PDF Learning

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 19

Assessing and Helping Challenging Students:

Part One, Why Do Some Students Have


Difficulty Learning?
William D. Hendricson, M.S., M.A.; John H. Kleffner, Ed.D.
Abstract: When students struggle with routine assignments and fall behind classmates, a busy teacher may pigeonhole them as
slow, give up on them, or become frustrated from failed efforts to bring them up to speed. Well-intentioned efforts to help
struggling students by providing repetitions of the same experiences may fail because the specific cause of the sub-par perfor-
mance was not identified. Six potential causes of inadequate student performance can serve as a diagnostic framework to help
teachers pinpoint why a student is struggling academically: 1) cognitive factors, including poorly integrated, compartmentalized
information, poor metacognition that hinders the student’s ability to monitor and self-correct performance, bona fide learning
disabilities that require professional assessment and treatment, and sensory-perceptual difficulties that may hinder performance in
certain health care disciplines; 2) ineffective study habits, which are more common among professional students than faculty
realize; 3) an inadequate educational experience (unclear objectives, poorly organized instruction, absence of coaching and
timely feedback) or a punitive environment in which students avoid approaching instructors for assistance; 4) distraction due to
nonacademic issues such as social relationships, health of a spouse, or employment; 5) dysfunctional levels of defensiveness that
hinder student-teacher communication; and 6) underlying medical conditions that may affect student attentiveness, motivation,
energy, and emotional balance. The objective of this article is to help faculty recognize potential underlying causes of a student’s
learning problems. Strategies for helping the academically struggling student are also introduced for several of these etiologies.
Mr. Hendricson is Director, and Dr. Kleffner is former Educational Development Specialist, both at the Division of Educational
Research and Development, Department of Academic Informatics Services, The University of Texas Health Science Center at San
Antonio. Direct correspondence and requests for reprints to William Hendricson, Director, Division of Educational Research and
Development, The University of Texas Health Science Center at San Antonio, 7703 Floyd Curl Drive, San Antonio, TX 78229-
3900; 210-567-2813 phone; hendricson@uthscsa.edu.
Key words: learning, dental students, curriculum, teaching methods
Submitted for publication 9/14/01; accepted 11/15/01
Editor’s Note: Part two of this study will appear in a subsequent issue of the Journal of Dental Education.

T
he article “Memory and the Brain” by Lee able to talk to ourselves (for example, conduct our
Robertson in this issue of the Journal of Den- private inner conversations)? And how are we able
tal Education reviews the neurobiology of to communicate our thoughts to others? Another
memory, focusing on the complex physiological and mystery, less theological in nature but just as pro-
chemical processes by which memories are created found for teachers, is why in any group of students a
and the influence of various factors on memory for- few individuals who have seemingly equivalent abili-
mation including stress, sleep, drugs, and aging. ties and incentives perform less well than others or
Memory is the foundation of learning: without the deviate so dramatically from the norms of expected
ability to retain and retrieve information and experi- behavior that they become labeled as problem stu-
ences, we could not perform the tasks of daily liv- dents.
ing, or take on the responsibilities associated with In a perfect world, all students would be highly
our occupations. One of the profound mysteries of motivated, energetic, enthusiastic, intellectually cu-
human life is how self-awareness and thought emerge rious, eager to learn, and academically successful.
from the movement of electrical charges from brain Most students in health professions education have
cell to brain cell. This electrical-to-chemical-to-elec- these desirable characteristics. However, over the
trical transmission process involves the axon of one course of a teaching career, faculty will encounter more
neuron connecting to a dendrite of another neuron, a than a few students who are challenging—the type of
process that occurs billions of times every second. student who provokes the teacher’s lament, “I spend
How does the miracle of thought emerge from this 90 percent of my time on 10 percent of the students.”
process? That is, how are we able to generate ideas, The term “challenging student” refers to a student with
observations, reactions, and questions? How are we one or more of the following characteristics:

January 2002 ■ Journal of Dental Education 43


• Has difficulty learning or performing up to ex- introduced. A companion article in a subsequent is-
pectations, sue of the Journal of Dental Education will review
• Is distracted and does not devote full attention to the causes of inappropriate and dysfunctional stu-
academic responsibilities, dent behavior such as acute defensiveness, apathy,
• Is difficult or unpleasant to work with; for ex- and belligerence; explore reasons for apparent lack
ample, has an attitude problem or is defensive, and of motivation among some students; and present a
• Does not appear to be motivated to learn. protocol for conducting an “educational intervention”
The senior author (Hendricson) frequently con- with a challenging student.
ducts workshops on teaching difficult students. The
first workshop exercise asks participants to propose
adjectives that describe students who are challeng- Assessing and Diagnosing
ing and students whom teachers enjoy. Adjectives
assigned to challenging students appear below on the the Etiology of Challenging
left; on the right are words that describe learners
whom teachers enjoy. Student Behavior
In the authors’ collective sixty years of experi-
Students who are Students whom ence in health professions education, most cases of
challenging or difficult teachers enjoy challenging student behavior can be classified into
Lethargic, listless, lazy Enthusiastic, energetic, the categories presented in Figure 1:
eager • learning problems, which are reflected in perfor-
Disorganized Motivated; have inner mance consistently below expectations or a sud-
(don’t use time wisely) drive den decline in performance by a student who has
a solid academic record; and
Frequently repeat the Learn from mistakes
• attitudinal issues, which include a variety of be-
same mistakes
havioral manifestations among students that are
No initiative Volunteer for tasks and perplexing, frustrating, and, in extreme cases, un-
(expect to be spoonfed) extra work acceptable to faculty and disruptive to the overall
Not punctual and Punctual and follow learning environment.
ignore rules directions Underlying medical conditions, including psy-
Indifferent (don’t Put in extra time (arrive chological problems, may contribute to substandard
appear to care; early; leave late) performance or undesirable behaviors and attitudes.
emotionless) The student may or may not be aware of medical
Defensive (hostile when Ask for feedback problems or may attempt to obscure medical condi-
feedback is given) on their performance tions, including chemical dependency, from student
peers and faculty.
We will focus here on the left hand column— In relation to the attitudinal issues displayed in
the challenging student. The objective is to help dental Figure 1, the authors have observed faculty to be
educators understand and assess factors that may particularly perplexed and frustrated by three types
cause dysfunctional student behavior in two areas: of behavior: acute defensiveness that hinders student-
performance (how students acquire knowledge and teacher communication and thus the quality of the
learn to perform skills) and attitude (the way stu- learning experience; lack of personal motivation; and
dents interact with others in the academic environ- the “cocky” know-it-all attitude of students who have
ment). Attitudinal issues are often intertwined with a high estimation of their ability, sometimes deserved
the student’s performance difficulties in either a and sometimes not. Defensiveness, lack of motiva-
causal or reactive relationship. This article will fo- tion, and know-it-all demeanor can be the behavioral
cus on the potential causes of substandard perfor- manifestations of underlying learning deficiencies
mance by presenting a series of diagnostic questions or medical problems. Defensiveness and a passive
designed to help faculty identify the underlying eti- approach to learning, which are often perceived as a
ology of the learning deficiency. Strategies for help- lack of motivation, appear to be intertwined with the
ing the student with learning difficulties will also be student’s sense of safety within the academic envi-

