Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

474121

2013
AUT0010.1177/1362361312474121AutismMazurek

Article
Autism

Loneliness, friendship, and well-being in 2014, Vol. 18(3) 223­–232


© The Author(s) 2013
Reprints and permissions:
adults with autism spectrum disorders sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1362361312474121
aut.sagepub.com

Micah O Mazurek

Abstract
This study examined the relations among loneliness, friendship, and emotional functioning in adults (N = 108) with
autism spectrum disorders. Participants completed self-report measures of symptoms of autism spectrum disorders,
loneliness, number and nature of friendships, depression, anxiety, life satisfaction, and self-esteem. The results indicated
that loneliness was associated with increased depression and anxiety and decreased life satisfaction and self-esteem, even
after controlling for symptoms of autism spectrum disorders. In addition, greater quantity and quality of friendships were
associated with decreased loneliness among adults with autism spectrum disorders. Multivariate models indicated that
friendship did not moderate the relationship between loneliness and well-being; however, number of friends provided
unique independent effects in predicting self-esteem, depression, and anxiety above and beyond the effects of loneliness.
This was the first study to examine the relations among these aspects of social and emotional functioning in adults with
autism spectrum disorders, and the results indicate that this topic warrants further clinical and research attention.

Keywords
adults, anxiety, autism spectrum disorders, depression, friendship, loneliness

Introduction
Individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) experi- with ASDs are at significantly greater risk of mood and
ence significant difficulties in social functioning, including anxiety disorders than are individuals in the general popu-
deficits in core social abilities and problems establishing lation (Gillott and Standen, 2007; Mazefsky et al., 2008;
and maintaining peer relationships (American Psychiatric Moseley et al., 2011). Thus, identifying factors that may be
Association, 2000). These social and interpersonal prob- associated with social and emotional functioning for adults
lems generally persist and sometimes worsen in adulthood, with ASDs is highly important.
contributing to functional impairment (Seltzer et al., 2004).
Despite the steady increase in ASD prevalence (Centers for Loneliness
Disease Control and Prevention, 2012), few studies have
focused on the experiences of adults with ASDs. There is considerable evidence that loneliness and social
Existing research indicates that adults with ASDs face support have a strong influence on overall well-being in the
many challenges in terms of occupational success, inde- general population. However, this has yet to be examined
pendent living, and relationship formation (Billstedt et al., among adults with ASDs. Contrary to historical assump-
2005; Eaves and Ho, 2008; Howlin et al., 2004). In previ- tions, individuals with ASDs do not always prefer social
ous studies, only a small percentage of adults with ASDs isolation. They often want to develop relationships but face
were reported to have friendships or romantic relationships
(Billstedt et al., 2011; Engstrom et al., 2003; Howlin et al.,
2004). There is also evidence that increased awareness of University of Missouri, USA
social deficits is associated with both depression and anx-
Corresponding author:
iety among individuals with ASDs (Mazurek and Kanne,
Micah O Mazurek, Department of Health Psychology, Thompson
2010; Sterling et al., 2008). Rates of depressive symp- Center for Autism and Neurodevelopmental Disorders, University of
toms increase with age among individuals with ASDs Missouri, 205 Portland Street, Columbia, MO 65211, USA.
(Ghaziuddin et al., 2002; Sterling et al., 2008), and adults Email: mazurekm@missouri.edu

