Free-Play Behaviors in Preschool and Kindergarten Children

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 4

Free-Play Behaviors in Preschool and Kindergarten Children

Author(s): Kenneth H. Rubin, Kathryn S. Watson and Thomas W. Jambor


Source: Child Development, Vol. 49, No. 2 (Jun., 1978), pp. 534-536
Published by: Wiley on behalf of the Society for Research in Child Development
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1128725 .
Accessed: 19/03/2013 19:45

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Wiley and Society for Research in Child Development are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and
extend access to Child Development.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 129.8.242.67 on Tue, 19 Mar 2013 19:45:46 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Free-Play Behaviors in Preschool and
Kindergarten Children

Kenneth H. Rubin and Kathryn S. Watson


University of Waterloo

Thomas W. Jambor
Universityof Alabamaat Birmingham

RUBIN, KENNETH H.; WATSON, KATHRYN S.; and JAMBOR, THOMAS W. Free-Play Behaviors in
Preschool and KindergartenChildren. CHILDDEVELOPMENT,1978, 49, 534-536. The present
study concernedage differencesin the socialand cognitiveplay of children.It was revealed
that kindergarten
childrendisplayedless unoccupied,onlooker,solitary,and functionalactivity
and more group and4ramaticplay than preschoolers.Examinationof the combinedParten-
SmilanskyPlay Scale revealedpreschoolersto engage in significantlymore solitary-functional
and parallel-functional
play and in less parallel-constructive,
parallel-dramatic,and group
dramaticplay than theirkindergartencounterparts.

Recently Rubin (e.g., Rubin, Maioni, & was to examine comparatively the free-play
Hornung 1976) has examined the free-play behaviorsof both preschool-and kindergarten-
behaviorsof preschoolersby utilizing an obser- aged children.
vational format which nests the cognitive play
categories of Smilansky (1968) within the so- The preschoolersparticipatingin the pres-
cial play categories of Parten (1932). Smilan- ent investigationincluded all 15 males (mean
sky's four sequential play categorieshave been age = 51.93 months) and 12 females (mean
labeled functional (simple, repetitive muscle age = 53 months) who attended a half-day
movements with or without objects), construc- program on the campus of a small teacher's
tive (creating something), and dramatic (sub- college in New York State (age range = 45-
stitution of imaginarysituationsto satisfy one's 59 months). The kindergarten sample con-
wishes or needs) play and games with rules, sisted of all 14 males (mean age = 62.30
months) and 14 females (mean age = 64.70
respectively. Parten'ssix sequential social play
categories included unoccupied behavior months) attending a half-day public school
of program in central Ontario (age range = 58-
(watching anything passing interest; walk- 69
ing around room; playing with body), solitary schools months). The pupil-teacher ratio in both
was 10:1. The curriculumof each class-
play (playing alone with materials different
from childrenwithin speakingdistance;no con- room would be considered"traditional"(Sears
versation with others), onlooker behavior & Dowley 1963) in nature. All play behavior
was observed in the one large classroomavail-
(watching others; perhaps talking to others able to each group of children. The children
but with no entry into the play situation),
parallel play (playing independentlywith toys were predominantly from lower-middle-class
similar to those being used by others in close homes.
proximity; no attempt to play with others), The procedure was essentially identical
associative play (playing with other children; with that followed by Rubin et al. (1976).
no role assignmentor organizationof Each child was observed during free play for
activity),
and cooperativeplay (playing in an 1 min on 30 consecutiveschool days. The order
organized
group). of observation was randomized daily. During
each minute, the observer recorded the num-
Investigations which have adopted the ber of seconds (to the nearest 5 sec) in which
strategy of nesting cognitive play within social the child engaged in particularforms of play.
play categories have been limited to children The durationof each child'scognitiveplay with-
of preschoolage. Thus, the intent of this
study in each social play category (e.g., solitary-func-
Requestsfor reprintsshouldbe sent to KennethH. Rubin,Departmentof
Universityof Waterloo,Waterloo,OntarioN2L 3G1. Psychology,
[ChildDevelopment,1978,49, 534-536. @ 1978 by the Society for Researchin Child
Development,Inc.
751
0009-3920/78/4902-00391;00.