44 Journal of Dental Education ■ Volume 66, No. 1


Figure 1. Categories of challenging student behavior

ronment and his or her “survival” strategy (that is, ability to recognize spatial relationships among ob-
making it to graduation day). jects and perceive small differences in shape, size,
position, and texture. For some students, difficulties
with spatial-perceptual tasks may be due to the way
Assessing Causes of Learning their brains process information received from the
environment.
Deficiency The second factor is study habits. Ineffective
study methods may contribute to substandard learn-
The etiology of learning deficiencies is com- ing. Quality of the academic environment is third.
plex and can involve a variety of factors often acting Sometimes the overall curriculum is so poorly con-
in concert. Six factors should be considered when a ceived or implemented that learning becomes diffi-
student has difficulty learning. These are briefly de- cult even for the best students. Student distraction
scribed here and then explored in more detail below. is fourth. Stressful, time-consuming, and energy-sap-
The first factor is cognition. Abnormalities can ping events in a student’s life outside school can over-
occur related to how the student acquires, processes, whelm even highly motivated students and contrib-
stores, and retrieves information. For example, stu- ute to inadequate academic performance. Fifth is the
dents may advance to the clinical phase of the cur- affective component of learning. Learners evolve
riculum with poorly integrated knowledge because through an emotional development continuum as they
of curriculum format, teaching methods, or the progress through the stages of professional training.
student’s study strategies.1 These students may have During this maturation process, a student’s self-con-
difficulty retrieving the information needed to an- cept as a learner and attitudes about the task of learn-
swer questions asked by instructors or “thinking on ing change substantially. These evolving attitudes play
their feet” to cope with unexpected events that occur an important role in student-teacher interactions, of-
during patient treatment. Metacognition, which is the ten by creating a wall of defensiveness that hinders
ability to self-assess the quality of performance and communication and helps shape the teacher’s per-
make corrections, varies considerably from person ception of the student, sometimes in a negative man-
to person. People with poor metacognition cannot ner. Sixth are underlying medical problems. If all
distinguish accuracy from error and have inflated other causes of inadequate performance are ruled out,
impressions of their abilities. It is not out of the realm is it possible that an underlying health problem is the
of possibility that an underperforming dental student catalyst for the student’s academic struggles? Chemi-
may have an undiagnosed learning disability that re- cal imbalances, chronic stress, the effects of aging,
quires professional intervention. People differ in their and certain systemic diseases can alter the chemical

January 2002 ■ Journal of Dental Education 45


basis of memory formation and contribute to atten- tion, and decision-making.6-7 The word “appear” in
tion deficits, loss of concentration, lack of energy, the previous sentence is emphasized because we are
mental confusion, and emotional disorders. still several decades away from definitive answers
This section presents diagnostic questions for about how expertise develops. For now, we can say
each of these six factors that may contribute to sub- that the available evidence is suggestive but not con-
standard learning. clusive.
Figure 2 schematically represents probable dif-
ferences in the way novices and experts mentally
Cognition: The Student’s Thinking structure information they acquire from various
Processes sources and experiences. Novices’ information, rep-
resented by the left panel, is vertical and compart-
Teachers should consider the following ques-
mentalized. When confronted with a task or prob-
tions when a student exhibits learning deficiencies
lem new to them, novices struggle in a trial and error
that cannot be explained by other factors.
manner to assemble isolated bits of information, rep-
Does the Student Have Poorly Networked
resented by the various symbols within the columns,
Knowledge? This question will receive the most at-
because they lack pre-existing networks that allow
tention because it has implications for the overall
fast retrieval of pertinent information. Students may
educational process. There have been dramatic break-
have encyclopedic information about topics intro-
throughs in our understanding of brain function and
duced in the curriculum, but this information is com-
the neurophysiological mechanisms of learning and
partmentalized and largely unlinked to other topics.
memory.2-5 A principle outcome of brain research is
To develop problem-solving ability, students must
a better understanding of the neurophysiological
convert the largely unorganized fragments of data
mechanisms involved in the creation of knowledge
absorbed from textbooks and lectures into interlinked
networks. These neurocognitive networks appear to
chains of networked knowledge. Knowledge is de-
underlie many aspects of memory, pattern recogni-

Source: Hendricson WD, Cohen P. Oral health care in the 21st century: implications for dental and
medical education. Acad Med 2001;76(12):1181-1206.

Figure 2. Differences between novices’ and experts’ mental organization of information

46 Journal of Dental Education ■ Volume 66, No. 1


fined as information that individuals can explain in nities for one or more of the reasons discussed later
their own words, has recognized value and utility, in this paper but manage to move ahead because of
and can be retrieved instantaneously to solve prob- testing that measures relatively low levels of cogni-
lems.8-11 The conversion process of taking isolated tion function.
bits of information and consolidating them into use- A number of studies have established an asso-
ful knowledge is the core of the constructivism theory ciation between the overall structure of the curricu-
of learning which is the prevailing educational model lum, the way students study and prepare for tests,
among cognitive psychologists today.12 Experts (see and the depth at which they learn the subject matter
the right panel of Figure 2) have developed networks of courses.19-24 An active-learning environment in
that allow rapid retrieval of chains of knowledge rel- which students are frequently asked to assess data,
evant to solving a problem. Cognitive psychologists make decisions, and explain the thinking underlying
refer to this interlinked configuration by various their decisions appears to be our best bet for helping
names such as horizontally networked knowledge, students build integrated knowledge.9,11,25-26 Unfor-
elaborated knowledge, or spider web knowledge.13-17 tunately, many health professions students are the
Classic symptoms of a student with poorly in- products of undergraduate or preclinical curricula in
tegrated information include the following: long- which they spent hundreds of hours sitting in a lec-
winded, circuitous answers to questions that never ture hall listening (perhaps) to lectures. These stu-
get to the point; the inability to look at clinical data dents arrive in the clinical environment ill-prepared
and recognize an obvious pattern (for example, to to assess patient problems or even to answer ques-
recognize that 2 + 2 = 4); the inability to move be- tions that teachers perceive to be very easy.
yond obvious surface features when discussing a The traditional smokestack or “silo” curricu-
patient’s problems (versus discussing important un- lum has been the prevailing model for the health pro-
derlying etiologies); difficulty explaining relation- fessions education for decades.27-28 In this type of
ships among patient care variables (for example, how curriculum, each discipline operates autonomous
two drugs such as coumadin and captopril might al- mini-curricula, educating students independently
ter each other’s pharmacokinetics and affect the with minimal cross-fertilization among disciplines.
patient’s cardiac and pulmonary function); the inabil- Structurally, the silo curriculum is organized much
ity to compare and contrast physiologic mechanisms like the left side of Figure 2. The student graduates
(for example, the differences between hypovalemic and is assumed to be competent when all courses in
and cardiogenic shock); and a phenomenon known each of the discipline-based silos are passed. How-
as “anchoring” in which the student fails to recog- ever, there is growing discontent with this model.
nize the need to change an opinion or a patient care The Institute of Medicine-National Academy of Sci-
plan when new or different information becomes ence report To Err Is Human: Building a Safer Health
available that changes the situation.18 Care System examined the reasons for patient care
We have observed three types of students who errors that contribute to approximately 75,000 deaths
exhibit poorly integrated knowledge. and several hundred thousand near-misses annually
Type I students are in over their heads; they within health care facilities.29 Numerous contribut-
attempt to solve problems or perform tasks without ing factors were identified including three educa-
adequate prerequisite learning experiences. Type II tional causes: 1) reliance on silo curricula and pas-
students are at a natural stage of cognitive develop- sive, sponge-learning during training produces
ment along the novice-to-expert continuum where a entry-level practitioners with poor knowledge inte-
fragmented, poorly integrated knowledge base is the gration, poor vigilance for errors, and inadequate cop-
norm. Type III students may have unique difficulties ing skills when problems occur; 2) entry-level prac-
putting it all together that extend beyond the normal titioners lack training in information management
struggles of the novice learner. Type I students are systems and thus rely on memorization on the job;
frequently the result of a curriculum that has inad- and 3) there is a lack of opportunity to practice prob-
equate assessment methods at the foundational level lem-solving during training and on the job.
of the program (for example, tests that emphasize Given the reliance on silo curricula in health
memorization of facts rather than the ability to ap- professions education, clinical teachers should an-
ply information to problems), or they are individuals ticipate that many students will struggle with prob-
who did not take full advantage of learning opportu- lem-solving tasks. Research indicates that one of the

