A Missing Piece? Neuropsychiatric Functioning in Untreated Patients With Tumors Within The Cerebellopontine Angle

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Neuro-Oncology (2018) 140:145–153

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11060-018-2944-z

CLINICAL STUDY

A missing piece? Neuropsychiatric functioning in untreated patients


with tumors within the cerebellopontine angle
Simone Goebel1 · Hubertus Maximilian Mehdorn2

Received: 27 May 2018 / Accepted: 27 June 2018 / Published online: 7 July 2018
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
Purpose To date, little is known about neuropsychiatric symptoms in patients with tumors within the cerebellopontine angle
(CPA). These, however, might be of clinical relevance. Aim of this study was thus to assess possible impairment in cognition,
elevation in mood symptoms, and fatigue in this specific patient group.
Methods Forty-five patients with an untreated CPA tumor (27 vestibularis schwannoma, 18 meningioma) were tested within
a cross-sectional observational study in a single institution prior to neurosurgical treatment. Patients were administered a
multifaceted battery comprising of widely-used tests for assessment of neuropsychiatric functioning.
Results The majority of the included patients (69%) showed neurocognitive impairment, most frequently in the areas of
attention and visuo-motor speed (e.g., alertness) (62%) as well as visuo-construction (44%). Impaired structural integrity of
the brain stem was accompanied by more serious neurocognitive deficits. About one-third of the sample reported clinically
relevant depression and/or anxiety and an even higher proportion (48%) described high levels of fatigue. Cognitive and
affective symptoms as well as fatigue contributed significantly to patients’ Quality of Life, indicating the clinical relevance
of neuropsychiatric symptoms in patients with CPA tumors.
Conclusions Although patients with untreated CPA tumors often suffer from devastating and prominent physical symptoms,
neuropsychiatric problems are also frequent. Including these aspects in the routine clinical assessment and initiating treat-
ment accordingly might thus improve clinical management of the patients and improve Quality of Life.

Keywords Tumor · Cerebellopontine angle · Neuropsychology · Cognition · Affect · HRQoL

Introduction patients’ Health Related Quality of Life (HRQoL), psycho-


social well-being, and participation [2–7]. Most CPA tumors
Tumors within the cerebellopontine angle (CPA) account can be classified as chronic illness due to their benign nature
for 5–10% of intracranial tumors [1]. CPA tumors are often and early impact on functioning [6]. Treatment options
accompanied by severe clinical symptoms due to their spe- include watchful waiting, radiosurgery, and neurosurgical
cific location close to vital anatomical structures, i.e., the treatment, each with possible detrimental effects [7–9].
cranial nerves and/or the brainstem. These include, amongst It might be due to these prominent and dominant neuro-
others, impaired facial nerve function, hearing loss, tinnitus, logical symptoms and neurosurgical challenges that there
vertigo, balance or visual problems, fatigue, and headache. is only little research regarding possible neuropsychiat-
These symptoms are of high clinical relevance and impede ric sequelae of CPA tumors. For the whole population of
patients with an intracranial neoplasm, it is known that about
* Simone Goebel 90% of untreated patients suffer from cognitive deficits [10]
goebel@psychologie.uni‑kiel.de and prevalence of depression, anxiety, and psychosocial dis-
Hubertus Maximilian Mehdorn tress is high [11, 12]. Also, many neurooncological patients
prof@mehdorn‑consil.de suffer from fatigue [13]. These aspects have, however, to
date not been assessed in patients with a CPA tumor.
1
Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Hio et al. [14] reported rates of depression in 27% and
Institute of Psychology, Christian-Albrechts University,
Olshausenstraße 62, 24118 Kiel, Germany high anxiety in 20% in their sample of 30 patients with an
2 acoustic neuroma prior to treatment, giving an indication
Mehdorn Consilium, Prüner Gang 7, 24103 Kiel, Germany

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
146 Journal of Neuro-Oncology (2018) 140:145–153

