Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/308033174

Color Constancy

Chapter · January 2016


DOI: 10.1007/978-3-642-27851-8_266-1

CITATIONS READS

3 750

1 author:

Bei Xiao
American University Washington D.C.
54 PUBLICATIONS 1,047 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Bei Xiao on 13 September 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Metadata of the chapter that will be visualized online
Chapter Title Color Constancy
Copyright Year 2015
Copyright Holder Springer Science+Business Media New York
Corresponding Author Family Name Xiao
Particle
Given Name Bei
Suffix
Division/Department Department of Computer Science
Organization/University American University
Street 4400 Massachusetts Avenue NW
City Washington
State DC
Postcode 20016-8058
Country USA
Email bei.xiao@gmail.com
Au1
1
C

2 Color Constancy source is tungsten light. If one were in the 25

scene, one would perceive the mug under both 26

3 Bei Xiao illumination conditions to have the same orange 27

4 Department of Computer Science, American color, though individual pixels from the same 28

5 University, Washington, DC, USA location from the mug in two photos appear dif- 29

ferent (see the patches above the photos). 30

Humans exhibit very good color constancy 31


Au2 6 Synonyms under natural viewing conditions (see a recent 32

review [16]). However, constancy can also be 33


7 Chromatic adaptation; White balancing poor under certain conditions. Figure 2 illustrated 34

a scenario when color constancy fails for most 35

observers. Color constancy is important in real- 36

8 Definition world tasks such as object and scene recognition 37

and visual search [65, 79]. 38

9 Color constancy refers to the ability of the human Unlike the human visual system, the image 39

10 visual system to perceive stable object color, captured by a digital camera has the issue that 40

11 despite significant variation of illumination. the color of the scene may be shifted by the 41

12 Color constancy is also a desired algorithm in change in the external illumination, even though 42

13 machine vision. In image processing, it is widely the intrinsic spectral property object in the scene 43

14 used in white balancing algorithms. Color con- (e.g., the mug) stays the same. The goal of a color 44

15 stancy has been an active research topic in the constancy algorithm is to correct the color shift 45

16 past 100 years. For a thorough understanding of caused by the illumination change and to extract 46

17 this subject, please see the following recent reliable color features that are invariant to the 47

18 reviews [4, 13, 16, 20, 34, 35, 56, 70]. change in illumination [23, 40]. The method of 48

correcting image color shift caused by changes in 49

the scene illumination in a camera is called white 50

19 Overview balance. 51

20 Figure 1 shows photos of the same scene under


21 two illumination conditions. The right photo was The Problem of Color Constancy 52

22 taken in the morning when the dominant light


23 source is the daylight, while the left photo was Figure 3 illustrates the problem of color con- 53

24 taken in the evening when the dominant light stancy. The color signal reaching the eye, C(l), 54

# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015


R. Luo (ed.), Encyclopedia of Color Science and Technology,
DOI 10.1007/978-3-642-27851-8_266-1
2 Color Constancy

Color Constancy, Fig. 1 Images of the samae thermal left was taken under two tungsten lamps. The image on the
mug lighted under two illuminants (tungsten illumination right was taken under the daylight from the windows. The
and window daylight). Top: the patches show rectangular photographs were taken by the author using a Canon EOS
regions filled with the color from roughly the same loca- Rebel T2 digital SLR camera with a 50 mm fixed lens and
tions of the mug in the two images. Bottom: the images the automatic white balance function of the camera
from which the patches were extracted. The image on the disabled

55 is a wavelength-by-wavelength product of the power distributions; for example, daylight has 59

56 spectral power distribution of the illumination I different spectral power distribution from that of 60

57 (l) and the surface reflectance function S(l). a tungsten light source. Surface reflectance func- 61

58 Different light sources have different spectral tion S(l) is an intrinsic property of a surface, and 62

Color Constancy, Fig. 2 Failures of color constancy. observers won’t be able to tell the color of the bell pepper
Left: a photograph of fruits taken under a monochromatic from the left image (the images were downloaded from
low-pressure sodium light. Right: the same scene was illu- http://www.soxlamps.com/advantages_sub.htm). But such
minated by normal broadband light sources. Most monochromatic light source is rare in the real world
Color Constancy 3

constancy. To answer this, we need to measure 81


E1 (λ) color constancy in a controlled way. Three com- 82

mon methods have been used in the past to mea- 83

sure color constancy in a laboratory setting: color 84

naming where observers name colors of surfaces 85


S1(λ)
under different illuminations [44, 45], asymmet- 86

ric matching where observers adjust a match sur- 87

face under one illuminant to match the color 88

C(λ) = E1(λ) S1(λ)


