Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

Journal of Vision (2024) 24(2):4, 1–14 1

Effects of binocular disparity on binocular luminance


combination
Goro Maehara* Department of Human Science,
Kanagawa University, Kanagawa, Japan

Yiqian Wang* Department of Psychology,


The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan

Ikuya Murakami Department of Psychology,


The University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan

This study aimed to examine the effects of binocular phenomenon is referred to as Fechner’s paradox.
disparity on binocular combination of brightness Levelt (1965) and Engel (1970) conducted controlled
information coming from luminance increments and experiments on binocular brightness combinations.
decrements. The point of subjective equality was They found that, when both eyes were sufficiently
determined by asking the observers to judge which illuminated, binocular brightness appeared to be
stimulus appeared brighter—a bar stimulus with perceptually matched among the conditions in which
variable disparity or another stimulus with zero the linear averages of the two monocular luminance
disparity. For the bar stimulus, the interocular values were equivalent. In contrast, when the luminance
luminance ratio was varied to trace an equal brightness in one eye was much lower than that in the other eye,
curve. Binocular disparity had no effect on luminance the eye with higher luminance determined the binocular
increments presented on a gray or black background. In brightness (winner-take-all), which is consistent with
contrast, when luminance decrements were presented
Fechner’s paradox.
on a gray background, non-zero disparities elevated
points of subjective equality for stimuli with interocular The way of binocular brightness combination
luminance differences. This means that the binocular is known to vary with the luminance polarity and
brightness combination of the two monocular signals the luminance difference between a stimulus and its
shifted from winner-take-all summation toward linear background. According to Anstis and Ho (1998), when
averaging. It has been argued that this effect may be a luminance decrement (a dark spot) was presented
caused by non-zero binocular disparities attenuating on a white background, the binocular brightness
interocular suppression, which is deemed to operate combination took the form of winner-take-all. That
normally with zero disparity. is, a greater decrement in one eye always determined
the binocular brightness without binocular averaging.
Moreover, Baker, Wallis, Georgeson, and Meese (2012)
adopted a gray background, instead of black or white,
Introduction to test the binocular brightness of light and dark spots.
Their results showed that the equal brightness functions
Researchers have examined how visual signals for for luminance increments deviated from the prediction
brightness delivered to the two eyes are combined of linear averaging, being close to winner-take-all. The
to yield brightness perception in binocular viewing, functions for luminance decrements have also exhibited
hereafter referred to as binocular brightness. In their compromised linearity for small decrements. Ding and
review of previous studies, Blake and Fox (1973) argued Levi (2017) reported similar results for a wider range of
that, although viewing with both eyes open is not target and background luminance. In Ding and Levi’s
dramatically different from viewing with only one eye, (2017) model, interocular suppression is assumed to be
sensitive experiments can reveal differences between weak when the total luminance of the stimuli is low and
binocular and monocular brightness. This trend dates the luminance values are balanced between the two eyes,
back to the work of Fechner (1860), who noted that resulting in linear averaging. If the total luminance is at
the binocular brightness of the light spot decreases a medium level, the binocular brightness combination
as the luminance in one eye increases from zero. This is expected to show compromised linearity for both

Citation: Maehara, G., Wang, Y., & Murakami, I. (2024). Effects of binocular disparity on binocular luminance combination. Journal
of Vision, 24(2):4, 1–14, https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.24.2.4.
https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.24.2.4 Received June 7, 2023; published February 20, 2024 ISSN 1534-7362 Copyright 2024 The Authors

This work is
Downloaded from jov.arvojournals.org onlicensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
03/23/2024
Journal of Vision (2024) 24(2):4, 1–14 Maehara, Wang, & Murakami 2

luminance increments and decrements. This model was away from winner-take-all toward averaging. There
recently expanded to explain depth perception (Ding & were three conditions in the luminance excursion (i.e., a
Levi, 2021). generic term to indicate a difference, either incremental
Interestingly, the binocular brightness of Ganzfeld or decremental, in the luminance values between the
stimuli followed linear averaging over the entire stimulus and background) of the comparison stimulus:
range of stimulus luminance, with no indication of a decrement on a gray background, an increment
Fechner’s paradox (Bourassa & Rule, 1994). This on a gray background, and an increment on a black
could be due to a weaker interocular suppression by background. We used a short vertical bar-shaped
Ganzfeld stimuli than by spatially localized stimuli. In stimulus as the comparison stimulus so that it did
contrast, binocular combinations have been reported not stimulate the corresponding retinal positions in
to shift from winner-take-all to linear averaging when the two eyes when presented with non-zero binocular
binocularly matched features are added to stimuli disparity. To avoid a masking effect on the comparison
in dichoptic color-saturation mixtures (Kingdom & stimulus, the standard stimulus was a pair of relatively
Libenson, 2015) and when the luminance contrasts of large rectangles presented within a region outside
surroundings are the same for both eyes in dichoptic the location of the comparison stimulus. Binocular
contrast mixtures (Wang, Ding, Levi, & Cooper, 2022). disparities within the fusion limit were selected
This evidence suggests that suppression of the eye with because preliminary observers had difficulty judging
the lower contrast by the eye with the higher contrast the brightness in double vision when the luminance
results in winner-take-all behavior. Binocularly matched excursions differed between the eyes.
features would keep the interocular suppression
balanced between the two eyes, resulting in a shift of the
binocular combination toward linear averaging. This
notion is supported by studies showing that binocularly Methods
matched features reduce the effects of dichoptic
masking (Jennings & Kingdom, 2019; Kingdom & Observers
Wang, 2015).
The aforementioned studies focused on situations Four adults naïve to the purpose of the experiment
in which binocular stimuli were presented at the and two authors (observers 5 and 6) participated
corresponding retinal positions. Recently, Maehara in Experiment 1. Four different naïve adults and
and Murakami (2020) reported that suprathreshold the two authors participated in Experiment 2. All
grating stimuli with non-zero binocular disparity observers had normal or corrected-to-normal visual
appeared to have a higher luminance contrast than acuity with normal stereopsis and provided written
those with zero binocular disparity. As simultaneous informed consent. The present study followed protocols
stimulations at corresponding retinal points are known approved by the institutional ethics committee and was
to elicit strong interocular suppression (Blake, 1989; in accordance with the tenets of the Declaration of
for an opposing view, see a dynamical model proposed Helsinki.
by Said & Heeger, 2013), the authors attributed
the perceptual enhancement of luminance contrast
to weaker interocular suppression in stimuli with Apparatus
binocular disparities. In contrast, binocular summation
is assumed to be functional even for stimuli with In a dimly lit room, a ViSaGe MKII Stimulus
binocular disparity, because the same mechanisms Generator (Cambridge Research Systems, Rochester,
may underlie binocular summation and stereopsis UK) with a 14-bit gray-level resolution was used
(Kingdom, Yared, Hibbard, & May, 2020; Lema & to present the stimuli on a CRT video monitor
Blake, 1977; May & Zhaoping, 2022; Rose, Blake, & (RDF223H; Mitsubishi, Tokyo, Japan). The display
Halpern, 1988). resolution was 800 × 600 pixels, with a refresh rate of
The present study aimed to examine the effects 100 Hz. The observers viewed the screen using a mirror
of binocular disparity on the binocular brightness stereoscope (Chuo Precision Industrial, Tokyo, Japan).
combination of luminance increments and decrements. The presentation area subtended a 13° × 13° arc in each
We measured the point of subjective equality (PSE) eye. The viewing distance was 57 cm.
in brightness between a standard stimulus with zero
disparity and a comparison stimulus with a certain
interocular excursion ratio and a zero, crossed, or Stimuli
uncrossed binocular disparity. If binocular disparity
caused weaker interocular suppression, then the There were three excursion conditions: a decrement
binocular brightness combination for a stimulus with on a gray background, an increment on a gray
non-zero as opposed to zero disparity would be shifted background, and an increment on a black background.

