Eur J of Neuroscience - 2023 - Corrone - The Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor Val66met Polymorphism Is Associated With

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

Received: 15 May 2023 Revised: 7 September 2023 Accepted: 10 September 2023

DOI: 10.1111/ejn.16153

RESEARCH REPORT

The brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66met


polymorphism is associated with better attention and
working memory performance and resilience to mild
chronic stress

Michelle Corrone 1 | Aleshia Nanev 1 | Isabella Amato 1 | Rowena Bicknell 1 |


Stefan Piantella 1 | Paul Maruff 2 | Maarten van den Buuse 1 | Bradley J. Wright 1

1
School of Psychology and Public Health,
La Trobe University, Melbourne, Victoria, Abstract
Australia The val66met polymorphism of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF)
2
Cogstate Ltd, Melbourne, Victoria, gene has been identified as a potential moderator for the relationship between
Australia
chronic stress and executive functioning. However, whether the presence of
Correspondence the met allele increases cognitive vulnerability or resilience to stress has yet to
Bradley J. Wright, Plenty Road and be determined. Given the established effects of autonomic activity and psycho-
Kingsbury Drive, Bundoora, VIC 3086,
Australia. logical arousal on executive functioning, in the present study, 56 healthy uni-
Email: b.wright@latrobe.edu.au versity students completed self-report measures of chronic stress, positive
arousal (vigour) and negative arousal (anxiety) and measured heart-rate vari-
Funding information
The authors did not receive support from ability to quantify autonomic activity. Participants then completed a cognitive
any organization for the submitted work. test battery that measured attention, decision-making, visual learning and
working memory. Regression analyses demonstrated that Val/met participants
Edited by: Gal Richter-Levin
performed better on attention and working memory tasks than Val/val partici-
pants, but no differences were seen in decision-making and visual learning.
Further, Val/met participants were protected from stress-related differences in
attention seen in Val/val participants. Val66met was not associated with physi-
ological or psychological arousal. This study demonstrates that val66met plays
an important but selective role in cognitive performance.

KEYWORDS
attention, autonomic activity, brain-derived neurotrophic factor, chronic stress, working
memory

List of abbreviations: AUDIT, Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test; BDNF, brain-derived neurotrophic factor; BMI, body mass index; DET,
detection test; HRV, heart rate variability; IDN, Identification Test; met, methionine; OCL, One Card Learning Task; ONB, One Back Test; POMS,
Profile of Mood States; PSS, Perceived Stress Scale; sAA, salivary alpha amylase; STAI, State–Trait Anxiety Inventory; val, valine; VR, virtual reality.

This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs License, which permits use and distribution in any
medium, provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non-commercial and no modifications or adaptations are made.
© 2023 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

Eur J Neurosci. 2023;58:3903–3916. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/ejn 3903


14609568, 2023, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ejn.16153 by Nat Prov Indonesia, Wiley Online Library on [20/01/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
3904 CORRONE ET AL.

1 | INTRODUCTION animal studies have provided evidence that individuals


carrying the met allele are more vulnerable to stress-
Because of the high density of glucocorticoid receptors in induced impairments in core executive functions, such as
the prefrontal cortex (Patel et al., 2000; Perlman working memory and cognitive flexibility (Gabrys
et al., 2007), stress exposure and its associated high levels et al., 2017; Gatt et al., 2009; Yu et al., 2012), but few
of circulating glucocorticoids can alter cognition and studies have been published on higher order executive
have long-lasting effects on executive functioning (Jett functions such as decision-making and problem-solving,
et al., 2017; Smith et al., 2018). However, these stress despite evidence that BDNF expression is associated
effects have been found to be highly variable, even within with decision-making in stressful situations (Smith
healthy populations, and this variability has been linked et al., 2014). Moreover, the cognitive load of the neuro-
to genetic, physiological and psychological factors. cognitive tasks administered may account for some of the
Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) is involved variability in the literature, with our recent work demon-
in neuronal maturation, growth and survival and binds strating that complex, top-down cognitive tasks were
to tropomyosin-related kinase B receptors (TrkB; Notaras more strongly associated with val66met than simpler,
et al., 2015) with high levels of BDNF found within the bottom-up cognitive tasks (Corrone et al., 2021). There-
prefrontal cortex (Ernfors, Ibañez et al., 1990; Ernfors, fore, this study will extend our previous findings and
Wetmore et al., 1990; Hofer et al., 1990). Glucocorticoids assess data taken from the same participants at the same
directly and indirectly influence BDNF signalling and time as the earlier work (Corrone et al., 2021) and mea-
expression (Gourley et al., 2009; Jeanneteau et al., 2008; sure a core executive function (working memory), a
Pradhan et al., 2019; Smith et al., 1995), whereas BDNF higher order executive function (decision-making) and
can regulate the stress response through modulation of a non-executive cognitive function (attention). Visual
corticotrophin-releasing factor (CRF) gene expression learning was also included to determine whether the lack
(Givalois et al., 2004; Jeanneteau et al., 2012). Animal of relationship between val66met and visual processing
studies have shown that increased BDNF signalling speed observed in our previous study (Corrone
induced by administration of an agonist at the BDNF et al., 2021) extends to other visual-based cognitive
receptor, TrkB, protected against stress-induced deficits functions.
in executive functioning (Barfield & Gourley, 2017). Con- Heart rate variability (HRV) provides a proxy measure
versely, TrkB deactivation impaired cortical neuroplasti- of autonomic activity, and although associations between
city following stress by blocking glucocorticoid receptor HRV and val66met have been identified, the direction of
phosphorylation (Arango-Lievano et al., 2015). For these this relationship is unclear. For example, some studies
reasons, BDNF may be central in the relationship have found that carriers of the Met/met genotype had
between stress and executive functioning. reduced parasympathetic activity, resulting in sympa-
The val66met polymorphism is a common BDNF gene thetic dominance, compared with Val/val and Val/met
variant, where a single nucleotide substitution at codon individuals (Tsuru et al., 2014; Yang et al., 2010), whereas
66 causes an amino acid substitution of valine (val) for others have found increased sympathetic activity in
methionine (met), resulting in reductions in activity- Val/met individuals compared with Val/val individuals,
dependent release of BDNF (Notaras et al., 2015; but no resultant sympathetic dominance (Corrone
Notaras & van den Buuse, 2018). Compared with val et al., 2021). Another study found that although individ-
homozygous (Val/val) carriers, neuronal BDNF release is uals carrying the met allele had reduced parasympathetic
reduced by 13% in single allele met (Val/met) carriers activity compared with Val/val individuals, the effect was
and 23% in met homozygous (Met/met) carriers (Chen only present in male participants, suggesting that the
et al., 2006, 2004; Egan et al., 2003). Although the pres- relationship between val66met and HRV may be
ence of the val66met polymorphism has been associated gender-specific (Chang et al., 2014). However, both Yang
with disrupted cognitive performance, the role of val66- et al. (2010) and Corrone et al. (2021) found no
met in executive functioning is less clear (Barha interactions between genotype and gender in their HRV
et al., 2019; Erickson et al., 2008; Gabrys et al., 2017; analysis.
Krueger et al., 2011; Wiłkosc et al., 2016). Specifically, it Mood state is also associated with cognitive perfor-
remains unclear whether val66met increases an individ- mance during neuropsychological testing (Bolte
ual’s vulnerability or resilience to stress-induced changes et al., 2003; Rowe et al., 2007). A positive mood state is
in executive functioning and which components of these typically associated with better cognition than negative
cognitive processes are most affected. Both human and mood states (Bolte et al., 2003; Schuch & Pütz, 2018), and
14609568, 2023, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ejn.16153 by Nat Prov Indonesia, Wiley Online Library on [20/01/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
CORRONE ET AL. 3905