appearance of a reference surface under another 89

illuminant [2, 19, 78], and achromatic adjustment 90


E2(λ) where observers adjust the chromaticity of a test 91

surface so that it appears achromatic and then 92

repeat the task when the test is embedded in 93


S2(λ) scenes with different illuminants [12]. It was 94

found that asymmetric matching and achromatic 95

adjustment reach similar conclusions of con- 96

stancy when the two tasks were compared using 97

C(λ) = E2(λ) S2(λ) the same scenes [19]. 98

Color Constancy, Fig. 3 Illustration of color constancy. How Good Is Human Color Constancy? 99 Au3
The spectrum of reflected light reaching the eye, C(l), is Overall, these methods show that human con- 100
wavelength-by-wavelength product of surface reflectance stancy is not perfect but generally very good. We 101
S and the illumination E. The problem of color constancy
is challenging because different combination of illumina- can compute a color constancy index from either 102

tion and surface reflectance can result in the same color asymmetric matching or achromatic adjustment 103
signal (This illustration is adapted from David Brainard experiments, where 0 % means no constancy and 104
[16]) 100 % means perfect constancy [2, 19, 21, 105

71]. Most studies in color constancy use simplified 106


63 it is determined by how the surface absorbs and laboratory stimuli that consist of flat and matte 107
64 reflects light. Under a neutral light source, objects surfaces under diffuse lighting conditions (for 108
65 with different surface reflectance functions reviews, see [2, 12, 13, 16, 54]). 109
66 appear to have different colors. The goal of For such flat-matte-diffuse surfaces in simple 110
67 color constancy is to extract the intrinsic surface scenes, especially when only the illuminant is 111
68 reflectance function from the color signal. Color varied in the scene, color constancy can be very 112
69 constancy is an ambiguous problem because dif- good with an average constancy index around 0.85 113
70 ferent combinations of illuminant and surface can for real scenes [12] and around 0.75 for graphical 114
71 give rise to the same color signal (see Fig. 3). simulated scenes [27]. However, constancy can be 115
72 Many computational models in the past suggest decreased significantly when both the illuminants 116
73 that the visual system first makes an estimate of and the surfaces are varied in a scene. Figure 4b 117
74 the illuminant and uses it to recover the surface shows an example of such manipulations. The 118
75 reflectance function {Brainard [14, 39, 40, 66]}. surface in the scene was manipulated so that the 119

color of the background wall reaching the eye 120

under illuminant A is the same as when a neutral 121

76 What Do We Know About Human Color colored wall is illuminated under illuminant B (the 122

77 Constancy? middle and the leftmost images). In this condition, 123

constancy index drops to 0.2 [78]. Similar effects 124

78 How Human Color Constancy Is Measured? have also been explored in previous studies [50, 125

79 After establishing the problem, now the question 51]. In these cases, constancy is reduced but not 126

80 is how good the human visual system is at color completely diminished. 127
4 Color Constancy

Color Constancy, Fig. 4 Various stimuli used in color color across fabric’s surface. Such color variation is
constancy and color perception experiments. (a) Stimuli important for material perception. The right image
used to study color constancy by [27]. Synthetic images shows the same photograph in gray scale. A recent study
contain a flat test surface embedded in a relative complex on tactile and visual matching of fabric properties shows
scene (b). The rendered images were used in a study of that observers make more mistakes predicting fabric’s
color constancy of 3D object by Xiao and Brainard tactile properties with grayscale images than color images
[78]. The leftmost scene was illuminated by a neutral (Photos taken by the author). (d) Translucent objects, such
illuminant, the middle scene was illuminated by a bluish as liquid, stone, skin, and wax, represent new challenges
illuminant, the rightmost scene was illuminated by the for color constancy research. The photo on the left shows a
same bluish illuminant as the middle scene, but the reflec- glass of fat-free milk illuminated from the front, and the
tance of the background surface has been changed so that photo on the right shows the same glass of fat-free milk
the light reflected from the background is the same as in illuminated from the back. One can observe that the color
the leftmost image. (c) Photograph of a piece of fabric appears to be slightly different when the illumination
draped over an object. The left image shows the original direction is varied (The photos were taken by Ioannis
photograph of the fabric. There is a significant variation of Gkioulekas from Harvard University)
Color Constancy 5