Downloaded from jov.arvojournals.org on 03/23/2024


Journal of Vision (2024) 24(2):4, 1–14 Maehara, Wang, & Murakami 3

The gray and black backgrounds had luminance values Left Eye Right Eye
of 35.1 cd/m2 and 0.2 cd/m2 , respectively. A square
frame subtending 9.9° × 9.9° was presented to each (a)
eye to aid binocular fusion (Figure 1). A fixation dot
was presented at the center of the presentation area
with two flanking dots positioned 1.5° to the left and
right (Figure 1a). Nonius dots were also presented 1.5°
above and below the fixation dot to the left and right
eyes, respectively. The luminance of these dots was 21.0
cd/m2 higher than the background luminance.
The standard stimulus was a pair of rectangles each
subtending 9.9° × 1.0° (Figures 1b and 1c). The stimuli (b)
were presented 3.0° above and below the fixation dot.
In Experiment 1, their luminance was 14 cd/m2 lower
in the luminance decrement condition (Figure 1b) and
14 cd/m2 higher in the luminance increment conditions
(Figure 1c) than the background luminance. In
Experiment 2, the luminance of the standard stimulus
was 21 cd/m2 lower than the background luminance.
As a comparison stimulus, a vertical bar subtending
0.2° × 1.1° was presented at the center (Figures 1d
and 1e). In Experiment 1, the excursion levels, either (c)
decremental or incremental, of the comparison stimuli
were chosen as 8.8, 10, 11.4, 13.1, 15, 17.2, and
19.7 cd/m2 except that, for one of the observers, the
excursion range had to be shifted to 10 to 22.5 cd/m2
for the luminance increment on a gray background
after the first session. In Experiment 1, three interocular
excursion ratios were chosen for the comparison
stimulus. In the equal-ratio (L:R = 1:1) condition, the
images of the left and right eyes had the same excursion. (d)
In the dichoptic conditions (L:R = 1:0.5 or 0.5:1), the
excursion in one eye was half the “base” excursion value
(i.e., one of the above-described excursion levels). In
Experiment 2, the excursion levels of the comparison
stimuli were 13.2, 15.1, 17.2, 19.7, 22.5, 25.8, and
29.6 cd/m2 , and five interocular excursion ratios (1:1,
1:0.75, 1:0.5, 0.75:1, and 0.5:1) were selected for the
comparison stimuli.
The binocular disparity of the comparison stimuli (e)
had three levels: zero (0°), crossed (−0.2°), and
uncrossed (0.2°) disparities. In the zero-disparity
condition, monocular images were displayed at the
center. The images were horizontally shifted by 0.1° in
opposite directions for the left and right eyes to create
non-zero disparities.

Procedures Figure 1. Stimulus examples. (a) A fixation dot and Nonius dots.
(b) The standard stimulus for the luminance decrement on a
The observers pressed a key to start each trial when gray background. (c) The standard stimulus for the luminance
the Nonius and fixation dots appeared to be aligned increment on a gray background. (d) The comparison stimulus
along the vertical meridian. After a 400-ms blank, the for the luminance decrement on a gray background. (e) The
standard stimulus was presented for 200 ms. After comparison stimulus for the luminance increment on a black
another 600-ms blank, the comparison stimulus was background.
presented for 200 ms. The fixation dot disappeared
100 ms before the onset of the comparison stimulus

Downloaded from jov.arvojournals.org on 03/23/2024


Journal of Vision (2024) 24(2):4, 1–14 Maehara, Wang, & Murakami 4

but the other dots remained. The duration of each


(a) Decrement on Gray BG stimulus presentation was too short to evoke vergence in
20 response to a disparate stimulus. The observers judged
Zero Cross. Uncross. which stimulus was darker in the luminance-decrement
condition or brighter in the luminance-increment
15 condition.
In Experiment 1, each experimental block had 189
trials, consisting of three repetitive trials for each
10 combination of three binocular disparities (zero,
L L L R R R crossed, and uncrossed), three interocular excursion
PSE of the Comparison Stimulus Matched to the Standard Stimulus (cd/m²)

ratios (1:1, 1:0.5, and 0.5:1), and seven excursion levels


5
(see above). The order of the trials was randomized
within a block. The observers rested after completing
0 a block and adapted to the background luminance
for 30 seconds before starting each block. During
1:1 1:0.5 0.5:1
this adaptation period, the standard stimulus of the
impending block was presented for 200 ms every 3
seconds. The observers completed four blocks in a
(b) Increment on Gray BG
20 session devoted to one of three excursion conditions
(a decrement on a gray background, an increment
on a gray background, and an increment on a black
15 background). Three sessions were conducted for each
excursion condition; thus, 6804 (189 × 4 × 3 × 3) trials
were conducted for each observer. The order of the
10 sessions was counterbalanced among the observers.
L L L R R R In Experiment 2, each experimental block had
210 trials consisting of two repetitive trials for each
5 combination of the three binocular disparities (zero,
crossed, and uncrossed), five interocular excursion
ratios (1:1, 1:0.75, 1:0.5, 0.75:1, and 0.5:1), and seven
0 excursion levels (see above). The observers completed
1:1 1:0.5 0.5:1 four blocks per session. Four sessions were conducted
for a luminance decrement on a gray background; thus,
3360 (210 × 4 × 4) trials were conducted for each
(c) Increment on Black BG observer. All other procedures were the same as those
20
in Experiment 1.
We determined the brightness PSEs and 95%
15 confidence intervals (CIs) of the comparison stimuli
that should be perceptual matches to the standard
stimuli by fitting the log-Quick function to the data
10 using the Palamedes toolbox (Kingdom & Prins,
2016). There were 252 trials for each fit (36 trials × 7
L L L R R R excursion levels) in Experiment 1 and 224 trials (32 ×
5 7 excursion levels) in Experiment 2. PSE was defined
as the excursion corresponding to a 50% probability

0
1:1 1:0.5 0.5:1

Interocular Excursion Ratio the standard stimulus that was fixed across all conditions. The
of the Comparison Stimulus larger excursions (base PSEs) are plotted in the dichoptic
conditions (1:0.5 and 0.5:1). The letters L and R in the panels
Figure 2. Mean brightness PSEs in three binocular disparity indicate the eye (left and right) that received the designated
conditions and three interocular excursion ratios. The ordinate excursion. The error bars indicate the 95% CIs of the mean PSEs.
indicates the excursion (i.e., the absolute value of (a) Results for the luminance decrement on a gray background.
increment/decrement from the background luminance) (b) Results for the luminance increment on a gray background.
required to establish the brightness match to the brightness of (c) Results for the luminance increment on a black background.