memory retrieval is enhanced when the stimuli’s emo- (HEC19036) and were compensated for their time with a
tional valence is congruent with the mood state of the $100 shopping voucher.
individual at the time of encoding (Bower, 1981). In
the present study, the relationship between val66met,
mood state (i.e., vigour and anxiety) and potential inter- 2.2 | Materials
action effects on cognition was examined in a healthy
population. 2.2.1 | Chronic stress
The overall aim of this study was to elucidate the
role of val66met in the relationship between chronic The Perceived Stress Scale (PSS; Cohen et al., 1983) is a
stress and attention, decision-making, visual learning 10-item self-report measure that assesses perceived
and working memory after considering the role of chronic stress on a 5-point Likert scale (0 = never to
physiological (autonomic activity) and psychological 4 = very often). An example item is ‘In the last month,
(mood states) arousal. It was hypothesized that carriers how often have you felt that things were going your
of the met allele would be more likely to have impair- way?’ (Cohen et al., 1983, p. 394). High scores denote
ments in working memory associated with high stress high levels of perceived stress. In this study, the PSS had
compared to Val/val individuals but would perform high internal consistency (α = 0.82).
better on cognitive tests of attention and decision-
making than individuals homozygous for the val allele
regardless of stress levels. No associations between 2.2.2 | Vigour—Positive mood state
val66met, stress and visual learning were anticipated.
Further, it was hypothesized that val66met would be The Profile of Mood States (POMS; McNair et al., 1971) is
associated with heightened sympathetic reactivity a 65-item self-report questionnaire that measures short-
because of the mild stress elicited by the cognitive term mood states on a 5-point Likert scale (0 = not at all
testing in Val/met participants compared with Val/val to 4 = extremely). To assess the participants’ positive
participants, but these changes would not be strong mood states, the 7-item vigour subscale was used. High
enough to alter autonomic dominance. It was further scores on the vigour subscale denote a positive mood
predicted that the relationship between val66met and state. The POMS vigour subscale showed high internal
autonomic activity would not be gender specific. Lastly, consistency (α = 0.89).
it was hypothesized that val66met would be associated
with higher self-reported state anxiety, but no
association with vigour was expected. 2.2.3 | Anxiety—Negative mood state

The six-item short-form of the State–Trait Anxiety Inven-


2 | METHODS tory (STAI-6; Marteau & Bekker, 1992) was used to assess
participants’ state anxiety levels. The STAI-6 is a self-
2.1 | Participants report questionnaire that utilizes a 4-point Likert scale
(0 = not at all to 4 = very much so), where high scores
Participants were healthy young adults (N = 56, denote a negative mood state. The STAI-6 showed high
females = 31), aged 18 to 28 years (M = 20.7, SD = 2.1). internal consistency (α = 0.78).
Participants were invited to participate face-to-face on
the grounds of La Trobe University, Melbourne,
Australia, using a recruitment script. Participants were 2.2.4 | Alcohol consumption screening
included if they were currently full-time students (crite-
rion for a related study) and could read English. Exclu- The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT;
sion criteria included a self-reported history of Saunders et al., 1993) was used to screen for frequent
concussion in the previous 6 months; psychiatric, cardiac, alcohol use. The 10-item self-report scale assesses alcohol
or neurological illness; dementia, head injury, insomnia consumption, drinking behaviour and consequences
or other sleep disorders; and the use of medications that related to drinking. An AUDIT score of ≥8 indicates
may affect concentration. Candidates were also excluded risky/hazardous levels of alcohol use. The AUDIT
if they reported feeling physically fragile or acutely has high internal consistency (α = 0.81; Shields &
unwell (cold, flu), or if they reported regular illicit drug Caruso, 2003) and strong test–retest reliability
or alcohol consumption. Participants provided written (Selin, 2003). No participants were excluded based on this
informed consent in line with institutional ethics criterion.
14609568, 2023, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ejn.16153 by Nat Prov Indonesia, Wiley Online Library on [20/01/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
3906 CORRONE ET AL.

2.2.5 | Heart rate variability were placed in a centrifuge tube and immediately stored
in a freezer (20 C). Samples were sent on dry ice to the
HRV measures heartbeat fluctuations, which are primar- Australian Genome Research Facility for DNA extraction
ily controlled by autonomic activity. Dysregulated HRV is and were analysed using Agena Mass Array for the single
commonly associated with alteration in cognitive perfor- nucleotide polymorphism rs6265 (BDNF val66met).
mance (Collins et al., 2012; Forte et al., 2019). Continu-
ous heart rate was measured using a digital ambulatory
ECG recorder (AR12plus, Medilog, Schiller AG, 2.2.7 | Cognitive test battery
Switzerland). The ECG recorder has five leads that attach
to the chest and records ECG signals at a sampling rate The cognitive test battery consisted of a collection of
of 1000 Hz. Recordings were examined using Darwin V2 4 Cogstate computerized neurocognitive tests: the Detec-
software (Medilog, Schiller AG, Switzerland) to deter- tion test (DET), the Identification test (IDN), the One
mine R-R peak intervals during the baseline and cogni- Card Learning test (OCL) and the One Back test (ONB).
tive testing phases. Non-sinus intervals were excluded. These tests measure attention, decision-making, visual
The duration of the baseline and testing periods were learning and working memory, respectively. Participants
both approximately 10 min and were analysed using a were provided with a practice test and began each trial
Poincaré plot, a non-linear method of measuring HRV when they selected the ‘enter’ button on the computer
where each data point refers to the relative change keyboard. The tests have moderate to high test–retest
between one R-R interval and the consecutive R-R reliability (DET, r = 0.90; IDN, r = 0.69; OCL, r = 0.45;
interval (Roy & Ghatak, 2013). Non-linear methods of ONB, r = 0.76) and have been validated for use in
analysing ECG data were developed to identify non- healthy young people (Collie et al., 2003). More informa-
linear patterns in HRV that occur because of the com- tion can be found at https://www.cogstate.com/clinical-
plexity of the autonomic nervous system (Roy & trials/computerized-cognitive-assessment/
Ghatak, 2013; Shaffer & Ginsberg, 2017). There is The DET uses simple reaction time as a measure of
insufficient evidence of non-linear HRV in val66met attention and psychomotor function. Participants are pre-
populations, as studies have predominantly been sented with a face-down playing card on screen with the
analysed by linear, time- and frequency-domain methods. question ‘Has the card turned over?’ to which they can
However, in a previous study, it was seen that stress- only respond by selecting the on-screen ‘Yes’ button by
induced alterations in HRV were identified in Val/met pressing the left button on the computer mouse. Partici-
participants but not Val/val participants (Corrone pants are instructed to respond as quickly and accurately
et al., 2021), highlighting that further examining as possible when the card turns over. The test is typically
non-linear patterns could elucidate the relationship administered in 3 min for healthy participants.
between HRV and val66met. The IDN uses choice reaction time as a measure of
The Poincaré plot provides three indices of HRV: decision-making. Participants are presented with a
SD1, SD2 and SD12. SD1 is a measure of parasympathetic face-down playing card on the screen with the question
activity and refers to the standard deviation of instanta- ‘Is the card red?’ to which they can respond by pressing
neous beat-to-beat interval variability. SD2 is a measure the ‘Yes’ or ‘No’ (left or right respectively) buttons with
of sympathetic activity, which refers to the standard devi- the computer mouse. When the card turns over, the face
ation of the continuous long-term R-R interval variabil- will either be red or black, and participants are required
ity. SD12 is the ratio of SD1/SD2 and indicates to respond as quickly and accurately as possible. The
autonomic balance. An SD12 change variable was calcu- test is typically administered in 3 min for healthy
lated by subtracting the baseline phase measure from the participants.
cognitive testing phase measure. Higher SD12 change The OCL uses a pattern separation paradigm as a
values denote increases in sympathetic dominance measure of visual learning and memory. Participants are
between baseline and cognitive testing phases. presented with a face-down playing card on the screen
with the question ‘Have you seen this card before?’ to
which the participant can respond by selecting either
2.2.6 | Genotyping ‘Yes’ or ‘No’. When the card turns over, the face will dis-
play a standard card face and the participants are
Participants provided an unstimulated saliva sample dur- required to recall whether the card has been previously
ing the baseline period by moving a cotton swab presented throughout the test, responding as quickly and
(Salivette®, Sarstedt, Chur, Switzerland) around their accurately as possible. The test is typically administered
mouth in a circular motion for 1 minute. The Salivettes® in 4 min for healthy participants.
14609568, 2023, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ejn.16153 by Nat Prov Indonesia, Wiley Online Library on [20/01/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
CORRONE ET AL. 3907