2 3
128 Natural scenes are rarely composed of flat- rL
129 matte-diffuse surfaces. First, objects usually r ¼ 4 rM 5 (1)
130 have 3D shapes and are made of non-matte mate- rS
131 rial such as metal, plastic, or wax, which looks
132 glossy and translucent. Second, the objects in the The cone signals are subjected by adaptation. 173 Au5
133 three-dimensional space are arranged at different Von Kries proposed that the LMS cone signals 174
134 depths from the viewing point. Lastly, the light- are scaled by a multiplicative factor, and at each 175
135 ing condition often has complex spatial and spec- retinal location the gains are scaled indepen- 176
136 tral distribution. How good is human color dently [74]. For each cone class, the gain is set 177
137 constancy in natural scenes? The factors in natu- in inverse proportion to the spatial mean of the 178
138 ral scenes that have been studied include the signals from the cones of the same class. This 179
139 effect of three-dimensional pose of surfaces algorithm is called von Kries adaptation. The 180
Au4 140 ([7–11], Boyaci et al. 2003, [28, 29, 43, 63, adapted cone signals, a, can be obtained by mul- 181
141 64]), the effect of lighting geometry on constancy tiplying the cone signals a by a diagonal 182
142 [1, 58–60], the stereo depth on constancy [77], matrix D, where the elements gl, gm, and gs 183
143 the 3D shape, and the material that an object is represent the three gains: 184
144 made of on color constancy [32, 57, 59, 78].
2 3 2 3
145 Cognitive factors have also been considered in aL gL 0 0
146 studying color constancy [61, 69]. Some objects a ¼ 4 aM 5 ¼ 4 0 gM 0 5 ¼ Dr (2)
147 have characteristic color, such as bananas are aS 0 0 gS
148 yellow and cucumbers are green. How do we
149 take the prior knowledge of objects’ color into The von Kries adaptation inspired Land’s famous 185

150 account in achieving color constancy? A recent retinex theory [52], which has very wide applica- 186

151 work by Kanematsu [49] suggests the effect of tion in camera color balance and can be used to 187

152 familiar contextual object on color constancy is explain human color constancy for the flat-matte- 188

153 small. diffuse stimuli. The central principle of the 189

154 Figure 4 depicts several scenes used previ- retinex theory is that the lightness values at each 190

155 ously in experiments on color constancy. The pixel are calculated independently for each cone 191

156 associated stimuli effectively challenge existing class. For an analysis of the retinex theory and 192

157 theories of color constancy. color constancy, see a study by Brainard and 193

Wandell [18]. 194

Further along the visual pathway, in addition 195


158 Theories of Color Constancy
to cone adaptation, secondary adaptation is also 196
159 How does the visual system achieve color con-
proposed. The effects include gain control after 197
160 stancy? One approach to understand constancy is
the combination of the cone signals and also 198
161 to explain it using low-level visual mechanisms
subtractive modulation instead of multiplicative 199
162 such as chromatic adaptation. The color signal
modulation [46, 47, 62, 67, 75, 76]. 200
163 reaching the eye, C(l), is encoded by the
The adaptation models predict color con- 201
164 responses of three classes of light-sensitive pho-
stancy quite well for flat-matte-diffuse scenes. 202
165 toreceptors in the retina, which are referred to as
However, it often fails to predict constancy for 203
166 long (L)-, medium (M)-, and short (S)-
rich scenes [50]. We do now know how to obtain 204
167 wavelength-sensitive cones [17, 48]. Let us rep-
the values of the multiplicative gain from images, 205
168 resent the spectral properties of the reflected light
which contain spatially rich information. That 206
169 reaching the eye by the quantal absorption rates
being said, the mechanistic approach sometimes 207
170 for the three classes of cones. The light signal, r,
can inspire new algorithms for color 208
171 can be represented by a three-dimensional col-
constancy [38]. 209
172 umn vector (Eq. 1):
6 Color Constancy

210 Another approach is to use a computational on the other hand, should aim at correcting illu- 255

211 method developed from a computer vision per- mination effects for real-world complex images 256

212 spective. As described above, color constancy is (see a recent review by [40]). 257

213 an ill-posed problem. Bayesian methods combine The best-known statistical method is the gray- 258

214 the information contained in the observed scene world theory, which assumes that the average 259

215 with information given a priori about the likely reflectance of a scene is gray [22]. Some other 260

216 physical configuration of the world [14]. In the similar algorithms include white patch and 261

217 case of color constancy, some prior knowledge max-RGB [36, 37, 52] and shade of gray 262

218 about the illuminants and surface reflectance [31]. The gray-world algorithm will fail if the 263

219 can resolve ambiguity. Earlier work has used average reflectance is not achromatic or if there 264

220 statistical constraints of illuminants and surface is a large uniform colored surface in the scene. 265

221 reflectance on solving color constancy such as the The incorporation of higher-order statistics in 266

222 gray-world, subspace, and physical realizability terms of image derivatives is proposed, where a 267

Au6 223 algorithms [22, 26, 33, 55]. Brainard and Free- framework called gray edge is presented 268

224 man [14] constructed prior distribution describ- [72]. Chakrabarti et al. [24] go beyond statistics 269

225 ing the probability of illuminants and surface in of per-pixel colors and model the spatial depen- 270

226 the world and then estimate the illuminant from dencies between pixels by decomposing the input 271

227 the posterior distribution conditioned on the images into spatial sub-bands and then model the 272

228 image intensity data. Brainard et al. [15] applied color statistics separately in each sub-band. 273