Downloaded from jov.arvojournals.org on 03/23/2024


Journal of Vision (2024) 24(2):4, 1–14 Maehara, Wang, & Murakami 5

of judging the comparison stimulus as having greater of binocular disparity on the binocular luminance
excursion than the standard stimulus. combination.
For the luminance decrement on a gray background
(Figure 2a), the main effect of interocular excursion
ratio was significant, F(2, 10) = 40.6, p < 0.001, ηp 2
Results of Experiment 1 = 0.890. Between the two bilaterally symmetrical
conditions for unequal ratios (1:0.5 and 0.5:1), the
Comparisons of PSEs base PSEs were expected to be comparable. Thus, the
significant main effect presumably reflects the result
Figure 2 shows the mean PSEs for the three binocular that the base PSE was smaller for the equal (1:1) ratio
disparities (zero, crossed, and uncrossed) and the (12.3 cd/m2 ) than for the unequal ratios (14.6 and 13.7
three interocular excursion ratios (L:R = 1:1, 1:0.5, cd/m2 for the 1:0.5 and 0.5:1 ratios, respectively). The
and 0.5:1) plotted separately for the three excursion two-way ANOVA also revealed a significant main effect
conditions. When the excursions were unequal between of binocular disparity, F(2, 10) = 11.2, p = 0.003, ηp 2
the eyes, a larger excursion, hereafter referred to as the = 0.691, and a significant interaction, F(4, 20) = 3.45,
base PSE, was plotted. When the luminance excursions p = 0.027, ηp 2 = 0.409. According to Dunnett’s test
were equal (1:1) for both eyes, the PSE was expected to to clarify the effect of binocular disparity, the crossed
be approximately 14 cd/m2 , which was the luminance disparity yielded a significant elevation in the base PSEs
excursion of the standard stimulus. The actual results in the dichoptic conditions (1:0.5 and 0.5:1) but not
were roughly consistent with the predictions. As in the equal ratio (1:1) condition (p > 0.05). The 95%
shown in Figure 2, the PSE for the equal ratio (1:1) CIs of the PSE elevations ranged from 0.230 to 1.80
was approximately 12 to 13 cd/m2 . Although these cd/m2 for the 1:0.5 ratio (p = 0.014) and from 0.320 to
measurements were slightly lower than the standard 1.81 cd/m2 for the 0.5:1 ratio (p = 0.008). However, the
excursion, this was possibly because the comparison uncrossed disparity showed no significant effect (p >
stimulus was presented near the fovea, whereas the 0.05).
standard stimulus was presented within the parafovea For the luminance increment on a gray background
(3.0° of eccentricity). (Figure 2b), the two-way ANOVA yielded a significant
If the binocular brightness combination was main effect of interocular excursion ratio, F(2, 10) =
winner-take-all, then the lengths of the bars would 88.7, p < 0.001, ηp 2 = 0.947, indicating that the base
be the same regardless of the interocular excursion PSE was smaller for the equal (1:1) ratio (12.9 cd/m2 )
ratio. However, Figure 2 suggests the contrary because than for the unequal ratios (16.6 and 15.7 cd/m2 for the
the bars are generally longer for unequal ratios 1:0.5 and 0.5:1 ratios, respectively). The main effect of
(1:0.5 and 0.5:1) than for the equal ratio (1:1). In binocular disparity was not statistically significant (p
contrast, if the binocular brightness combination > 0.05). The interaction between the two factors was
was a linear average, then the length of the bars significant, F(4, 20) = 4.81, p = 0.007, ηp 2 = 0.490.
would be 4/3 times longer for the unequal ratios However, in Dunnett’s multiple-comparison tests,
than for the equal ratio. This prediction was more neither crossed nor uncrossed disparities produced
or less applicable to the luminance increment on a significant changes in the base PSEs from the PSE under
black background (Figure 2c). On the other hand, the control condition (zero disparity) in any of the
for the luminance decrement on a gray background interocular excursion ratios (p > 0.05). The significant
(Figure 2a), the bars in the unequal ratios (1:0.5 and interaction may reflect some ratio-dependent differences
0.5:1) did not support this linear-average prediction. between the crossed and uncrossed disparities, the
The data in the luminance increment on a gray origin of which could not be identified.
background were somewhat intermediate (Figure 2b). For the luminance increment on a black background
Similar trends were observed in the individual (Figure 2c), the main effect of interocular excursion
data, as shown in Figure 3 for two representative ratio was significant, F(2, 10) = 53.1, p < 0.001, ηp 2
observers. = 0.914. In the same way as the conditions described
We subjected the base PSE values to a two-way above, the base PSE was smaller for the equal (1:1) ratio
repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) (11.7 cd/m2 ) than for the unequal ratios (15.7 and 14.8
with the interocular excursion ratio (1:1, 1:0.5, or cd/m2 for the 1:0.5 and 0.5:1 ratios, respectively). The
0.5:1) and binocular disparity (zero, crossed, or main effects of disparity and the interaction were not
uncrossed) as factors. In addition to the two-way significant (p > 0.05). Dunnett’s multiple-comparison
ANOVAs, we conducted Dunnett’s multiple- test showed no significant effect of binocular disparity
comparison tests for each interocular excursion ratio. (p > 0.05).
In this multiple comparison, the base PSE at zero Because the standard and comparison stimuli had
binocular disparity was considered as the control different shapes and appeared in a fixed temporal
condition because our main concern was the effect order, the observers were always able to distinguish

Downloaded from jov.arvojournals.org on 03/23/2024


Journal of Vision (2024) 24(2):4, 1–14 Maehara, Wang, & Murakami 6

Observer 1 Observer 2

(a) Decrement on Gray BG


20 Zero Cross. Uncross.

15
PSE of the Comparison Stimulus Matched to the Standard Stimulus (cd/m²)

10
L L L R R R L L L R R R
5

0
1:1 1:0.5 0.5:1 1:1 1:0.5 0.5:1

(b) Increment on Gray BG


20

15

10
L L L R R R L L L R R R
5

0
1:1 1:0.5 0.5:1 1:1 1:0.5 0.5:1

(c) Increment on Black BG


20

15

10
L L L R R R L L L R R R
5

0
1:1 1:0.5 0.5:1 1:1 1:0.5 0.5:1

Interocular Excursion Ratio of the Comparison Stimulus

Figure 3. Brightness PSEs for two representative observers plotted separately in the left and right panels. The error bars indicate the
95% CIs of the estimated PSEs. (a) Results for the luminance decrement on a gray background. (b) Results for the luminance
increment on a gray background. (c) Results for the luminance increment on a black background.

those stimuli; thus, response bias could have caused standard deviation of PSE was only 1.21 cd/m2 for the
an overall shift of PSEs in individual data. However, decremental stimuli with zero disparity. The mean PSEs
this type of constant error is supposed to influence the at the equal ratio (1:1) were also comparable with the
measurements equally among different conditions and standard excursion, as described above. Taken together,
therefore is orthogonal to our experimental purpose. it is unlikely that response bias artificially created the
In addition, the six observers’ PSEs were distributed observed differences across conditions in the present
within a limited range of excursion; for example, the experiment.