January 2002 ■ Journal of Dental Education 47


most effective strategies to help students develop the ronal connections; the more repetition, the easier it
mental muscle needed for problem-solving—as well is for the same neuronal pathway to reactivate in sub-
as one of the easiest and least resource-intensive to sequent situations. The process of making it easier
implement—is to create a learning environment in for neuronal pathways to reactivate may be the basis
which instructors make it a goal to frequently ask for how memories are created. 38 Students who
questions that help students “connect the dots.”30-32 struggle with rudimentary problem-solving tasks
Connect-the-dot questions provide students with appear to particularly benefit from frequent partici-
opportunities to think about and verbalize their un- pation in very focused problem-solving conferences
derstanding of the relationships between isolated bits in which teachers use the questioning techniques pre-
of information acquired throughout the curriculum. viously described to help learners practice answer-
Responding to connect-the-dot questions, which re- ing questions and then provide specific feedback to
quires students to pull together information acquired help students compare and contrast their responses
from different sources, has been shown to be a pow- to an ideal response.39-40
erful technique for building deeper understanding of We must add one note of caution. In our ca-
concepts and principles. The students’ process of reers, we have observed countless health professions
building personal meaning by articulating their un- faculty unfairly label students as substandard learn-
derstanding and interpretation of information in their ers before they have even begun the lengthy process
own words may be the most critical step in the pro- of creating these knowledge networks. Faculty ex-
cess of learning.11,13,33 Connect-the-dot questions in- pectations, particularly early in the clinical phase of
clude: decision questions (What do you think we training, are often skewed too high. Novice learners
should do for this patient?), why questions (Why do may be expected to assess patient problems and reach
you think this is our best option?), how questions decisions just as quickly as experienced faculty,
(How can we perform that procedure given the which is an unreasonable and inappropriate expecta-
patient’s condition?), what-if questions (What should tion. To help faculty guard against the unreasonable-
we do if the patient cannot tolerate the splint?), and expectation phenomenon in medical education,
dig deeper questions (That’s a good idea, but can Pangaro et al. developed the R-I-M-E model and a
you think of any other dietary plans we can recom- variation of this model adapted for dental education
mend for this patient?). These types of questions re- appears in Figure 3.41 This model identifies desir-
quire students to assemble their explanations from able levels of performance that can be expected of
different information compartments and thus have learners as they progress through the educational
been linked to higher levels of comprehension.33-36 continuum (reporter, interpreter, manager, educator).
The cognitive tasks involved in assessing the The level of performance appropriate and expected
intent of a decision question (for example, what for students at each of the four levels of training is
should we do next for this patient and why?), men- identified in the text. The sentence in bold at the con-
tally selecting information pertinent to the query, link- clusion of each skill-level description indicates level-
ing this information to construct an answer, verbal- appropriate assessment criteria for students at that
izing (or writing) the answer, offering a supportive particular phase of the curriculum. For example, as-
rationale for the decision, and answering follow-up sessment of students at the reporter level (an early
questions to further clarify, modify, or justify the re- phase of clinical training) should measure the com-
sponse can help learners start to make sense of the pleteness and accuracy of the students’ data collec-
mass of information they have absorbed in the cur- tion (by history-taking and patient examination) and
riculum.11 Through repetitions of similar cognitive the clarity and accuracy of their verbal and written
events (such as frequently answering connect-the-dots reports about the patient’s health. Students at this
questions), neuronal linkages are established and a ground floor level should not be assessed on their
memory of the response to a particular situation or ability to make diagnostic decisions about patient care
question becomes more deeply ingrained, thus in- that is appropriate only for learners on subsequent
creasing the potential for prompt retrieval in the fu- rungs of the ladder. This model is used in faculty
ture.37 As described in the Robertson article, it ap- development sessions and clinical orientations to re-
pears that neuron-to-neuron connections are mind faculty of the expectations that are appropriate
strengthened by repetition of experience. Repetition for learners at each stage of development as they pro-
produces coordinated activation of a network of neu- ceed through training.

48 Journal of Dental Education ■ Volume 66, No. 1


Figure 3. A clinical development continuum for setting level-appropriate expectations for learner performance

Does the Student Have Poor Metacognition? rect actions and decisions. People with well-devel-
Some people who have poorly developed skills or oped metacognition constantly self-correct and fine-
limited knowledge are simply oblivious to their in- tune their behavior and actions. This self-monitor-
competence. In any group of professional students, ing process allows people to make an appropriate
some individuals will fall into a category that has assessment of their capabilities and to display a level
been labeled “unskilled and unaware of it.”42-44 Un- of confidence that corresponds to actual ability.47-48
skilled and unaware students often reach erroneous Students with poorly developed metacognition blun-
conclusions, perform tasks ineptly, reach amazingly der from one error to the next, but have the mistaken
wrongheaded decisions, and repeatedly make unfor- impression that they are doing just fine. Researchers
tunate choices in their academic lives (as well as in in neuroscience and cognition believe that poor
their nonacademic lives), but are remarkably unaware metacognition is a fundamental component of incom-
of their incompetence. In fact, these individuals of- petence.
ten have very high, although inaccurate, estimates Unskilled and unaware students who overesti-
of their abilities and are very confident, if not arro- mate their capabilities and are overly confident can
gant.45 As Charles Darwin once observed, “Ignorance be challenging to teach. Research has shown that
more frequently begets confidence than does knowl- these individuals 1) rarely receive feedback that might
edge.”46 help them develop an accurate view of reality, 2) do
Unskilled and unaware students have poorly not learn from feedback unless it is very precise and
developed metacognition, which is the brain’s self- frequent, and 3) do not learn by observing how other
assessment process that analyzes what we say and people function.49-50 It is unreasonable to expect that
do (it allows us, for example, to think about what these students will fine-tune their performance by
we’re doing) and distinguishes correct from incor- simply observing clinical teachers as they interact