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE LAVAL on January 24, 2015


224 Autism 18(3)

barriers in developing relationships due to social compe- samples to examine the relations between characteristics of
tence difficulties (Bauminger et al., 2003; Causton- the broader autism phenotype and social-emotional func-
Theoharis et al., 2009). Thus, loneliness appears to be a tioning. Jobe and White (2007) found that autism traits
clinically important consideration for this population. among young adults in the general population were associ-
Loneliness is generally understood to reflect a negative ated with increased self-reported loneliness. Pollmann et al.
emotional state arising from a perception of unfulfilled (2010) also used a nonclinical population to examine the
social needs (Peplau and Perlman, 1982; Russell et al., associations between autism traits and relationship satisfac-
1984). By contrast, social isolation is an objective measure tion within romantic relationships. The results indicated
of social contact, and does not imply subjective appraisal that higher autism traits were associated with decreased
(either positive or negative). In fact, solitude may be per- self-reported relationship satisfaction among men, but
ceived positively by some individuals, while loneliness loneliness was not specifically assessed.
may be experienced by others even in the presence of
objective social relationships (Heinrich and Gullone, 2006).
Friendship in individuals with ASDs
As such, loneliness and social isolation are related, yet con-
ceptually distinct. Individuals with ASDs are at risk of difficulties forming
A need for social connection has long been considered a and maintaining friendships, which may place them at
fundamental aspect of the human experience. As argued by greater risk of experiencing loneliness. Previous studies
Baumeister and Leary (1995), this basic drive is believed to have found that children with ASDs have fewer friends
underlie a range of other psychological processes, and to than typically developing children (Bauminger and
represent a universal human motivation. Likewise, a lack of Shulman, 2003; Koning and Magill-Evans, 2001), and that
belonging is associated with a range of negative psychoso- their friendships are characterized by shorter duration and
cial effects, ranging from physical health to psychological less frequent get-togethers (Bauminger and Shulman,
well-being. Among individuals in the general population, 2003). Furthermore, there is evidence that peer engage-
loneliness has been consistently linked with negative emo- ment appears to decline over the course of childhood for
tional outcomes, including decreased self-esteem (Davis children with ASDs (Rotheram-Fuller et al., 2010).
et al., 1992; Schultz and Moore, 1988), reduced satisfaction Rowley et al. (2012) found that only 34% of children with
with life and perceived meaning (Goodwin et al., 2001; ASDs were reported by parents to have at least one good
Stillman et al., 2009), increased depression (Cacioppo et al., friend, as compared to 71% of children with other special
2006b; Wei et al., 2005), and increased anxiety (Cacioppo educational needs and 93% of typically developing chil-
et al., 2006a). dren. Locke et al. (2010) also found that adolescents with
ASDs reported poorer quality friendships than typically
developing peers, particularly with regard to companion-
Loneliness in individuals with ASDs ship and helpfulness.
There has been an increasing emphasis on understanding Relatively fewer studies have examined the role of
the potential impact of social difficulties on emotional friendship in the lives of older adolescents and adults with
functioning among individuals with ASDs. However, the ASDs. Orsmond et al. (2004) examined the social experi-
vast majority of this research has been conducted among ences of a large sample of adolescents and adults with
children. Across most studies, children with ASDs have ASDs who were living with parents. Only 8.1% of the
reported greater loneliness than typically developing chil- sample had at least one close reciprocal friendship, and
dren, including both social and emotional aspects of loneli- 20.9% had at least one relationship with a peer that
ness (Bauminger et al., 2003, 2004; Bauminger and Kasari, involved some shared activities. However, almost half the
2000; Lasgaard et al., 2010; Locke et al., 2010). Children sample had no peer relationships at all. In addition, few of
with ASDs also experience high rates of loneliness despite the participants socialized weekly with either friends
the fact that their understanding of the affective compo- (20.9%) or people from work or school (13%). More
nents of the construct may be less complete than that of recently, Shattuck et al. (2011) examined social participa-
their typically developing peers (Bauminger et al., 2003). tion among adolescents (ages 13–17 years) with ASDs
There is also evidence that loneliness is associated with using data from the National Longitudinal Transition
negative emotional outcomes for children with ASDs. For Study-2 (NLTS2). They found that 43% of adolescents
example, in previous studies of children with ASDs, loneli- with ASDs never got together with friends outside of
ness has been associated with decreased feelings of self- school or organized activities. Similarly, over half the
worth (Bauminger et al., 2004) and increased social anxiety sample never received phone calls from friends or were
(White and Roberson-Nay, 2009). invited to social activities by friends. In addition, findings
In contrast, studies have yet begun to explore the expe- revealed that these indicators of social participation were
rience of loneliness among adults with ASD diagnoses. significantly lower than among adolescents with other
However, some studies have used general population types of disabilities.

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE LAVAL on January 24, 2015


Mazurek 225

Friendship as a protective factor not significantly associated with anxiety and depression in
a sample of children with ASDs.
Given that individuals with ASDs may be at risk of experi-
encing loneliness, and that loneliness is associated with
negative emotional outcomes, identifying factors that may Current study
serve a protective function is important. Close friendship In summary, there have been no previous studies of the
may represent a potential buffer against negative effects of associations among loneliness, friendship, and well-being
social difficulties on well-being among adults with ASDs. In in adults with ASDs. This knowledge would provide impor-
fact, there is a precedent for this in previous research among tant information about whether friendship relationships
both typically developing children and those with ASDs. may serve an emotionally protective role for adults with
Among typically developing children, having at least one ASDs. Thus, based on previous research, the following
good friend is associated with greater well-being even after hypotheses were proposed: (1) loneliness will be associated
controlling for overall social success in the larger peer group with decreased well-being among adults with ASDs, (2)
(Bukowski et al., 1994; Hodges et al., 1999; Linsey, 2002). friendship will be associated with decreased loneliness
Parker and Asher (1993) found that children who had a close among adults with ASDs, and (3) friendship will moderate
friend experienced less loneliness than those without a close the relationship between loneliness and well-being among
friend, even when they were not accepted by the larger peer adults with ASDs.
group. In addition, greater friendship quality was associated
with decreased loneliness, even after controlling for social
acceptance. More recent research has shown that both quan- Methods
tity and quality of children’s friendships provide unique
contributions to emotional functioning, including loneliness
Participants
and depression (Nangle et al., 2003). The sample for this study included 108 adults with ASDs
Among children and adolescents with ASDs, friendship ranging in age from 18 to 62 years (M = 32.4 years, stand-
and social support also appear to influence perceived lone- ard deviation (SD) = 12.5 years), who were recruited with
liness. For example, Bauminger et al. (2004) found that the assistance of a national open enrollment ASD registry,
children with ASDs who reported higher quality friend- the Interactive Autism Network (IAN) Research Database
ships also reported less loneliness and greater overall self- at Kennedy Krieger Institute, and Johns Hopkins Medicine,
worth. In a more recent study, Whitehouse et al. (2009) Baltimore, Maryland. Participants were recruited by email
found that adolescents with Asperger’s syndrome (AS) and completed all measures online using a web-based for-
reported significantly poorer quality friendships, and lower mat. Participants were provided with a US$15 gift card for
scores on a measure of friendship motivation as compared participation in the study, and recruitment continued until
to typically developing adolescents. In addition, the adoles- the target sample size was achieved. Eligibility require-
cents with AS also reported higher levels of loneliness and ments included age range (18 years of age or above), ability
depression than did typically developing adolescents. to complete measures independently, and a previous pro-
Friendship quality was negatively associated with loneli- fessional diagnosis of ASDs, including autism or autistic
ness in both groups, but was not significantly associated disorder (29.6%), AS (63.9%), or pervasive developmental
with depressive symptoms in either group. Similarly, disorder, not otherwise specified (NOS) (6.5%). The major-
Lasgaard et al. (2010) found that self-reported loneliness ity of the sample was male (52.8%) and Caucasian (88.0%).
among adolescent boys with ASDs was negatively corre- Additional sample characteristics are presented in Table 1.
lated with perceived social support from classmates,
friends, and parents.
There is also some evidence that friendship may offer Measures
additional protective benefits for children with ASDs. For Participants completed a demographic and history form
example, having at least one reciprocal friendship in ele- designed for this study. Information included age, race,
mentary school was found to be associated with greater diagnostic information, relationship status, employment,
peer acceptance among children with ASDs (Rotheram- living arrangements, and specific information about friend-
Fuller et al., 2010). However, the associations between ship, as described below.
friendship and emotional functioning have been somewhat
mixed in other previous studies. For example, Mazurek and
Kanne (2010) found that children and adolescents with ASD symptoms
ASDs who had friendships that were limited in reciprocity Symptoms of ASDs were assessed using the Autism
had greater anxiety and depression than either those with Spectrum Quotient–Short (AQ-Short) (Hoekstra et al.,
good quality friendship or those with no friends. However, 2011), an abbreviated 28-item version of the original
Kelly et al. (2008) found that peer relationship quality was 50-item self-report questionnaire, the AQ (Baron-Cohen