Downloaded from jov.arvojournals.org on 03/23/2024


Journal of Vision (2024) 24(2):4, 1–14 Maehara, Wang, & Murakami 7

Model fitting then divided by the denominator expression. Following


the division, two monocular responses are summed to
Kingdom and Libenson (2015) used a binocular yield binocular brightness (B).
processing model to describe the relationship between The weighting parameter, w, is important for the
binocular color-saturation perception and dichoptic analysis of binocular luminance combinations. If the
stimuli with an identical hue but different saturation weighting parameter is approximately 1, then the raised
values. We used their model to visualize the effects signals from the two eyes are simply summed in the
of binocular disparity on binocular brightness denominator expressions. Because this process works
combination. The model is based on a model of as interocular suppression, the eye receiving the higher
contrast normalization proposed by Legge and Foley excursion predominates the binocular brightness when
(1980) and its extensions for binocular processing the luminance is unbalanced between the two eyes. On
(Ding & Sperling, 2006; Maehara & Goryo, 2005; the other hand, if the weighting parameter is near 0,
Meese, Georgeson, & Baker, 2006). Although Ding then each monocular response is virtually free from
and Levi (2017) recently proposed a binocular the divisive suppression from the other eye. Therefore,
processing model that can explain various aspects of binocular brightness is determined based on the linear
binocular perception, including binocular brightness summation of the two monocular responses. See
combinations, its computations are much more complex Kingdom and Libenson (2015) for more details about
than the model used by Kingdom and Libenson (2015). the weighting parameters.
Therefore, for simplicity, we adopted their model as a We fit this descriptive model to the data using a
descriptive model for the present data, which can be procedure similar to that of Kingdom and Libenson
expressed by the following equation: (2015). Each PSE was normalized to the PSE at an equal
ratio (1:1). For example, if the luminance excursions in
CLp CRp the left and right eyes are 15 and 7.5 cd/m2 , respectively,
B= +
z + CLq + wCRq z + CRq + wCLq at the PSE for an unequal ratio (1:0.5) and if the PSE
for the equal ratio is 12 cd/m2 , then their normalized
The right side of the equation consists of two terms values are 1.25 and 0.625, respectively. The normalized
that represent two monocular responses. CL and CR luminance excursions in the two eyes, L and R, are at
are the luminance excursions in the left and right eyes, the PSE when the binocular brightness (B) equals 1.
respectively. These monocular inputs are first raised An equal brightness curve, which corresponds to the
to the power of p for the numerator and q for the family of PSEs to the standard stimulus predicted by
denominator. The raised signals from the two eyes and the model, can be plotted on the plane in which the x-
a constant (z) are summed in denominators. Before and y-axes indicate normalized luminance in the left
this summation, the raised signal from the other eye is and right eyes, respectively. The model prediction was
multiplied by a weighting parameter (w) that controls constrained to pass through the PSE for an equal ratio
how much one eye is suppressed by the other eye. The because its normalized value was one by definition
numerator expression in each monocular response is and a constant z was fixed to be near zero. Therefore,

wzero wcross wuncross RMSEzero RMSEcross RMSEuncross

Decrement on a Gray Background


Mean 0.780 0.631 0.675 0.0573 0.0531 0.0291
95% CI
Upper limit 0.985 0.689 0.785
Lower limit 0.575 0.573 0.565
Increment on a Gray Background
Mean 0.464 0.440 0.406 0.0550 0.0677 0.0251
95% CI
Upper limit 0.588 0.535 0.496
Lower limit 0.340 0.345 0.317
Increment on a Black Background
Mean 0.246 0.260 0.245 0.0814 0.0698 0.0654
95% CI
Upper limit 0.324 0.394 0.376
Lower limit 0.169 0.125 0.114
Table 1. Mean w Values, 95% CI Limits, and Mean RMSEs for Experiment 1.

Downloaded from jov.arvojournals.org on 03/23/2024


Journal of Vision (2024) 24(2):4, 1–14 Maehara, Wang, & Murakami 8

each fit aimed to find the curve that ran closest to Table 1 presents the mean w values, their 95% CIs,
the two data points in the dichoptic conditions. The and mean RMSEs. The √ mean RMSEs were reasonably
model contained four parameters, but p, q, and z small compared with 2 of the radial distance to the
were fixed at 4.25, 3.0, and 0.001, respectively. These standard value, suggesting that the model fit the data
parameter values are the same as the constants used by well. The smooth curves in the left column of Figure 4
Kingdom and Libenson (2015). We used the MATLAB correspond to the model fits with interobserver mean
(MathWorks, Natick, MA) “fminbnd” function to find w values. In this figure, the normalized luminance was
the w value that minimized the root mean squared error reconverted back to cd/m2 before plotting. The best
(RMSE) between the PSE data and the predicted PSEs fit to the data from the two representative observers is
in the radial direction. The w-value range was set to be 0 shown in the middle and right columns (see Figure A1
to 1.25. in the Appendix for all individual data in the condition

Mean Parameters Observer 1 Observer 2


25 25 25
Decrement Decrement Decrement
20 20 20
Gray BG Gray BG Gray BG
15 15 15

10 10 10
Luminance Excursion in the Right Eye (cd/m²)

5 5 5
n=6
0 0 0
0 5 10 15 20 25 0 5 10 15 20 25 0 5 10 15 20 25

25 25 25
Increment Increment Increment
20 20 20
Gray BG Gray BG Gray BG
15 15 15

10 10 Zero 10

5 5 Cross. 5
n=6 Uncross.
0 0 0
0 5 10 15 20 25 0 5 10 15 20 25 0 5 10 15 20 25

25 25 25
Increment Increment Increment
20 20 20
Black BG Black BG Black BG
15 15 15

10 10 10

5 5 5
n=6
0 0 0
0 5 10 15 20 25 0 5 10 15 20 25 0 5 10 15 20 25

Luminance Excursion in the Left Eye (cd/m²)

Figure 4. Equal brightness curves for the decrement on a gray background (top panels), the increment on a gray background (middle
panels), and the increment on a black background (bottom panels). Smooth curves in the left panels correspond to the model curves
with mean w values for the zero-disparity (blue), crossed-disparity (red), and uncrossed-disparity (yellow) conditions. The blue dotted
lines represent the winner-take-all prediction (vertical and horizontal lines) and the linear-averaging prediction (diagonal line) in the
zero-disparity condition. The central and right panels show the fit to the individual data of two representative observers. Symbols in
the figure represent the PSEs. The error bars indicate the 95% CIs of the estimated PSEs. Most error bars are smaller than the symbols.

