Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 52

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/342052612

The Shape of Knowledge: Situational Analysis in Counseling Psychology


Research

Article in Journal of Counseling Psychology · June 2020


DOI: 10.1037/cou0000527

CITATIONS READS
6 1,605

1 author:

Patrick Grzanka
University of Tennessee
78 PUBLICATIONS 1,292 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Science, Research, and Racism View project

White Racial Affect View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Patrick Grzanka on 09 June 2020.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Running head: SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 1

© 2020, American Psychological Association. This paper is not the copy of record and may
not exactly replicate the final, authoritative version of the article. Please do not copy or cite
without authors' permission. The final article will be available, upon publication, via its
DOI: 10.1037/cou0000527

Please cite accordingly:

Grzanka, P. R. (in press). The shape of knowledge: Situational analysis in counseling psychology
research. Journal of Counseling Psychology. Advance online publication.
http://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000527

The Shape of Knowledge:

Situational Analysis in Counseling Psychology Research

Patrick R. Grzanka

The University of Tennessee, Knoxville

Author Note

Patrick R. Grzanka, Department of Psychology, The University of Tennessee, Knoxville.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Patrick R. Grzanka, The

University of Tennessee, Department of Psychology, 1404 Circle Drive, Knoxville, TN 37996.

Email: patrick.grzanka@utk.edu
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 2

Abstract

Situational analysis (SA) is a powerful method for visually mapping qualitative data. As an

extension of constructivist grounded theory developed by Charmaz and others, Clarke’s

situational analysis encourages researchers to transform qualitative data into various visual maps

that can illuminate dynamics that may be obscured by more traditional analytic approaches.

Fifteen years since Fassinger’s landmark paper on grounded theory in counseling psychology

research, I make an argument for SA’s potential uses in counseling psychology using data from a

mixed-methods dissertation on White racial affect. I outline the exigency of SA and its

epistemological and methodological underpinnings in detail, with a focus on SA as a critical,

structural analysis. Each primary mapping procedure—situational, positional, and social

worlds/arenas maps—is introduced and examples are provided that illustrate SA’s unique

analytic capacities and insights. By way of SA, I argue for a “critical-cartographic” turn in

counseling psychology along four axes: promoting systems-level research and advocacy,

deepening consideration of intersectionality, cultivating alternative epistemologies beyond post-

positivism, and invigorating qualitative research on counseling and psychotherapy.

Keywords: qualitative methods, grounded theory, research methodology, situational analysis,

critical theory

Public Significance Statement: This paper explores situational analysis, an approach to

visualizing qualitative data that can help researchers conduct critical, structural analysis through

three mapping procedures: situational, positional, and social worlds/arenas maps.


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 3

The Shape of Knowledge:

Situational Analysis in Counseling Psychology Research

In 2009, just as I was finishing my interdisciplinary, mixed-method dissertation, I found

myself constantly referencing Ruth Fassinger’s 2005 paper on grounded theory, which was

published in the last Journal of Counseling Psychology special issue on qualitative research and

which has now been cited nearly 1,000 times1 by scholars working across diverse fields,

including but certainly not limited to counseling psychology. That piece, like so many other

papers in Haverkamp and colleagues’ (2005) special issue, sparked a qualitative imagination in

me. Fassinger’s paper, in particular, specified a vision and version of grounded theory (GT) that

negotiated the complex methodological debates that are unavoidable in otherwise positivist-

rooted social science disciplines. From her standpoint as a feminist counseling psychologist,

Fassinger articulated pivotal questions: How can critical counseling psychologists use GT to

advance social justice research? How can we use GT responsibly and reflexively to represent the

experiences of marginalized groups and communities? How can we use this as a tool for actual

social transformation?