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE LAVAL on January 24, 2015


226 Autism 18(3)

Table 1. Social and demographic characteristics.

Total sample (N = 108)


Residence
Live alone 19.4%
Live with parent(s) 38.0%
Live with partner, spouse, or roommate 28.7%
Other 13.9%
Employment
Full-time job 24.1%
Part-time job 25.0%
Not employed, student 24.1%
Not employed, not student 26.9%
Relationship status
Married 22.2%
In a romantic relationship 2.8%
Divorced/separated 6.5%
Single 58.3%
Friendship status
Has a close or best friend 60.2%
Does not have a close or best friend 39.8%
Sample with best friend (n = 64)
Frequency of visits with close friend
About once a day 10.8%
3–5 days per week 18.5%
1–2 days per week 18.5%
Every few weeks 27.7%
Less than once per month 24.6%
Frequency of phone contact with close friend
Several times per day 4.6%
About once a day 9.2%
3–5 days per week 13.8%
1–2 days per week 18.5%
Every few weeks 23.1%
Less than once per month 30.8%
Frequency of electronic communication with close friend
Several times per day 29.2%
About once a day 18.5%
3–5 days per week 16.9%
1–2 days per week 13.8%
Every few weeks 9.2%
Less than once per month 12.3%

et al., 2001). The AQ was originally developed to assess Hoekstra et al., 2011). This study utilized this approach for
symptoms of autism along a continuum, using a quantita- scoring the AQ-Short, resulting in a total score ranging
tive approach. Items are rated on a 4-point scale, ranging from 28 to 112, with higher scores indicating greater autism
from definitely agree to definitely disagree. The AQ-Short traits. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of .85 in the current
has demonstrated good psychometric properties, including sample indicated solid internal consistency.
strong reliability and validity among general population
and clinical samples. The original scoring for the AQ uti-
Loneliness
lized a dichotomous approach for each item (with a possi-
ble range of scores of 0–50) (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). Loneliness was assessed using the 8-item version of the
However, subsequent studies have utilized the full range of University of California, Los Angeles Loneliness Scale
responses in order to provide a more fine-grained differen- (ULS-8) (Hays and DiMatteo, 1987). The ULS is one of the
tiation among scores in subsequent analysis (Austin, 2005; most widely used measures of loneliness and has been used

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE LAVAL on January 24, 2015


Mazurek 227

Table 2. Ms, SDs, and correlation coefficients (N = 108).


Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
AQ–Short 80.6 12.2
Loneliness 20.9 4.7 .30**
Depression 8.4 6.2 .09 .48**
Anxiety 7.4 5.4 .11 .34** .73**
Life satisfaction 18.8 8.1 –.23* −.46** −.40** −24*
Self–esteem 17.7 4.9 −.22* −.38** −.57** −.54** .54**
Number of friends 3.1 3.1 −.41** −.34** .03 .06 .17 −.01
URCS 5.1 1.2 −.23 −.29* −.001 −.05 .14 −.03 .24

AQ–Short: Autism Spectrum Quotient–Short; URCS: Unidimensional Relationship Closeness Scale; M: Mean; SD: standard deviation.
*p < .05; **p < .01; **p < .001.