January 2002 ■ Journal of Dental Education 49


with patients. However, there is evidence that con- sual, spatial, and tactile perception.52-55 Although
ducting debriefings with these students in which their more research is needed, a person’s unique way of
decisions are carefully analyzed and compared to an processing sensory stimuli may also influence his or
ideal will help them develop metacognitive skills.40,44 her creativity, for example, the ability to think out-
Does the Student Have an Undiagnosed side the box and see ways of doing things other people
Learning Disability? Students in demanding pro- do not see. A person’s unique style of cognitive func-
fessional training programs are unlikely to have an tioning also appears to influence the amount of guid-
undiagnosed learning disability, but it should not be ance and structure desired when learning.
automatically ruled out. Often, students can cope with Much of the information-processing research
a learning disability at lower academic levels, only is based on the field dependent-field independent
to fall apart upon entry into a professional program construct of cognitive function.52,56 This model was
that requires extensive reading or careful attention proposed at the time of the Korean War (1950-53) to
to written instructions in laboratory manuals. Clas- explain why some experienced World War II fighter
sic signs of a potential learning disability among late pilots could maneuver the new and faster jet fighters
adolescents and adults include: 1) difficulty reading and why others who had been successful in slower,
aloud, 2) difficulty comprehending the meaning of propeller-driven aircraft had difficulty in jets. In fact,
written material, 3) difficulty completing written tests there were so many crashes in the early phases of
on time, 4) failure to follow written instructions, 5) training that the military sought the help of cogni-
difficulty comprehending diagrams and flow charts, tive psychologists to determine why skilled pilots
and 6) difficulty expressing thoughts and ideas in could not safely fly the new generation of jet planes.
written form.51 None of these problems, alone, is di- A series of cognitive tests was developed that in-
agnostic for a learning disorder, but it should raise volved such tasks as tracing images when they can
your index of suspicion if it occurs in combination be observed only by an inverted image in a mirror,
with other signs of substandard performance. Nota- stacking objects of various sizes and shapes, discern-
bly, there is a trend in the research literature—which ing geometric figures embedded within a larger and
is not conclusive but still frequently reported—of an more complex image, and determining body posi-
association between a certain style of cognitive pro- tion in space when subjects are physically rotated
cessing (reviewed in the next section of this paper) through various positions in a darkened room in
and a learning disability, particularly in curricula which they can see only a rod within a square frame.
emphasizing perceptual and motor skills.39 This battery of brain function tests identified
If you have concerns about a student, alert the two distinct ways in which the pilots processed in-
academic dean or program director by phone or in formation received from their senses. One group was
person. Teachers should not communicate a guess or determined to be field dependent (FD) because they
hunch about a student’s possible learning disability relied heavily on their external environment (instru-
in a memo or email message that can become a pub- ments, geographic landmarks, and the horizon) to
lic record or verbally in a public forum. Your respon- make maneuvering decisions. When flying their air-
sibility is to alert the program director and allow the craft at supersonic speeds (roughly twice as fast as
university to follow up with a professional assess- the earlier propeller-driven planes), FD pilots did not
ment. have time to look around and locate the horizon, find
Does the Student Have Difficulties with Per- geographic landmarks, or take more than a quick
ceptual Tasks or Motor Skills? Research by psy- glance at their instruments. Without these external
chologists and neurophysiologists over the past fifty cues, many FD pilots could not determine if they
years demonstrates that human beings mentally pro- were right side up or upside down, or whether their
cess information and use visual, tactile, and audi- aircraft was gaining altitude or diving toward the
tory cues from their environment in dramatically dif- earth. The other group of pilots was determined to
ferent ways. These different types of be field independent (FI) because they had their own
information-processing apparently influence how internal sense of direction and body position and thus
individuals attempt to structure the environment in did not rely extensively on external cues to make
which they work or learn, how much they trust their decisions. FI pilots could make the transition from
own judgment versus the opinions of others, and how the propeller-driven plane to the jet fighter because
well they can perform certain tasks that require vi- they did not need constant visual contact with the

50 Journal of Dental Education ■ Volume 66, No. 1


horizon or the landscape below and did not rely cific advice about how to improve performance; and
heavily on instrument readings to make decisions. 10) students have ample opportunities to refine their
There have been over 500 investigations of field performance.33,67-70 Research on the learning of pro-
dependence-independence (FDI) and how it influ- cedural skills indicates that students who struggle
ences personality variables (including mental health with tasks requiring eye-to-hand coordination and
pathologies), academic performance, and athletic and perceptual accuracy (such as judging depth, width,
musical tasks, as well as FDI’s relationship to physi- angle, and separation distance) benefit from a pre-
ological mechanisms such as motion sickness. These cise application of these steps.33
studies indicate that our unique way of processing The three most significant factors in the learn-
information from the surrounding environment in- ing of procedural tasks are the quality of the initial
fluences how we prefer to learn, how adept we are at instructor demonstrations, the quality and prompt-
certain fine motor visual-spatial-perceptual tasks, and ness of feedback, and the total amount of time the
how we interact with others in school or work envi- student devotes to learning the task. Instructors who
ronments. Tinajero et al. provide a comprehensive employ the teaching steps identified above and who
review of research based on the field independence supplement demonstrations with illustrations, even
and field dependence construct.56 hand-drawn sketches, and three-dimensional mod-
Students who have difficulty learning psycho- els produce better learning than instructors who do
motor tasks (such as administering an intravenous not.70-73 Prompt feedback and access to information
injection) that require eye-to-hand coordination and about the results of a task are the techniques most
ability to interpret tactile and visual stimuli may not likely to improve the performance of learners, par-
be deficient because they have not studied or prac- ticularly those who struggle with the skill.58,70,74
ticed. It may have something to do with the way they Supplemental practice on a task after criterion levels
are “seeing and feeling” the object or, in fact, not are achieved enhances long-term retention. Fifty per-
seeing and feeling it. Notably, several studies have cent overlearning is appropriate, but overlearning
found that students with sub-par performance in reaches a point of diminishing return. If it takes a
motor skills training tend to have a field dependent dental student four attempts, for example, to master
orientation.57 As demonstrated in research pertinent a particular type of restorative procedure, two more
to dental education, primarily conducted by Feil, practice trials will enhance skill retention. However,
Guenzel, and Knight,58-66 as well as other investiga- additional trials may not improve performance.72,75-76
tions of psychomotor learning, all students and par-
ticularly those who struggle with motor skills ben-
efit from a well-organized instructional process that
Additional Factors That May Cause
has the following characteristics introduced in the Inadequate Performance
following sequence: 1) students can clearly see and
In addition to cognition issues, a number of
analyze the desired end product; 2) students practice
additional factors can contribute to substandard stu-
visualizing and/or drawing the desired end product;
dent learning. When instructors are trying to pinpoint
3) students have opportunities to compare and con-
the reasons students are struggling, they should there-
trast the desired outcome to examples of outcomes
fore ask themselves the following questions related
that are not acceptable; 4) students can observe the
to study habits, educational experience, distractions,
performance of the task by an expert practitioner who
the student’s self-concept, and possible underlying
explains movements and procedures as they are ex-
medical problems.
ecuted; 5) during time-outs in the instructor’s dem-
Does the Student Have Effective Study Hab-
onstration, students can ask questions and request
its? It is unlikely that a large number of students will
repeat demonstrations of components of the task; 6)
reach an advanced level of professional training with
students are actively coached during several initial
poor study habits. However, we routinely encounter
attempts to perform the task; 7) students receive
students who have sufficient native intelligence to
prompt comparison of how their work corresponds
get by with ineffective study methods for much of
to the ideal outcome; 8) students are asked to ana-
their academic career, but who struggle when they
lyze their work products and identify reasons for dis-
run into conceptually difficult material. Figure 4 re-
crepancies from the ideal; 9) instructors provide spe-
views the characteristic study habits of high-achiev-

January 2002 ■ Journal of Dental Education 51


Figure 4. Characteristic study habits of high-achieving and underachieving students

ing and underachieving students. It is important to sionary activities like talking to friends on the phone
note that this figure presents general characteristics. or taking a break to watch television. High-achiev-
Students who are high achievers may not exactly ing students are more likely to use active learning
match the profile, and students who are underachiev- strategies such as writing detailed notes in class, re-
ers may employ some of the desirable study meth- viewing class notes daily, interacting frequently with
ods. However, a large body of research supports the instructors, and self-quizzing. In contrast, under-
conclusion that the study methods of high-achieving achieving students tend to approach their studies in
and underachieving university students are often a more passive manner. Finally, high-achieving stu-
strikingly different.20,22-23,77-79 dents more often than not study in a quiet location,
High-achieving students, for example, tend to whereas students who are underachievers may pre-
be persistent and schedule a specific time block ev- fer background sound from a radio or CD player and
ery day for reading and review. Underachievers rarely often study in high-traffic areas where they are likely
block out time for study and are easily distracted, to encounter people who might stop and talk.
often succumbing to social opportunities or diver-

