-action 502 persons contacted, 344 (68.52%) were low and
(1  pki) is given by the difference between k’s 158 (31.48%) high in their environmental engage-
overall environmental engagement (k) and the ment. The definition for low was a maximally
composite of all the figurative costs of this average engagement score, whereas high engage-
behavior (i). Note that in this mathematical ment was a score minimally one standard devia-
formalization, people are distinguishable with tion above the average.
respect to their overall levels of engagement, and Of the 502 people contacted, 68 (13.55%) could
behaviors are distinguishable by how ‘‘costly’’ they not be reached, which left a sample of 434
are to realize. By applying the Rasch model, (86.45%). Of those, 303 (69.82%) did not accept
Kaiser and colleagues were additionally able to our request to participate—of whom 259 (59.68%)
circumvent some well-known technical problems explicitly refused. Of the 434, 293 (67.51%)
of conventional factor analytical approaches that persons were low and 141 (32.49%) high in their
can occur when behaviors are involved (see e.g., environmental engagement. Of the 131 (30.18%)
Kaiser, Oerke, & Bogner, 2007). who actually came to the laboratory, 79 (60.31%)
were men and 52 (39.69%) were women, with a
mean age of 54.93 (SD ¼ 16.16).
RESEARCH GOALS Note that generally in this research, the
alpha level was set at p ¼ .05. Of the participants
In the current study, we expected environmental- who came to the laboratory, a larger proportion
ists to generally act comparatively more of females were high in environmental engagement
ENVIRONMENTALISM AS A TRAIT 75

(50.87%) compared to males (31.08%), the two measures of r ¼ .67. When conventionally
2(1) ¼ 5.27, 2 ¼ .04. Persons high in environ- corrected for measurement error attenuation, the
mental engagement were older (M ¼ 59.49, correlation coefficient translated into r ¼ .96,
SD ¼ 14.42, range: 26–83) than persons low in representing an impressive stability across time.
engagement (M ¼ 51.42, SD ¼ 16.63, range: 18– We additionally included a measure of social
83), F(1, 130) ¼ 8.50, Z2 ¼ .06. value orientation using nine questions containing
three possible response alternatives (see Van
Lange, de Bruin, Otten, & Joireman, 1997). The
Measures three options represent (a) prosocial, (b) indivi-
dualistic, or (c) competitive choices for how a
Our dependent variable (prosocial behavior) was person agrees to grant credit points to another
whether the subject personally appeared on site to person and to himself or herself. A particular value
participate (show vs. no-show). Environmental orientation is assigned when a person prefers the
engagement was measured using a composite of
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 02:23 13 December 2014

same type of choice at least six out of nine times.