extensively in both the general population and clinical sam- Items are rated on a 7-point scale, ranging from strongly
ples (Russell, 1996; Russell et al., 1980). The ULS-8 has disagree to strongly agree with higher total scores indicat-
demonstrated strong reliability and validity in previous ing greater overall life satisfaction. The SWLS has been
studies (Hays and DiMatteo, 1987), and Cronbach’s alpha used extensively in clinical and nonclinical population
in the current sample was .80. Items are rated on a 4-point samples, and demonstrates excellent construct validity and
scale, ranging from never to always, with higher total test–retest reliability (Pavot and Diener, 1993), and
scores indicating greater loneliness. Cronbach’s alpha in the current sample was .89.
Self-esteem was assessed using the Rosenberg Self-
Esteem Scale (RSE) (Rosenberg, 1965), a 10-item self-
Friendship
report measure assessing general self-concept. The RSE
Friendship status was assessed using a questionnaire devel- represents the most widely used instrument designed to
oped for this study. Participants responded to the following assess self-esteem, and has shown strong psychometric
questions: “How many friends do you have right now?” properties, including reliability, validity, and cross-cultural
and “Do you have a close or best friend (who is not a mem- equivalence (Corwyn, 2000; Schmitt and Allik, 2005) Items
ber of your family)?” Participants who reported having a are rated on a 4-point scale, ranging from strongly agree to
close friend (n = 64), then responded to the following ques- strongly disagree with higher total scores indicating greater
tions: “How often do you get together with your close self-esteem. Cronbach’s alpha in the current sample was .80.
friend?” “How often do you talk on the phone with your Depression and anxiety were assessed using the Patient
close friend?” and “How often do you use email, Facebook, Health Questionnaire (PHQ) (Spitzer et al., 1999). The
instant messaging, or texting to talk with your close friend?” PHQ contains separate modules designed for specific
See Table 2 for response options. symptom types, and was designed to be consistent with
Friendship quality was assessed using the Unidimensional Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders cri-
Relationship Closeness Scale (URCS), a recently developed teria for particular diagnoses (American Psychiatric
self-report measure of perceived closeness in personal rela- Association, 1994). For this study, the 9-item depression
tionships (Dibble et al., 2012). The URCS is a 12-item self- and the 7-item generalized anxiety modules were used. All
report scale that assesses the relationship closeness between items are rated on a 4-point scale ranging from not at all to
two persons. Items are rated on a 7-point scale, ranging from nearly every day, with total scores indicating greater symp-
strongly disagree to strongly agree, with higher total scores tom severity. Both the depression and generalized anxiety
indicating greater degree of intimacy. As suggested by the modules of the PHQ have demonstrated excellent validity,
authors, item 6 was omitted from the scale for this study due sensitivity, and specificity (Kroenke et al., 2001, 2010;
to poor discriminant validity, resulting in an 11-item scale. Spitzer et al., 1999). Cronbach’s alpha in the current sam-
In the development study, the URCS demonstrated very ple ranged from .86 for the depression scale to .88 for the
good construct validity, convergent validity, and reliability anxiety scale.
for both romantic and friendship relationships. Cronbach’s
alpha in the current sample was .88. Only participants who
reported having a close friend completed the URCS (n = 64) Data analyses
in this study. Descriptive statistics, including percentage, mean, standard
deviation, and range, were calculated to characterize the
sample. To test hypothesis 1, Pearson’s correlations were
Well-being computed to examine the bivariate relationships between
Life satisfaction was assessed with the widely used 5-item loneliness and each well-being variable (including life
Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) (Diener et al., 1985). satisfaction, self-esteem, depression, and anxiety). To test

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE LAVAL on January 24, 2015


228 Autism 18(3)

hypothesis 2, Pearson’s correlations were computed to F(1, 102) = 5.2, p = .02. To determine whether friendship
examine the relationship between loneliness and both num- contributed to decreased loneliness even after controlling
ber of friends, and friendship closeness (for those with a for the effects of ASD symptoms, a subsequent linear
best friend). One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was regression analysis was conducted with loneliness as the
conducted to examine differences in loneliness between dependent variable, and close friend (dichotomous) and AQ
individuals with and without a close friend. Finally, to test as independent variables. The results indicated that both
hypothesis 3, linear regression analyses were conducted to close friendship (β = −.22, p = .02) and AQ (β = .28, p =
examine the unique contributions of friendship and loneli- .004) provided significant main effects in predicting
ness as well as the interaction between the two in contribut- loneliness.
ing to well-being. Separate analyses were conducted for Also consistent with predictions from the second hypoth-
each dependent variable of interest, including life satisfac- esis, number of friends was negatively correlated with lone-
tion, self-esteem, depression, and anxiety. liness (r = −.34, p = .001). Again, to determine whether
number of friends contributed to decreased loneliness even
after controlling for the effects of ASD symptoms, a linear
Results regression analysis was conducted with loneliness as the
Initial one-way ANOVAs were conducted to test for poten- dependent variable, and number of friends and AQ as inde-
tial differences in study variables with regard to gender, pendent variables. The results indicated that number of
race, employment, marital status, or residence. Groups did friends provided significant main effects (β = −.29, p =
not differ at the p < .05 level with regard to loneliness, anxi- .007), but that AQ was not a significant predictor.
ety, depression, life satisfaction, self-esteem, number of Finally, for those who reported having a best friend, the
friends, or friendship quality. In addition, age was not sig- closeness of the friendship relationship was negatively
nificantly correlated with any of these variables of interest. correlated with loneliness (r = −.29, p = .02). To deter-
Finally, Chi-square tests revealed no significant differences mine whether relationship closeness contributed to
in any of the demographic variables when comparing those decreased loneliness after controlling for the effects of
with and without a close friend. ASD symptoms, a subsequent linear regression analysis
was conducted with loneliness as the dependent variable,
and friendship closeness and AQ as the independent vari-
Loneliness and well-being ables. The results indicated that AQ (β = .26, p = .04) pro-
As predicted, loneliness was significantly correlated with vided significant main effects in predicting loneliness,
all emotional functioning variables. Specifically, loneliness and that friendship closeness approached significance
was positively correlated with depression (r = .48, p < .001) (β = −.23, p = .06).
and anxiety (r = .34, p = .001), and negatively correlated
with both satisfaction with life (r = −.46, p < .001) and self-
esteem (r = −.38, p < .001).
Friendship, loneliness, and well-being
Given that AQ was also correlated with loneliness and To examine the extent to which friendship moderates the
with some dependent variables of interest (see Table 2), a relationship between loneliness and well-being (after
series of linear regression analyses were conducted to controlling for ASD symptoms), a series of hierarchical
determine whether loneliness contributed to the prediction linear regression analyses were conducted with the well-
of these variables after controlling for AQ. Loneliness pro- being variable as the outcome. Within each model, loneli-
vided significant main effects in the models predicting ness, friendship, and AQ were entered as independent
depression (β = .49, p < .001), anxiety (β = .32, p = .002), variables in step 1, and the interaction between loneliness
satisfaction with life (β = −.42, p < .001), and self-esteem (β and friendship was entered in step 2. Prior to analysis, the
= −.32, p = .001). In contrast, AQ did not contribute to the dependent variables were mean centered, and the interac-
prediction of well-being in any model. Thus, the results tion terms were computed using centered variables.
indicate that loneliness contributes significantly to Interaction terms were not significant predictors in any
increased depression and anxiety and decreased life satis- model; thus, interaction terms were dropped from all sub-
faction and self-esteem, even after controlling for the sequent models.
effects of ASD symptoms. Results from these models are presented in Table 3. For
the models examining the contributions of having a best
friend to the prediction of well-being, loneliness provided
Friendship and loneliness
significant main effects in all models (ranging from p =
Consistent with the second hypothesis, one-way ANOVA .001 to p < .001), while neither having a best friend nor AQ
results revealed that adults with a close friend reported sig- score were significant predictors in any model (including
nificantly lower levels of loneliness (M = 20.1, SD = 4.8) those predicting life satisfaction, self-esteem, depression,
than those without a close friend (M = 22.2, SD = 4.2), or anxiety).