229 the similar Bayesian model to predicate the Forsyth [33] introduced the gamut-mapping 274

230 degree of human color constancy across method. It is based on the assumption that only 275

231 different manipulations and connect the variation a limited set of colors (canonical gamut) can 276

232 in constancy to the prior distribution of occur under a given illuminant. The model learns 277

233 the illuminant. a model based on training images (the canonical 278

gamut) and estimate the illuminant based on the 279

input features. 280

234 What Do We Know About Machine Color Inspired by Brainard [14], Rosenberg 281

235 Constancy? et al. [66] introduced a Bayesian model of color 282

constancy utilizing a non-Gaussian probabilistic 283

236 While human visual system is equipped with model of the image formation process and dem- 284

237 good color constancy, the digital camera has to onstrated that it can outperform the gamut- 285

238 rely on color balancing algorithm to discount the mapping algorithm. Gehler et al. [39] extended 286

239 illumination effect and extract the invariable the Bayesian algorithm using new datasets, 287

240 object color. This process is also called color which allows the algorithm to learn more precise 288

241 balance or white balance. The most popular priors of the illuminations. 289

242 method is based on adaptation such as the von A new thread of algorithms estimates the illu- 290

243 Kries coefficient rule and Land’s retinex theory minant using high-level features [5, 41, 53, 291

244 discussed above [30, 52, 74]. But this of this type 73]. For example, Gijsenij and Gervse [42] pro- 292

245 of model is restricted to simple scenes. posed to dynamically determine which color con- 293

246 In some sense, there is a significant overlap stancy algorithm to be used for a specific image 294

247 between the development of algorithms for based on the scene category. Bianco and Schettini 295

248 machine color constancy and the modeling of [6] proposed a model to estimate illuminant from 296

249 human color constancy. However, one distinction color statistics extracted from faces automati- 297

250 is the choice of stimuli. To understand human cally detected in the image. It takes advantage 298

251 color constancy, simple synthetic stimuli that that the skin color tends to cluster in the color 299

252 allow systematic manipulation of scene parame- space, which provides a valid cue to estimate the 300

253 ters are often used as experimental stimuli. illuminant. Inspired by human visual system 301

254 A successful machine color constancy algorithm, mechanisms, a recent study by Gao et al. [38] 302
Color Constancy 7

303 built a physiologically based color constancy Retrieved from http://www.opticsinfobase.org/ 346

304 model that imitated the double-opponent mecha- abstract.cfm?&id=2483 347


3. Barnard, K., Martin, L., Funt, B., Coath, A.: A data set 348
305 nism of the visual system. The illuminant is esti- for color research. Color Res. Appl. 27(3), 147–151 349
306 mated by searching for the maxima of the (2002). Retrieved from http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/ 350
307 separate RGB channels of the responses of viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.3.4123&rep=rep1& 351

308 double-opponent cells in the RGB space. type=pdf 352


4. Bianco, S., Schettini, R.: Computational color con- 353 Au8
309 As the technology of camera sensor develops, stancy (2011) 354
310 several datasets captured by high-quality SLR 5. Bianco, S., Ciocca, G., Cusano, C., Schettini, R.: Improv- 355
311 camera in RAW format have been used in the ing color constancy using indoor  outdoor image clas- 356

312 community. The popular datasets include sification. IEEE Trans. Image Process. 17(12), 357
2381–2392 (2008). Retrieved from http://www.ivl. 358
313 Ciurea’s dataset [25], Middlebury color database disco.unimib.it/publications/pdf/bianco2008improving- 359
314 [24], and Barnard’s datasets [3, 68]. color.pdf 360
6. Bianco, S., Schettini, R.: Color constancy using faces 361
(2012) 362
7. Bloj, M., Ripamonti, C., Mitha, K., Hauck, R., 363
315 Future Directions Greenwald, S., Brainard, D.H.: An equivalent illuminant 364
model for the effect of surface slant on perceived light- 365

316 Color constancy is an important and practical ness. J. Vis. 4(9), 735–746 (2004). doi:10.1167/4.9.6 366
8. Bloj, M.G., Hurlbert, A.C.: An empirical study of the 367
317 problem in both human and computer vision. traditional Mach card effect. Perception Lond. 31(2), 368
318 Color constancy provides an excellent model sys- 233–246 (2002). Retrieved from http://www. 369
319 tem to understand how the visual system solves perceptionjournal.com/perception/fulltext/p31/p01sp.pdf 370

320 ambiguity. Significant progress has been made to 9. Bloj, M.G., Kersten, D., Hurlbert, A.C.: Perception of 371
three-dimensional shape influences colour perception 372
321 understand color constancy of simple scenes. through mutual illumination. Nature 402(6764), 373
322 A big challenge is how to extend the theoretical 877–879 (1999). Retrieved from http://www.nature. 374
323 model of color constancy for simple scenes to com/nature/journal/v402/n6764/abs/402877a0.html 375