50 self-reported behaviors from the General Of the 131 participants, 113 (86.25%) were
Ecological Behavior scale (e.g., Kaiser & Wilson, classifiable. The vast majority of our participants
2004). The behavioral self-reports represent var- were prosocials (n ¼ 86; 65.64%), with only 27
ious conservation domains, such as energy con- (20.61%) proselfs (i.e., 18 (13.74%) ‘‘individu-
servation, transportation, consumerism, and alists’’ and 9 (6.87%) ‘‘competitors’’). For the
recycling. Eighteen relatively more permanent purposes of this study, we collapsed the individu-
behaviors, such as membership in a car pool, alists and competitors into one category, which we
were examined with a yes/no format (indicating called the proselfs. Note that a person’s social
either ‘‘ecological engagement’’ or ‘‘non-engage- value orientation and the second assessment of a
ment’’). Other more reoccurring behaviors person’s environmental engagement (with the
(n ¼ 32), such as buying milk in returnable bottles, General Ecological Behavior scale) could be
were later recoded from a polytomous response carried out with only the 131 persons who actually
format to a dichotomous one by regarding the came to the site.
responses ‘‘never,’’ ‘‘seldom,’’ and ‘‘occasionally’’
as rejections (indicating ‘‘unreliable ecological The decision task
engagement’’) and ‘‘often’’ and ‘‘always’’ as
confirmations (indicating ‘‘reliable ecological The decision task for which people were invited
engagement’’). was a typical commons dilemma game. Each
The scale calibration was implemented in line participant was seated in front of a computer
with previous such calibrations using the Rasch and was led to believe that four other persons were
model (for details and fit statistics, see Byrka, simultaneously involved in the game in separate
2009). In 2003, the environmental engagement cubicles (whereas the computer randomly gener-
measure had a separation reliability of r ¼ .71 ated the other four participants’ requests following
(N ¼ 1,746). With a time lag of about 24 months, a conventional procedure). Each participant
we reassessed environmental engagement with the played 10 rounds in the game. Depending on the
131 persons who came to the laboratory. To avoid experimental condition, half of the participants
bothering them with too many overlapping ques- requested energy amounts in kW for personal
tions, we randomly selected a subset of 20 items for amenities. The other half of the participants
each participant out of the 50 behaviors previously requested points. The individual pay-off was
employed. Persons with an originally high determined by the overall group request. If the
environmental engagement level did not receive total demand exceeded 10 units of the common
extremely easy behaviors, and persons originally resource, no one received anything. If the demand
low on environmental engagement did not receive remained within 10 units of the resource, the
extremely difficult ones. Such a procedure is requested amount was granted. Each round was
possible due to the technical features of Rasch terminated with feedback concerning the indivi-
models (i.e., uniform item discrimination). Again, dual and the collective pay-offs (for more details,
assessing people’s environmental engagement see Byrka, 2009).
based on the Rasch model was feasible. Once
more, the engagement measure had a separation RESULTS
reliability of r ¼ .70 (N ¼ 131). People’s environ-
mental engagement in the two assessments was In this research, we expected a person’s propensity
highly comparable given the correlation between to comply with a request to participate in a
76 KAISER AND BYRKA

psychological experiment to depend on the extent TABLE 1


of the person’s environmental engagement. Based Frequencies of persons with high and low environmental
engagement complying or not complying with a selfless
on a one-way ANOVA, we found that persons behavioral response (i.e., participation in a psychological
who actually showed up in 2005 for the experiment experiment)
had originally scored higher in environmental
engagement in 2003 (Mshow ¼ .09, SD ¼ 1.00, Environmental engagement
n ¼ 131) than the ones who failed to show up Behavioral response High Low
2 years later (Mno-show ¼ .33, SD ¼ .97, n ¼ 303),
F(1, 432) ¼ 17.22, Z2 ¼ .04. Show 57 (13.13%) 74 (17.05%)
Table 1 reports the numbers of participants with 43 (9.81%) 88 (20.37%)
No-show 84 (19.36%) 219 (50.46%)
high and low engagement who participated or did
98 (22.68%) 205 (47.14%)
not participate in the experiment. Of the 131
persons who effectively participated in the experi- Relative frequencies are presented in parentheses. In all cells,
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 02:23 13 December 2014