52 Journal of Dental Education ■ Volume 66, No. 1


Are You Providing a Quality Educational consider the research support for these strategies to
Experience? As the saying goes, “We have met the be extremely strong: each item has been found con-
enemy, and he is us.” This is a hard pill for teachers sistently effective over decades of research in numer-
to swallow, but the sad truth is that substandard stu- ous educational settings with many different kinds
dent performance is frequently associated with a sub- of learners. As a review of educational quality in your
standard educational experience. When confronted teaching program, readers are encouraged to check
with a consistent pattern of inadequate performance [ ] those methods you use frequently when working
by more than a few students, the educational envi- with students and mark methods with an “X” that
ronment needs to be assessed. There are well-recog- are frequently employed by other instructors in your
nized ingredients for good practice in higher educa- teaching program.
tion programs,80-82 and the characteristics of quality Is the Student Unusually Distracted? Stu-
teaching in the patient care environment of health dents who are present in body but not in spirit or
care facilities have been identified in several litera- who seem easily distracted from routine tasks often
ture reviews.83-88 have so much going on in their nonacademic lives
Although countless questions can be asked to that they cannot fully attend to the school compo-
assess the learning environment, the authors have nent. Key symptoms of dysfunctional nonacademic
found the following items to be useful. Several “no” distraction include forgetfulness, excessive amount
responses may be a red flag for a potentially sub- of time on the phone or away from the clinical facil-
standard learning environment. ity during work hours, sudden requests for time off,
For all types of instruction: moodiness, concentration errors, unproductive time
• Do teachers communicate clear learning objectives (daydreaming), dramatic changes in demeanor, sud-
to the student? den decline in performance, and unexplained tardi-
• Do students know how they will be evaluated and ness or absences.
are the evaluation methods suitable to measure stu- Faculty who sense such distractions should ask
dent attainment of course/rotation objectives? themselves two questions. First, what do I know about
• Is coursework well organized and presented in a student’s personal/domestic situation? Many variables
stimulating manner that includes variety, fun, op- can become time- and energy-consuming distractions
portunities for frequent student interaction, includ- for the student, diverting attention away from the
ing peer teaching, and frequent student-teacher academic program. Financial concerns, domestic
contact both inside and outside of class? conflict with a spouse, outside employment, or the
• Is coursework structured so that students have health of a spouse, children, or parents can become
opportunities to promptly use new information and major sources of distraction. Students tend to be
employ active learning strategies such as analysis close-mouthed about these issues, and faculty often
of cases or research projects? do not think of the students’ “other life.” Exploring
For clinical rotations and preceptorships: nonacademic issues can be tricky and should be ap-
• Do students routinely receive coaching and sug- proached with caution. However, if the student’s level
gestions before they encounter patients, or are stu- of distraction is such that his or her performance is
dents basically left to their own devices? unsatisfactory (especially if it has declined dramati-
• Are students routinely observed while they inter- cally) or, in clinical education, if the overall func-
act with patients? tioning of the facility is affected, then it is your re-
• Are instructors enthusiastic, available, and ap- sponsibility to share your concerns with the student.
proachable, so that students are not afraid to re- The second question to ask is: is the student
quest help or ask questions? maintaining a lifestyle conducive to learning? Stu-
• Do instructors routinely give constructive feedback dent readiness to learn can be dramatically influenced
to students and reserve time in their schedules to by lifestyle variables including diet, sleep (or lack
meet with students to review progress? thereof), caffeine consumption, alcohol consumption,
• Do students have ample opportunity to observe and level of physical activity. A number of studies
clinicians’ providing patient care and to ask ques- have documented an increase in drug use among
tions about techniques the students observed? postsecondary students over the past decade includ-
ing several studies of health professions students.89-
Figure 5 presents eleven teaching strategies that 92
The most frequently consumed drug in all studies
teachers can use to enhance learning. The authors

January 2002 ■ Journal of Dental Education 53


Educational Quality Assessment:

Figure 5. Methods and activities that teachers can use to enhance learning

54 Journal of Dental Education ■ Volume 66, No. 1


is alcohol, with more than 50 percent of students re- competence as they progress through the curriculum
porting at least weekly alcohol consumption and 10- and through the stages of professional develop-
20 percent of students reporting daily alcohol con- ment.10,95 A student’s self-concept as a learner evolves
sumption. Alcohol consumption has been associated dramatically through predictable phases as they grow
with inefficient study habits, poor academic perfor- from novice status when they enter professional
mance, and an increase in the number of physical school to a competent entry-level professional at the
assaults by students.90 Unfortunately, the academic time of graduation.
rigors of dental school leave little time for physical The self-concept continuum depicted in Fig-
exercise, so many students drift into a couch potato ure 6 illustrates how students see themselves at vari-
routine in their limited free time. Declining physical ous stages of the competency development process.
condition, a fatty, fast-food diet, associated weight Students matriculate as unconscious incompetents
gain, and lack of sleep are ingredients for low en- who are excited to be entering professional training,
ergy and lethargic behavior. optimistic about their performance, but naive about
What Is the Student’s Self-Concept and How the rigors of the training and unaware of their limita-
Does This Affect Student-Teacher Interactions? tions. In other words, they don’t know what they don’t
The amygdala plays a crucial role in the creation of know. The unconscious incompetent stage is the natu-
memories associated witih emotional arousal. Not ral entry point of competency development and is
surprisingly, heightened emotional arousal has been different from the unskilled and unaware student dis-
associated with increased learning.11,33 As a conse- cussed earlier. The unskilled and unaware student
quence of this research and the recognition that stu- lacks the ability to self-monitor, which prevents the
dents’ attitudes play a profound role in how they ap- student from recognizing substandard performance
proach learning tasks, measurement of the emotional and making corrections.
sophistication of college and professional students At some point in the first year of the curricu-
has become a hot topic in recent years.93 lum, the learner’s self-concept shifts dramatically to
A number of instruments have been developed conscious incompetent, which is the most character-
to assess emotional intelligence, which is defined as istic stage of the novice learner. Students are now
the ability to monitor one’s own emotions and the acutely aware of their limitations and realize that the
emotions of others and to use this information to road to competence will be long and arduous. Nega-
guide thinking and actions.94 It is important for health tive self-talk can dominate the novice learner’s inner
professions educators to understand that learners conversations. This produces an undue focus on per-
travel along an emotional development continuum, ceived deficiencies, which in turn stimulates efforts
which in many ways parallels the development of to hide these weaknesses from instructors, thus hin-

Figure 6. Evolution of learner self-concept during progression from novice to expert status