The ONB uses the n-back paradigm as a measure of and directly comparable with previous research using
working memory. Participants are presented with a face- Cogstate tests, all cognitive measures were transformed
down playing card on the screen with the question ‘is the using either logarithmic transformation (IDN, DET,
previous card the same?’ to which they can respond by ONB) or arcsine transformation (OCL).
using the computer mouse to select either ‘Yes’ or ‘No’.
When the card turns over, the face will display a standard
card face and the participants are required to recall the 3.2 | Demographic equivalence and
previous card and respond as quickly and accurately as normative comparisons
possible. The test is typically administered in 4 min for
healthy participants. The descriptive statistics are provided for each genotype
and the entire sample for gender, age and BMI data
(Table 1). The genotype groups did not differ in gender
2.3 | Statistical analysis distribution (X 2[1, N = 56] = 0.30, p = .420), mean age (t
[54] = 0.51, p = .610), or mean BMI (t[54] = 0.136,
IBM SPSS statistics computer software package (version p = .892). Participants reported lower perceived chronic
26) was used for all statistical analyses. An independent stress and state anxiety and higher vigour than age-
samples t-test was used to compare Val/val or Val/met matched normative samples (Table 2).
genotype groups on age and BMI. Chi-square was used to
examine relationships between BDNF genotypes and
gender. A one-sample t-test was used to compare partici- 3.3 | Effect of testing and genotype on
pants’ self-reported chronic stress (PSS), vigour (POMS) autonomic activity
and state anxiety (STAI) with age-matched normative
samples. A paired t-test compared HRV (i.e., SD1, SD2 Increases in autonomic activity were observed in the test-
and SD12) differed across baseline and testing phases. A ing phase for all variables except SD1 (Table 3). The effect
2 (phase; baseline, testing)  2 (genotype; val/val, Val/- size for SD2 was very large (Cohen, 1988), indicating a
met) analysis of variance (ANOVA) assessed if changes substantial change in sympathetic activity during cogni-
in HRV between phases differed by genotype. A one-way tive testing. Changes in autonomic arousal did not differ
analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) assessed whether vig- between genotypes (Figure 1; SD1: F(1,52) = 0.50,
our, anxiety or chronic stress differed by genotype after p = 0.49; SD2: F(1,52) = 0.54, p = 0.47; SD12: F(1,52)
controlling for age and gender. Associations between par- = 0.12, p = 0.73), nor genders; SD1: F(1,52) = 0.98,
ticipant characteristics (genotype, gender, age and BMI), p = 0.33; SD2: F(1,52) = 0.07, p = 0.79; SD12: F(1,52)
self-report measures of mental state (PSS, vigour and anx- = 1.50, p = 0.23). No interaction effects were identified
iety), HRV (change in SD12) and cognitive measures between genotype and gender (SD1: F(1,52) = 0.21,
(DET, IDN, OCL and ONB) were assessed with Pearson p = 0.65; SD2: F(1,52) = 2.09, p = 0.16; SD12: F(1,52)
and Spearman correlations as appropriate. Four moder- = 2.32, p = 0.13).
ated regression analyses were conducted using the SPSS
PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2013) with bootstrapping
(N = 5000 resamples) to assess whether genotype inter- 3.4 | Effect of genotype on perceived
acted with PSS to predict performance on the cognitive mood states
test battery after controlling for age, gender, vigour and
changes in autonomic arousal. The differences in self-reported state vigour and anxiety
between genotypes after controlling for age and gender
were assessed. Neither vigour (F(1,52) = 0.77, p = 0.38)
3 | R E SUL T S nor anxiety (F(1,52) = 0.25, p = 0.62) differed between
genotypes (Figure 2).
3.1 | Data management

One participant carried the Met/met genotype, and there- 3.5 | Associations between key variables
fore, data for this subject were removed from the study.
Data were free from outliers as assessed by Mahalanobis The associations between participant characteristics, self-
distance and violations of normality (z-skewness > 2.58), report measures of stress, vigour and anxiety, physiological
multicollinearity in the regression tests (all tolerance arousal and cognitive measures were assessed (Table 4).
statistics > .10) and homoscedasticity. To be consistent Individuals carrying the met allele were associated with
14609568, 2023, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ejn.16153 by Nat Prov Indonesia, Wiley Online Library on [20/01/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
3908 CORRONE ET AL.

TABLE 1 Demographics of study sample compared with genotype.

Genotype

Variable Total sample Val/val Val/met


Gender (Male: Female) M = 25: F = 31 M = 11: F = 18 M = 14: F = 13
Age M (SD) 20.66 (2.15) 20.52 (2.22) 20.81 (2.22)
BMI M (SD) 24.02 (3.99) 24.10 (4.07) 23.95 (3.98)

TABLE 2 Age-matched normative comparisons of means and standard deviations for self-report measures.

Sample data Normative data

Scale M SD M SD t d
PSS 15.47 5.78 19.62 7.49 4.08*** 0.56
POMS–Vigour 17.16 4.29 15.00 6.90 2.17* 0.35
STAI 10.98 3.12 12.96 3.26 3.81*** 0.62

Note: Normative data: PSS (Örücü & Demir, 2009), n = 508, age 15–29; POMS–Vigour (Nyenhuis et al., 1999), n = 132, age 18–24; STAI (Taylor &
Deane, 2002), n = 122, age = 19–80.
Abbreviations: POMS, Profile of Mood States; PSS, Perceived Stress Scale; STAI, State–Trait Anxiety Inventory.
*p < .05.
***p < .001.

TABLE 3 Comparison of autonomic activity measures at baseline and cognitive testing.