ment, 57 were high in engagement (40.42% of the the discrepancy between the observed count (italics) and the
expected count (bold face) was significant.
successfully contacted highly engaged persons) and
74 were low in environmental engagement (25.25%
of the successfully contacted lowly engaged per-
sons). As predicted, a person high in environ- When combined, our results speak of environ-
mental engagement was more likely to show up for mental engagement as a sensible indicator of
the experiment than a person with a comparatively people’s prosocial propensities. Overall, people’s
low engagement, 2(1) ¼ 10.40, 2 ¼ .08. differential prosocial personalities seem reflected
As this effect was anticipated to reflect people’s in their pro-environmental deeds. Additional
prosocial nature, we also expected to find it evidence for our claim comes from the experiment
represented in people’s differential prosocial dis- in which the participants partook. The original
position, measured with their social value orienta- purpose of this research was to test the hypothesis
tion (Van Lange, 1999). In line with our that individual environmental engagement deter-
expectation, we found all 113 (86.25% of the mines cooperation with others for the greater
sample) classifiable participants who effectively collective good in a laboratory commons dilemma
showed up for the experiment to be more likely to experiment (for more details, see Byrka, 2009).
be prosocials than proselfs (note that 18 persons Specifically, we had anticipated that cooperation
were not classifiable). would depend on environmental engagement
Among the classifiable persons, we found, as provided that the resource was environment-
expected and in line with previous findings relevant (e.g., energy). With a planned compar-
(McClintock & Allison, 1989), relatively more ison, the predicted interaction was corroborated:
prosocials than proselfs when comparing persons t(127) ¼ 2.65, Z2 ¼ .05.
high in environmental engagement to persons low As can be seen in Figure 1, if the common
in engagement, 2(1) ¼ 10.15, 2 ¼ .09. Specifically, resource meant energy, persons with high levels of
the proportion of prosocials (i.e., 90.20%) was environmental engagement removed less of the
comparatively higher for persons high in resource than the persons in the other three
environmental engagement than the 64.52% conditions combined (those with low levels
prosocials among persons low in environmental of engagement considering either environment-
engagement. Conversely, only 9.80% of the persons relevant energy or environment-irrelevant points,
high in environmental engagement were proselfs, and those with high levels of engagement
35.48% of the ones low in environmental considering points). Note that this recognized
engagement were proselfs. Employing a one-way interaction does not depend on persons with low
ANOVA, we found also that proselfs levels of environmental engagement removing an
(Mproselfs ¼ .28, SD ¼ .20, n ¼ 27) scored lower in environment-irrelevant resource (e.g., points).
their environmental engagement than prosocials With a comparison of persons with high levels of
(Mprosocials ¼ .40, SD ¼ .12, n ¼ 86), F(1, 111) ¼ engagement considering an environment-relevant
7.45, Z2 ¼ .06. Note that this effect held even resource to the remaining two conditions com-
when we used people’s environmental engagement bined (i.e., persons with high levels of engagement
2 years prior to the assessment of their social value removing an environment-irrelevant resource and
orientation: Again, proselfs (Mproselfs ¼ .38, persons with low levels of engagements removing
SD ¼ .17, n ¼ 27) scored lower in their environ- an environment-relevant resource), the effect
mental engagement than prosocials (Mprosocials ¼ remains statistically significant: t(127) ¼ 1.80,
.25, SD ¼ .11, n ¼ 86), F(1, 111) ¼ 8.15, Z2 ¼ .07. Z2 ¼ .02.
ENVIRONMENTALISM AS A TRAIT 77

excessive incidence ratios, we found also that


active participants and highly engaged persons
were more likely to be prosocials—measured with
social value orientation (e.g., Van Lange, 1999).
Despite the presented circumstantial evidence,
there is still room for alternative explanations.
Biased subsamples in terms of general intelligence
(indicated by IQ and/or educational level, or by
socioeconomic status), of future-orientation, and
of agreeableness, rather than prosociality are three
candidates that could additionally explain our
findings (as we did not control for these three
variables).
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 02:23 13 December 2014