324 predict human performance in rich scenes. 10. Boyaci, H., Doerschner, K., Snyder, J., Maloney, L.: 376
Surface color perception in three-dimensional scenes. 377
325 Even though many computer vision algo- Vis. Neurosci. 23(3/4), 311 (2006). Retrieved from 378
326 rithms are successful at correcting color bias http://www.bilkent.edu.tr/hboyaci/Vision/Boyaci_Do 379
327 caused by illumination, whether or not the erschner_Snyder_Maloney_VisNeuro_2006.pdf 380

328 human visual system uses similar algorithms to 11. Boyaci, H., Doerschner, K., Maloney, L.T.: Perceived 381
surface color in binocularly viewed scenes with two 382
329 achieve constancy is poorly understood. A major light sources differing in chromaticity. J. Vis. 383
330 challenge to reconcile the latter with the former is 4(9) (2004). Retrieved from http://ww. 384
331 a matter of scene and stimulus complexity. Often, journalofvision.org/content/4/9/1.full 385

332 images used in computer vision algorithms are 12. Brainard, D.H.: Color constancy in the nearly natural 386
image. 2. Achromatic loci. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 15(2), 387
333 real photos or videos, which contain rich scenes. 307–325 (1998). Retrieved from http://www. 388
334 To model color constancy in humans, however, opticsinfobase.org/viewmedia.cfm?URI=josaa-15-2- 389
335 experiments require more simplified scenes and 307&seq=0 390

336 conditions. How to bridge the two fields is a 13. Brainard, D.H.: Color constancy. In: The Visual 391
Neurosciences, vol. 1, pp. 948–961. MIT Press, 392
Au7 337 promising endeavor for future research. Cambridge, MA (2004). Retrieved from http://www. 393
cns.nyu.edu/csh04/Articles/Brainard-02.pdf 394
14. Brainard, D.H., Freeman, W.T.: Bayesian color con- 395
stancy. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 14(7), 1393–1411 (1997). 396
338 References Retrieved from http://www.opticsinfobase.org/ 397
viewmedia.cfm?uri=josaa-14-7-1393&seq=0 398
339 1. Adelson, E.H.: Lightness perception and lightness 15. Brainard, D.H., Longère, P., Delahunt, P.B., Freeman, 399
340 illusions. In: The New Cognitive Neurosciences, W.T., Kraft, J.M., Xiao, B.: Bayesian model of human 400
341 p. 339. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA (2000). Retrieved color constancy. J. Vis. 6(11) (2006). Retrieved from 401
342 from http://www.cs.tau.ac.il/hezy/Vision Seminar/ http://w.journalofvision.org/content/6/11/10.full 402
343 Lightness Perception and Lightness Illusions.htm 16. Brainard, D.H., Radonjić, A.: Color constancy. In: 403
344 2. Arend, L., Reeves, A.: Simultaneous color constancy. Werner, J.S., Chalupa, L.M. (eds.) The New Visual 404
345 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A. 3(10), 1743–1751 (1986).
8 Color Constancy