of the possible relevance of neuropsychiatric symptoms in 1. What is the proportion of cognitive impairment, elevated
patients with a CPA tumor. Traditionally, neurocognitive mood symptoms and fatigue in patients with a CPA
decline has been attributed to infratentorial lesions [15]. tumor?
However, in recent years, it has been consistently reported 2. Of which nature are possible deficits, i.e., which
that supratentorial lesions are frequently accompanied by aspects of cognition and affect are especially at risk in
neuropsychiatric symptoms as well. For example, patients this patient group? This helps to characterize patients’
with cerebellar lesions are often impaired in cognitive and impairments, better understand the patients’ clinical
affective functioning, suffering, amongst others, from defi- situation, apply specific and focused test procedures
cits in the areas of executive functioning, attention, spa- relevant to this specific patient group, and plan targeted
tial cognition, and emotion regulation [16]. In addition, rehabilitative treatment.
impaired structural and functional integrity of the brain 3. Are there specific tumor attributes (e.g., tumor size or
stem has been linked to the decline in various cognitive histological features) associated with neuropsychiatric
domains including attention and executive functioning [17, symptoms? This can help to identify patients of special
18]. Moreover, Kim et al. [19] reported cognitive deficits risk to develop symptoms in the assessed areas.
in patients with infratentorial stroke, which was associated 4. What is the clinical relevance of these deficits? To test
with white matter hyperintensities, and Meskal et al. [20] this, we assessed the impact of cognition, affect, and
included seven patients with an infratentorial meningioma fatigue on Health Related Quality of Life (HRQoL).
in their study who showed similar neurocognitive impair-
ment compared to patients with supratentorial lesions.
Thus, it is plausible to expect neuropsychiatric impairment Materials and methods
also in patients with CPA tumors.
Considering neurocognitive impairment and elevations Study design
in moods symptoms is clinically recommended:
All these three aspects—cognitive deficits, elevated This cross-sectional observational study was conducted
mood symptoms and fatigue—relevantly impede HRQoL between January 2009 and December 2010. We tested a
in brain tumor patients [13]. Also, patients’ neuropsycho- consecutive sample of patients scheduled for neurosurgical
logical status gives important information with regard to tumor removal who fulfilled the following inclusion criteria:
patients’ participation and might indicate necessary treat- diagnosis of an untreated CPA tumor, age 18 years or above,
ment [21]. Neuropsychological impairment is robustly approval of the ward physician, patients’ consent. The final
associated with real-life change, including, amongst oth- sample (Table 1), consisted of patients with either a menin-
ers, the impairment in activities of daily living (ADL), gioma or a vestibularis schwannoma. Surgery was performed
Employability, the ability to drive, or social integration. 1–4 days after the examination (Mx = 1.64, Sx = .88). The
This prediction of everyday functioning by neuropsy- study was approved by the appropriate ethics committee and
chological assessment data is referred to as ‘ecological patients gave written, informed consent. This study was not
validity’ [21]. Moreover, neuropsychological change may part of another study.
indicate tumor progression before visible change on CT
or MRI, stressing the relevance of an adequate baseline Variables
assessment [22]. In addition, the patients’ cognitive abil-
ity is an important part of evaluation and quality control Patients participated in a comprehensive neuropsychological
regarding the success and the appropriateness of medical examination consisting of standardised tests with published
treatments [23]. normative data. Sociodemographic (age, gender, estimated
Despite this clinical relevance, no previous study has, premorbid intelligence, employment status) and medical
to the best of our knowledge, focused on cognitive and information (Karnofsky Performance Status, tumor size,
affective symptoms as well as fatigue in patients with CPA lateralization, histological diagnosis and whether there was
tumors to date. Thus, aim of this study was the assessment visible brain stem suppression on MRI) was drawn from
of the cognitive and affective state as well as of fatigue in patients’ charts (Table 1).
untreated patients with a CPA tumor in order to analyse
whether including these aspects in the routine diagnosis Neurocognitive variables
and treatment might help to improve clinical management
of the patients. More specifically, the following research Following the recommendations from Taphoorn and Klein
questions were addressed: [25], we included the following cognitive domains:

13
Journal of Neuro-Oncology (2018) 140:145–153 147

Table 1  Sample description (n = 45) – Memory Verbal learning and memory Rey Auditory
Variable Mean SD Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) [35]; Implicit visual-
spatial memory RCFT—delay [27]
Age in years (range 22–71) 53.91 13.01 – Language the four following subtests of the Aachener
Estimated premorbid intelligence (range 86–125) 111.66 7.872 Aphasie Test (AAT) [36], the Gold Standard for the
Karnofsky-Performance Status (range 50–100)a 84.1 12.1 assessment of aphasia in Germany, were used: Naming,
Tumor size in cm (maximal dimension) (range 2.41 1.54 Token Test (Language comprehension), Reading, and
0.4–5.3 cm)
Writing
Variable N %