That year also marked the publication of Situational Analysis: Grounded Theory After the

Postmodern Turn by Adele Clarke (2005), one of GT-inaugurator Anselm Strauss’s students. As

I will argue in this paper, the introduction of situational analysis represented a landmark moment

of methodological innovation in GT. Situational analysis is what Clarke (2005) termed a

supplement to—not a replacement for—the “theory-methods package” that is GT. Through three

primary modes of analysis linked by a commitment to exposing the invisible-but-meaningful in

qualitative data, situational analysis involves cartographically mapping qualitative data. Given

1
956 citations according to Google Scholar, May 11, 2020.
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 4

(1) the well-documented challenges to teaching and training qualitative researchers to move

beyond description (Luker, 2008; Poulin, 2007), (2) psychologists’ stated goals of maintaining

rigor, trustworthiness, and fidelity across all forms of inquiry in psychology, including

qualitative research (Levitt et al., 2017a; Morrow, 2005; Yeh & Inman, 2007), and (3) situational

analysis’s explicit foundation in critical scholarship and Clarke’s (2005, 2012; Clarke et al.,

2015; Clarke et al., 2018) framing of situational analysis as a tool for social justice (e.g., Pérez &

Cannella, 2013), situational analysis represents an exciting methodological opportunity for

counseling psychologists and critical researchers across the discipline.

In this paper, I argue that situational analysis is a valuable tool for qualitative inquiry

both with and beyond GT methods. I will briefly review the epistemic and methodological

origins of GT, as well as Clarke’s argument for cartography as a heuristic in qualitative analysis.

Using previously unpublished dissertation data (Grzanka, 2010), I introduce the three modes of

situational analysis: situational, positional, and social worlds/arenas maps, and identify

important ways these mapping practices can deepen, enhance, and extend traditional GT and GT-

adjacent qualitative methods, such as critical discourse analysis. Finally, I identify four axes on

which situational analysis might fulfil recent calls within counseling psychology to improve the

methodological and transformative potential of research, teaching, practice, and advocacy. My

goals here are invitational and pedagogical: as Fassinger’s (2005) paper invited counseling

psychologists into the generative possibilities of GT research and taught some key ways to

employ GT, I hope to stimulate interest in situational analysis as a underused and nonetheless

potent tool for qualitative and mixed-methods research in counseling psychology and related

fields.

Situating Situational Analysis


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 5

Grounding Grounded Theory

Sociologists Barney Glaser and Anselm Strauss (1965) originally developed grounded

theory while studying how terminally ill patients and families respond to their diagnoses with

physicians, nurses, and each other. Glaser and Strauss (1965, 1967) demonstrated the power of

inductive qualitative inquiry to illuminate social dynamics in ways that produced explanatory

theory grounded in data rather than existing ideas (e.g., performativity, habitus) or frameworks

(e.g., Marxism). Taking an unconventional approach, they criticized the landscape of mid-

century sociology for its reliance on theory-testing and philosophy-style deduction. In its place,

they offered a radically inductive approach to qualitative inquiry that prioritized the development

of explanatory frames from the situation at-hand, rather than deduction based on pre-existing

ideas: theory grounded in the data.

Though it was initially not enthusiastically received by the social scientific mainstream

(Kenny & Fourie, 2014), GT eventually achieved a superior status among qualitative methods

(Clarke & Charmaz, 2014). GT’s preeminence as an approach is attributable to its precise,

reproducible, and (at least if applied consistently) rigorous methods, including: coding,

memoing, saturation, and other concepts that today are synonymous with good qualitative

research, regardless of whether it is GT research (Levitt et al., 2017a; Morrow, 2005; Yeh &

Inman, 2007). GT’s use within and beyond psychology has resulted in methodological debates

and various iterations (Fassinger, 2005). While the Glasserian approach is sometimes associated

with realism and post-positivism, Strauss and student Juliet Corbin’s branch of the GT tradition

tends to reflect an epistemic orientation toward strong social constructionism; however, both

approaches have been used across epistemological frameworks. Strauss and Glaser’s student

Kathy Charmaz (2014), for example, developed a version—“constructivist” GT—that further


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 6

extended the social constructionist undercurrent of GT and embraced an expressly political

orientation (Charmaz, 2016; Mills et al., 2006). Charmaz’s is a flexible though no less rigorous

GT that she posits as a tool for social justice (Charmaz, 2011). Further, Charmaz’s approach

rejects the idea of the researcher encountering the empirical universe naively or in ways that do

not fundamentally affect the knowledge production process. In conversation with the debates of

the latter part of the 20th century regarding the relationship between science and feminism

(Haraway, 1998; Harding, 1987), Charmaz offered a deeply critical version of GT, wherein

critical signifies a sensitivity to structural power relations, especially the relationship between the

researcher and the researched (Charmaz, 2017; Denzin, 2015). Though not all critical uses of GT

are indebted to a single scholar or tradition, this type of critical, constructivist work generally

characterizes GT’s use in in counseling psychology (Levitt et al., 2017b).