Baseline M (SD) Testing M (SD) Cohen’s d p


SD1 28.77 (18.09) 25.65 (12.17) 0.413 .14
SD2 76.16 (26.34) 95.93 (39.89) 1.029 <.001
SD12 0.38 (0.17) 0.27 (0.09) 1.26 <.001

higher chronic stress (PSS) and better attention (DET) and participants with the Val/met genotype demonstrated fas-
working memory (ONB). Genotype was not associated ter/better attention and working memory performance
with mood state or physiological arousal. than participants with the Val/val genotype. Lower vig-
our was also a significant predictor of attention and
decision-making performance (Table 5).
3.6 | Effect of chronic stress on the Genotype did not moderate the association between
relationships between genotype and chronic stress and IDN; b = 0.01 [0.01, 0.01], p = 0.07,
cognitive performance ONB; b = 0.00 [0.02, 0.01], p = 0.85 or OCL; b = 0.00
[0.01, 0.01], p = 0.85 as outcomes. The attention (DET)
A series of multivariable regressions were conducted to model, however, demonstrated an association between
assess if any of the demographic, psychological, or physi- genotype and chronic stress (PSS); b = 0.01 [0.01, 0.02],
ological measures were associated with cognition (DET, p = 0.04. Specifically, the association revealed that for the
IDN, OCL and ONB). Given that anxiety was not corre- Val/val genotype, higher chronic stress was associated
lated with any cognitive measure but was correlated with with poorer attention performance; b = 0.01, p = 0.05,
both chronic stress and vigour (Table 4), anxiety was not whereas for the Val/met group, perceived stress was not
included as a measure of state affect in any regression related to attention scores (Figure 3).
model to preserve statistical power and potential issues of
multicollinearity.
The regression models identified that genotype was 4 | DISCUSSION
associated with attention (DET; Table 5) and working
memory (ONB; Table 6) but not decision-making (IDN; In line with our hypotheses, Val/met individuals demon-
Table 5) or visual learning (OCL; Table 6). Specifically, strated better attention than Val/val participants and
14609568, 2023, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ejn.16153 by Nat Prov Indonesia, Wiley Online Library on [20/01/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
CORRONE ET AL. 3909

F I G U R E 2 No difference in mood states between genotypes.


Top panel: vigour; bottom panel: anxiety. Box plots show the 5th
and 95th percentiles, the interquartile range and the median.

present in Val/met participants. The results of this study


F I G U R E 1 Changes in autonomic activity between baseline suggest that, after controlling for gender, age, autonomic
and cognitive testing phases between genotypes. Panels show SD1, a arousal and mood state at baseline, Val/met participants
measure of parasympathetic activity that refers to the standard demonstrate better attention and working memory than
deviation of instantaneous beat-to-beat interval variability; SD2, a
Val/val individuals and are protected against stress-
measure of sympathetic activity that refers to the standard deviation
induced impairments to attention.
of the continuous long-term R-R interval variability; and SD12, the
ratio of SD1/SD2 and indicates autonomic balance. Box plots show
the 5th and 95th percentiles, the interquartile range and the median.
4.1 | The Val/met genotype is associated
with better attention and working memory
genotype was not associated with visual learning. How-
ever, contrary to our hypotheses, Val/met individuals Val/met participants performed better on working mem-
showed better working memory than Val/val participants ory and attention tests than Val/val participants, but no
but val66met was not associated with decision-making, differences between genotypes were observed for
physiological arousal, or psychological arousal. Further, decision-making or visual learning in the unadjusted
Val/val individuals with higher self-reported stress levels analyses. In our previous study, we explained the discrep-
demonstrated poorer attention, but this effect was not ancies between val66met and various cognitive tasks as
14609568, 2023, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ejn.16153 by Nat Prov Indonesia, Wiley Online Library on [20/01/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
3910 CORRONE ET AL.

T A B L E 4 Correlations comparing genotype, gender, age, self-report measures of stress, vigour and anxiety, physiological arousal, and
cognitive measures.

Variable Genotype Gender Age PSS Vigour Anxiety SD12Δ IDN DET OCL
Gender 0.14 a

Age 0.07 0.09


PSS 0.28* 0.21 0.13
Vigour 0.07 0.28* 0.12 0.28*
Anxiety 0.10 0.22 0.05 0.26* 0.50**
Δ
SD12 0.07 0.15 0.09 0.02 0.00 0.09
IDN 0.18 0.24 0.01 0.11 0.39** 0.05 0.05
DET 0.31* 0.05 0.02 0.02 0.23 0.02 0.00 0.63**
OCL 0.03 0.16 0.04 0.08 0.02 0.11 0.17 0.16 0.29*
ONB 0.35** 0.29* 0.02 0.00 0.27* 0.05 0.09 0.70** 0.52** 0.01
Δ
Note: PSS, Perceived Stress Scale; SD12 , change in SD12 between baseline and testing phases; IDN, decision-making; DET, attention; OCL, visual learning;
ONB, working memory. Gender: male = 1, female = 2, Genotype 1 = Val/val, 2 = Val/met.
a
Spearman’s correlation.
*p < 0.05.
**p < 0.01.

TABLE 5 Linear regression model of predictors of attention and decision-making.

DET IDN

B (95% CI) SE B t p B (95% CI) SE B t p


Genotype 0.06 (0.01–0.02) 0.01 3.06 <0.01 0.03 (0.07–0.01) 0.02 1.69 0.10
PSS 0.02 (0.06–0.17) 0.06 0.98 0.33 0.01 (0.00–0.02) 0.01 1.88 0.07
Gender 0.03 (0.07–0.01) 0.02 1.52 0.13 0.01 (0.01–0.05) 0.02 0.65 0.52
Age 0.00 (0.01–0.01) 0.00 0.65 0.52 0.00 (0.01–0.01) 0.00 0.72 0.48
Vigour 0.01 (0.01–0.00) 0.00 2.78 0.01 0.01 (0.01–0.00) 0.00 3.28 < 0.01
SD12Δ 0.01 (0.17–0.14) 0.08 0.15 0.88 0.01 (0.15–0.14) 0.07 0.10 0.93

Note: PSS, Perceived Stress Scale; SD12Δ, change in SD12 between baseline and testing phases; DET, attention; IDN, decision-making. Total model: DET
R 2 = .27, p = .03; Total model: IDN R 2 = .27, p = .03. Genotype: 1 = Val/val, 2 = Val/met; Gender: 1 = male, 2 = female.

TABLE 6 Linear regression model of predictors of visual learning and working memory.

OCL ONB

B (95% CI) SE B t p B (95%CI) SE B t p


Genotype 0.01 (0.21–0.24) 0.04 0.11 0.92 0.06 (0.10–0.01) 0.02 2.45 0.02
PSS 0.00 (0.02–0.03) 0.01 0.39 0.70 0.00 (0.02–0.01) 0.01 0.21 0.84
Gender 0.04 (0.12–0.03) 0.04 1.12 0.27 0.03 (0.01–0.08) 0.02 1.47 0.15
Age 0.00 (0.01–0.02) 0.01 0.40 0.69 0.00 (0.01–0.01) 0.01 0.80 0.43
Vigour 0.00 (0.01–0.01) 0.00 0.32 0.75 0.00 (0.01–0.00) 0.00 1.73 0.09
SD12Δ 0.15 (0.14–0.45) 0.15 1.05 0.30 0.07 (0.11–0.26) 0.09 0.83 0.41

Note: PSS, Perceived Stress Scale; SD12Δ, change in SD12 between baseline and testing phases; OCL, visual learning; ONB, working memory. Total model: OCL
R 2 = .07, p = .85; Total model: ONB R 2 = .26, p = .04. Genotype: 1 = Val/val, 2 = Val/met; Gender: 1 = male, 2 = female.

potentially related to cognitive load of the tasks (Corrone presence of val66met than low cognitive load tasks, this
et al., 2021). However, where we found that tasks with study found that relatively low and high cognitive load
high cognitive load are more likely to be affected by the tasks (but not moderate) were performed better by
14609568, 2023, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ejn.16153 by Nat Prov Indonesia, Wiley Online Library on [20/01/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
CORRONE ET AL. 3911

not reported independently and were incorporated in an


information processing factor score. Therefore, it is possi-
ble that choice reaction time measures in isolation are
more specific than accuracy or capacity measures and
therefore better demonstrate the cognitive differences
between val66met genotypes.
This study supports previous evidence that val66met
is not an important predictor of visual learning. Although
very few studies have investigated the role of val66met in
visual learning, neither an effect of val66met nor serum
BDNF levels have been established (Pitts et al., 2020;
Wheeler et al., 2020).