January 2002 ■ Journal of Dental Education 55


dering the learning process. Students at this point of studying a problem and quickly leap to a solution
the continuum are defensive, which may manifest with a high probability of success. The length of time
itself in passive-aggressive behavior (often incor- and amount of experience required to reach an un-
rectly perceived by faculty as apathy or lack of moti- conscious competent, expert status have been the
vation) or overt hostility (perceived correctly as a subject of speculation, with estimates ranging from
bad attitude). Teachers may view students in the con- ten to fifteen years.26,97
scious incompetent stage as frustrating and difficult The self-concept continuum has significant
to teach. Indeed, conscious incompetents may be se- implications for teachers. The primary communica-
cretive and attempt to minimize interaction with tion dyad in health professions education is a teacher
teachers. who is an expert and an unconscious competent, in-
At this stage, students hesitant to deviate from teracting with a student who is a conscious incom-
rules and guidelines, and are reluctant to make their petent. A review of the words and phrases below that
own decisions for fear of making errors. They have describe each of these individuals underscores the
little patience for abstractions or alternatives, desir- potential for misunderstanding and conflict when
ing instead precise directions from instructors, such unconscious competents and conscious incompetents
as “Just tell me what to do!” Instructors who under- attempt to work together. In some ways, this expert-
stand the etiology of such defensiveness can be par- novice dyad is like mixing oil and water.
ticularly helpful to the novice student with a fragile
conscious incompetent mind-set by providing encour- Unconscious competent Conscious incompetent
agement, practical suggestions consistent with the (teacher/expert) (student/novice)
student’s skill level, extensive hands-on but Quick; uses mental Hesitant
nonjudgmental assistance, and praise for accomplish- short-cuts (automaticity)
ments, even partial successes. Students in profes-
Accurate Frequent errors
sional school may spend the bulk of the curriculum
in the conscious incompetent phase. Confident Low confidence; acutely
Students slowly evolve with repetitive practice aware of limitations
and instructor coaching into conscious competents. Impatient Defensive; secretive;
Students at this level perform somewhat mechani- seeks to hide weaknesses
cally by the numbers because they must think care-
fully about everything they do (thus the conscious Because of their quickness and accuracy and
competent label). During the conscious competent the confidence that results from frequent success,
phase, students drop their defensive shields and ac- experts tend to expect everyone to perform at a simi-
tively seek assistance from teachers. They worry less lar high level of proficiency and become impatient
about what others think of their performance, focus- with a student’s less-developed skills. A student’s slow
ing instead on the quality of the work they perform and hesitant approach to a task, frequent errors, and
for the patient. Work performance is not as fluid and defensiveness may lead the teacher to conclude that
effortless as the expert practitioner, but the conscious the student is dim-witted or hopelessly incompetent.
competent can, with adequate time and limited dis- In reality, the student may be demonstrating an ap-
tractions, perform the tasks expected of the unsuper- propriate level of skill for his or her training level.
vised entry-level practitioner. However, many teachers lose sight of what is level-
Expert practitioners are known as unconscious appropriate performance and focus on the student’s
competents because they routinely perform the tasks deficiencies. This further reinforces the student’s
of their profession at a high level of quality and effi- conscious incompetent self-concept, fostering more
ciency without expending a great deal of brainpower. defensiveness by the student and more frustration by
Over the years, expert practitioners have hardwired the teacher and sending the relationship into a down-
needed knowledge and skills into their brains and, ward spiral.
consequently, do not overtly think about each step in The P-E-T model presented in Figure 7 (Prime,
a task, a level of intellectual development called au- Partition and Praise, Empathy, Teach) was developed
tomaticity by cognitive psychologists.96 Unconscious by the authors for use in faculty development work-
competents can take mental short-cuts because they shops. It is intended to remind teachers of strategies
can automatically recognize important cues when that can be employed to reduce the prototypical de-

56 Journal of Dental Education ■ Volume 66, No. 1


fensive posture of the conscious incompetent learner. sential early in the learning curve and sends a signal
Strategies included in P-E-T are derived from com- that you are not a malignant instructor on the prowl
prehensive reviews of the literature on clinical teach- for opportunities to criticize errors. Getting down off
ing, supervision in the health care professions, and the pedestal of perfection and sharing examples of
Baron’s review of strategies to minimize defensive- problems you encountered when you were a student
ness.84,87,98-100 These strategies are likely to benefit all is an effective door-opener for student-teacher com-
students, but we believe they are particularly useful munication. Make it clear to the student that you rec-
for students who are struggling. ognize that professional training is a developmental
The components of the model are as follows. continuum and that mistakes will occur, especially
Before students undertake a task, prime (prepare) early in the learning curve. As discussed previously
them by reminding them of key elements of the task (Pangaro’s R-I-M-E Model), an on-target assessment
and alerting them to possible problems. Ask students of the student’s level of training is critical to suc-
how they will deal with problems that may arise. cessful teaching in the clinical environment.
Novice learners often are more successful if an en- Finally, the “T” in P-E-T stands for “teach.”
tire task is subdivided into manageable components; Amazingly, the most common criticism we hear from
thus, it is advisable to allow students to build from dental students about their clinical education is that
success by assigning manageable tasks early in the many instructors only give suggestions after the pro-
rotation and then expand the scope of responsibili- cedure is completed or wait until the student’s work
ties. Praise for successful completion of tasks is es- has gone so far off course that instructor interven-

Figure 7. The P–E–T model (Prime, Partition & Praise, Empathy, Teach) for helping conscious incompetent students

January 2002 ■ Journal of Dental Education 57


tion is required. Reviews of clinical teaching effec- slow or hopeless, give up on the student, or grow
tiveness consistently indicate that the teachers per- frustrated from failed efforts to bring the student up
ceived to be the most helpful by learners take a pro- to speed. Well-intentioned efforts to rehabilitate a
active coaching role before, during, and after patient struggling student by providing more repetitions or
appointments using demonstrations, reminders, re- more time in a clinical environment may fail because
hearsals, prompting questions, and nonjudgmental the specific cause of the subpar performance was
feedback to guide the student’s learning.84,87,88 These not clearly identified.
exemplary teachers primarily focus on helping stu- Simply “throwing more education at the stu-
dents develop skill and learn how to correct mistakes, dent” may not be the answer. Rather, six potential
rather than evaluating performance. causes of inadequate student performance can serve
Is There an Underlying Medical Problem as a diagnostic framework to help teachers more
That Should Be Considered? Lee Robertson’s ar- clearly pinpoint why a student is struggling academi-
ticle in this issue, “Memory and the Brain,” provides cally: 1) cognitive factors including poorly integrated,
an excellent review of the complex cellular and mo- compartmentalized information, poor metacognition
lecular mechanisms that control memory formation. which hinders the student’s ability to accurately moni-
Abnormal brain chemistry, induced by high levels tor and self-correct performance, learning disabili-
of stress, hormonal imbalances, thyroid dysfunction, ties which require professional referral, and sensory-
other systemic diseases such as hypertension, and perceptual difficulties which may hinder performance
insufficient sleep, can contribute to attention disor- in certain health care disciplines; 2) ineffective study
ders, concentration difficulties, and affective (emo- habits which, from our experience, are more com-
tional) abnormalities that may impair the brain’s ca- mon among professional students than most faculty
pacity to create memories and thus interfere with a realize; 3) an inadequate educational experience in-
person’s capacity for learning. For example, stress cluding substandard curriculum quality (unclear ob-
appears to have a particularly adverse effect on the jectives, poorly organized instruction, lack of oppor-
hippocampus, which is centrally involved in memory tunity to practice problem-solving, absence of
formation.101 Loss of sleep, thyroid dysfunction, and coaching and feedback) or a punitive environment
alcohol can also impair normal hippocampal func- in which students are hesitant to approach instruc-
tion. The scope of abnormalities at the cellular and tors for assistance; 4) a high level of student distrac-
molecular level as well as dysfunctions in the brain tion due to nonacademic issues including social re-
structures involved in memory creation are too com- lationships, employment obligations, and worry over
plex to address here, but are mentioned because health and financial issues; 5) student defensiveness
teachers should include chemical and physiological and hesitancy to interact with faculty during the con-
abnormalities in their index of suspicion when other scious incompetent stage of learning; and 6) under-
causes of substandard learning have been ruled out. lying, and perhaps undetected, medical etiologies
If you have well-founded reasons to be concerned which may affect student attentiveness, concentra-
about a student’s health, contact the academic dean tion, and emotional balance.
at your school by telephone and share your observa-
tions. Again, do not communicate unconfirmed
hunches about student performance or behavior in a REFERENCES
memo or email. 1. Norman GR, Schmidt HG. The psychological basis of
problem-based learning: a review of the evidence. Acad
Med 1992;67:557-65.
2. Harrier RJ, Siegel BV, et al. Regional glucose metabolic
Summary: Potential Causes changes after learning a complex visual-spatial/motor
task: a positron emission tomographic study. Brain Re-
of Learning Deficiency 3.
search 1992;570:134-43.
Cabezza R, Nyberg L. Imaging cognition II: an empiri-
When a student inexplicably struggles with cal review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J Cognitive
Neuroscience 2000;12:1-47.
routine assignments and falls behind classmates, busy 4. McGaugh JL. Memory: a century of consolidation. Sci-
teachers who are juggling patient care, educational, ence 2000;287:248-51.
research, and even administrative responsibilities 5. Rolls ET. Memory systems in the brain. Annu Rev
might be tempted to either pigeonhole the student as Psychol 2000;1:599-630.