GT in Counseling Psychology

Fassinger (2005) noted that by the publication of Haverkamp, Morrow, and Ponterotto’s

JCP special issue on qualitative methods, GT was already becoming popular in counseling

psychology research since Rennie et al.’s (1989) landmark paper exploring the method’s promise

in psychology. Researchers throughout the field have used GT methods to investigate a wide

range of topics, including: feminist group supervision (Burnes et al., 2012); client perspectives

on multicultural counseling competence (Pope-Davis et al., 2002); acculturation experiences of

sexual and gender minority immigrants (Fuks et al., 2018); intimate partner violence (Latta &

Goodman, 2011); perceptions of racist and xenophobic discrimination (Inman et al., 2015); and

ally identity development (Grzanka et al., 2015). In other words, counseling psychology

researchers have used GT to study all the kinds of things that counseling psychologists study,

including psychotherapy (Levitt et al., 2017b). Alongside consensual qualitative research (Hill et
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 7

al., 2005), thematic analysis (Braun & Clarke, 2006), case study (Stake, 2005), narrative analysis

(Frost, 2018), phenomenology (Giorgi, 1985), and participatory action research (Kidd & Kral,

2005), GT became a dominant tool for qualitative psychological inquiry (Creswell et al., 2007),

particularly research on members of socially marginalized, stigmatized, or

systematically/historically under-represented groups and work that purports to take a critical

approach to mental health, well-being, and inequities (i.e., disparities) (Fassinger & Morrow,

2013).

Just as no method is imbued with magical properties (Grzanka, 2016), it is important to

recognize that GT is not a perfect method. Indeed, belief in a perfect method would, as Bowleg

(2008) argued, betray a kind of latent positivism, or faith in an unobscured and disembodied

representation of Reality or Truth. Further, GT is not always the best tool for the job (Creswell et

al., 2007). For example, there are circumstances under which theory-testing is more appropriate

than theory generation (e.g., Moradi et al., 2019), or times when thematic observations more

adequately answer research questions than new (explanatory) theories (e.g., Grzanka et al.,

2019). On the other hand, sociologists have observed how many studies that claim to use GT do

not actually produce new theories (Timmermans & Tavory, 2012), and yet GT procedures (e.g.,

inductive coding, memoing) may be useful even when theory-generation is not the primary

objective of a study. GT has been critiqued for its methodological rigidity—particularly the

analytic prescriptions of Glaserian and “classical” approaches (Charmaz, 2014), including

mandates for the discreteness of codes and somewhat arbitrary distinctions between coding

stages (e.g., open, axial). It has also been criticized for an oblique relationship to positivism and

scientism that undermines the potential for GT to actually ground theory-generation in

participants’ lived experiences and self-representations (Clarke, 2005). Yet, some of GT’s most
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 8

vocal critics are its biggest advocates, including Fassinger (2005), Charmaz (2014), and Clarke

(2005).

Even as counseling psychologists have largely embraced constructionist strands of GT

and some have conducted explicitly critical work (Levitt et al., 2017b), criticisms of GT’s

epistemic and methodological foundations merit engagement in order to understand the exigency

of situational analysis and its potential benefit to counseling psychology researchers. Note that

Harding (1987) distinguished methods from methodology by specifying methodology as the

theory and assumptions that guide of use of particular tools, i.e., methods. In this feminist

framing, epistemology (i.e., theories of knowledge) and methodology are deeply intertwined.

Accordingly, from her disciplinary standpoint at the nexus of science and technology studies and

medical sociology, Clarke (2005) outlined five primary “recalcitrancies” of traditional GT that

prevent it from being “pushed” around the “postmodern turn” (p. 11). To Clarke, these

recalcitrancies motivate situational analysis as a complementary analytic package or extension of

GT, because some traditional formulations of GT remain invested in positivist (i.e., modernist)

notions of truth, objectivity, and disembodied knowing that are dismantled by critical,

postmodern epistemologies (see also Gergen, 1973). Clarke encourages all GT researchers to

consider how the underlying logic of theory discovery may inadvertently affect even those

projects that aspire toward critical social constructionism. Accordingly, I elaborate Clarke’s five-

point typology to suggest traditional GT (pp. 11-18):

1. Lacks reflexivity, particular in terms of interrogating the co-constitutive dynamic

between researchers and researched, subjects and objects, knowledge and the material

world; fails to understand research as a process of representation that is always

political.
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 9

2. Oversimplifies complex dynamics in the interest of conceptual parsimony and

explanatory power; can effectively reduce qualitative data in ways that are analogous

if not directly comparable to more primarily reductionist qualitative approaches (e.g.,

content analysis) and quantitative methods.