4.2 | The Val/met genotype buffers the


F I G U R E 3 Association between chronic stress and attention association between stress and poor
between genotypes. The perceived stress scale was mean centred attention
prior to analysis but the untransformed values are presented on the
X-axis to assist interpretation. The association between higher perceived stress and
poorer attention observed in Val/val participants could
be the result of heightened dopaminergic activity in the
participants with the Val/met genotype. Yet, the atten- prefrontal cortex (PFC). This is supported by evidence
tion and decision-making tasks of this cognitive battery, that stress increases dopaminergic activity in the PFC to
simple and choice reaction time tasks, are analogous to the point of cognitive impairment (Finlay et al., 1995;
the simple and choice reaction time tasks used in the Murphy et al., 1996; Oh et al., 2021; Roth et al., 1988) and
Corrone et al. (2021) study and therefore should demon- dopaminergic hyperfunctioning throughout mesocortico-
strate similar findings. The discrepancies between the limbic pathways has been linked to inattention (Viggiano
current and previous study (Corrone et al., 2021) may be et al., 2003; Zhuang et al., 2001). Although studies of a
explained by the increased level of arousal (HRV and direct role of val66met in the mesocortical pathway are
heart rate) elicited by the CONVIRT virtual-reality test- limited, there is evidence that met-carrying individuals
ing environment compared to the Cogstate computer- exhibit reduced dopamine transmission in the nucleus
based testing (Horan et al., 2020). Additionally, carriers accumbens (Pecina et al., 2014). However, it is possible
of the met allele are less likely to be distracted by irrele- that the observed relationship between val66met, stress
vant stimuli during goal-directed behaviour than Val/val and attention may be related to a more direct action of
individuals (Getzmann et al., 2013). Therefore, Val/met TrkB, independent of dopamine activity. Suppressed
participants may have been advantaged in our previous TrkB activity is associated with reduced glucocorticoid
study using the CONVIRT virtual-reality task because of receptor phosphorylation and has been seen to block
better tolerance to distractors, whereas the Val/met par- stress-induced changes in cortical dendritic growth
ticipants had no such advantage during the present (Arango-Lievano et al., 2015). Given the val66met-
study’s standard computerized cognitive testing. associated decrease in activity-dependent BDNF secre-
Although the positive association between val66met tion, carriers of the met allele have less BDNF available
and working memory is in line with the theory that high to bind to TrkB receptors (Egan et al., 2003).
cognitive load tasks elicit differences between genotypes,
the result that Val/met participants had better working
memory than Val/val participants differs from previous 4.3 | The potential role of ventral and
val66met research, where no association was found dorsal attention networks
between val66met and working memory (Cerasa
et al., 2010; Chen et al., 2015; Gatt et al., 2009; LeMoult The ventral attention network is important for inhibition
et al., 2015). However, these studies generally focus on and filtering out distractors but is suppressed during
working memory accuracy and capacity, rather than multi-elemental tasks (Zhao et al., 2022), whereas the
choice reaction time (Cerasa et al., 2010; Chen dorsal attention network is related to spatial attention
et al., 2015; LeMoult et al., 2015). Where choice reaction and working memory (Vossel et al., 2014; Zhao
time was measured (Gatt et al., 2009), these results were et al., 2022). It is likely that the more complex measures
14609568, 2023, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ejn.16153 by Nat Prov Indonesia, Wiley Online Library on [20/01/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
3912 CORRONE ET AL.

of decision-making, working memory and visual learning primarily driven by the PFC. Additionally, participants
require more dynamic attentional control than the atten- with the Met/met genotype were not included, with gen-
tion measure (simple reaction time) and therefore would otyping conducted after recruitment and only one
rely on the dorsal attention network while suppressing Met/met carrier identified. This meant we could not fully
the ventral attention network. Further, it has been dem- examine the effects of all val66met genotypes. Further,
onstrated that chronic stress increases PFC connectivity there have been some concerns surrounding the use of a
to and activation of both attention networks (Ginty candidate gene approach (Sullivan, 2007). However,
et al., 2019; Soares et al., 2013). However, although few genome-wide association studies are limited when asses-
studies have compared differences in the dorsal and ven- sing genotypes that are moderated by environmental fac-
tral attention networks between val66met genotypes, pre- tors (Moore, 2017). Therefore, evaluation of the
liminary evidence suggests met carriers may have relationship between val66met and stress was best
stronger ventral attention networks than individuals addressed using a candidate gene approach.
homozygous for the val allele (Hashimoto et al., 2016), Although our sample size was modest, it was
and no functional differences have been identified in the adequately powered and the study controlled for several
dorsal attention network (Li et al., 2016; Pietzuch potential confounds that are not typically considered
et al., 2021). Therefore, it is possible that the met allele in val66met research (e.g., psychological arousal).
protects against stress-induced deficits in attention due Additionally, a pre-post design strengthened this study by
to a more robust ventral attention network, but once allowing for analysis of autonomic change within
the dorsal attention network plays a more dominant participants, whereas direct comparisons could be drawn
role during decision-making, working memory and between genotypes. Nonetheless, the results of this
visual learning tasks, this effect can no longer be study should be replicated in a larger and higher
seen behaviourally. stressed sample.

4.4 | Psychological and physiological 5 | CONCLUSION


arousal are not associated with Val66met
In conclusion, this study provides evidence that val66met
Although positive psychological arousal (vigour) was plays an important role in cognitive performance, dem-
associated with better attention and working memory onstrating better attention and working memory perfor-
performance, no interactions between val66met and psy- mance in Val/met participants compared with Val/val
chological or physiological arousal were found. Previous participants. We demonstrated, when controlling for age,
studies have found that Val/met participants report gender and psychological and physiological arousal, that
higher levels of vigour and respond to emotional stimuli Val/met participants are protected from chronic stress-
differently than Val/val individuals (Tsuru et al., 2014; related decreases in attention. This study provides a foun-
Wang et al., 2012). However, the results of the current dational basis for animal model studies to establish a
study are consistent with evidence derived from clinical direct role of val66met in the mesocortical dopamine
samples that have also found no relationship between pathway and the ventral attention network.
val66met and mood states (Surtees et al., 2007; Verhagen
et al., 2010). Further, the lack of relationship between A C KN O WL ED G EME N T S
val66met and HRV differs from previous research, which N/A.
has found that individuals carrying the met allele have
altered autonomic activity compared to Val/val partici- C O N F L I C T O F I N T E R E S T S T A TE M E N T
pants (Chang et al., 2014; Corrone et al., 2021; Tsuru P.M. is an employee and equity holder in CogState Ltd.
et al., 2014; Yang et al., 2010). It is possible that the par- The other authors have nothing to disclose.
ticipants of this study did not reach an adequate level of
arousal during computer-based testing to elicit differ- PE ER RE VI EW
ences between genotypes. The peer review history for this article is available at
https://www.webofscience.com/api/gateway/wos/peer-
review/10.1111/ejn.16153.
4.5 | Limitations
DA TA AVAI LA BI LI TY S T ATE ME NT
A limitation of this study was that although the working Data will be available from the Open Science Foundation
memory assessment involves hippocampal input, it is platform at OSF j Cogstate Val/Met.
14609568, 2023, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ejn.16153 by Nat Prov Indonesia, Wiley Online Library on [20/01/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
CORRONE ET AL. 3913