405 Neuroscience, pp. 545–556. MIT Press, Cambridge 31. Finlayson, G.D., Hordley, S.D., Hubel, P.M.: Color 464
406 (2013) by correlation: a simple, unifying framework for color 465
407 17. Brainard, D.H., Stockman, A.: Colorimetry. (1995). constancy. IEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 466
408 Retrieved from http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/ 23(11), 1209–1221 (2001). Retrieved from http://th. 467
409 summary?doi=10.1.1.140.3027 physik.uni-frankfurt.de/triesch/courses/275vision/ 468
410 18. Brainard, D.H., Wandell, B.A.: Analysis of the papers/finlayson_et_al_pami_2001.pdf 469
411 retinex theory of color vision. J. Opt. 32. Fleming, R.W., B€ ulthoff, H.H.: Low-level image cues 470
412 Soc. Am. A. 3(10), 1651–1661 (1986). Retrieved in the perception of translucent materials. ACM 471
413 from http://www.opticsinfobase.org/viewmedia. Trans. Appl. Percept. (TAP) 2(3), 346–382 (2005). 472
414 cfm?URI=josaa-3-10-1651&seq=0 Retrieved from http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id= 473
415 19. Brainard, D.H., Brunt, W.A., Speigle, J.M.: Color con- 1077409 474
416 stancy in the nearly natural image I. Asymmetric 33. Forsyth, D.A.: A novel algorithm for color constancy. 475
417 matches. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis 14(9), Int. J. Comp. Vis. 5(1), 5–35 (1990). Retrieved from 476
418 2091–2110 (1997). Retrieved from http://www.ncbi. http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/BF00056770 477
419 nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db= 34. Foster, D.H.: Color constancy. Vision Res. 51(7), 674–700 478
420 PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=9291602 (2011). Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect.com/sci 479
421 20. Brainard, D.H., Stockman, A.: Colorimetry. In: Bass, ence/article/pii/S0042698910004402 480
422 M. (ed.) OSA Handbook of Optics. McGraw-Hill, 35. Foster, D.H.: Does colour constancy exist? Trends 481
423 New York (2010) Cogn. Sci. 7(10), 439–443 (2003). Retrieved from 482
424 21. Brainard, D.H., Wandell, B.A.: A bilinear model of http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd= 483
425 the illuminant’s effect on color appearance. In: Com- Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids= 484
426 putational Models of Visual Processing, pp. 171–186. 14550490 485
427 MIT Press, Cambridge, MA (1991) 36. Funt, B., Shi, L.: The effect of exposure on MaxRGB 486
428 22. Buchsbaum, G.: A spatial processor model for object color constancy (2010a) 487
429 colour perception. J. Franklin Inst. 310(1), 1–26 37. Funt, B., Shi, L.: The rehabilitation of MaxRGB 488
430 (1980). Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect. (2010b) 489
431 com/science/article/pii/0016003280900587 38. Gao, S., Yang, K., Li, C., Li, Y.: A color constancy 490
432 23. Chakrabarti, A., Hirakawa, K., Zickler, T.: Color con- model with double-opponency mechanisms. In: Pro- 491
433 stancy with spatio-spectral statistics. IEEE Trans. ceedings from ICCV (2013) 492
434 Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 34(8), 1509–1519 (2012). 39. Gehler, P. V., Rother, C., Blake, A., Minka, T., Sharp, 493
435 http://cilab.knu.ac.kr/seminar/Seminar/2013/ T.: Bayesian color constancy revisited (2008) 494
436 20130330ColorConstancywithSpatio- 40. Gijsenij, A., Gevers, T., van de Weijer, J.: Computa- 495
437 SpectralStatistics.pdf tional color constancy: survey and experiments. IEEE 496
438 24. Chakrabarti, A., Scharstein, D., Zickler, T.: Color Trans. Image Process. 20(9), 2475–2489 (2011). 497
439 datasets. Retrieved from http://vision.middlebury. doi:10.1109/TIP.2011.2118224 498
440 edu/color/ (2011) 41. Gijsenij, A., Gevers, T.: Color constancy using natu- 499
441 25. Ciurea, F., Funt, B. A large image database for color ral image statistics (2007) 500
442 constancy research (2003) 42. Gijsenij, A., Gevers, T.: Color constancy using natu- 501
443 26. D’Zmura, M., Iverson, G., Singer, B.: Probabilistic ral image statistics and scene semantics. IEE Trans. 502
444 color constancy. In: Geometric representations of per- Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 33(4), 687–698 (2011). 503
445 ceptual phenomena, pp. 187–202. (1995) Retrieved from http://staff.science.uva.nl/gevers/ 504
446 27. Delahunt, P.B., Brainard, D.H.: Does human color pub/GeversPAMI11.pdf 505
447 constancy incorporate the statistical regularity of nat- 43. Gilchrist, A.L.: When does perceived lightness 506
448 ural daylight? J. Vis. 4(2) (2004). Retrieved from depend on perceived spatial arrangement? Percept. 507
449 http://ww.journalofvision.org/content/4/2/1.full Psychophys. 28(6), 527–538 (1980). Retrieved from 508
450 28. Doerschner, K., Boyaci, H., Maloney, L.T.: Human http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi= 509
451 observers compensate for secondary illumination 10.1.1.211.7842&rep=rep1&typ e=pdf 510
452 originating in nearby chromatic surfaces. J. Vis. 44. Helson, H.: Fundamental problems in color vision. 511
453 4(2) (2004). Retrieved from http://wwww. I. The principle governing changes in hue, saturation, 512
454 journalofvision.org/content/4/2/3.full and lightness of non-selective samples in chromatic 513
455 29. Epstein, W.: Phenomenal orientation and perceived illumination. J Exp. Psychol. 23(5), 439 (1938). 514
456 achromatic color. J. Psychol. 52(1), 51–53 (1961). Retrieved from http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/xge/ 515
457 Retrieved from http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/pdf/ 23/5/439/ 516
458 10.1080/00223980.1961.9916503 45. Helson, H., Jeffers, V.B.: Fundamental problems in 517
459 30. Finlayson, G.D., Drew, M.S., Funt, B.V.: Color con- color vision. II. Hue, lightness, and saturation of 518
460 stancy: generalized diagonal transforms suffice. J. Opt. selective samples in chromatic illumination. J. Exp. 519
461 Soc. Am. A 11(11), 3011–3019 (1994). Retrieved from Psychol. 26(1), 1 (1940). Retrieved from http:// 520
462 http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi= psycnet.apa.org/journals/xge/26/1/1/ 521
463 10.1.1.121.872&rep=rep1&type=pdf
Color Constancy 9