Downloaded from jov.arvojournals.org on 03/23/2024


Journal of Vision (2024) 24(2):4, 1–14 Maehara, Wang, & Murakami 9

with the decrement on a gray background). The equal 30 30


brightness curves had greater curvatures, especially 25 Observer 5 25 Observer 6
at the center, for the luminance decrement on a gray 20 20
background (top row), compared to the conditions
15 15
of the luminance increments (middle and bottom Zero

Luminance Excursion in the Right Eye (cd/m²)


rows). The curves show a quasilinear function for 10 10
Cross.
the luminance increment on a black background 5
Uncross. 5
(bottom row). The curves for the luminance increment 0 0
on a gray background were in between (middle 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 0 5 10 15 20 25 30
row).
As noted previously, ANOVAs and Dunnett’s 30 30
multiple comparison tests of PSEs indicate that crossed 25 Observer 7 25 Observer 8
disparity had a significant effect on how the visual 20 20
system integrates luminance decrements presented to
15 15
the left and right eyes. This significant effect can be
observed as a change in the equal brightness curves 10 10
for the luminance decrement on a gray background 5 5
(top rows of Figure 4). The curves appear to be less 0 0
bent for the crossed disparity (red) than for the zero 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 0 5 10 15 20 25 30
disparity (blue). The mean w values reflect this trend
(Table 1a). This was smaller for the crossed disparity 30 30
(0.631) than for the zero disparity (0.780). Although 25 Observer 9 25 Observer 10
we subjected w values to Dunnett’s test, the difference 20 20
between zero and crossed disparity reached a statistical 15 15
significance level of p = 0.064, which is rather marginal.
10 10
However, this may be because three (p, q, and z) of
the four model parameters were fixed at the constants 5 5
used by Kingdom and Libenson (2015) and thus are 0 0
not guaranteed to be optimal for our observers. The 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 0 5 10 15 20 25 30
uncrossed disparity also yielded a smaller exponent Luminance Excursion in the Left Eye (cd/m²)
parameter (0.675) than zero disparity, resulting in
slightly less bent curves (Figure 4, yellow); however,
Figure 5. Equal brightness curves for luminance decrement on a
this trend was not significant in Dunnett’s test. For
gray background in Experiment 2. Each panel shows the fit to
the luminance increments on the gray and black
the individual data. Symbols represent the PSEs. Error bars
backgrounds (Figure 4, middle and bottom rows),
indicate the 95% CIs of the estimated PSEs. Most error bars are
the curves almost overlap. The mean w values were
smaller than the symbols.
comparable across the three binocular disparities under
these conditions (Tables 1b and 1c), indicating that the
binocular disparity had little effect when luminance
increments were presented. in which binocular combination at zero disparity is
apparently winner-take-all. According to Baker et
al. (2012) and Ding and Levi (2017), the binocular
combination of decrements is intermediate between
Results of Experiment 2: Luminance winner-take-all and linear averaging at small excursions
decrement with a larger excursion but gradually gets closer to winner-take-all with
increasing excursion. Therefore, in the next experiment,
In Experiment 1, the crossed disparity produced we used stimuli with a larger decremental excursion
a significant change in the binocular brightness (1.5 times) than those in Experiment 1. The levels of
combination of luminance decrement on a gray interocular excursion ratios were also increased from
background, although there was no significant effect of three to five for more precise estimations of equal
disparity for luminance increment on a gray and black brightness curves.
background. The effect of disparity may depend on Figure 5 illustrates the individual results for the
the pattern of binocular combination; that is, disparity six observers. As expected from the above-mentioned
had no effect on luminance increments because the literature, the equal brightness curves were more bent
binocular combination was already close to linear than those in Experiment 1, indicating that binocular
averaging at zero disparity. If this was the case, then the brightness was determined in a winner-take-all manner.
effect of disparity might become larger in a situation However, observer 10 showed a somewhat different

Downloaded from jov.arvojournals.org on 03/23/2024


Journal of Vision (2024) 24(2):4, 1–14 Maehara, Wang, & Murakami 10

wzero wcross wuncross RMSEzero RMSEcross RMSEuncross

Mean 1.15 0.916 0.904 0.0427 0.0285 0.0213


95% CI
Upper limit 1.26 1.18 1.11
Lower limit 1.04 0.656 0.700
Table 2. Mean w Values, 95% CI Limits, and Mean RMSEs for Decrement on a Gray Background in Experiment 2.

