Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 3

1858 Animal Behaviour, 36, 6

winged choughs, Corcorax melanorhamphos. Ibis, 120, modified on a computer. All twitter parts were
178-197. removed, and the remaining motif songs were used
Vehrencamp, S. L. 1977. Relative fecundity and parental to construct six test tapes each lasting 3 min. The
effort in communally nesting anis, Crotophaga sulciros- first control tape had unaltered motif songs. The
tris. Science, N.Y., 197, 403 405. remaining five tapes had motif songs with all
Woolfenden, G. E. & Fitzpatrick, J. W. 1984. The Florida
frequencies multiplied by one of the following
Scrub Jay. Demography o f a Cooperative-breeding Bird.
Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press. values: 0.75, 0-875, 1.1, 1.2, or 1.4. Three of the
Yom-Tov, Y. 1975. Synchronization of breeding and modifications changed the frequency level up to
intraspecific interference in the carrion crow. Auk, 92, (0.75, 1.2) or beyond (1.4) its natural limits of
778-785. variation making it abnormal for motif songs.
Multiplication by 0.875 and 1.1 changed the level
(Received 22 January 1988; revised 27 April 1988; within its natural limits of variation making it code
MS. number: sc-423) for decreased and increased arousal, respectively
(Dabelsteen & Pedersen 1985).
Ten females were made receptive to song stimu-
lation by long day conditions (16:8 light: dark)
and subcutaneous injections of 1 mg oestradiol-
Do Female Blackbirds, Turdus merula, Decode Song valerat, and then tested in an anechoic chamber.
in the Same Way as Males? Each bird was tested in two experimental series
each consisting of six double tests. In a double test
Most studies of the decoding of bird song have used the bird was confronted twice with one of the six
males only. Several authors (for example Becker types of song. Two hours passed between the two
1982) have pointed out that this is unsatisfactory, presentations in a double test. Each test consisted
not only because song may convey information to of three 3-min playback sessions with two 3-min
both males and females, but also because the two pauses inserted between the sessions, plus a 3-min
sexes differ in behaviour and may sometimes post-playback period. For further details of pro-
occupy slightly different habitats. Differences in cedure see Dabelsteen & Pedersen (in press).
habitat use may result in males and females The responses of the females were recorded on
experiencing different degrees of song degradation. an NEC PVC-9507 videotape recorder. Three
If the difference is marked, sex differences in song response types (R-I-3) and eight behaviours be-
decoding may evolve. As an example, the range of longing to them were quantified: R-I: sexual
variation within which certain song features elicit a behaviours, duration of copulation displays; R-2:
response may differ in males and females, i.e. the nest-building behaviours, number of pecks and
two sexes may have different song variation toler- scrape-turnings in straw deposited on the nest-site
ances (Shiovitz & Lemon 1980). We report here on platform, ae-sounds (a female vocalization) and
experiments testing this possibility in blackbirds. transport of straw between the floor, perches and
Blackbird song consists of a long-ranging motif platform; R-3: other behaviours, number of orien-
followed by a higher-pitched and short-ranging tating head movements, jumps, and pecks at the
twitter (Dabelsteen 1984). Both the motif and the bottom of the cage (see description in Dabelsteen
twitter song convey information to territorial males 1988). A behaviour was regarded as released by the
and receptive females (Dabelsteen & Pedersen song if its duration or frequency exceeded that of
1985, in press; Dabelsteen 1988). However, there is the spontaneous behaviour during the 3-min pre-
a marked sexual difference in receiving conditions test period. If not, the score was set at zero. The
of forest-living blackbirds. Males often hear song response types were released when one (R- 1) or two
in relatively open conditions, for example when (R-2 and R-3) of their behaviours were released.
patrolling territorial borders or when singing, Any response type released in a test scored one
whereas females usually hide in vegetation at or point, i.e. the maximum score of a double test was
near ground level. A change of certain song six. Within each experimental series the durations
features (for example frequency level) up to or and frequencies obtained or points scored for each
beyond their natural limits of variation is not bird in each double test (N = 20) were expressed as
tolerated by males (Dabelsteen & Pedersen 1985). percentages of the highest value possible. A Fried-
Because of the inferior receiving conditions the man two-way ANOVA was used to test the overall
song variation tolerances could be wider in females heterogeneity between the values obtained for the
than in males. We tested this by presenting females six types of song.
with motif songs whose frequencies had been Both the duration of the copulation display and
altered (cf. Dabelsteen & Pedersen 1985). the total reproductive response, as expressed by
A recording of blackbird song lasting 2 min was points scored, varied significantly between the six
Animal Behaviour, 36, 6 1859