3. Reduces social processes so as to emphasize singular explanatory dynamics rather

than overlapping, multifarious, and even contradictory processes in a given situation;

isolates situations to represent social worlds and contexts as discrete rather than

interactive and plural.

4. Interprets variation in data as “negative” cases or inexplicable, whereby coding

practices privileges consistency rather than diversity; reproduces quantitative

tendencies to identify and eliminate “outliers” in the data.

5. Implicitly and sometimes explicitly purports to “discover” (a la Glaser) the singular

Truth or achieve a methodological and epistemic purity; positions impersonality and

stability of data as desirable, rather than as reflective of a normative investment in

scientism and positivism.

In introducing situational analysis, Clarke (2003, 2005) did not argue that most GT research is

fundamentally flawed; likewise, I am not suggesting that counseling psychologists have

generally misapplied or misused GT. Nevertheless, Clarke’s framing of these problems as

recalcitrancies—stubborn tendencies that can be difficult to break away from—is helpful for

reinvigorating GT with critical self-reflexivity, a hallmark of feminist and social justice-oriented

research (Hesse-Biber & Piatelli, 2012; Levitt et al., in press). Accordingly, Clarke (2005)

suggested pushing GT around the postmodern or “interpretive” turn (Clarke et al., 2018) can

open up qualitative imaginations to generative, critical, and ultimately grounded accounts of


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 10

complex social problems, especially those faced by members of oppressed populations (Espín,

1993). The postmodern turn, in Clarke’s framing, involves identifying and marking socially

constructed binaries, interrogating the ways that power shapes the methodological process, and

situating research contextually in local and broader struggles for social justice.

In this sense, Clarke’s (2005) postmodern turn is thoroughly consonant with qualitative

research in counseling psychology. Accordingly, situational analysis speaks directly to (1)

longstanding calls in the field for social justice action (Spanierman & Poteat, 2005; Goodman et

al., 2004) and (2) to counseling psychologists’ training in strengths-based and multicultural

approaches in the interest of alternative ways of knowing and doing psychology (Fine, 2018).

While many counseling psychologists are already using GT as a tool for social justice (see

references), I follow Fine’s (2012, 2018) recent provocations to widen the methodological

imagination in “revolting times” as consonant with the self-reflexivity so central to Clarke’s

(2005) argument. Furthermore, this special issue marks an occasion to refine, expand, and be

self-critical of even long-established best methodological practices (e.g., Frankfurt et al., 2016).

As I detail in the following section, situational analysis leverages the strengths of extant critical

approaches to GT by encouraging reflexivity at all stages of the research process; embracing

complexity; envisioning “situations” rather than discrete constructs or isolated processes;

sensitizing analyses to the centrality and meaning of difference; and valuing the researcher’s

subjective interpretive skills as central to theory construction. The remainder of this paper is

dedicated to introducing Clarke’s situational analysis (hereafter “SA”), with a focus on how SA

can advance critical psychology (Fine, 2012; Prilletensky & Nelson, 1997), including a

counseling psychology rooted in social justice (Moradi & Grzanka, 2017).

Doing Situational Analysis


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 11

Because SA follows Clarke’s (2005) call to understand discourse as the flows of

power/knowledge through institutions (Foucault, 1972) and attention to the role of discourse in

producing subjects and ordering differences (Foucault, 1978), SA is particularly adept at

attending to sites of silence in data. In other words, by not thinking of qualitative data

exclusively as manifest evidence but as a discourse(s) about a situation, SA helps researchers

uncover what is present and not present in a dataset (cf. Gordon, 1997). What is not being said or

articulated often offers insight into the form and shape of knowledge as structural, rather than

(simply) individual or subjective. And the shape of knowledge—what is known and what is not,

positions taken and not taken, actors and actants implicated and implicating, historical

contexts—facilitates understanding the structural dimensions of a situation. Such a structural

perspective, I argue, moves qualitative inquiry beyond the descriptive or the particular and

toward the critical and explanatory, which is a key goal of GT. This analytic pivoting toward the

explanatory is a unique strength of SA: when stuck in the process of merely describing

qualitative data, SA’s three primary analytic exercises facilitate conceptualizing data relationally

and visually. In the following three sections, I describe procedures for and provide examples of

situational, positional, and social worlds/arenas mapping using data collected as part of my own

dissertation study.