pattern (see the bottom-right panel in Figure 5). Even no significant elevation in PSEs (p > 0.05), but the
when the excursion was larger for the left eye than for base PSEs were significantly lower for the uncrossed
the right eye, binocular combination suggested a strong disparity (18.8 cd/m2 ) than for zero disparity (19.6
dominance of the right eye. Due to these asymmetrical cd/m2 ) at the equal ratio (1:1). The 95% CIs of the
results between the two eyes, the model failed to fit the PSE reduction ranged from 0.152 to 1.37 cd/m2 (p =
data for observer 10. In addition, observer 10 exhibited 0.018).
very large 95% CIs (error bars in Figure 5) compared The crossed disparity caused some elevation of PSEs
with those for the other observers. Notably, despite the in the dichoptic conditions but not in the equal-ratio
principle that the range should ideally be spanning 0% condition, resulting in the equal brightness curve
to 100% to derive a reliable sigmoidal psychometric being less bent for the crossed disparity than for zero
function by best-fitting, inspection of the raw data disparity. In contrast, the uncrossed disparity caused
confirmed that in only five of the 15 conditions did the the reduction of PSEs at the equal ratio but not in
response rates in the method of constant stimuli span the dichoptic conditions. This result is also consistent
<25% to >75%. Due to these indications of atypical with the shape change in the equal brightness curve in
binocular vision and poor brightness discriminability, a manner similar to the above-described case of the
we excluded observer 10 from the following crossed disparity. The aforementioned shape changes
analysis. from winner-take-all to slightly less curvatures at
non-zero disparities can be seen in Figure 5, especially
in the curves of observers 5 and 9.
Comparisons of PSEs
We subjected the base PSE values to a two-way Model fitting
repeated-measures ANOVA with the interocular
excursion ratio (1:1, 1:0.75, 1:0.5, 0.75:1, or 0.5:1) and We fit the model to the PSE data to visualize the
binocular disparity (zero, crossed, or uncrossed) as changes in equal brightness curves using the same
factors. The base PSEs significantly differed across procedure as that in Experiment 1. Table 2 presents
interocular excursion ratios, F(4, 16) = 7.09, p < 0.001, the mean w values, their 95% CIs, and mean RMSEs.
ηp 2 = 0.639. This significant main effect indicates The mean RMSEs were comparable to those in
that the base PSE was smaller for the equal (1:1) Experiment 1 and reasonably small. The smooth curves
ratio (19.1 cd/m2 ) than for the dichoptic conditions in Figure 5 are the fits to the individual data. Figure 6
(20.4, 20.3, 20.2, and 20.2 cd/m2 for the 1:0.75, 1:0.5, shows the model curves with the interobserver mean
0.75:1, and 0.5:1 ratios, respectively). Although the w values. Consistent with the results of previous
main effect of binocular disparity was not significant studies (Baker et al., 2012; Ding & Levi, 2017), the
(p > 0.05), the two factors significantly interacted, curvatures were greater in Experiment 2 (Figure 6) than
F(8, 32) = 3.59, p < 0.001, ηp 2 = 0.473. To clarify in Experiment 1 (Figure 4). Dunnett’s test confirmed
the interaction, we conducted Dunnett’s multiple that the crossed and uncrossed disparities produced
comparison test for each interocular ratio. The base smaller w values (0.916 and 0.904, respectively) than
PSEs were significantly higher for the crossed disparity those in the zero-disparity condition (1.15). The 95%
than for zero disparity at the interocular excursion CIs of the difference ranged from 0.0841 to 0.381 for
ratios of 0.5:1 (20.7 cd/m2 vs. 19.6 cd/m2 ) and 0.75:1 the crossed disparity (p = 0.006) and from 0.0957 to
(20.6 cd/m2 vs. 19.7 cd/m2 ). The 95% CIs of the 0.393 for the uncrossed disparity (p = 0.004). Because
PSE elevations ranged from 0.410 to 1.75 cd/m2 of the reduction in w values, the curvatures were less
for the 0.5:1 ratio (p = 0.005) and from 0.758 to bent for the crossed and uncrossed disparities than for
1.56 cd/m2 for the 0.75:1 ratio (p = 0.033). These zero disparity (Figures 5 and 6). These results indicate
elevations in the PSEs are consistent with the results that, in the situation of luminance decrement with
of Experiment 1, although there was no significant a larger excursion, binocular disparities weakened
difference at the interocular excursion ratios of 1:0.5 interocular suppression, and consequently the binocular
and 1:0.75 (p > 0.05). The uncrossed disparity produced combination was less of a winner-take-all scenario.

Downloaded from jov.arvojournals.org on 03/23/2024


Journal of Vision (2024) 24(2):4, 1–14 Maehara, Wang, & Murakami 11

30 2012; Ding & Levi, 2017; Engel, 1970; Levelt, 1965). The
in the Right Eye (cd/m²)
reduction in interocular suppression due to non-zero
Luminance Excursion
25 n=5 disparities may be effective only when the binocular
brightness combination for stimuli with zero disparity
20 is strongly nonlinear in the first place. According to
Baker et al. (2012), the effect of luminance excursion is
15 asymmetrical between increments and decrements. In
Zero their results, the equal brightness curves for decrements
10 became slightly shifted toward linear averaging as the
Cross. luminance excursion decreased, whereas the curves for
5 increments on a gray background did not change. The
Uncross. effects of excursion level on decrements could also be
0 due to the reduction in interocular suppression because
0 5 10 15 20 25 30
interocular suppression is generally considered to
Luminance Excursion weaken as the luminance contrast of stimuli decreases
in the Left Eye (cd/m²) (Maehara, Huang, & Hess, 2009; Schor & Heckmann,
1989). That is, the results of Baker et al. (2012) are
Figure 6. The model fits with mean w values for the arguably consistent with the present results in that
zero-disparity (blue), crossed-disparity (red), and the reduction of interocular suppression is a viable
uncrossed-disparity (yellow) conditions in Experiment 2. The idea in both cases to explain the reduction of strong
blue dotted lines represent the winner-take-all prediction nonlinearity in the binocular combination of luminance
(vertical and horizontal lines) and the linear-averaging decrements.
prediction (diagonal line) in the zero-disparity condition. Moreover, the luminance contrasts of the stimuli
might have caused the difference between the results of
luminance decrements and increments. In Experiment 1,
we equated Weber contrasts between the decrement
Discussion and increment on a gray background. Consequently,
the Michelson contrast, (Lmax − Lmin )/(Lmax + Lmin ),
The present study examined how binocular disparity of the decremental standard (25%) was higher than that
changed binocular brightness combination patterns. of the incremental standard (17%). Because interocular
The results of this study were consistent with those of suppression is assumed to be greater for stimuli with
previous studies in that luminance decrements in the higher contrasts, the effects of binocular disparity
left and right eyes were combined in a winner-take-all could increase for decrements more than for increments
fashion, whereas the combination of luminance due to the difference in the Michelson contrast.
increments was close to linear averaging when the Nonetheless, according to Ding and Levi (2017), who
luminance values in the two eyes were not very different examined binocular combinations within a wider range
(Anstis & Ho, 1998; Baker et al., 2012; Ding & Levi, of luminance excursions, incremental stimuli show little
2017). Our main finding was that the binocular change in the equal brightness curve among different
brightness combination slightly deviated from the excursions when background luminance is sufficiently
winner-take-all prediction when a luminance decrement high (16.2 cd/m2 ). In light of their finding, we expect
was accompanied by a crossed disparity (Experiment 1) that the present results of the incremental conditions
or by a crossed or uncrossed disparity (Experiment 2). would not change dramatically even if the luminance
In contrast, for luminance increments, binocular contrast was increased. Future investigations with a
disparity had no effect on the binocular brightness wider range of excursions with disparity manipulations
combination. will resolve this interesting issue.
It was expected that the presence of binocular Lu and Sperling (2012) summarized psychophysical
disparity would cause binocular brightness studies concerning the asymmetry between luminance
combinations to approach linear averaging if stimuli increments and decrements by proposing that
with non-zero binocular disparity weakened interocular black (decremental) stimuli are represented with
suppression. However, the effect of binocular disparity larger magnitudes in the visual system than white
was significant only for a luminance decrement and (incremental) stimuli when they have the same
not for increments. Previous studies have reported that physical intensity in virtually all asymmetric cases.
the monocular luminance values of decrements are The asymmetry between luminance increments and
combined in a nonlinear fashion (that is, winner-take- decrements may reflect the characteristics of natural
all), whereas those of increments are combined in a images. Based on natural image statistics, researchers
linear fashion, except for the range corresponding to have suggested that retinal images contain a greater
Fechner’s paradox (Anstis & Ho, 1998; Baker et al., number of dark features than bright features (Cooper