58 Journal of Dental Education ■ Volume 66, No. 1


6. Eichenbaum H, Cahill LF, Gluck MA, et al. Learning 28. Hendricson WD, Cohen P. Oral health care in the 21st
and memory: systems analysis. In: Zigmond MJ, Bloom century: implications for dental and medical education.
FE, Landis SC, Roberts JL, Squire LR, eds. Fundamen- Acad Med 2001;76(12):In press.
tal neuroscience. San Diego: Academic Press, 1999. 29. Institute of Medicine, Division of Health Care Services.
7. Nyberg L, Persson J, Habib R, Tulving E, et al. Large To err is human: building a safer health system. Quality
scale neurocognitive networks underlying episodic of health care in America. Washington, DC: National
memory. J Cognitive Neuroscience 2000;12:163-73. Academy of Science, 2000.
8. Horton DL, Mills CB. Human learning and memory. Ann 30. Katona G. Organizing and memorizing. New York: Co-
Rev Psychol 1984;35:361-94. lumbia University Press, 1940.
9. Regehr G, Norman GR. Issues in cognitive psychology: 31. Ross BH, Kennedy PT. Generalizing from the use of
implications for professional education. Acad Med earlier examples in problem solving. J Exp Psychol Learn
1996;71:988-1001. Mem Cognit 1990;16:42-55.
10. Hendricson WD, Kleffner JH. Curricular and instruc- 32. Kurfiss JG. Critical thinking: theory, research, practice
tional implications of competency-based dental educa- and possibilities. ASHE ERIC Higher Education Report
tion. J Dent Educ 1998;62:183-96. Number 2. Washington, DC: Association for the Study
11. Bransford JD, Brown AL, Cocking RR, eds. How people of Higher Education, 1988.
learn: brain, mind, experience and school. Washington, 33. Druckman D, Bjork AR, eds. In the mind’s eye: enhanc-
DC: National Academy Press, 1999. ing human performance. Washington, DC: National
12. Whitman N. Review of constructivism: understanding and Academy Press, 1991.
using a relatively new theory. Fam Med 1993;25:517-21. 34. Gall MD. The use of questions in teaching. Rev Educ
13. Anderson J. Cognitive psychology and its implications. Res 1970;40:707-21.
New York: Springer-Verlag, 1985. 35. Redfield DL, Rousseau EW. A meta-analysis of experi-
14. Bordage G, Lemieux M. Semantic structures and diag- mental research on teacher questioning behavior. Rev
nostic thinking of novices and experts. Acad Med Educ Res 1981;51:237-45.
1991;66:S70-S72. 36. Connell KJ, Bordage G, Chang RW, et al. Measuring the
15. Caine RN. Making connections: teaching and the hu- promotion of thinking during precepting encounters in
man brain. Menlo Park: Addison Wesley, 1991. outpatient settings. Acad Med 1999;74(suppl):S10-S12.
16. Bordage G. Elaborated knowledge: a key to diagnostic 37. Cohen NJ, Ryan J, Hunt C, Romine L, et al. Hippocam-
thinking. Acad Med 1994;69:883-5. pal system and declarative (relational) memory: sum-
17. Jensen E. Teaching with the brain in mind. Alexandria, marizing the data from functional neuroimaging studies.
VA: ASCD Press, 1998. Hippocampus 1999;9:83-98.
18. Voytovich AE, Rippey RM, Suffredini A. Premature con- 38. Martin SJ, Grimwood PD, Morris RG. Synaptic plastic-
clusions in diagnostic reasoning. J Med Educ ity and memory: an evaluation of the hypothesis. Annu
1985;60:302-7. Rev Neurosci 2000;23:649-711.
19. Hendricson WD, Berlocher WC, Herbert RJ. A four-year 39. Blackman S, Goldstein KM. Cognitive styles and learn-
longitudinal study of dental student learning styles. J ing disabilities. J Learn Disabilities 1982;15:106-15.
Dent Educ 1987;51:175-81. 40. McPherson SL, Thomas JR. Relation of knowledge and
20. Brown G. Studies of student learning: implications for performance in boys’ tennis: age and expertise. J Exp
medical teaching. Med Teacher 1983;5:52-6. Child Psych 1989;48:190-211.
21. Coles C. Differences between conventional and prob- 41. Pangaro L. A new vocabulary and other innovations for
lem-based curricula in their students’ approaches to improving descriptive in-training evaluations. Acad Med
studying. Med Educ 1985;19:308-9. 1999;74:1203-7.
22. Newble DI, Clarke RM. The approaches to learning of 42. Moreland R, Miller J, Laucka F. Academic achievement
students in a traditional and an innovative problem-based and self-evaluations of academic performance. J Educ
medical school. Med Educ 1986;20:267-73. Psych 1981;73:335-44.
23. Martenson D. Students’ approaches to studying in four 43. Sinkavich FJ. Performance and meta memory: do stu-
medical schools. Med Educ 1986;20:532-4. dents know what they don’t know? Instruct Psych
24. Arnold L, Feighny KM. Students’ general learning ap- 1995;22:77-87.
proaches and performance in medical school: a longitu- 44. Kruger J, Dunning D. Unskilled and unaware of it: how
dinal study. Acad Med 1995;70:715-22. difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead
25. Ericsson KA, Kampe RT, et al. The role of deliberate to inflated self-assessments. J Personality and Social
practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychol Psych 1999;77:1121-34.
Rev 1993;100:363-406. 45. Weinstein ND, Lachendro E. Egocentrism as a source of
26. Ericsson KA. Expert performance: its structure and ac- unrealistic optimism. Personality and Social Psych Bul-
quisition. Am Psychol 1994;49:725-47. letin 1982;8:195-200.
27. Hendricson W, Cohen P. Future directions in dental 46. Darwin C. The descent of man. London: John Murray,
school curriculum, teaching and learning. In: Leader- 1871.
ship for the future: the dental school in the university. 47. Everson HT, Tobias S. The ability to estimate knowl-
Washington, DC: Center for Educational Policy and edge and performance in college: a metacognitive analy-
Research, American Association of Dental Schools, sis. Instruct Science 1998;26:65-79.
1999.