522 46. Hurvich, L.M., Jameson, D.: An opponent-process 58. Obein, G., Knoblauch, K., Viéot, F.: Difference scal- 580
523 theory of color vision. Psychol. Rev. 64(6p1), ing of gloss: nonlinearity, binocularity, and con- 581
524 384 (1957). Retrieved from http://cogsci.bme.hu/ stancy. J. Vision. 4(9), 711–20 (2004). Retrieved 582
525 gkovacs/letoltes/HurvichJameson1957.pdf from http://ww.journalofvision.org/content/4/9/4.full 583
526 47. Jameson, D., Hurvich, L.: Opponent-response func- 59. Olkkonen, M., Brainard, D.H.: Perceived glossiness 584
527 tions related to measured cone photopigments*. and lightness under real-world illumination. J. Vis. 585
528 J. Opt. Soc. Am. 58(3), 429–430 (1968). Retrieved 10(9), 5 (2010). doi10.1167/10.9.5 586
529 from http://www.opticsinfobase.org/abstract.cfm? 60. Olkkonen, M., Brainard, D.H.: Joint effects of illumi- 587
530 URI=josa-58-3-429_1 nation geometry and object shape in the perception of 588
531 48. Kaiser, P.K., Boynton, R.M.: Human Color Vision surface reflectance. Iperception 2(9), 1014–1034 589
532 (287). Optical Society of America, Washington, DC (2011). doi:10.1068/i0480 590
533 (1996). Retrieved from http://www.getcited.org/pub/ 61. Olkkonen, M., Hansen, T., Gegenfurtner, K.R.: Color 591
534 100154932 appearance of familiar objects: effects of object shape, 592
535 49. Kanematsu, E., Brainard, D.H.: No measured effect of texture, and illumination changes. J. Vis. 8(5) (2008). 593
536 a familiar contextual object on color constancy. Color Retrieved from http://www.journalofvision.orgwww. 594
537 Res. Appl. 39(4), 347–359 (2013). Retrieved from journalofvision.org/content/8/5/13.full 595
538 http://color.psych.upenn.edu/brainard/papers/ 62. Poirson, A.B.., Wandell, B.A.: Appearance of colored 596
539 Kanematsu_Brainard_13.pdf patterns: pattern—color separability. J. Opt. 597
540 50. Kraft, J.M., Brainard, D.H.: Mechanisms of color Soc. Am. A. 10(12), 2458–2470 (1993). Retrieved 598
541 constancy under nearly natural viewing. Proc. Natl. from http://white.stanford.edu/brian/papers/color/ 599
542 Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 96(1), 307–312 (1999). Retrieved smatch.pdf 600
543 from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi? 63. Radonjić, A., Todorović, D., Gilchrist, A.: Adjacency 601
544 cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_ and surroundedness in the depth effect on lightness. 602
545 uids=9874814 J. Vis. 10(9) (2010). Retrieved from http://171.67. 603
546 51. Kraft, J.M., Maloney, S.I., Brainard, D.H.: Surface- 113.220/content/10/9/12.full 604
547 illuminant ambiguity and color constancy: effects of 64. Ripamonti, C., Bloj, M., Hauck, R., Mitha, K., 605
548 scene complexity and depth cues. Perception 31(2), Greenwald, S., Maloney, S.I., Brainard, D.H.: Measure- 606
549 247–263 (2002). Retrieved from http://www.ncbi. ments of the effect of surface slant on perceived light- 607
550 nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db= ness. J. Vis. 4(9) (2004). Retrieved from http://www. 608
551 PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=11922136 journalofvision.orgwww.journalofvision.org/content/4/ 609
552 52. Land, E.H.: The retinex theory of color vision. Sci. 9/7.full 610
553 Am. 237, 108–128 (1977). Retrieved from http://xa. 65. Robilotto, R., Zaidi, Q.: Limits of lightness identifi- 611
554 yimg.com/kq/groups/18365325/470399326/name/E. cation for real objects under natural viewing condi- 612
555 Land_-_Retinex_Theory%255B1%255D.pdf tions. J. Vis. 4(9) (2004). Retrieved from http://ww.w. 613
556 53. Li, B., Xu, D., Lang, C.: Colour constancy based on journalofvision.org/content/4/9/9.full 614
557 texture similarity for natural images. Color. Technol. 66. Rosenberg, C., Ladsariya, A., Minka, T.: Bayesian 615
558 125(6), 328–333 (2009). Retrieved from http:// color constancy with non-gaussian models (2003) 616
559 onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1478-4408.2009. 67. Shevell, S.K.: The dual role of chromatic backgrounds 617
560 00214.x/full in color perception. Vision Res. 18(12), 1649–1661 618
561 54. Maloney, L.T.: Physics-based approaches to model- (1978). Retrieved from http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/ 619
562 ing surface color perception. In: Color Vision: From bitstream/2027.42/22782/1/0000337.pdf 620
563 Genes to Perception, pp. 387–416. Cambridge Uni- 68. Shi, L., Funt, B. V.: Re-processed version of the 621
564 versity Press, Cambridge, New York (1999). Gehler color constancy database of 568 images 622
565 Retrieved from http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/ (2010) 623
566 download?doi=10.1.1.211.8602&rep=rep1&type=pdf 69. Smet, K., Ryckaert, W.R., Pointer, M.R., Deconinck, 624
567 55. Maloney, L.T., Wandell, B.A.: Color constancy: a G., Hanselaer, P.: Colour appearance rating of famil- 625
568 method for recovering surface spectral reflectance. iar real objects. Color Res. Appl. 36(3), 192–200 626
569 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A. 3(1), 29–33 (1986). Retrieved (2011). Retrieved from http://www.esat.kuleuven.be/ 627
570 from http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download? electa/publications/fulltexts/pub_2070.pdf 628
571 doi=10.1.1.6.4745&rep=rep1&type=pdf 70. Smithson, H.E.: Sensory, computational and cogni- 629
572 56. Maloney, L. T., Brainard, D. H.: Color and material tive components of human colour constancy. Philos. 630
573 perception: achievements and challenges. J. Vis. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 360(1458), 631
574 10(9). doi:10.1167/10.9.19 (2010) 1329–1346 (2005). doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1633 632
575 57. Motoyoshi, I., Nishida, S., Sharan, L., Adelson, E.H.: 71. Troost, J.M., De Weert, C.M.M.: Naming versus 633
576 Image statistics and the perception of surface quali- matching in color constancy. Percept. Psychophys. 634
577 ties. Nature 447(7141), 206–209 (2007). Retrieved 50(6), 591–602 (1991). Retrieved from http://link. 635
578 from http://www.cns.nyu.edu/msl/courses/2223/Read springer.com/article/10.3758/BF03207545 636
579 ings/MotoyoshiNishidaSharanAdelson.Nature.2007.pdf
10 Color Constancy