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE LAVAL on January 24, 2015


Mazurek 229

Table 3. Linear regression analyses: loneliness and friendship variables predicting well-being.

Independent variables Life satisfaction Self-esteem Depression Anxiety

Adjusted R2 β Adjusted R2 β Adjusted R2 β Adjusted R2 β


Model 1 .20 .12 .21 .087
Loneliness −.44*** −.33** .51*** .34**
Best friend (yes/no) −.11 −.02 .11 .11
AQ-Short −.11 −.12 −−.05 .16
Model 2 .20 .17 .26 .12
Loneliness −.42*** −.39*** .56*** .38***
Number of friends −.02 −.24* .22* .22*
AQ-Short −.14 −.22* .05 .09
Model 3 .26 .19 .31 .16
Loneliness −.50*** −.48*** .62*** .44**
Friendship closeness −.03 −.19 .16 .09
AQ-Short −.11 −.07 −.07 .07

AQ-Short: Autism Spectrum Quotient–Short.


Interaction terms (loneliness × friendship variable) were not significant predictors in any model, and were dropped from all subsequent models.
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

Similarly, in the models examining the contributions of Interestingly, the associations between loneliness and
friendship closeness to the prediction of well-being, loneli- emotional functioning remained significant even after con-
ness provided significant main effects in all models (again trolling for the effects of ASD symptoms. This finding
ranging from p = .001 to p < .001), while neither friendship lends support to the idea that loneliness may be a secondary
closeness nor AQ score were significant predictors in any consequence of social difficulties for individuals with
model (including those predicting life satisfaction, self- ASDs, and that these consequences may have emotional
esteem, depression, or anxiety). repercussions above and beyond the effects of social
Finally, in the models examining the contributions of impairment. Adults with ASDs who desire social connec-
number of friends to the prediction of well-being, signifi- tion, but who perceive that those social needs are unful-
cant main effects were observed for number of friends in filled, may be particularly vulnerable to depressed mood
most models. While loneliness was the only significant and a decreased sense of self-worth. These findings are
predictor of life satisfaction (p < .001), number of friends consistent with an emerging body of work examining the
provided additional significant effects in the models pre- associations between loneliness and emotional functioning
dicting self-esteem (p = .03), depression (p = .03), and in children with ASDs (Bauminger et al., 2004; White and
anxiety (p = .04), even after controlling for the effects of Roberson-Nay, 2009), and indicate that the interplay
ASD symptoms. between social and emotional experiences is similar for
adults with ASDs. Alternatively, it is also possible that
underlying depression and anxiety may give rise to feelings
Discussion of loneliness and increased social isolation. Longitudinal
This was the first study to examine the associations between studies are needed to examine the relationships between
loneliness, friendship, and well-being among adults with these constructs over time.
ASDs. A primary finding from this study is that loneliness A second important finding from this study was that
was significantly correlated with increased depression and friendship was associated with decreased loneliness for
anxiety, and decreased life satisfaction and self-esteem. adults with ASDs. This is highly consistent with findings
The effect sizes ranged from medium to large, with the from studies of children and adolescents in the general pop-
strongest association existing between loneliness and ulation (Nangle et al., 2003; Parker and Asher, 1993), and
depression. These findings are consistent with a large body from studies of children and adolescents with ASDs
of literature on loneliness in the general population (Bauminger et al., 2004; Whitehouse et al., 2009). These
(Cacioppo et al., 2006a, 2006b; Davis et al., 1992), and results indicate that different aspects of friendship, includ-
extend our knowledge about the social and emotional lives ing having a close friend, the strength of the friendship rela-
of adults with ASDs. Contrary to the idea that individuals tionship, and the size of the friendship network are all
with ASDs do not experience or are not affected by loneli- significantly related to loneliness in adults with ASDs.
ness, these results show that loneliness can be associated Additionally, friendship (i.e. having a best friend and num-
with negative emotional experiences for adults with ASDs, ber of friends) was associated with decreased loneliness
and is worthy of increased clinical and research attention. even when controlling for the effects of ASD symptoms.