Downloaded from jov.arvojournals.org on 03/23/2024


Journal of Vision (2024) 24(2):4, 1–14 Maehara, Wang, & Murakami 12

& Norcia, 2015; Ratliff, Borghuis, Kao, Sterling, & to add a high-contrast rectangular contour to support
Balasubramanian, 2010). Notably, one study suggested binocular fusion of our comparison stimulus, whereas
that non-zero binocular disparities appear more the luminance inside the contour varies for each eye.
frequently in dark features than in bright features in This will enable us to examine the effects of binocular
natural images (Cooper & Norcia, 2015). Therefore, it is disparity under conditions of greater luminance
possible that our visual system represents dark features differences between the eyes. However, in such cases,
more dominantly than bright features during binocular the luminance of the contour can affect the brightness
disparity processing. This asymmetry may have caused perception of the interior. In addition, a process of
a difference in the effects of binocular disparity between perceptual filling-in may be triggered by the spatial
luminance increments and decrements. Cooper and profiles of luminance around the contour. Therefore,
Norcia (2015) reported that dark features were more elucidating the effects of contours on the binocular
prominent at lower spatial frequencies. In our study, the brightness combination requires future studies.
observers had to judge the brightness of the surfaces but Maehara and Murakami (2020) reported that
not the luminance contrast of the edges. Judgment of perceived contrast was approximately 1.1 times higher
the former depends on information from components at 1° binocular disparity than at zero disparity. If this
with relatively low spatial frequencies. This may be perceptual enhancement had occurred, the position
another reason why the effect of binocular disparity of the equal brightness curves would shrink in the
was significant only for the luminance decrement. lower-left direction when the stimuli had a non-zero
The effect of binocular disparity was significant binocular disparity. However, this effect was not
for crossed disparity but not for uncrossed disparity observed in Experiment 1 (Figure 4) and was smaller in
in Experiment 1, although both types of disparity Experiment 2 (Figure 6) than that reported by Maehara
produced significant effects in Experiment 2. Various and Murakami (2020). The absence or reduction of
studies have demonstrated separate mechanisms that perceptual enhancement may be owing to differences in
selectively process crossed and uncrossed disparities the range of binocular disparities. We presented a short,
(see for a review, see Mustillo 1985). The results of thin bar as a stimulus for the binocular brightness
Experiment 1 presumably reflected the differential combination and set its binocular disparity within the
characteristics of these mechanisms. Manning, Finlay, fusion limit. This is because brightness judgment in
Neill, and Frost (1987) found that the duration double vision is difficult under dichoptic conditions.
threshold for detection was substantially longer for In contrast, the Gabor patches used in the study by
stimuli with uncrossed disparity than for those with Maehara and Murakami (2020) presented disparities
crossed disparity. Patterson et al. (1995) also showed over the fusion limit, but within the stereopsis limit.
that a longer duration was required to perceive depth Another possible explanation for the above discrepancy
from an uncrossed disparity than from a crossed is that binocular luster is involved in the perceptual
disparity. These studies suggest that the mechanism enhancement. Previous studies have suggested that
for crossed disparity is more sensitive than that its occurrence depends on stimulus size (for a review,
for uncrossed disparity. Although our stimuli were see Wendt & Faul, 2022). The size of the comparison
suprathreshold and 100% detectable, they are only stimulus in the present study was arguably smaller
briefly presented. The stimuli with crossed disparity than the lower limit of binocular luster, whereas the
might have elicited a larger neural response than those Gabor patches used in the study by Maehara and
with uncrossed disparity in the present experiment, Murakami (2020) were sufficiently large. Nonetheless,
causing a difference in the effectiveness of the binocular the perceptual enhancement observed in their study
brightness combination in Experiment 1. was also vigorous when the left-eye and right-eye
In the present experiments, the largest excursion images were vertically disparate with virtually no
difference between the two eyes was found in the spatial overlap, which is an unfavorable situation for
excursion ratios of 1:0.5 and 0.5:1. If we could measure luster.
PSEs in even greater unbalanced ratios, we could
make a more precise evaluation of the nonlinearity
of equal-brightness data. We tested the interocular
excursion ratios of L:R = 1:0.25 and 0.25:1 in a Conclusions
preliminary experiment. When non-zero disparities
were added, however, observers were not able to fuse We found that, although the binocular brightness
the stimuli as in cyclopean vision but instead saw combination of the luminance decrement tended
only two dichoptic images, one brighter and the other to be nonlinear and more like winner-take-all, the
darker, making it difficult to perform the brightness combination became less of a winner-take-all scenario
task as in the main experiment. Therefore, our PSE when presented with non-zero disparity. This shift
measurements had to be limited to range between 1:0.5 in binocular combination is possibly due to weaker
and 0.5:1 cases. One possible extension of the stimuli is interocular suppression caused by non-zero binocular