Gender Assessment of patients’ emotional state, fatigue and HRQoL


Female 26 58
Male 19 42 The following questionnaires were applied:
Employment status
Currently employed 30 67 1. For assessment of patients’ emotional state, the Hospital
Retired 12 27 Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) [37], was applied.
Housekeeper/unemployed 3 7 The HADS is one of the most widely used instruments
Tumor lateralization for assessment of anxiety and depression via seven items
Right 19 42 each in patients with physical complaints. We used the
Left 26 58 cutoff scores of ≥ 10 as indicators for clinically relevant
Histological diagnosis depression/anxiety and the sumscore of both scales as
Vestibularis schwannoma 27 60 measure for patients’ overall emotional state.
Meningioma 18 40 2. For assessment of fatigue, the short form of the Profile
Visible brain stem suppression on MRI of Mood States (POMS) [38] was applied. We used the
No 36 80 tiredness/fatigue score consisting of 7 items for opera-
Yes 9 20 tionalization of patients’ fatigue.
3. For assessment of global HRQoL, question 29 (“How
a
Global measure of the patients’ physical status, ranging from 0 would you rate your overall health during the past
(death) to 100 (no complaints; no evidence of disease) [24]
week?”) and question 30 (“How would you rate your
overall quality of life during the past week?”) from the
– Premorbid intellectual functioning was estimated via “European Organisation for Research and Treatment of
the demographic formula provided by Barona [26]. Six Cancer core Quality of Life Questionnaire” (EORTC
cognitive domains represented by the following test pro- QLQ C-30) [39] were administered. Each question is
cedures were included in the assessment: answered on a Likert scale, ranging from 1 (“very poor”)
– Visuo-construction Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure to 7 (“excellent”) recoded in 0–100%. We used the mean
Test (RCFT)—copy [27]; score of these two items as dependent variable.
– Visuo-perception Object recognition Object Decision
from the Visual Object and Space Perception Battery Study size
(VOSP) [28]; Space perception number location from
the VOSP; Visual field/neglect line bisection [29]; During the study period, 54 patients with a CPA tumor were
– Attention and visuo-motor speed Verbal short term eligible. Patients were excluded if they reported any previ-
memory digit Span Forward from the Wechsler Mem- ous brain impairment or if MRI scans showed supratento-
ory Scale—Revised (WMS-R) [30], Visuo-motor speed rial lesions (n = 2). Two patients could not be seen due to
Trail-Making-Test (TMT)-A [31]; Alertness subtest organizational reasons and five patients declined partici-
“Alertness” from the computerized test battery of the pation due to lack of physical well-being. Completing the
“Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung (TAP)”, the whole test-battery lasted for 90–120 min. Not all patients
German Gold Standard for assessment of attention [32]; were administered all tests due to patients’ exhaustion or
– Executive functions Verbal working memory Digit Span organizational reasons.
Backwards [30]; Flexibility/shifting TMT-B [31]; Verbal
Lexical Fluency German version of the Controlled Oral
Word Administration test COWAT, F-A-S) [33]; Figural Statistical analyses
Fluency (Five-point Test FPT) [34];
Patients were classified as impaired in any cognitive domain
if they scored below the 7th percentile (i.e., 1.5 standard

13
148 Journal of Neuro-Oncology (2018) 140:145–153

deviations below the respective mean). For comparability, all vomiting (24%), coordination (24%). During the structured
data were transformed to the z-scale with a mean (Mx) of 0 exploration, patients affirmed the following neuropsychiatric
and a standard deviation (SD) of 1. T-Tests were applied for changes: attention (43%), memory (38%), spatial navigation
comparing the following groups: patients with/without visible (33%), and mood changes (58%).
brain stem impairment; patients with meningioma/vestibularis
schwannoma; patients with left/right sided tumors. Pearson’s Patients’ cognitive and affective state
correlations were used for calculation of associations between
variables. Correlations were calculated between our depend- Proportion and nature of cognitive impairment, elevated
ent variables and medical characteristics. Moreover, associa- mood symptoms and fatigue
tions between HRQoL, fatigue, patients’ emotional state (total
score of the HADS) and cognitive measures were calculated. Overall, 31 (69%) of the patients scored below the 7th per-
For examining which variables impact Quality of Life in centile in at least one of the applied cognitive tests. The
CPA-patients, we conducted linear regression analyses. As most frequently impaired domains were visuo-construction
our sample size did not allow for including cognition, affect, (Fig. 1), executive functioning as well as attention and visuo-
and fatigue in one model, they were analyses separately. For motor speed (Table 2). Regarding patients’ affective state, we
analysing the impact of cognition on HRQoL, we included found high anxiety in 20% and clinically relevant depression
cognitive measures correlated with HRQoL (i.e., TMT-B in 33% of the sample. Finally, about half of the patients (48%)
and alertness) in a multiple regression. Due to our explora- reported high fatigue.
tory approach, we chose a backward stepwise approach. Fur-
thermore, two linear regression analyses were carried out in Relationships between tumor attributes
which the sum score of the HADS as indicator for patients’ and neuropsychiatric functioning
psychological state, respectively the POMS fatigue score
were analysed as possible predictors for patients’ HRQoL. Patients with visible brain stem suppression on MRI (n = 9)
Statistical analyses were performed with SPSS 21.0 with a performed worse compared to the remaining sample in a vari-
level of significance of alpha = .05. Due to the exploratory ety of cognitive tasks (Table 2). All of these nine patients suf-
nature of our study, we did not control for multiple testing. fered from cognitive impairment in at least one of the applied
measures. Neither tumor size nor the patients’ physical state
(Karnofsky Performance Status) was related to cognitive
Results performance (p > .2). No differences were found between
patients with vestibular schwannoma resp. meningioma as
Sample description well as between patients with left- resp. right-sided tumors
(p > .2). Neither affect nor fatigue were associated with any of
Table 1 presents patient information. WHO tumor grading the included medical characteristics (p > .2). Also, we did not
was WHO° I for all patients. In one patient, the tumor was find differences in affect or fatigue between groups of patients
found by incident. This patient displayed no neurological with different tumor attributes (p > .2).
symptoms. All other participants suffered from at least one of
the following problems: hearing impairment (71%), fatigue
(71%), balance (62%), tinnitus (43%), vertigo (43%), impaired
facial nerve function (38%), headache (38%), nausea and/or

Fig. 1  Original of the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (RCFT) and results of the copy task of two impaired patients