Figure 1. Allocation of a common resource (points/energy in Intelligence is known to be linked with research
kW) as a function of the environmental relevance of the
common resource and of people’s environmental engagement.
participation (Rosenthal & Rosnow, 1975), as is
N ¼ 131 (n ¼ 38, n ¼ 29, n ¼ 36, n ¼ 28; from left to right); future-orientation (Harber, Zimbardo, & Boyd,
vertical bars indicate the 95% confidence intervals. 2003). Agreeableness entails a certain prosocial
orientation in individuals (cf. McCrae & Costa,
Another look at Figure 1 makes it obvious that 1999). Previously, it has been found to be
in addition to the theoretically anticipated and positively related to cooperation in resource
corroborated interaction, a second effect is appar- dilemmas (Koole, Jager, van den Berg, Vlek, &
ent in the presented data; this one, however, is a Hofstee, 2001) and to environmentalism (Hirsh
main effect. People high in environmentalism & Dolderman, 2007).
generally demanded less and, thus, acted more At the same time, our research speaks of two
cooperatively in the commons dilemmas, regard- more issues worth mentioning. First, if environ-
less of the resource the dilemma was about, mentalism turns out to be grounded in a compara-
t(127) ¼ 3.29, Z2 ¼ .08. In other words, we found tively durable trait—apparent in its remarkable
comparatively more cooperation in general, that stability over 24 months—rather than a more
is, more prosocial behavior from people high in malleable motive, this finding also cautions against
environmental engagement irrespective of the overly high hopes for moral exhortation in
resource that was considered. Note also that environmental conservation as a viable means for
while cooperation in social dilemmas could have effectively changing behavior. In other words, our
been expected from prosocial individuals, thus far, findings imply that environmentalism might not be
differences in environmental engagement have not as malleable in large-scale one-shot interventions
been found to be relevant for individual coopera- by means of promoting moral norms as conserva-
tion in laboratory experiments involving resource tion psychologists occasionally seem to believe.
dilemmas (see Smith & Bell, 1992). Second, if the reported self-selection bias toward
prosocial individuals can eventually be confirmed
in the social sciences, we may have two rather
DISCUSSION fundamental problems: (a) Generalizing findings
of (potentially) prosocially biased samples to the
As anticipated, environmentalists were more likely general population may be unwarranted at times;
to volunteer, show up, and actually participate in a and (b) researching prosocial performance in
psychological experiment. This finding not only psychology might be problematic due to sample-
confirms our hypothesis that a prosocial trait selection-dependent range restrictions in the target
would be reflected in people’s environmental variable.
engagement, but also has some bearing on how Four limitations are also worth mentioning as
to promote environmentalism and on how we look they may possibly have affected our research and,
at experimental research in psychology. thus, seem relevant for our conclusions. First, our
As an extension of previous work, we found that data were originally collected for another purpose
people high in environmental engagement (estab- (cf. Byrka, 2009). Thus, our hypothesis was at least
lished with a self-report measure of past conserva- partly inspired by an apparent pattern in some
tion behavior; e.g., Kaiser & Wilson, 2004) were already available data. In other words, our test
comparatively more likely to participate in a was not truly prospective. Nevertheless, we would
psychological experiment than people low in like to stress the convergence of our results with
environmental engagement. As indicated by multiple other findings involving different models,
78 KAISER AND BYRKA

measures, designs, and samples (e.g., Bamberg & pro-environmental behavior. Journal of
Möser, 2007; Kaiser, 2006). Environmental Psychology, 27, 14–25.
Bond, T. G., & Fox, C. M. (2001). Applying the Rasch
Second, financial compensation was granted model: Fundamental measurement in the human sci-
for participating in our experiment. Each ences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
participant was offered E6.50 (approx. US$8.50) Byrka, K. (2009). Attitude–behavior consistency:
at the end of the experiment, which seems to Campbell’s paradigm in environmental and health
contradict the subsequent labeling of participation domains. Doctoral dissertation, Eindhoven Univ-
as a selfless, prosocial behavior. However, because ersity of Technology, Eindhoven, The Netherlands.
Gifford, R., & Hine, D. W. (1997). Towards coopera-
the extent of the compensation was economically tion in the commons dilemma. Canadian Journal of
insufficient to cover expenditure in terms of time Behavioural Science, 29(3), 167–178.
loss and transportation costs, participation in the Hardin, G. (1968). The tragedy of the commons.
experiment remained a comparatively selfless act. Science, 162, 1243–1248.
Third, the two subsamples must be regarded as Harber, K. D., Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (2003).
Participant self-selection biases as a function of
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 02:23 13 December 2014