R EF E RE N C E S Collie, A., Maruff, P., Darby, D. G., & McStephen, M. (2003). The
Arango-Lievano, M., Lambert, W. M., Bath, K. G., effects of practice on the cognitive test performance of neuro-
Garabedian, M. J., Chao, M. V., & Jeanneteau, F. (2015). logically normal individuals assessed at brief test-retest inter-
Neurotrophic-priming of glucocorticoid receptor signaling vals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9,
is essential for neuronal plasticity to stress and 419–428. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617703930074
antidepressant treatment. Proceedings of the National Academy Collins, O., Dillon, S., Finucane, C., Lawlor, B., & Kenny, R. A.
of Sciences, 112, 15737–15742. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas. (2012). Parasympathetic autonomic dysfunction is common in
1509045112 mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiology of Aging, 33, 2324–
Barfield, E. T., & Gourley, S. L. (2017). Adolescent corticosterone 2333. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.11.017
and TrkB pharmaco-manipulations sex-dependently impact Corrone, M., Nanev, A., Amato, I., Bicknell, R.,
instrumental reversal learning later in life. Frontiers in Behav- Wundersitz, D. W. T., van den Buuse, M., & Wright, B. J.
ioral Neuroscience, 11, 237. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017. (2021). Brain-derived neurotropic factor val66met is a strong
00237 predictor of decision-making and attention performance on
Barha, C. K., Liu-Ambrose, T., Best, J. R., Yaffe, K., & Rosano, C. the CONVIRT virtual reality cognitive battery. Neuroscience,
(2019). Sex-dependent effect of the BDNF Val66Met polymor- 455, 19–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.12.004
phism on executive functioning and processing speed in older Egan, M. F., Kojima, M., Callicott, J. H., Goldberg, T. E.,
adults: Evidence from the health ABC study. Neurobiology of Kolachana, B. S., Bertolino, A., Zaitsev, E., Gold, B.,
Aging, 74, 161–170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging. Goldman, D., Dean, M., Lu, B., & Weinberger, D. R. (2003).
2018.10.021 The BDNF val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent
Bolte, A., Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Emotion and intuition. secretion of BDNF and human memory and hippocampal
Psychological Science, 14, 416–421. https://doi.org/10.1111/ function. Cell, 112, 257–269. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0092-
1467-9280.01456 8674(03)00035-7
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. The American Psycholo- Erickson, K., Kim, J., Suever, B., Voss, M., Francis, B. M., &
gist, 36(2), 129–148. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.36. Kramer, A. (2008). Genetic contributions to age-related decline
2.129 in executive function: A 10-year longitudinal study of COMT
Cerasa, A., Tongiorgi, E., Fera, F., Gioia, M. C., Valentino, P., and BDNF polymorphisms. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,
Liguori, M., Manna, I., Zito, G., Passamonti, L., Nisticò, R., & 2, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.09.011.2008
Quattrone, A. (2010). The effects of BDNF Val66Met polymor- Ernfors, P., Ibañez, C. F., Ebendal, T., Olson, L., & Persson, H.
phism on brain function in controls and patients with multiple (1990). Molecular cloning and neurotrophic activities of a pro-
sclerosis: An imaging genetic study. Behavioural Brain tein with structural similarities to nerve growth factor: Devel-
Research, 207, 377–386. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2009. opmental and topographical expression in the brain.
10.022 Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
Chang, C.-C., Chang, H.-A., Chen, T.-Y., Fang, W.-H., & United States of America, 87, 5454–5458. https://doi.org/10.
Huang, S.-Y. (2014). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor 1073/pnas.87.14.5454
(BDNF) Val66Met polymorphism affects sympathetic tone in a Ernfors, P., Wetmore, C., Olson, L., & Persson, H. (1990). Identifica-
gender-specific way. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 47, 17–25. tion of cells in rat brain and peripheral tissues expressing
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.04.019 mRNA for members of the nerve growth factor family. Neuron,
Chen, C.-C., Chen, C.-J., Wu, D., Chi, N.-F., Chen, P.-C., 5, 511–526. https://doi.org/10.1016/0896-6273(90)90090-3
Liao, Y.-P., Chiu, H.-W., & Hu, C.-J. (2015). BDNF Val66Met Finlay, J. M., Zigmond, M. J., & Abercrombie, E. D. (1995).
polymorphism on functional MRI during n-back working Increased dopamine and norepinephrine release in medial pre-
memory tasks. Medicine, 94, e1586. https://doi.org/10.1097/ frontal cortex induced by acute and chronic stress: Effects of
MD.0000000000001586 diazepam. Neuroscience, 64, 619–628. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Chen, Z.-Y., Jing, D., Bath, K. G., Ieraci, A., Khan, T., Siao, C.-J., 0306-4522(94)00331-X
Herrera, D. G., Toth, M., Yang, C., Mcewen, B. S., Forte, G., Favieri, F., & Casagrande, M. (2019). Heart rate variabil-
Hempstead, B. L., & Lee, F. S. (2006). Genetic variant BDNF ity and cognitive function: A systematic review. Frontiers in
(Val66Met) polymorphism alters anxiety-related behavior. Sci- Neuroscience, 13, 710. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2019.00710
ence, 314, 140–143. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1129663 Gabrys, R. L., Dixon, K., & Anisman, H. (2017). Traumatic life
Chen, Z. Y., Patel, P. D., Sant, G., Meng, C. X., Teng, K. K., events in relation to cognitive flexibility: Moderating role of
Hempstead, B. L., & Lee, F. S. (2004). Variant brain-derived the BDNF Val66Met gene polymorphism. Frontiers in Behav-
neurotrophic factor (BDNF) (Met66) alters the intracellular ioral Neuroscience, 11, 241. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.
trafficking and activity-dependent secretion of wild-type BDNF 00241
in neurosecretory cells and cortical neurons. The Journal of Gatt, J. M., Nemeroff, C. B., Dobson-Stone, C., Paul, R. H.,
Neuroscience, 24, 4401–4411. https://doi.org/10.1523/ Bryant, R. A., Schofield, P. R., Gordon, E., Kemp, A. H., &
JNEUROSCI.0348-04.2004 Williams, L. M. (2009). Interactions between BDNF Val66Met
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences polymorphism and early life stress predict brain and arousal
(2nd ed.). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203771587 pathways to syndromal depression and anxiety. Molecular Psy-
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global mea- chiatry, 14, 681–695. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2008.143
sure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, Getzmann, S., Gajewski, P. D., Hengstler, J. G., Falkenstein, M., &
24, 385–396. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136404 Beste, C. (2013). BDNF Val66Met polymorphism and goal-
14609568, 2023, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ejn.16153 by Nat Prov Indonesia, Wiley Online Library on [20/01/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
3914 CORRONE ET AL.