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE LAVAL on January 24, 2015


230 Autism 18(3)

The third aim of the study was to examine the extent to a study focused on social and emotional functioning.
which friendship may moderate the effects of loneliness on Although there were no gender differences in any of the
emotional functioning in adults with ASDs. Counter to social or emotional variables assessed, the potential for this
these predictions, the results did not show evidence of mod- bias remains possible. Similarly, the percentage of individ-
eration. Specifically, loneliness by friendship interaction uals in the current sample who reported having close friends
terms were not significant predictors in any of the regres- was much higher than has been reported in previous studies
sion models. Instead, loneliness appeared to account for the (e.g. Orsmond et al., 2004; Shattuck et al., 2011). Again,
largest amount of variance in all models, above and beyond this may reflect participation bias, or it may reflect differ-
the effects of both ASD symptoms and friendship variables; ences in methodology. For example, this study utilized self-
and this relationship did not differ as a function of friend- report of friendship number and quality, while previous
ship status. This was the case for models examining self- studies (e.g. Orsmond et al., 2004; Shattuck et al., 2011)
esteem, life satisfaction, depression, and anxiety. However, used parent-report for these constructs.
further examination of these results reveals that the quan- Finally, the participants were enrolled in a national
tity of friendships does appear to play an important role in autism network, and may differ from the general ASD pop-
emotional functioning. In fact, the total number of friend- ulation in additional undetermined ways. Aside from AQ
ships provided significant main effects in predicting self- scores and DSM-IV diagnostic categories, additional infor-
esteem, depression, and anxiety, above and beyond the mation about participants’ ASD diagnoses was not availa-
effects of both loneliness and ASD symptoms. These find- ble. Specifically, information was not collected regarding
ings suggest that decreasing loneliness and increasing the the professional qualifications of the clinicians who pro-
total social network may have a significant impact on over- vided the diagnoses, nor about the diagnostic battery. A bet-
all well-being for adults with ASDs. ter characterization of the participants, including objective
measures of ASD symptoms, cognitive functioning, and
adaptive skills would be informative in future research.
Limitations This study also relied on self-report for all measured
This study was limited in several ways. First, the study uti- constructs. Because internalizing symptoms, self-esteem
lized a cross-sectional design. Therefore, issues of timing and life satisfaction, are primarily subjective experiences,
or causality among these variables cannot be determined. self-report is typically preferred in general population stud-
In future studies, longitudinal designs will be most inform- ies of these constructs. However, previous studies have
ative in examining these issues as they unfold over time. suggested that individuals with ASD struggle with under-
Longitudinal studies among both child and adult samples standing and describing emotional experiences (Ben
would allow for a better understanding of potential causal Shalom et al., 2006; Losh and Capps, 2006). Future work in
relationships among variables, as well as an examination of this area may also benefit from the inclusion of multiple
developmental shifts in social and emotional functioning. reporters, including perceptions of friendship quality from
Findings from longitudinal studies in the general popula- both members of the friendship dyad. This would allow for
tion indicate that loneliness predicts increases in depression a more nuanced understanding of the quality of dyadic
over time, even after controlling for initial levels of depres- friendship relationships, and the extent to which concord-
sion and other psychosocial variables (Cacioppo et al., ance or discordance in characterizing the relationship
2006b). Although there has been some evidence of a recip- relates to other social and emotional variables.
rocal relationship between these variables over time
(Cacioppo et al., 2006b), a recent 5-year longitudinal study
Conclusion
found evidence that loneliness predicted changes in depres-
sive symptoms across time, while the reverse was not true This study provides an important first look at the social and
(Cacioppo et al., 2010). These issues require further study emotional experiences of adults with ASDs. These findings
among adults with ASDs to determine whether the relation- extend current knowledge in several important ways, and
ships between loneliness and emotional functioning are highlight the clinical importance of loneliness. Significant
unidirectional or bidirectional over time. correlations were found between loneliness and a number
A second limitation of this study is that the sample may of negative emotional experiences, including increased
not be representative of the larger population of adults with depression and anxiety, and reduced well-being. Given that
ASDs. The gender ratio in the current sample, with nearly individuals with ASDs are already at increased risk of anxi-
equal numbers of males and females, was not comparable ety and depression (Gillott and Standen, 2007; Mazefsky
to that of the broader ASD population, which is closer to a et al., 2008; Moseley et al., 2011), loneliness may represent
4:1 male:female ratio (Fombonne, 2005). The reasons for an important mechanism in the developmental course of
this are not clear, but may be an indication of potential sam- internalizing problems through adolescence and adulthood.
pling bias. That is, females, or those who are more socially A better understanding of the developmental course and
interested, may have been more motivated to participate in consequences of loneliness will be essential to inform our