13
Journal of Neuro-Oncology (2018) 140:145–153 149

Table 2  Results of the No of patients Min Max Mean SD Impaired


neurocognitive and affective N (%)
measures
Visuo-construction 16 of 38 (44%)
RCFT-copy*** 38 − 3.30 1.00 − 1.20 1.50 16 (44%)
Visuo-perception 7 of 45 (16%)
VOSP object decision 45 − 1.56 3.30 .19 1.54 3 (7%)
VOSP number location 45 − 3.30 3.30 .33 1.66 4 (9%)
Line bisection 45 – – – – 0
Attention and speed 28 of 45 (62%)
Digit span forward 43 − 2.05 2.05 − .46 1.07 9 (21%)
TMT-A** 38 − 3.30 .84 − 1.00 1.74 17 (45%)
Alertness—reaction time* 28 − 2.33 1.88 − .68 1.37 9 (32%)
Alertness—standard deviation of 28 − 2.33 2.33 − .036 1.54 5 (18%)
reaction times**
Phasic alertness 28 -2.33 2.33 .011 1.25 3 (11%)
Executive functioning 23 of 45 (51%)
Digit span backwards 43 -2.05 1.88 − .73 − 2.05 8 (19%)
“FAS”COWAT​ 42 -2.33 1.75 .40 1.21 6 (14%)
FPT—productivity* 45 -3.30 1.65 − 1.10 1.71 18 (40%)
FPT—perseveration 45 − 3.30 1.00 − .32 1.40 6 (13%)
TMT-B** 38 − 3.30 1.08 − 1.1 2.01 17 (45%)
Memory 15 of 42 (36%)
AVLT—learning 38 − 3.30 1.65 − .89 1.22 12 (32%)
AVLT—delayed free recall 38 -2.05 1.88 − .78 1.55 9 (24%)
AVLT—recognition 38 − 1.41 1.18 − .31 .91 0%
RCFT-delay** 36 − 3.30 1.80 − .70 1.37 10 (28%)
Language 0 of 45 (0%)
AAT—token test (0–120) 44 0 1 .04 .208 0
AAT—naming (0–120) 44 118 120 119.53 .757 0
AAT—reading (0–30) 44 30 30 30.00 .000 0
AAT—writing (0–30) 44 28 30 29.91 .358 0
All cognitive tests 31 of 45 (69%)
Affective measures 22 of 45 (49%)
HADS-anxiety (0–21) 45 0 14 5.78 4.11 9 (20%)
HADS-depression (0–21) 45 0 17 6.69 5.60 15 (33%)
Fatigue 22 of 45 (49%)
POMS-fatigue (0–42) 31 0 34 18.85 10.91 15 (48%)
Global HRQoL (0–100)
EORTC-QLQ question 29 + 30/2 45 16.67 100.00 67.59 25.55 –

Cognitive test results are presented in z-scores (Mx = 0, SD = 1). Patients were classified as impaired if
scoring below the 7th percentile (z < − 1.5) respectively below the suggested cutoff score according to pub-
lished norms. Tests with greater impairment of patients with brain stem lesions visible on CT/MRT are
marked by asterisks
Patients with brain stem impairment performed worse compared to the rest of the sample with *p < .1,
**p < .05, ***p < .01

Relationships between HRQoL, fatigue, cognition, For estimation of a patients’ overall neurocognitive state,
and emotional well‑being we calculated the mean z-score from all completed neu-
ropsychological tests. This score was not correlated with
Correlation analyses patients’ global HRQoL. Regarding the individual cognitive
tests, both Alertness and TMT-B (but no other measures)
Table 3 presents details regarding the associations between were correlated with HRQoL. The sumscore of the HADS
variables. HRQoL and fatigue were highly intercorrelated. as indicator for patients’ emotional state and fatigue were
significantly correlated with HRQoL.