I00 the responsiveness of the two sexes is difficult to


explain simply by the laboratory conditions of the

o
50 a 1 b female experiments. We did not test the male and
female results directly against each other; however,
the difference in responsiveness may be real. High-
pitched motif songs may resemble twitters in
frequency, modulations, etc. (e.g. Dabelsteen
"6 0.750.875 I.I I-2 1.4 0.750.875 I'1 1.2 1.4 1984). Therefore, one possibility is that relatively
strong female responses were obtained with songs
I00 whose frequencies were elevated by factors 1.2 and
c)
1.4 because the females somehow perceived the
high-pitched motif songs as powerful twitters. A
50 second interpretation is that the decoding mecha-
nism of female blackbirds is less critical than that of
males as regards the upper part of the spectrum.
The two interpretations are not mutually exclusive,
0-75 0.875 I-I 1"2 I-4 0-75 0-875 I-2 1.4 and they both make sense as proximate explana-
Motif type tions since twitters in another experiment released
strong female responses (Dabelsteen & Pedersen, in
Figure 1. Responses of female blackbirds to playback of press).
unmodified motifs (closed histograms) and motifs We suggest habitat-induced variations in sound
whose frequencies were multiplied by 0.75, 0.875, 1.1, degradation as one of the forces ultimately respon-
1.2, or 1.4 (open histograms). (a) Means for the
duration of sexual behaviour. (b) Overall means for the sible for the sexual variation in the decoding
frequencies of nest-building behaviours. (c) Overall mechanism of blackbirds. Females should be less
means for the other bebaviours. (d) Means for points discriminatory than males when using high song
scored (see text for explanation). SESare indicated for frequencies as identifying cues, because frequencies
sexual behaviour and for points. higher than 3-4 kHz may degrade more when
passing through dense vegetation to females than
when passing the more open communication chan-
types of song (P<0.01 and <0.001, respectively; nel to other males (Wiley & Richards 1982). Other
Fig. 1). A pairwise comparison using the Wilcoxon forces, e.g. sexual selection, may tend to make
critical range method (Colquhoun 1971) showed females more discriminating towards song struc-
that unaltered motif songs and motif songs modi- ture. This may also apply to the blackbird, mani-
fied by the factor 1.1 both scored more points than festing itself in the song variation tolerances for
the 0.75 modification (P < 0-05), and that the 1.1 song features other than the frequency level.
modification released more copulation displays We thank M. Calford, O. N. Larsen and H. Lind
than the 0.75 modification (P<0.01) and the 1.4 for their comments on the manuscript. The study
modification (P < 0.05). Similar trends were found was made possible by a Niels Bohr fellowship given
for the other behaviour patterns, and orientating by the Royal Danish Academy of Sciences and
head movements and jumps varied significantly Letters and the Tuborg Foundation, and by a
between the test songs (P<0.01 and <0.05, re- fellowship given by the Carlsberg Foundation, and
spectively). Orientating head movements were by the Danish Nature Research Council (grant no.
released less frequently by the 0-75 modification 11-4153).
than by unaltered motif songs (P<0.01) and the
TORBEN DABELSTEEN*
modifications 1.1 (P < 0.05) and 1-4 (P < 0-05), and
SIMON BOEL PEDERSENt
the unaltered motif songs released more jumps than
* Institute o f Population Biology,
the 0-75 modification ( P < 0.05).
Copenhagen University,
The variation in females' responsiveness to
Universitetsparken 15,
modifications 0"75, 0.875 and 1-1 was expected
DK-2100 Copenhagen O, Denmark.
from the way the coding of the frequency para-
t The Acoustics Laboratory,
meters was changed, and it is consistent with the
Technical University,
results of the previous experiments with males
DK-2000 Lyngby, Denmark.
(Dabelsteen & Pedersen 1985). However, unlike
those of the females, the males' responses were also
reliably reduced by modifications 1-2 and i -4. As all References
modifications resulted in frequencies well within Becker, P. H. 1982. The coding of species-specificcharac-
the hearing range of passerines, such a difference in teristics in bird sounds. In: Acoustic Communication in
1860 Animal Behaviour, 36, 6

Birds. Vol. 1 (Ed. by D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller), Dabelsteen, T. & Pedersen, S. B. In press. Song parts
pp. 131-181. New York: Academic Press. adapted to function both at long and short ranges may
Colquhoun, D. 1971. Lectures on Biostatistics. Oxford: communicate information about the species to female
Clarendon Press. blackbirds Turdus merula. Ornis Scand.
Dabelsteen, T. 1984. An analysis of the full song of the Shiovitz, K. A. & Lemon, R. E. 1980. Species identifica-
blackbird Turdus merula with respect to message tion of song by the indigo bunting as determined by
coding and adaptations for acoustic communication. responses to computer-generated sounds. Behaviour,
Ornis Scand., 15, 227-239. 74, 167-199.
Dabelsteen, T. 1988. The meaning of the full song of the Wiley, R. H. & Richards, D. G. 1982. Adaptations for
blackbird Turdus merula to untreated and estradiol- acoustic communication in birds: sound transmission
treated females. Ornis Seand., 19, 7-16. and signal detection. In: Acoustic Communication in
Dabelsteen, T. & Pedersen, S. B. 1985. Correspondence Birds Vol. 1 (Ed. by D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller),
between messages in the full song of the blackbird pp. 131-181. New York: Academic Press.
Turdus merula and meanings to territorial males, as
inferred from responses to computerized modifications (Received 20 January 1988; revised 27 April 1988;
of natural song. Z. Tierpsychol., 69, 149-165. MS. number: sc-422)

You might also like