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE LAVAL on January 24, 2015


Mazurek 231

understanding of social and emotional well-being in indi- Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC and Thisted RA (2010) Perceived
viduals with ASDs. Finally, strategies to reduce and prevent social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses
loneliness in adults with ASDs appear to be important tar- of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago
gets for future research and clinical practice. Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. Psychology and
Aging 25(2): 453–463.
Declaration of conflicting interest Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC, Ernst JM, et al. (2006a) Loneliness
within a nomological net: an evolutionary perspective.
The author declares that there is no conflict of interest. Journal of Research in Personality 40(6): 1054–1085.
Cacioppo JT, Hughes ME, Waite LJ, et al. (2006b) Loneliness as a
Funding specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: cross-sectional and
This work was supported by the University of Missouri School of longitudinal analyses. Psychology and Aging 21(1): 140–151.
Health Professions Catalyst Award. Causton-Theoharis J, Ashby C and Cosier M (2009) Islands of
loneliness: exploring social interaction through the auto-
biographies of individuals with autism. Intellectual and
References Developmental Disabilities 47(2): 84–96.
American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2012) Prevalence
Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American of autism spectrum disorders—Autism and Developmental
Psychiatric Association. Disabilities Monitoring Network, 14 sites, United States,
American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and 2008. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report: Surveillance
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR). 4th ed., Summaries 61(SS03): 1–19.
text rev. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Corwyn RF (2000) The factor structure of global self-esteem
Austin EJ (2005) Personality correlates of the broader autism phe- among adolescents and adults. Journal of Research in
notype as assessed by the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ). Personality 34(4): 357–379.
Personality and Individual Differences 38(2): 451–460. Davis SF, Hanson H, Edson R, et al. (1992) The relationship between
Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, et al. (2001) The optimism-pessimism, loneliness, and level of self-esteem in col-
Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger lege students. College Student Journal 26(2): 244–247.
syndrome/high-functioning Autism, males and females, Dibble JL, Levine TR and Park HS (2012) The Unidimensional
scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Relationship Closeness Scale (URCS): reliability and valid-
Developmental Disorders 31(1): 5–17. ity evidence for a new measure of relationship closeness.
Baumeister RF and Leary MR (1995) The need to belong: desire Psychological Assessment 24(3): 565–572.
for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human moti- Diener E, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, et al. (1985) The Satisfaction
vation. Psychological Bulletin 117(3): 497–529. with Life Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment 49(1): 71–75.
Bauminger N and Kasari C (2000) Loneliness and friendship in Eaves LC and Ho HH (2008) Young adult outcome of autism
high-functioning children with autism. Child Development spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental
71(2): 447–456. Disorders 38(4): 739–747.
Bauminger N and Shulman C (2003) The development and Engstrom I, Ekstrom L and Emilsson B (2003) Psychosocial func-
maintenance of friendship in high-functioning children with tioning in a group of Swedish adults with Asperger syndrome
autism: maternal perceptions. Autism 7(1): 81–97. or high-functioning autism. Autism 7(1): 99–110.
Bauminger N, Shulman C and Agam G (2003) Peer interaction and Fombonne E (2005) The changing epidemiology of autism.
loneliness in high-functioning children with autism. Journal Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 18(4):
of Autism and Developmental Disorders 33(5): 489–507. 281–294.
Bauminger N, Shulman C and Agam G (2004) The link between Ghaziuddin M, Ghaziuddin N and Greden J (2002) Depression in
perceptions of self and of social relationships in high-func- persons with autism: implications for research and clinical care.
tioning children with autism. Journal of Developmental and Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 32(4): 299–306.
Physical Disabilities 16(2): 193–214. Gillott A and Standen PJ (2007) Levels of anxiety and sources of
Ben Shalom D, Mostofsky SH, Hazlett RL, et al. (2006) Normal stress in adults with autism. Journal of Intellectual Disabilities
physiological emotions but differences in expression of con- 11(4): 359–370.
scious feelings in children with high-functioning autism. Journal Goodwin R, Cook O and Yung Y (2001) Loneliness and life sat-
of Autism and Developmental Disorders 36(3): 395–400. isfaction among three cultural groups. Personal Relationships
Billstedt E, Gillberg C and Gillberg C (2005) Autism after ado- 8(2): 225–230.
lescence: population-based 13- to 22-year follow-up study of Hays RD and DiMatteo MR (1987) A short-form measure of lone-
120 individuals with autism diagnosed in childhood. Journal liness. Journal of Personality Assessment 51(1): 69–81.
of Autism and Developmental Disorders 35(3): 351–360. Heinrich LM and Gullone E (2006) The clinical significance of
Billstedt E, Gillberg IC and Gillberg C (2011) Aspects of quality loneliness: a literature review. Clinical Psychology Review
of life in adults diagnosed with autism in childhood. Autism 26(6): 695–718.
15(1): 7–20. Hodges EVE, Boivin M, Vitaro F, et al. (1999) The power of
Bukowski WM, Hoza B and Boivin M (1994) Measuring friend- friendship: protection against an escalating cycle of peer vic-
ship quality during pre- and early adolescence: the development timization. Developmental Psychology 35(1): 94–101.
and psychometric properties of the Friendship Qualities Scale. Hoekstra R, Vinkhuyzen A, Wheelwright S, et al. (2011) The
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 11(3): 471–484. construction and validation of an abridged version of the