13
150 Journal of Neuro-Oncology (2018) 140:145–153

Table 3  Associations between HRQoL, fatigue, cognition, and emo- (69%) was impaired in at least one neuropsychological test
tional state procedure, about one-third of our sample suffered from anxi-
Fatigue Alertness TMT-B HADS ety and/or depression, and 48% described high fatigue.
Both language and visuo-perceptive abilities were spared
HRQoL − .607** .480** − .469** − .597**
in the majority of our participants. Thus, patients did not
Fatigue – − .330 .243 .602**
exhibit the typical profile of cognitive and affective symp-
Alertness – – .336 − .587**
toms after cerebellar lesions: the “cerebellar cognitive affec-
TMT-B – – – .233
tive syndrome” (CCAS) is marked by deficits in the areas of
**Significant with p < .01 visual-spatial processing, linguistic function, executive func-
tions, and affective regulation [16, 40, 41]. Thus, our results
suggest that patients with CPA tumors suffer frequently from
Prediction of HRQoL via neuropsychiatric symptoms neurocognitive deficits whilst exhibiting a distinct neuropsy-
chological profile. However, it should be noted that not all
We performed a linear regression analysis to analyse whether aspects of language functioning which rely on cerebellar
cognitive functioning predicted HRQoL with alertness and processing were included in this study (e.g., agrammatism
TMT-B as possible predictors. In the resulting model, only or dysprosodia) [16, 40, 42].
alertness was a significant predictor of HRQoL ­(R2 = .37; In addition, the ability to encode and maintain informa-
T = 3.01; p = .007). The sum score of the HADS as indica- tion was intact in all patients: Memory was only impaired
tor for the patients’ psychological state predicted 34.8% of if it did require either free recall of information (which
the variance of patients’ global HRQoL (­ R2; T = − 4.958; demands executive and attentional resources) [43] or visuo-
p = .001). Also, fatigue significantly predicted HRQoL constructive abilities.
­(R2 = .33; T = 3.233; p = .005). Regarding executive functioning—one of the most fre-
quently impaired domains in patients with lesions to the cer-
Additional analyses ebellum [16, 40–42] or brain stem [18]—we found material-
specific results. Thus, only 13–19% of the patients exhibited
Visuo‑motor cognitive abilities deficits in tasks independent of visuo-motor functioning
(e.g., verbal fluency) whereas 40–45% of the patients were
Five of the applied cognitive tests rely on visuo-motor pro- impaired in tasks requiring visuo-motor coordination (e.g.,
cessing: Rey-copy, Rey-recall, TMT-A, TMT-B, and FPT pro- figural fluency). Generally, we observed a pattern of impair-
ductivity. These five tests measure a variety of cognitive con- ment in our sample which reflected greater difficulties in test
structs and are assigned to four different cognitive domains procedures relying on visuo-motor abilities—independent
(see "Neurocognitive variables" section). However, patients of the underlying cognitive construct the tests were in fact
in our sample showed impairments in these tasks independ- designed to measure. A variety of reasons might attribute to
ent of the cognitive domain the tests were originally designed these visuo-motor difficulties: deficits in the areas of visual
to measure (Table 2). Thus, we searched for a pattern as we [41, 44], motor [41, 45], or vestibular [46, 47] functioning,
hypothesized that this variety of deficits might be attributable impaired visuo-motor coordination [42, 48], as well as the
to a common course. In accordance with this assumption, we disruption of cerebro-cerebellar circuits as a form of “dia-
conducted an exploratory factor analysis of these five tests. schisis” [49]. Future studies could include tasks for assess-
This revealed that all five consisted single factor, explain- ment of basic visuo-motor functions, e.g., fine motor skills
ing 64% of the variance (factor loadings from .700 to .891). or visual processing, to test for their influence on cognitive
All these five tests heavily rely on visuo-motor functioning. tasks in patients with CPA tumors.
Moreover, this is the only basic function linking these five The second domain in which deficits were frequently (i.e.,
tasks. Thus, we hypothesize that generally, test procedures in about one-third of our sample) observed, was attention,
demanding visuo-motor abilities were prone to be impaired. predominantly alertness and thus the general wakefulness
of an organism. Alertness is a fundamental function of the
brain stem [18, 50, 51]. In concordance with this, patients
with brain stem lesions were more severely impaired in the
Discussion area of alertness compared to the rest of the sample.
Completion of our whole test-battery took about
This was, to the best of our knowledge, the first comprehen- 90–120 min. This was not manageable for a considerable
sive assessment of cognitive and affective functioning in an proportion of patients due to exhaustion. Thus, our test-
unselected sample of patients with an untreated tumor within battery is not suited for the application in the routine neu-
the CPA. We found that the majority of participating patients ropsychiatric assessment in this patient group. Our results