637 72. Van De Weijer, J., Gevers, T., Gijsenij, A.: Edge-based 694–698 (1995). Retrieved from http://www.nature. 651
638 color constancy. IEEE Trans. Image Process. 16(9), com/nature/journal/v373/n6516/abs/373694a0.html 652
639 2207–2214 (2007). Retrieved from http://hal.archives- 77. Werner, A.: Color constancy improves, when an 653
640 ouvertes.fr/docs/00/54/86/86/PDF/IP07_vandeweijer.pdf object moves: high-level motion influences color per- 654
Au9 641 73. Van De Weijer, J., Schmid, C., Verbeek, J.: Using high- ception. J. Vis. 7(14) (2007). Retrieved from http:// 655
642 level visual information for color constancy (2007) wwww.journalofvision.org/content/7/14/19.full 656
643 74. von Kries, J.: Chromatic adaptation. Festschrift der 78. Xiao, B., Hurst, B., MacIntyre, L., Brainard, D.H.: 657
644 Albrecht-Ludwigs-Universität, pp. 145–158. (1902) The color constancy of three-dimensional objects. 658
645 75. Walraven, J.: Discounting the background – the missing J. Vis. 12(4), 6 (2012). doi:10.1167/12.4.6 659
646 link in the explanation of chromatic induction. Vision 79. Zaidi, Q., Bostic, M.: Color strategies for object iden- 660
647 Res. 16(3), 289–295 (1976). Retrieved from http://www. tification. Vision Res. 48(26), 2673–2681 (2008). 661
648 sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0042698976901127 doi:10.1016/j.visres.2008.06.026 662
649 76. Webster, M.A., Mollon, J.D.: Colour constancy
650 influenced by contrast adaptation. Nature 373(6516),
Author Queries
Encyclopedia of Color Science and Technology
Chapter No: 266-1

___________________________________________________________________
Query Refs. Details Required Author’s response
AU1 If you are using material from other works please
make sure that you have obtained the necessary
permission from the copyright holders and that
references to the original publications are included.
AU2 The heading of ‘Similar Concepts’ has been
changed to ‘Synonyms’. Please check if okay.
AU3 Please check if identified heading levels are okay.
AU4 Please provide details for Boyaci et al. (2003).
AU5 Equations have renumbered please check if okay.
AU6 Please check if "Brainard and Freeman. . ." is okay
as edited.
AU7 Please provide “Cross-References”.
AU8 Please provide publisher name and location details
for ref. [4].
AU9 Please provide complete details for proceedings
and conferences in refs. [4, 6, 24, 25, 26, 36, 37,
38, 39, 41, 66, 68, 73, 74].

View publication stats

You might also like