This content downloaded from 129.8.242.67 on Tue, 19 Mar 2013 19:45:46 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Rubin, Watson, and Jambor 535
tional) was noted. Following each time sample, of the preschool and kindergartenwritten re-
the observerrecorded exactly what it was the cordings of behavior were randomly selected.
child was doing and with whom (e.g., subject The senior author then assigned these written
at table independently building block tower- descriptions (made by the two junior authors)
sitting beside Amy and Leslie who are using to social-cognitiveplay categories. Percentages
play dough). The only deviation from Rubin of agreementwere 95 for the preschool sample
et al. (1976) concerned the associative and and 90 for the kindergartensample.
cooperative play categories. Upon reviewing The mean numbers of seconds children
the behavioral data, we found that particular
engaged in all play forms are presented in
activities were being consistently coded as table 1. A grade (2) x sex (2) x social play
"associative"in preschoolersbut "cooperative" (3) x cognitive play (3) ANOVA was calcu-
in kindergartners.As a result, these two cate- lated. Onlooker, unoccupied, and game be-
gories were collapsed to form a single "group haviors were not included in this analysis.
play" category. In addition, unoccupied and These three categories either occurred alone
onlookerbehaviorswere scored. (unoccupied and onlookerbehaviors) or across
a limited range of social-cognitive play com-
Interjudge reliability was calculated in binations (games). The importantdata to con-
two ways. First, priorto formaldata collection, sider are the grade (G) x social play (S),
two observers simultaneously gathered 20 G x cognitive play (C), and G x S x C
1-min time samples of play for each of the interactions.A significant G x C interaction
preschool and kindergarten groups. Percent- was found, F (2,204) = 12.01, p < .02. New-
ages of agreement (where an agreement was man-Keuls analyses (p < .05) revealed pre-
defined as identical codings of behavior with schoolers to display significantly more func-
durationsnot exceeding a 5-sec difference be- tional play and less dramatic play than did
tween observers) of 92.5 and 87.5 were found kindergartners.The latter finding may be a
for the preschool and kindergartensample, re- function of the elder group's more advanced
spectively. Second, 20 1-minsamplesfromeach conceptionsof social decentrationand recipro-
TABLE 1
MEANS AND PERCENTAGESFOR COMBINEDSOCIAL
AND COGNITIVEPLAY CATEGORIES

PRESCHOOL KINDERGARTEN
RMHa
MEASURE Mean %b Mean %
o
Solitary:
Functional....... 139.89 7.77 44.27 2.46 4.88
Constructive..... 247.11 13.73 241.85 13.44 7.67
Dramatic........ 34.19 1.90 31.61 1.76 2.41
Total......... 421.19 23.40 318.03 17.66 14.96
Parallel:
Functional........ 118.87 6.60 24.65 1.37 12.96
Constructive..... 300.48 16.69 392.14 21.79 15.07
Dramatic...... .. 24.81 1.38 125.00 6.94 1.04
Total.......... 444.16 24.67 541.79 30.10 29.07

Group:
Functional........ 41.99 2.33 43.04 2.39 10.47
Constructive..... 326.37 18.13 301.19 16.73 17.81
Dramatic........ 190.89 10.61 382.14 21.23 10.81
Games........... 33.19 1.84 37.50 2.08 .30
Total......... 592.44 32.91 763.57 42.43 38.77

Unoccupied ........ 118.19 6.57 16.43 .09 17.200


Onlooker .......... 223.72 12.43 160.72 8.93

Percentagesof play for middle-classchildrentaken from Rubin et al. (1976).


b Percentagescalculated by dividing mean numberof seconds per category by total seconds (1,800 sec).
* Combinedunoccupiedand onlookerpercentage.