13
Journal of Neuro-Oncology (2018) 140:145–153 151

can help to select and focus neuropsychological test proce- Future studies might add a healthy control group to account
dures, which likely is of special importance in more severely for this. Another factor that might have distorted our results
impaired patients. is that not all patients completed all test procedures. Patients
Patients with brain stem impairment were also more with more severe symptoms (e.g., high fatigue) possibly
severely impaired in the area of visuo-construction. In both were more likely to discontinue the assessment and thus,
the study of Garrard et al. [18] and ours, all patients with our data might underestimate neurocognitive symptomatol-
lesions of the brain stem showed neuropsychological impair- ogy. As tumour localization, not histopathology or any other
ment. Thus, our data might be supportive of the postulation clinical feature, was the tumor feature of main interest of this
of Garrad et al. [18] that neurocognitive impairment is not study, we included patients with any CPA tumor resulting
always appropriately accounted for in this patient group. in a sample composed of patients with either meningioma
Regarding the impact on HRQoL, we did not find an or acoustic neuroma. We did not find any differences in
influence of patients’ average cognitive test scores. However, cognitive or affective measures between these two groups.
both deficits in alertness and TMT-B (demanding executive However, due to the small sample size, future research
and visuo-motor functioning) were associated with reduced might reveal that it could be more appropriate to distinguish
HRQoL. Moreover, alertness relevantly and significantly between these groups due to specific cognitive or affective
predicted patients’ HRQoL. This effect might however have symptoms. Moreover, as this was the first study to assess
been moderated by brain stem impairment and thus more neuropsychiatric functioning in patients with CPA tumors,
severe physical symptoms. Due to our sample size, our data we chose an exploratory approach, including a variety of
did not allow to control for possible moderators. Future stud- measures of potential interest and not accounting for multi-
ies could assess possible moderators as well as the influence ple testing. Thus, our results can only be seen as preliminary.
of specific cognitive impairment on HRQoL and participa- On basis of our results, future studies might address more
tion in order to identify possible areas of neurocognitive focused and specific aspects of cognition (e.g., attention,
functioning with relevant influence on patients’ well-being. visuo-motor functioning). Due to the small sample size and
Hereby, the most important targets for cognitive rehabilita- hypothesis-generating approach, we refrained from carrying
tion could be identified. out a multiple regression analysis for prediction of patients’
About one-third of our sample described relevant anxiety HRQoL as this would have been severely underpowered and
and/or depression. This is comparable to the proportion of thus not been able to detect predictors of clinical relevance.
preoperative brain tumor patients in general [52]. Previous We presume that HRQoL in patients with a CPA tumor can
studies demonstrated that depression is a major predictor best be explained by complex relationships within a bio-
of HRQoL in patients with brain tumors [53]. Consistently, psycho-social model. Choosing multiple regression analysis
elevated mood symptoms were a relevant and significant in future studies with larger sample sizes will help to deepen
predictor of patients’ HRQoL in our study. This stresses the the understanding of the patients’ clinical situation. Finally,
high clinical relevance of patients’ emotional state. On basis we solely included preoperative patients and thus, our data
of our data, we therefore recommend the routine screening do not allow accounting for the effect of surgery. Future
of patients for affective disorders and psychosocial distress. studies might use neurocognitive baseline assessments for
About half of the patients described high fatigue. Both comparison of the effects of different treatment approaches
emotional and physical symptoms might contribute to this. on cognition and affect [9] in order to be able to inform
In our study, fatigue was strongly associated with Quality of patients about possible effects of different treatment modali-
Life and depression, but not with neurocognitive variables. ties [7].
Fatigue also significantly predicted patients’ HRQoL.
The following shortcomings and specifics should be con-
sidered regarding our results: All patients were recruited
from a single centre and scheduled for a specific treatment, Conclusion
i.e., neurosurgical intervention. Thus, patients were both
highly selected with regard to their tumor attributes and In summary, this was the first extensive study of neuropsy-
in a highly specific situation: shortly before neurosurgical chiatric symptoms in patients with tumors within the CPA.
tumor removal during inpatient hospital stay. This might Our results demonstrate that patients with an untreated CPA
have influenced patients’ affect ratings and thus differ from tumor frequently suffer from cognitive disorders, fatigue,
the emotional state patients usually experience in their all- and elevated mood symptoms. These symptoms affect
day life. An important shortcoming is that our patients were patients’ HRQoL and are thus of clinical relevance. Includ-
solely compared to published norms. Thus, differences ing these aspects in the routine clinical assessment and initi-
between tests might in part be attributable to differences ating treatment accordingly might improve clinical manage-
in the underling norms, thereby impeding comparability. ment of the patients and improve Quality of Life.