biased in terms of age and gender composition— individual differences in time perspective. Basic and
yet two other alternative explanations of our Applied Social Psychology, 25, 255–264.
findings—and in terms of extremity. While low Hirsh, J. B., & Dolderman, D. (2007). Personality
environmental engagement meant maximally aver- predictors of consumerism and environmentalism:
age scores, high engagement meant scores mini- A preliminary study. Personality and Individual
mally one standard deviation above the average. Differences, 43, 1583–1593.
Joireman, J. A., van Lange, P. A. M., Kuhlman, D. M.,
In other words, low was less low than high was van Vugt, M., & Shelley, G. P. (1997). An inter-
high. Note that such a reduced and not perfectly dependence analysis of commuting decisions.
centered gap was actually to our disadvantage as it European Journal of Social Psychology, 27, 441–463.
created conditions that made it more difficult to Kaiser, F. G. (2006). A moral extension of the theory of
detect differences in the two subsamples. planned behavior: Norms and anticipated feelings of
regret in conservationism. Personality and Individual
Fourth, our research design was quasi-experi-
Differences, 41, 71–81.
mental. That means participants were not ran- Kaiser, F. G., Hübner, G., & Bogner, F. X. (2005).
domly allocated to conditions. Strictly speaking, Contrasting the theory of planned behavior with the
such a design does not allow for a conclusive test of value–belief–norm model in explaining conservation
causality (i.e., for the direction of the relationship). behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35,
Since we proposed environmental engagement as 2150–2170.
Kaiser, F. G., Oerke, B., & Bogner, F. X. (2007).
an indicator of a person’s overall prosocial disposi- Behavior-based environmental attitude: Develop-
tion, only a reciprocal relationship and not ment of an instrument for adolescents. Journal of
causality—between the general disposition and a Environmental Psychology, 27, 242–251.
specific prosocial behavior—was formally required. Kaiser, F. G., & Wilson, M. (2004). Goal-directed
With our research, we demonstrated that conservation behavior: The specific composition of a
general performance. Personality and Individual
environmentalism can be used to forecast people’s
Differences, 36, 1531–1544.
propensity to act prosocially even outside the Komorita, S. S., & Parks, C. D. (1995). Interpersonal
environmental domain and even with specific overt relations: Mixed-motive interaction. Annual Review
prosocial actions by cooperating in a commons of Psychology, 46, 183–207.
dilemma and by participating in a psychological Koole, S. L., Jager, W., van den Berg, A. E., Vlek, C. A. J.,
experiment. Most remarkably, however, our find- & Hofstee, W. K. B. (2001). On the social nature of
personality: Effects of extraversion, agreeableness,
ings speak of environmentalism as a virtue and of
and feedback about collective resource use on coop-
environmentalists as being generally ‘‘better,’’ eration in a resource dilemma. Personality and Social
more prosocially acting persons. Psychology Bulletin, 27, 289–301.
Kramer, R. M., McClintock, C. G., & Messick, D. M.
Manuscript received October 2009 (1986). Social values and cooperative response to a
Revised manuscript accepted July 2010 simulated resource conservation crisis. Journal of
First published online November 2010 Personality, 54, 576–592.
McClintock, C. G., & Allison, S. T. (1989). Social value
orientation and helping behavior. Journal of Applied
REFERENCES Social Psychology, 19, 353–362.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa Jr, P. T. (1999). A five-factor
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. theory of personality. In L. A. Pervin, & O. P.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and
Processes, 50, 179–211. research (2nd ed., pp. 139–153). New York, NY:
Bamberg, S., & Möser, G. (2007). Twenty years Guilford Press.
after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: A new Milfont, T. L., & Duckitt, J. (2004). The structure of
meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of environmental attitudes: A first- and second-order
ENVIRONMENTALISM AS A TRAIT 79
confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of Environmen- simulated commons dilemma. Journal of Social
tal Psychology, 24, 289–303. Psychology, 132, 461–468.
Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. (1975). The volunteer Stern, P. C. (2000). Toward a coherent theory of
subject. New York, NY: Wiley. environmentally significant behavior. Journal of
Schultz, P. W. (2001). The structure of environmental Social Issues, 56, 407–424.
concern: Concern for self, other people, and the Van Lange, P. A. M. (1999). The pursuit of joint
biosphere. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 21, outcomes and equality in outcomes: An integrative
327–339. model of social value orientation. Journal of
Schultz, P. W., & Oskamp, S. (1996). Effort as a Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 337–349.
moderator of the attitude–behavior relationship: Van Lange, P. A. M., de Bruin, E. M. N., Otten, W., &
General environmental concern and recycling. Joireman, J. A. (1997). Development of prosocial,
Social Psychology Quarterly, 59, 375–383. individualistic, and competitive orientations: Theory
Smith, J. M., & Bell, P. A. (1992). Environmental and preliminary evidence. Journal of Personality and
concern and cooperative–competitive behavior in a Social Psychology, 73, 733–746.
Downloaded by [University of California, San Francisco] at 02:23 13 December 2014

You might also like