This content downloaded from 129.8.242.67 on Tue, 19 Mar 2013 19:45:46 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
536 Child Development
cal relations-two propertiesthought to be pre- ties while males engaged in dramatic play in
requisite to sociodramaticinterchanges (Smi- close proximitybut not with their peers.
lansky 1968). The lesser frequency of func-
tional play in kindergartnersmay be illustra- At this point, it would be worthwhile to
tive of their attainmentof more complex cog- compare the present preschool findings with
nitive skills. This hypothetical link between those of Rubin et al. (1976). Given the data
for Rubin et al.'s middle-class sample, there
play and cognitive developmentdraws support was more constructive and solitary play and
from the Rubin and Maioni (1975) study in
which the frequency of functional play was less functionalplay in our sample. The former
related negatively to performance on classifi- two differences may be accounted for almost
cation and role-takingtasks, while the frequen- entirely by the greater incidence of solitary-
constructive play found herein. The present
cy of dramatic play was related positively to
performanceon these measures. group also emitted less group play than did
the previous sample. However, this difference
A significantG x S interactionwas also is entirely accountedfor by the greateramount
found, F (2,204) = 4.49, p < .02. Newman- of group-functionalplay. In short, the Rubin
Keuls analyses indicated preschoolers engage et al. (1976) sample displayed less mature
in significantly more solitary and less group modes of play-a finding which may have re-
play than kindergartners.In addition, a series sulted from the children being 6.1 months
of t tests between grades revealed preschoolers younger on the average than those observed
to display significantlymore unoccupied,t(53) herein.
= 4.10, p < .001, and onlooker behaviors,
In conclusion, this report extended the
t(53) = 2.64, p < .02, than kindergartners.It growing literatureconcerningthe play of young
is noteworthythat the amount of parallel play
children. The report also revealed a number
was not different for the two age groups.
of weaknessesin the often cited Parten (1932)
Moreover, the finding of solitary-play differ- scale of child's
ences may have been of some surprise given play. For one, observers had
the previous Rubin et al. (1976) belief that difficulty distinguishing between associative
and cooperativeplay. For another,the singular
this category was not as reflective of imma-
use of the Partenscale would not have allowed
as
turity once thought (Parten 1932). the discovery that the social-cognitiveplay of
The G X S X C significant interaction, preschoolerswas qualitativelyless mature than
F (4,204) = 3.95, p < .01, clarifies the above that of kindergartners.These
findings support
findings. Simultaneous consideration of both the use of the nested play scale in future
scales revealed (Newman-Keulsanalyses) that studies of children'splay.
preschoolers engaged in significantly more
parallel-functionaland less parallel-construc- References
tive and parallel-dramaticplay than the older Parten,M. B. Socialparticipation
children. Moreover, the kindergartenchildren amongpreschool
children. Journal of Abnormaland Social Psy-
displayed less solitary-functional and more chology, 1932, 27, 243-269.
group-dramaticplay than preschoolers.These Rubin, K. H., & Maioni,T. L. Play preference
data indicate that within each Partenplay cate- and its relationshipto egocentrism,popularity,
gory, the preschoolers tended to emit less and classificationskillsin preschoolers.
Merrill-
mature cognitive-play behaviors while kinder- Palmer Quarterly, 1975, 25, 171-179.
gartners displayed more mature cognitive-play Rubin,K. H.; Maioni,T. L.; & Hornung,M. Free
behaviors.' play behaviorsin middle-and lower-classpre-
As for sex differences,a significantsex X schoolers:Partenand Piaget revisited.Child
S x C interaction,F(4,204) = 3.49, p < Development, 1976, 47, 414-419.
.01,
followed by Newman-Keulsmultiple compari- Sears,P., & Dowley, E. Researchon teachingin
sons, revealed that females displayed more the nurseryschool.In N. Gage (Ed.), Hand-
book of research on teaching. Skokie, Ill.:
parallel-constructiveand less parallel-dramatic RandMcNally,1963.
play than males. These data supportthe earlier Smilansky, S. The effects of sociodramaticplay on
findings of Rubin et al. (1976), who reported disadvantaged children: preschool children.
that females displayed more sedentary activi- New York:Wiley,1968.
1 A completedescriptionof ANOVAand Newman-Keuls resultsconcerningdifferencesin
socialand cognitiveplaywithineachgradeis availablefromK. H. Rubin
uponrequest.

This content downloaded from 129.8.242.67 on Tue, 19 Mar 2013 19:45:46 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like