13
152 Journal of Neuro-Oncology (2018) 140:145–153

Acknowledgements The authors are grateful to Maren Hinck for valu- 13. Taphoorn MJB, Sizoo EM, Bottomley A (2010) Review on quality
able support during manuscript preparation. of life issues in patients with primary brain tumors. Oncologist
15(6):618–626. https​://doi.org/10.1634/theon​colog​ist.2009-0291
14. Hio S, Kitahara T, Uno A et al (2013) Psychological condition
Compliance with ethical standards in patients with an acoustic tumor. Acta Otolaryngol 133:42–46.
https​://doi.org/10.3109/00016​489.2012.70932​2
Conflict of interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of 15. Kolb B, Whishaw IQ (2015) Fundamentals of human neuropsy-
interest. chology, 7th edn. Worth Publ, New York
16. Schmahmann J (1998) The cerebellar cognitive affective syn-
Ethical approval All procedures performed in studies involving human drome. Brain 121:561–579. https ​ : //doi.org/10.1093/brain​
participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the insti- /121.4.561
tutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki 17. Mariën P, D’aes T (2015) “Brainstem cognitive affective syn-
declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. drome” following disruption of the cerebrocerebellar net-
work. Cerebellum 14:221–225. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1231​
Informed consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual 1-014-0624-x
participants included in the study. 18. Garrard P (2002) Cognitive dysfunction after isolated brain stem
insult. An underdiagnosed cause of long term morbidity. J Neu-
rol Neurosurg Psychiatry 73:191–194. https​://doi.org/10.1136/
jnnp.73.2.191
19. Kim TW, Kim Y-H, Kim KH et al (2014) White matter hyperin-
References tensities and cognitive dysfunction in patients with infratentorial
stroke. Ann Rehabil Med 38:620–627. https​://doi.org/10.5535/
1. Tos M, Charabi S, Thomsen J (1998) Clinical experience with arm.2014.38.5.620
vestibular schwannomas: epidemiology, symptomatology, diag- 20. Meskal I, Gehring K, van der Linden SD et al (2015) Cognitive
nosis, and surgical results. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol 255:1–6. improvement in meningioma patients after surgery: clinical rel-
https​://doi.org/10.1007/s0040​50050​012 evance of computerized testing. J Neurooncol 121:617–625. https​
2. Foley RW, Shirazi S, Maweni RM et al (2017) Signs and symp- ://doi.org/10.1007/s1106​0-014-1679-8
toms of acoustic neuroma at initial presentation: an exploratory 21. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, Tranel D (2012) Neuropsy-
analysis. Cureus 9:e1846. https​://doi.org/10.7759/cureu​s.1846 chological Assessment, 5th edn. University Press, Oxford
3. Carlson ML, Tveiten ØV, Driscoll CL et al (2015) What drives 22. Armstrong CL, Goldstein B, Shera D et al (2003) The predictive
quality of life in patients with sporadic vestibular schwan- value of longitudinal neuropsychologic assessment in the early
noma? Laryngoscope 125:1697–1702. https​://doi.org/10.1002/ detection of brain tumor recurrence. Cancer 97(3):649–656. https​
lary.25110​ ://doi.org/10.1002/cncr.11099​
4. McLaughlin EJ, Bigelow DC, Lee JYK et al (2015) Quality of life 23. Talacchi A, d’Avella D, Denaro L et al (2012) Cognitive outcome
in acoustic neuroma patients. Otol Neurotol 36:653–656. https​:// as part and parcel of clinical outcome in brain tumor surgery.
doi.org/10.1097/MAO.00000​00000​00067​4 J Neurooncol 108(2):327–332. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1106​
5. Bateman N, Nikolopoulos TP, Robinson K et al (2000) Impair- 0-012-0818-3
ments, disabilities, and handicaps after acoustic neuroma 24. Karnofsky DA, Burchenal JH (1949) The clinical evaluation of
surgery. Clin Otolaryngol 25:62–65. https​: //doi.org/10.104 chemotherapeutic agents in cancer. In: MacLeod CM (ed) Evalua-
6/j.1365-2273.2000.00326​.x tion of chemotherapeutic agents. Columbia University Press, New
6. Ribeyre L, Frère J, Gauchard G et al (2015) Preoperative balance York, pp 191–205
control compensation in patients with a vestibular schwannoma: 25. Taphoorn MJB, Klein M (2004) Cognitive deficits in adult
does tumor size matter? Clin Neurophysiol 126:787–793. https​:// patients with brain tumours. Lancet Neurol 3:159–168. https​://
doi.org/10.1016/j.clinp​h.2014.07.022 doi.org/10.1016/S1474​-4422(04)00680​-5
7. Sandooram D, Grunfeld EA, McKinney C et al (2004) Quality of 26. Barona ACRL (1986) An improved estimate of premorbid IQ
life following microsurgery, radiosurgery and conservative man- for blacks and whites on the WAIS-R. Int J Clin Neuropsychol
agement for unilateral vestibular schwannoma. Clin Otolaryngol 8:169–173
29:621–627. https​://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2273.2004.00881​.x 27. Osterrieth PA (1944) Le test de copie d’une figure complexe; con-
8. Martin HC, Sethi J, Lang D et al (2001) Patient-assessed outcomes tribution à l’étude de la perception et de la mémoire. Arch Psychol
after excision of acoustic neuroma: postoperative symptoms and 30:206–356
quality of life. J Neurosurg 94:211–216. https​://doi.org/10.3171/ 28. Warrington EK, James M (1992) Testbatterie für visuelle
jns.2001.94.2.0211 584Objekt- und Raumwahrnehmung (VOSP). 585Thames Val-
9. British Association of Otolaryngologists – Head & Neck Surgeons ley Test Company, Burry St Edmunds
(ed) (2002) Clinical effectiveness guidelines on acoustic neuroma 29. Schenkenberg T, Bradford DC, Ajax ET (1980) Line bisection and
(vestibular schwannoma). http://www.dohns​.org/DOHNS​/Resou​ unilateral visual neglect in patients with neurologic impairment.
rces_files​/Vesti​bular​%20Sch​wanno​ma%20Gui​delin​es.pdf Neurology 30(5):509. https​://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.30.5.509
10. Tucha O, Smely C, Preier M et al (2000) Cognitive deficits 30. Härting C, Markowitsch HJ, Neufeld H, Calabrese P, Deisinger
before treatment among patients with brain tumors. Neurosurgery K, Kessler J (eds) (2000) Wechsler Gedächtnistest - Revidierte
47:324–334. https:​ //doi.org/10.1097/000061​ 23-200008​ 000-00011​ Fassung (WMS-R). Huber, Bern
11. Goebel S, Stark AM, Kaup L et al (2011) Distress in patients with 31. Tombaugh T (2004) Trail Making Test A and B: normative
newly diagnosed brain tumours. Psychooncology 20:623–630. data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol
https​://doi.org/10.1002/pon.1958 19(2):203–214. https​://doi.org/10.1016/S0887​-6177(03)00039​-8
12. Pringle A-M, Taylor R, Whittle IR (1999) Anxiety and depression 32. Zimmermann P, Fimm B (1994) Testbatterie zur Aufmerksam-
in patients with an intracranial neoplasm before and after tumour keitsprüfung. PSYTEST, Herzogenrath
surgery. Br J Neurosurg 13:46–51. https​://doi.org/10.1080/02688​ 33. Benton AL, Hamsher Kd (1989) Multilingual Aphasia Examina-
69994​4177 tion. AJA Associates, Iowa City