January 2002 ■ Journal of Dental Education 59


48. Maki RH, Jonas D, Kallod M. The relationship between 68. Hagman JD, Rose AM. Retention of military skills: a
comprehension and metacomprehension ability. review. Human Factors 1983;25:199-213.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1994;1:126-9. 69. Johnson P. The acquisition of skill. In: Smyth MM, Wing
49. Tesser A, Rosen S. The reluctance to transmit bad news. AM, eds. The psychology of human movement. Lon-
In: Berkowitz L, ed. Advances in experimental psychol- don: Academic Press, 1984.
ogy, vol. 8. New York: Academic Press, 1975. 70. Schmidt RA. Summary: knowledge of results of skill
50. Gilbert DT, Giesler RB, Morris KE. When comparisons acquisition—support for the guidance hypothesis. J Exp
arise. J Personality Social Psych 1995;69:227-36. Psych: Learn Mem Cognit 1989;15:352-9.
51. Welford AT. Skilled performance. Glenview, IL: Scott 71. Smith E, Goodman L. Understanding instructions: the
Foresman, 1976. role of explanatory material. Cognition and Instruction
52. Witkin HA, Goodenough DR, Oltman PK. Role of field 1984;1:359-96.
dependent and field independent cognitive styles in aca- 72. Schendel JD, Hagman JD. On sustaining procedural skills
demic evolution: a longitudinal study. J Educ Psych over a prolonged retention interval. J Applied Psychol
1977;69:197-211. 1982;67:605-10.
53. Wilson S, Suddick RP, Shay JS, Hustmyer FE. Correla- 73. Kieras DE, Boviar S. The role of mental modeling in
tions of scores on embedded figures and mirror tracing learning how to operate a devise. Cog Science
with preclinical technique grades and PMAT scores of 1984;8:255-73.
dental students. Perceptual Motor Skills 1981;53:31-5. 74. Davis DA, Thomson MA, et al. Evidence for the effec-
54. Suddick RP, Yancey JM, Devine S, Wilson S. Field de- tiveness of CME: a review of 50 randomized controlled
pendence-independence and dental students’ clinical trials. JAMA 1992;268:1111-7.
performance. J Dent Educ 1982;46:227-32. 75. Melnick MJ. Effects of overlearning on the retention of
55. Suddick RP, Yancey JM, Wilson S. Mirror-tracing and a gross motor skill. Research Quarterly 1971;42:60-9.
embedded figures tests as predictors of dental students’ 76. Loftus GR. Evaluating forgetting curves. J Exper
performance. J Dent Educ 1983;47:149-54. Psychol: Learn Mem Cognit 1985;11:397-406.
56. Tinajero C, Paramo M, Cadaveira F, Rodriguez-Holguin 77. Price GE, Dunn K, Dunn R. Learning style inventory/
S. Field dependence-independence and brain organiza- productivity environmental preference survey, 2nd ed.
tion: the confluence of two different ways of describing Lawrence: University of Kansas, 1982.
general forms of cognitive functioning? a theoretical 78. Duckwall J, Arnold L, Hayes J. Approaches to learning
review. Perceptual Motor Skills 1993;77:787-802. by undergraduate students: a longitudinal study. Research
57. Huang J, Chao L. Field dependence versus field inde- High Educ 1991;32:1-13.
pendence of students with and without learning disabili- 79. Meyer J, Dunne T. Study approaches of nursing students:
ties. Perceptual Motor Skills 2000;90:343-6. effects of an extended clinical context. Med Educ
58. Feil P, Reed T, Hart JK. Continuous knowledge of re- 1991;25:497-516.
sults and psychomotor skill acquisition. J Dent Educ 80. Chickering AW, Gamson ZF. Seven principles for good
1986;50:300-3. practice in undergraduate education. AAHE Bulletin
59. Feil P, Reed T. The effect of knowledge of the desired 1987;39:3-7.
outcome on dental motor performance. J Dent Educ 81. Sorcinelli MD. Research findings on the seven principles.
1988;52:198-201. New Directions Teach Learn 1991;47:13-25.
60. Feil P, Reed T, Hart JK. The transfer effect of leadup 82. Curry L, Wergin J, eds. Educating professionals: respond-
activities. J Dent Educ 1990;54:609-11. ing to new expectations for competence and account-
61. Feil P, Reed TR, McMillen SD, Killip JW, Purk JH. An ability. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1993.
experimental evaluation of the effectiveness and efficiency 83. Romberg E. A factor analysis of students’ ratings of clini-
of the leadup activity. J Dent Educ 1991;55:560-4. cal teaching. J Dent Educ 1984;48:258-62.
62. Feil P. An assessment of the application of psychomotor 84. Irby DM. Teaching and learning in ambulatory care set-
learning theory constructs in preclinical laboratory in- tings: a thematic review of the literature. Acad Med
struction. J Dent Educ 1992;56:176-82. 1995;70:898-931.
63. Killip JW, Feil P, McMillen S. Effect of planning the 85. Speer AJ, Elnicki DM. Assessing the quality of teach-
desired outcome on the quality of tooth preparation. J ing. Am J Med 1999;106:381-4.
Dent Educ 1993;57:804-6. 86. Wright SM, Kern DE, Kolodner K. Attributes of excellent
64. Knight GW, Guenzel PJ. Design and validation of mir- physician role models. N Eng J Med 1998;339:1986-93.
ror skills instruction. J Dent Educ 1994;58:752-61. 87. Milne D, James I. A systematic review of effective cog-
65. Feil PH, Guenzel PJ, Knight GW, Geistfeld R. Design- nitive-behavioural supervision. Br J Clin Psychol
ing preclinical instruction for psychomotor skills (I)— 2000;39(Pt 2):111-27.
theoretical foundations of motor skill performance and 88. Heidenreich C, Lye P, Simpson D, Lourich M. The search
their applications to dental education. J Dent Educ for effective and efficient ambulatory teaching methods
1994;58:806-12. through the literature. Pediatrics 2000;105:231-7.
66. Knight GW, Guenzel PJ, Feil P. Using questions to fa- 89. Smart RG, Ogbourne AC. Drinking and heavy drinking
cilitate motor skill acquisition. J Dent Educ 1997;61:56- by students in 18 countries. Drug Alcohol Dependence
65. 2000;60:315-8.
67. Fischman MG, Christina RW, Vercruyssen MJ. Reten- 90. Newbury-Birch D, White M, Kamali F. Factors influ-
tion and transfer of motor skills: a review for the practi- encing alcohol and illicit drug use among medical stu-
tioner. Quest 1982;33:181-94. dents. Drug Alcohol Dependence 2000;59:125-30.

60 Journal of Dental Education ■ Volume 66, No. 1


91. McCartan BE, Sadlier D, O’Mullane DM. Smoking hab- 96. Shiffrin R, Schneider W. Controlled and automatic hu-
its and attitudes of Irish dental students. J Irish Dent man information processing: perceptual learning, auto-
Assoc 2000;39:26-9. matic attending and general theory. Psych Rev
92. Ashton CH, Kamali F. Personality, lifestyles, alcohol and 1977;84:127-90.
drug consumption in a sample of British medical stu- 97. Hoffman RR, ed. The psychology of expertise: cogni-
dents. Med Educ 1995;29:187-92. tive research and empirical AI. New York: Springer-
93. Salovey P, Mayer JD, Goldman SL, Turvey D, et al. Emo- Verlag, 1991.
tional attention, clarity and repair: exploring emotional 98. McGee SR, Irby DM. Teaching tips in the outpatient
intelligence using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. In: clinic. J Gen Internal Med 1997;12(April,Suppl 2):S34-
Pennebaker JW, ed. Emotion, disclosure, and health. S40.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 99. Lesky LG, et al. Strategies to improve teaching in the
1995. ambulatory medicine setting. Arch Inter Med
94. Elam C, Stratton TD, Andrykowski MA. Measuring the 1990;150:2133-7.
emotional intelligence of medical school matriculants. 100. Baron RA. Countering the effects of destructive criti-
Acad Med 2001;5:507-8. cism: the relative efficacy of four interventions. J Ap-
95. Schwenk TL, Whitman N, eds. Residents as teachers: a plied Psychol 1990;75:235-45.
guide to educational practice. Salt Lake City: Univer- 101. McEwen BS. The neurobiology of stress: from seren-
sity of Utah Press, 1984. dipity to clinical relevance. Brain Res 2000;886:172-89.

January 2002 ■ Journal of Dental Education 61

You might also like