directed behavior in healthy elderly—Evidence from auditory transition to university: The roles of BDNF and working mem-
distraction. NeuroImage, 64, 290–298. https://doi.org/10.1016/ ory capacity. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience,
j.neuroimage.2012.08.079 15, 95–103. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-014-0305-8
Ginty, A. T., Kraynak, T. E., Kuan, D. C., & Gianaros, P. J. (2019). Li, W., Liu, B., Xu, J., Jiang, T., & Yu, C. (2016). Interaction of
Ventromedial prefrontal cortex connectivity during and after COMTrs4680 and BDNFrs6265 polymorphisms on functional
psychological stress in women. Psychophysiology, 56, e13445. connectivity density of the left frontal eye field in healthy
https://doi.org/10.1016/10.1111/psyp.13445 young adults. Human Brain Mapping, 37, 2468–2478. https://
Givalois, L., Naert, G., Rage, F., Ixart, G., Arancibia, S., & Tapia- doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23187
Arancibia, L. (2004). A single brain-derived neurotrophic fac- Marteau, T. M., & Bekker, H. (1992). The development of a six-item
tor injection modifies hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenocortical short-form of the state scale of the Spielberger State—Trait
axis activity in adult male rats. Molecular and Cellular Neuro- Anxiety Inventory (STAI). British Journal of Clinical Psychol-
science, 27, 280–295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mcn.2004.07.002 ogy, 31, 301–306. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1992.
Gourley, S. L., Kedves, A. T., Olausson, P., & Taylor, J. R. (2009). A tb00997.x
history of corticosterone exposure regulates fear extinction and McNair, D., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L. (1971). Manual for the pro-
cortical NR2B, GluR2/3, and BDNF. Neuropsychopharmacol- file of mood states (POMS). Educational and Industrial Testing
ogy, 34, 707–716. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2008.123 Service.
Hashimoto, T., Fukui, K., Takeuchi, H., Yokota, S., Kikuchi, Y., Moore, S. R. (2017). Commentary: What is the case for candidate
Tomita, H., Taki, Y., & Kawashima, R. (2016). Effects of the gene approaches in the era of high-throughput genomics? A
BDNF Val66Met polymorphism on gray matter volume in typ- response to Border and Keller (2017). Journal of Child Psychol-
ically developing children and adolescents. Cerebral Cortex, 26, ogy and Psychiatry, 58, 331–334. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.
1795–1803. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw020 12697
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and con- Murphy, B. L., Arnsten, A. F., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., & Roth, R. H.
ditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford (1996). Increased dopamine turnover in the prefrontal cortex
Press. https://doi.org/10.1111/jedm.12050 impairs spatial working memory performance in rats and
Hofer, M., Pagliusi, S. R., Hohn, A., Leibrock, J., & Barde, Y. A. monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93,
(1990). Regional distribution of brain-derived neurotrophic 1325–1329. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.93.3.1325
factor mRNA in the adult mouse brain. The EMBO Journal, 9, Notaras, M., Hill, R., & van den Buuse, M. (2015). The BDNF gene
2459–2464. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1460-2075.1990.tb07423.x Val66Met polymorphism as a modifier of psychiatric disorder
Horan, B., Heckenberg, R., Maruff, P., & Wright, B. (2020). Devel- susceptibility: Progress and controversy. Molecular Psychiatry,
opment of a new virtual reality test of cognition: Assessing the 20, 916–930. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2015.27
test-retest reliability, convergent and ecological validity of Notaras, M., & van den Buuse, M. (2018). Brain-derived neuro-
CONVIRT. BMC Psychology, 8, 61. https://doi.org/10.1186/ trophic factor (BDNF): Novel insights into regulation and
s40359-020-00429-x genetic variation. The Neuroscientist, 25, 434–454. https://doi.
Jeanneteau, F., Garabedian, M. J., & Chao, M. V. (2008). Activation org/10.1177/1073858418810142
of Trk neurotrophin receptors by glucocorticoids provides a Nyenhuis, D. L., Yamamoto, C., Luchetta, T., Terrien, A., &
neuroprotective effect. Proceedings of the National Academy of Parmentier, A. (1999). Adult and geriatric normative data and
Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 4862–4867. validation of the profile of mood states. Journal of Clinical Psy-
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0709102105 chology, 55, 79–86. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679
Jeanneteau, F. D., Lambert, W. M., Ismaili, N., Bath, K. G., (199901)55:1<79::AID-JCLP8>3.0.CO;2-7
Lee, F. S., Garabedian, M. J., & Chao, M. V. (2012). BDNF and Oh, W. C., Rodríguez, G., Asede, D., Jung, K., Hwang, I.-W.,
glucocorticoids regulate corticotrophin-releasing hormone Ogelman, R., Bolto, M. L. M., & Kwon, H.-B. (2021). Dysregu-
(CRH) homeostasis in the hypothalamus. Proceedings of the lation of the mesoprefrontal dopamine circuit mediates an
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, early-life stress-induced synaptic imbalance in the prefrontal
109, 1305–1310. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1114122109 cortex. Cell Reports, 35, 109074. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Jett, J. D., Bulin, S. E., Hatherall, L. C., McCartney, C. M., & celrep.2021.109074
Morilak, D. A. (2017). Deficits in cognitive flexibility induced Örücü, M. Ç., & Demir, A. (2009). Psychometric evaluation of per-
by chronic unpredictable stress are associated with impaired ceived stress scale for Turkish university students. Stress and
glutamate neurotransmission in the rat medial prefrontal cor- Health, 25, 103–109. https://doi.org/10.1002/smi.1218
tex. Neuroscience, 346, 284–297. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Patel, P. D., Lopez, J. F., Lyons, D. M., Burke, S., Wallace, M., &
neuroscience.2017.01.017 Schatzberg, A. F. (2000). Glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid
Krueger, F., Pardini, M., Huey, E. D., Raymont, V., Solomon, J., receptor mRNA expression in squirrel monkey brain. Journal
Lipsky, R. H., Hodgkinson, C. A., Goldman, D., & Grafman, J. of Psychiatric Research, 34, 383–392. https://doi.org/10.1002/
(2011). The role of the Met66 brain-derived neurotrophic fac- 10.1016/S0022-3956(00)00035-2
tor allele in the recovery of executive functioning after Pecina, M., Martinez-Jauand, M., Love, T., Heffernan, J.,
combat-related traumatic brain injury. The Journal of Neuro- Montoya, P., Hodgkinson, C., Stohler, C. S., Goldman, D., &
science, 31, 598–606. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI. Zubieta, J. K. (2014). Valence-specific effects of BDNF Val66-
1399-10.2011 Met polymorphism on dopaminergic stress and reward proces-
LeMoult, J., Carver, C. S., Johnson, S. L., & Joormann, J. (2015). sing in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 5874–5881.
Predicting change in symptoms of depression during the https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2152-13.2014
14609568, 2023, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ejn.16153 by Nat Prov Indonesia, Wiley Online Library on [20/01/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
CORRONE ET AL. 3915