13
Journal of Neuro-Oncology (2018) 140:145–153 153

34. Goebel S, Fischer R, Ferstl R et al (2009) Normative data and 44. Jacobson S, Marcus EM, Pugsley S (2018) Brain stem functional
psychometric properties for qualitative and quantitative scoring localization. In: Jacobson S, Marcus EM, Pugsley S (eds) Neuro-
criteria of the Five-point Test. Clin Neuropsychol 23(4):675–690. anatomy for the neuroscientist, vol 5. Springer, Cham, pp 169–204
https​://doi.org/10.1080/13854​04080​23891​85 45. Perciavalle V, Apps R, Bracha V et al (2013) Consensus paper:
35. Helmstaedter C, Durwen HF (1990) VLMT: a useful tool to assess current views on the role of cerebellar interpositus nucleus in
and differentiate verbal memory performance. Schweizer Archiv movement control and emotion. Cerebellum 12(5):738–757. https​
für Neurologie Neurochirurgie Psychiatrie 14(1):21–30 ://doi.org/10.1007/s1231​1-013-0464-0
36. Hubert W, Poeck K, Willmes-von-Hinckeldey K et al (1983) 46. Cronin T, Arshad Q, Seemungal BM (2017) Vestibular deficits
Aachener aphasie test. Hogrefe Verlag GmbH & Co., Göttingen in neurodegenerative disorders: balance, dizziness, and spatial
37. Hermann-Lingen C, Buss U, Snaith RP (2011) HADS-D Hospital disorientation. Front Neurol 8:538. https​://doi.org/10.3389/fneur​
Anxiety and Depression Scale-German Version: Deutsche Adap- .2017.00538​
tation der Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) von R. 47. Karnath H-O, Dieterich M (2006) Spatial neglect—a vestibular
P. Snaith und A. S. Zigmond disorder? Brain 129(Pt 2):293–305. https​://doi.org/10.1093/brain​
38. Grulke N, Bailer H, Schmutzer G et al (2006) Standardiza- /awh69​8
tion of the German short version of “profile of mood states” 48. Kralj-Hans I, Baizer JS, Swales C et al (2007) Independent roles
(POMS) in a representative sample—short communication. Psy- for the dorsal paraflocculus and vermal lobule VII of the cerebel-
chother Psychosom Med Psychol 56(9–10):403–405. https​://doi. lum in visuomotor coordination. Exp Brain Res 177(2):209–222.
org/10.1055/s-2006-94012​9 https​://doi.org/10.1007/s0022​1-006-0661-x
39. Aaronson NK, Ahmedzai S, Bergman B et al (1993) The European 49. Carrera E, Tononi G (2014) Diaschisis: past, present, future. Brain
Organization for Research and Treatment of Cancer QLQ-C30: 137(Pt 9):2408–2422. https​://doi.org/10.1093/brain​/awu10​1
a quality-of-life instrument for use in international clinical trials 50. Obrador S (1975) Brain stem vascular lesions affecting alertness
in oncology. JNCI J Natl Cancer Inst 85(5):365–376. https​://doi. and responsiveness. J Neurosurg Sci 19(4):211–214
org/10.1093/jnci/85.5.365 51. Sturm W, Willmes K (2001) On the functional neuroanatomy of
40. Smet HJ de, Paquier P, Verhoeven J et al (2013) The cerebel- intrinsic and phasic alertness. Neuroimage 14(1 Pt 2):S76–S84.
lum: its role in language and related cognitive and affective func- https​://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0839
tions. Brain Lang 127(3):334–342. https:​ //doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl​ 52. Rooney AG, Carson A, Grant R (2011) Depression in cerebral
.2012.11.001 glioma patients: a systematic review of observational studies. J
41. Bodranghien F, Bastian A, Casali C et al (2016) Consensus Natl Cancer Inst 103(1):61–76. https:​ //doi.org/10.1093/jnci/djq45​
paper: revisiting the symptoms and signs of cerebellar syndrome. 8
Cerebellum 15(3):369–391. https ​ : //doi.org/10.1007/s1231​ 53. Bunevicius A, Tamasauskas S, Deltuva V et al (2014) Predic-
1-015-0687-3 tors of health-related quality of life in neurosurgical brain tumor
42. Kansal K, Yang Z, Fishman AM et al (2017) Structural cerebel- patients: Focus on patient-centered perspective. Acta Neuro-
lar correlates of cognitive and motor dysfunctions in cerebellar chir (Wien) 156(2):367–374. https​: //doi.org/10.1007/s0070​
degeneration. Brain 140(3):707–720. https:​ //doi.org/10.1093/brain​ 1-013-1930-7
/aww32​7
43. Kopelman MD (2002) Disorders of memory. Brain 125(10):2152–
2190. https​://doi.org/10.1093/brain​/awf22​9

13

You might also like