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE LAVAL on January 24, 2015


232 Autism 18(3)

Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ-Short). Journal of Autism Peplau LA and Perlman D (1982) Loneliness: A Sourcebook of
and Developmental Disorders 41(5): 589–596. Current Theory, Research and Therapy. New York: Wiley.
Howlin P, Goode S, Hutton J, et al. (2004) Adult outcome for chil- Pollmann M, Finkenauer C and Begeer S (2010) Mediators of the
dren with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry link between autistic traits and relationship satisfaction in a
45(2): 212–229. non-clinical sample. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Jobe LE and White SW (2007) Loneliness, social relation- Disorders 40(4): 470–478.
ships, and a broader autism phenotype in college students. Rosenberg M (1965) Society and the Adolescent Self-Image.
Personality and Individual Differences 42(8): 1479–1489. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Kelly A, Garnett M, Attwood T, et al. (2008) Autism spectrum Rotheram-Fuller E, Kasari C, Chamberlain B, et al. (2010) Social
symptomatology in children: the impact of family and peer involvement of children with autism spectrum disorders in
relationships. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 36(7): elementary school classrooms. Journal of Child Psychology
1069–1081. and Psychiatry 51(11): 1227–1234.
Koning C and Magill-Evans J (2001) Social and language skills in Rowley E, Chandler S, Baird G, et al. (2012) The experience of
adolescent boys with Asperger syndrome. Autism 5(1): 23–36. friendship, victimization and bullying in children with an
Kroenke K, Spitzer R and Williams J (2001) The PHQ-9: valid- autism spectrum disorder: associations with child character-
ity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General istics and school placement. Research in Autism Spectrum
Internal Medicine 16(9): 606–613. Disorders 6(3): 1126–1134.
Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, et al. (2010) The Patient Russell D, Cutrona CE, Rose J, et al. (1984) Social and emotional
Health Questionnaire Somatic, Anxiety, and Depressive loneliness: an examination of Weiss’s typology of loneliness.
Symptom Scales: a systematic review. General Hospital Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 46(6): 1313–1321.
Psychiatry 32(4): 345–359. Russell D, Peplau LA and Cutrona CE (1980) The revised UCLA
Lasgaard M, Nielsen A, Eriksen ME, et al. (2010) Loneliness and Loneliness Scale: concurrent and discriminant validity evidence.
social support in adolescent boys with autism spectrum disor- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39(3): 472–480.
ders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 40(2): Russell DW (1996) UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): reli-
218–226. ability, validity, and factor structure. Journal of Personality
Linsey EW (2002) Preschool children’s friendships and peer Assessment 66(1): 20–40.
acceptance: links to social competence. Child Study Journal Schmitt DP and Allik J Jr (2005) Simultaneous administration of
32(3): 145–156. the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale in 53 nations: exploring the
Locke J, Ishijima EH, Kasari C, et al. (2010) Loneliness, friend- universal and culture-specific features of global self-esteem.
ship quality and the social networks of adolescents with high- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 89(4): 623–642.
functioning autism in an inclusive school setting. Journal of Schultz NR and Moore D (1988) Loneliness: differences across
Research in Special Educational Needs 10(2): 74–81. three age levels. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships
Losh M and Capps L (2006) Understanding of emotional experi- 5(3): 275–284.
ence in autism: insights from the personal accounts of high- Seltzer MM, Shattuck P, Abbeduto L, et al. (2004) Trajectory of
functioning children with autism. Developmental Psychology development in adolescents and adults with autism. Mental
42(5): 809–818. Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research
Mazefsky CA, Folstein SE and Lainhart JE (2008) Reviews 10(4): 234–247.
Overrepresentation of mood and anxiety disorders in adults Shattuck PT, Orsmond GI, Wagner M, et al. (2011) Participation
with autism and their first-degree relatives: what does it in social activities among adolescents with an autism spec-
mean? Autism Research 1(3): 193–197. trum disorder. PLoS One 6(11): e27176.
Mazurek M and Kanne S (2010) Friendship and internalizing Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JW, et al. (1999) Validation and
symptoms among children and adolescents with ASD. Journal utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD: the PHQ pri-
of Autism and Developmental Disorders 40(12): 1512–1520. mary care study. JAMA 282(18): 1737–1744.
Moseley DS, Tonge BJ, Brereton AV, et al. (2011) Psychiatric Sterling L, Dawson G, Estes A, et al. (2008) Characteristics
comorbidity in adolescents and young adults with autism. associated with presence of depressive symptoms in adults
Journal of Mental Health Research in Intellectual Disabilities with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and
4(4): 229–243. Developmental Disorders 38(6): 1011–1018.
Nangle DW, Erdley CA, Newman JE, et al. (2003) Popularity, Stillman TF, Baumeister RF, Lambert NM, et al. (2009) Alone and
friendship quantity, and friendship quality: interactive influ- without purpose: life loses meaning following social exclusion.
ences on children’s loneliness and depression. Journal of Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45(4): 686–694.
Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 32(4): 546–555. Wei M, Russell DW and Zakalik RA (2005) Adult attachment,
Orsmond GI, Krauss MW and Seltzer MM (2004) Peer relation- social self-efficacy, self-disclosure, loneliness, and subse-
ships and social and recreational activities among adolescents quent depression for freshman college students: a longitudinal
and adults with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental study. Journal of Counseling Psychology 52(4): 602–614.
Disorders 34(3): 245–256. Whitehouse AJO, Durkin K, Jaquet E, et al. (2009) Friendship,
Parker JG and Asher SR (1993) Friendship and friendship loneliness and depression in adolescents with Asperger’s syn-
quality in middle childhood: links with peer group accept- drome. Journal of Adolescence 32(2): 309–322.
ance and feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction. White SW and Roberson-Nay R (2009) Anxiety, social defi-
Developmental Psychology 29(4): 611–621. cits, and loneliness in youth with autism spectrum disor-
Pavot W and Diener E (1993) Review of the Satisfaction with Life ders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 39(7):
Scale. Psychological Assessment 5(2): 164–172. 1006–1013.

Downloaded from aut.sagepub.com at UNIVERSITE LAVAL on January 24, 2015

You might also like