Perlman, W. R., Webster, M. J., Herman, M. M., Kleinman, J. E., & prefrontal cortex in the working memory maintenance of one’s
Weickert, C. S. (2007). Age-related differences in glucocorti- own emotional responses. Scientific Reports, 8, 3460. https://
coid receptor mRNA levels in the human brain. Neurobiology doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-21896-8
of Aging, 28, 447–458. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Smith, M. A., Makino, S., Kvetnansky, R., & Post, R. M. (1995).
neurobiolaging.2006.01.010 Stress and glucocorticoids affect the expression of brain-
Pietzuch, M., Bindoff, A., Jamadar, S., & Vickers, J. C. (2021). Inter- derived neurotrophic factor and neurotrophin-3 mRNAs in the
active effects of the APOE and BDNF polymorphisms on func- hippocampus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 1768–1777.
tional brain connectivity: The Tasmanian Healthy Brain https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-03-01768.1995
Project. Scientific Reports, 11, 14514. https://doi.org/10.1038/ Soares, J. M., Sampaio, A., Ferreira, L. M., Santos, N. C.,
s41598-021-93610-0 Marques, P., Marques, F., Palha, J. A., Cerqueira, J. J., &
Pitts, B. L., Wen, V., Whealin, J. M., Fogle, B. M., Southwick, S. M., Sousa, N. (2013). Stress impact on resting state brain networks.
Esterlis, I., & Pietrzak, R. H. (2020). Depression and cognitive PLoS ONE, 8, e66500. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.
dysfunction in older U.S. military veterans: Moderating effects 0066500
of BDNF Val66Met polymorphism and physical exercise. The Sullivan, P. F. (2007). Spurious genetic associations. Biological Psy-
American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 28, 959–967. https:// chiatry, 61, 1121–1126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.
doi.org/10.1016/j.jagp.2020.02.001 11.010
Pradhan, J., Noakes, P. G., & Bellingham, M. C. (2019). The role of Surtees, P. G., Wainwright, N. W. J., Willis-Owen, S. A. G.,
altered BDNF/TrkB signaling in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Sandhu, M. S., Luben, R., Day, N. E., & Flint, J. (2007). No
Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience, 13, 368. https://doi.org/10. association between the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism and
3389/fncel.2019.00368 mood status in a non-clinical community sample of 7389 older
Roth, R. H., Tam, S.-Y., Ida, Y., Yang, J.-X., & Deutch, A. Y. (1988). adults. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 41, 404–409. https://
Stress and the mesocorticolimbic dopamine systems. Annals of doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.01.004
the New York Academy of Sciences, 537, 138–147. https://doi. Taylor, J., & Deane, F. P. (2002). Development of a short form
org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1988.tb42102.x of the Test Anxiety Inventory (TAI). The Journal of
Rowe, G., Hirsh, J. B., & Anderson, A. K. (2007). Positive affect General Psychology, 129, 127–136. https://doi.org/10.1080/
increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings of 00221300209603133
the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 383–388. https://doi. Tsuru, J., Tanaka, Y., Ishitobi, Y., Maruyama, Y., Inoue, A.,
org/10.1073/pnas.0605198104 Kawano, A., Ikeda, R., Ando, T., Oshita, H., Aizawa, S.,
Roy, B., & Ghatak, S. (2013). Nonlinear methods to assess changes Masuda, K., Higuma, H., Kanehisa, M., Ninomiya, T., &
in heart rate variability in type 2 diabetic patients. Arquivos Akiyoshi, J. (2014). Association of BDNF Val66Met polymor-
Brasileiros de Cardiologia, 101, 317–327. https://doi.org/10. phism with HPA and SAM axis reactivity to psychological and
5935/abc.20130181 physical stress. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 10,
Saunders, J. B., Aasland, O. G., Babor, T. F., De La Fuente, J. R., & 2123–2133. https://doi.org/10.2147/ndt.s68629
Grant, M. (1993). Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Verhagen, M., Van Der Meij, A., Van Deurzen, P. A. M.,
Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on Janzing, J. G. E., Arias-Vasquez, A., Buitelaar, J. K., &
early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption- Franke, B. (2010). Meta-analysis of the BDNF Val66Met poly-
II. Addiction, 88, 791–804. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443. morphism in major depressive disorder: Effects of gender and
1993.tb02093.x ethnicity. Molecular Psychiatry, 15, 260–271. https://doi.org/
Schuch, S., & Pütz, S. (2018). Mood state dissociates conflict adapta- 10.1038/mp.2008.109
tion within tasks and across tasks. Journal of Experimental Psy- Viggiano, D., Vallone, D., Ruocco, L. A., & Sadile, A. G. (2003).
chology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 1487–1499. Behavioural, pharmacological, morpho-functional molecular
https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000530 studies reveal a hyperfunctioning mesocortical dopamine sys-
Selin, K. H. (2003). Test-retest reliability of the alcohol use disorder tem in an animal model of attention deficit and hyperactivity
identification test in a general population sample. Alcoholism: disorder. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 27, 683–689.
Clinical and Experimental Research, 27, 1428–1435. https://doi. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.08.011
org/10.1097/01.ALC.0000085633.23230.4A Vossel, S., Geng, J. J., & Fink, G. R. (2014). Dorsal and ventral
Shaffer, F., & Ginsberg, J. P. (2017). An overview of heart rate vari- attention systems. The Neuroscientist, 20, 150–159. https://doi.
ability metrics and norms. Frontiers in Public Health, 5, 258. org/10.1177/1073858413494269
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258 Wang, L., Ashley-Koch, A., Steffens, D. C., Krishnan, K. R. R., &
Shields, A. L., & Caruso, J. C. (2003). Reliability generalization of Taylor, W. D. (2012). Impact of BDNF Val66Met and
the alcohol use disorders identification test. Educational and 5-HTTLPR polymorphism variants on neural substrates
Psychological Measurement, 63, 404–413. https://doi.org/10. related to sadness and executive function. Genes, Brain and
1177/0013164403063003004 Behavior, 11, 352–359. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-183X.
Smith, J. P., Achua, J. K., Summers, T. R., Ronan, P. J., & 2012.00764.x
Summers, C. H. (2014). Neuropeptide S and BDNF gene Wheeler, M. J., Green, D. J., Ellis, K. A., Cerin, E., Heinonen, I.,
expression in the amygdala are influenced by social decision- Naylor, L. H., Larsen, R., Wennberg, P., Boraxbekk, C.-J.,
making under stress. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, Lewis, J., Eikelis, N., Lautenschlager, N. T., Kingwe, B. A.,
121. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00121 Lambert, G., Owen, N., Dunstan, D. W., & Dunstan, D. W.
Smith, R., Lane, R. D., Alkozei, A., Bao, J., Smith, C., Sanova, A., (2020). Distinct effects of acute exercise and breaks in sitting
Nettles, M., & Killgore, W. D. S. (2018). The role of medial on working memory and executive function in older adults: A
14609568, 2023, 8, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/ejn.16153 by Nat Prov Indonesia, Wiley Online Library on [20/01/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
3916 CORRONE ET AL.

three-arm, randomised cross-over trial to evaluate the effects span. Human Brain Mapping, 43, 1941–1954. https://doi.org/
of exercise with and without breaks in sitting on cognition. 10.1002/hbm.25765
British Journal of Sports Medicine, 54, 776–781. https://doi.org/ Zhuang, X., Oosting, R. S., Jones, S. R., Gainetdinov, R. R.,
10.1136/bjsports-2018-100168 Miller, G. W., Caron, M. G., & Hen, R. (2001). Hyperactivity
Wiłkosc, M., Szałkowska, A., Skibi
nska, M., Zając-Lamparska, L., and impaired response habituation in hyperdopaminergic
Maciukiewicz, M., & Araszkiewicz, A. (2016). BDNF gene mice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98,
polymorphisms and haplotypes in relation to cognitive perfor- 1982–1987. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.98.4.1982
mance in Polish healthy subjects. Acta Neurobiologiae Experi-
mentalis, 76, 43–52. https://doi.org/10.21307/ane-2017-004
Yang, A. C., Chen, T.-J., Tsai, S.-J., Hong, C.-J., Kuo, C.-H.,
Yang, C.-H., & Kao, K.-P. (2010). BDNF Val66Met polymor- How to cite this article: Corrone, M., Nanev, A.,
phism alters sympathovagal balance in healthy subjects. Amato, I., Bicknell, R., Piantella, S., Maruff, P., van
American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric den Buuse, M., & Wright, B. J. (2023). The
Genetics, 153B, 1024–1030. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b. brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66met
31069
polymorphism is associated with better attention
Yu, H., Wang, D.-D., Wang, Y., Liu, T., Lee, F. S., & Chen, Z.-Y.
(2012). Variant brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met
and working memory performance and resilience
polymorphism alters vulnerability to stress and response to to mild chronic stress. European Journal of
antidepressants. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 4092–4101. Neuroscience, 58(8), 3903–3916. https://doi.org/10.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5048-11.2012 1111/ejn.16153
Zhao, J., Wang, J., Huang, C., & Liang, P. (2022). Involvement of
the dorsal and ventral attention networks in visual attention

You might also like