Downloaded from jov.arvojournals.org on 03/23/2024


Journal of Vision (2024) 24(2):4, 1–14 Maehara, Wang, & Murakami 13

disparity. This is presumably because simultaneous Baker, D. H., Wallis, S. A., Georgeson, M. A., &
stimulations at corresponding retinal points are required Meese, T. S. (2012). Nonlinearities in the binocular
for strong suppression. Another possible mechanism combination of luminance and contrast. Vision
is that stereopsis, which is processed when disparity is Research, 56, 1–9.
signaled, inhibits interocular suppression to avoid eye Blake, R. (1989). A neural theory of binocular rivalry.
rivalry and to promote binocular fusion, resulting in the Psychological Review, 96(1), 145.
maximal use of precise position information originating
Blake, R., & Fox, R. (1973). The psychophysical
from two monocular signals. The mechanism of
inquiry into binocular summation. Perception &
attenuation of the interocular suppression we found still
Psychophysics, 14(1), 161–185.
remains unclear, and we argue that one of the possible
extensions would be to introduce vertical disparity to Bourassa, C. M., & Rule, S. J. (1994). Binocular
ascertain whether the attenuation requires the process brightness: A suppression-summation trade off.
for stereopsis specifically or just any misalignment from Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology,
the corresponding retinal points. 48(3), 418–434.
Previous studies have suggested that interocular Cooper, E. A., & Norcia, A. M. (2015). Predicting
suppression is reduced in Ganzfeld stimuli (Bourassa cortical dark/bright asymmetries from natural
& Rule, 1994) and balanced between the two eyes in image statistics and early visual transforms. PLoS
binocularly matched features (Jennings & Kingdom, Computational Biology, 11(5), e1004268.
2019; Kingdom & Libenson, 2015; Kingdom & Ding, J., & Levi, D. M. (2017). Binocular combination
Wang, 2015; Wang et al., 2022). These techniques of luminance profiles. Journal of Vision, 17(13):4,
could be effective tools for training binocular vision 1–32, https://doi.org/10.1167/17.13.4.
in stereoblind observers or patients with amblyopia.
Ding, J., & Levi, D. M. (2021). A unified model for
Here, we suggest that binocular disparity manipulation
binocular fusion and depth perception. Vision
potentially offers another technique to make both eyes
Research, 180, 11–36.
functional. However, the effects of binocular disparity
were small and limited to a luminance decrement in Ding, J., & Sperling, G. (2006). A gain-control theory of
the present experiment. For such training purposes, binocular combination. Proceedings of the National
future studies are required to determine the optimal Academy of Sciences, USA, 103(4), 1141–1146.
stimulus conditions to elicit the largest attenuation Engel, G. (1970). Tests of a model of binocular
effect on interocular suppression in the domain of brightness. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 24(5),
brightness. 335–352.
Keywords: brightness, stereopsis, interocular Fechner, G. T. (1860). Elemente der Psychophysik.
suppression, binocular perception Leipzig, Germany: Breitkopf & Härtel.
Jennings, B. J., & Kingdom, F. A. A. (2019). Unmasking
the dichoptic mask. Journal of Vision, 19(6):3, 1–8,
https://doi.org/10.1167/19.6.3.
Acknowledgments Kingdom, F. A. A., & Libenson, L. (2015). Dichoptic
color saturation mixture: Binocular luminance
IM was supported by a JSPS KAKENHI contrast promotes perceptual averaging. Journal of
Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (21H04426). Vision, 15(5):2, 1–15, https://doi.org/10.1167/15.5.2.
Commercial relationships: none. Kingdom, F. A. A., & Prins, N. (2016). Psychophysics:
Corresponding author: Goro Maehara. A practical introduction (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA:
Email: goro@kanagawa-u.ac.jp. Academic Press.
Address: Department of Human Science, Kanagawa Kingdom, F. A. A., & Wang, D. (2015). Dichoptic
University, Kanagawa, Japan. colour-saturation masking is unmasked by
* binocular luminance contrast. Vision Research, 116,
GM and YW contributed equally to this work. 45–52.
Kingdom, F. A. A., Yared, K.-C., Hibbard, P. B., &
May, K. A. (2020). Stereoscopic depth adaptation
References from binocularly correlated versus anti-correlated
noise: Test of an efficient coding theory of
stereopsis. Vision Research, 166, 60–71.
Anstis, S., & Ho, A. (1998). Nonlinear combination of
luminance excursions during flicker, simultaneous Legge, G. E., & Foley, J. M. (1980). Contrast masking
contrast, afterimages and binocular fusion. Vision in human vision. Journal of the Optical Society of
Research, 38(4), 523–539. America, 70(12), 1458–1471.

Downloaded from jov.arvojournals.org on 03/23/2024


Journal of Vision (2024) 24(2):4, 1–14 Maehara, Wang, & Murakami 14

Lema, S. A., & Blake, R. (1977). Binocular summation on stereopsis and fusion. Vision Research, 29(7),
in normal and stereoblind humans. Vision Research, 837–847.
17(6), 691–695. Wang, M., Ding, J., Levi, D. M., & Cooper, E. A.
Levelt, W. J. (1965). On binocular rivalry (dissertation, (2022). The effect of spatial structure on binocular
Leiden University). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. contrast perception. Journal of Vision, 22(12):7,
Lu, Z.-L., & Sperling, G. (2012). Black–white 1–19, https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.22.12.7.
asymmetry in visual perception. Journal of Vision, Wendt, G., & Faul, F. (2022). Binocular luster – A
12(10):8, 1–21, https://doi.org/10.1167/12.10.8. review. Vision Research, 194, 108008.
Maehara, G., & Goryo, K. (2005). Binocular,
monocular and dichoptic pattern masking. Optical
Review, 12(2), 76–82. Appendix
Maehara, G., Huang, P.-C., & Hess, R. F. (2009).
Importance of phase alignment for interocular
suppression. Vision Research, 49(14), 1838–1847. 25 25
Maehara, G., & Murakami, I. (2020). Perceptual Observer 1 Observer 2
20 20
enhancement of suprathreshold luminance
modulation in stereoscopic patterns. Journal of 15 15
Vision, 20(12):8, 1–11, https://doi.org/10.1167/jov. 10 10
20.12.8. Zero

Luminance Excursion in the Right Eye (cd/m²)


Manning, M. L., Finlay, D. C., Neill, R. A., & Frost, 5 Cross. 5
B. G. (1987). Detection threshold differences to 0
Uncross. 0
crossed and uncrossed disparities. Vision Research, 0 5 10 15 20 25 0 5 10 15 20 25
27(9), 1683–1686.
May, K., & Zhaoping, L. (2022). Li and Atick’s 25 25
theory of efficient binocular coding: A tutorial and 20
Observer 3
20
Observer 4
mini-review. Vision Research, 201, 107950.
Meese, T. S., Georgeson, M. A., & Baker, D. H. 15 15
(2006). Binocular contrast vision at and above 10 10
threshold. Journal of Vision, 6(11):7, 1224–1243,
https://doi.org/10.1167/6.11.7. 5 5

Mustillo, P. (1985). Binocular mechanisms mediating 0 0


crossed and uncrossed stereopsis. Psychological 0 5 10 15 20 25 0 5 10 15 20 25
Bulletin, 97(2), 187–201.
Patterson, R., Cayko, R., Short, G. L., Flanagan, R., 25 25
Moe, L., Taylor, E., . . . Day, P. (1995). Temporal 20
Observer 5
20
Observer 6
integration differences between crossed and
uncrossed stereoscopic mechanisms. Perception & 15 15
Psychophysics, 57, 891–897. 10 10
Ratliff, C. P., Borghuis, B. G., Kao, Y.-H., Sterling, P.,
5 5
& Balasubramanian, V. (2010). Retina is structured
to process an excess of darkness in natural scenes. 0 0
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 0 5 10 15 20 25 0 5 10 15 20 25
USA, 107(40), 17368–17373.
Luminance Excursion in the Left Eye (cd/m²)
Rose, D., Blake, R., & Halpern, D. L. (1988). Disparity
range for binocular summation. Investigative
Figure A1. Individual results for the decrement on a gray
Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 29(2), 283–290.
background in Experiment 1. Smooth curves in the left panels
Said, C. P., & Heeger, D. J. (2013). A model of binocular correspond to the model fit with mean w values for the
rivalry and cross-orientation suppression. PLoS zero-disparity (blue), crossed-disparity (red), and
Computational Biology, 9(3), e1002991. uncrossed-disparity (yellow) conditions. Symbols in the figure
Schor, C., & Heckmann, T. (1989). Interocular represent the PSEs. Error bars indicate the 95% CIs of the
differences in contrast and spatial frequency: Effects estimated PSEs. Most error bars are smaller than symbols.

Downloaded from jov.arvojournals.org on 03/23/2024

You might also like