The Illustrative Dataset

In the examples that follow, I use two types of qualitative data, print media coverage and

interview transcripts, collected as part of a mixed-methods dissertation on White racial emotions

(Grzanka, 2010). The White Racial Affect Project (WRAP) was expressly interdisciplinary and

involved three intertwined studies drawing on theory and methods from counseling psychology,

critical social psychology, sociology, and American studies. The research was organized around
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 12

questions about White racial affect—in other words, the feelings of race and antiracism (Grzanka

et al., 2020). This research extended scholarship on Whiteness by taking an intersectional

approach to the gendered and sexualized dynamics of emotion, as well as the fundamental racial

politics of feeling. Part 1 involved a critical discourse analysis (Clarke, 2005) and SA of media

coverage of three moments of particularly intersectional, public controversy at the end of the 20th

century: the firing of Black actor Isaiah Washington from the ABC television series Grey’s

Anatomy (headed by a Black woman showrunner) for allegedly twice calling a White gay

colleague a gay slur; the fallout of radio “shock jock” Don Imus’s referring to the championship-

winning Rutgers University women’s basketball team with a gendered racial slur; and White gay

journalist Anderson Cooper’s perceived emotional coverage of Hurricane Katrina’s aftermath in

New Orleans and the Gulf Coast region. These controversies were selected from a range of

similar events (e.g., Janet Jackson and Justin Timberlake’s controversial performance at the 2004

Super Bowl) for their temporal proximity to the actual interviews, prominence in national

culture, and capacity to facilitate direct conversation about issues of race, gender, and sexuality.

Part 2 composed the initial development and validation of a psychometric scale to measure

White guilt and shame (Grzanka et al., 2020), items of which were developed from the study of

these public controversies. And part 3 involved two in-depth, semi-structured interviews each

with White college students (N = 10), during which the participants watched footage from these

public controversies and then talked through their thoughts and feelings with the interviewer and

principal investigator, a White cisgender queer man (me). The interviews took place at least one

week and no more than one month apart to facilitate the development of trust and rapport-

building, particularly in the context of discussing painful emotions related to racism,

homophobia, and misogyny. Data were analyzed using GT and SA. I describe the key tenets
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 13

(goals, procedures) for conducting SA maps using illustrative data from parts 1 and 2 in the

sections that follow.

Though in my own work I have tended to move through these exercises sequentially,

Clarke (2005) stressed that SA is an iterative and non-linear process meant to facilitate data

interpretation during all stages of the research process. Further, SA is meant to supplement—not

supplant—existing GT procedures, namely coding, memoing, and auditing (Fassinger, 2005).

Though SA may be valuable in other interpretivist-constructivist projects outside the formal

boundaries of GT (i.e., in related inductive approaches such as critical discourse analysis or

narrative analysis), Clarke posits SA as an extension of GT, not a replacement. In SA, the

situation itself becomes the object of analysis. According to Clarke et al. (2018), the

“fundamental focus of a GT analysis is on action in interaction. What is happening?” (p. 108). In

SA, they assert the fundamental focus is on relationality via mapping the situation. The situation

is an important concept for a range of theorists ranging from American pragmatist John Dewey

to feminist philosopher Donna Haraway (Clarke et al., 2018); thinking situationally, as the

cartographic (i.e., map-making) exercises below will illustrate, facilitates taking into account

discursive materials encountered in the study (even if they were not conceptualized as “primary”

data points) and historical elements that, again, might not initially seem relevant.

Thinking situationally means, according to Clarke (2005), understanding the problem or

issue under investigation in terms of the social worlds/arenas that co-constitute the situation. In

the WRAP, for example, White participants’ experiences of racial guilt and/or shame was

understood as inextricable from the 2008 election and the political ascension of Barack Obama.

The study was not about Obama, but Obama was certainly part of the situation. This emphasis on

the situation (i.e., the social-historical embeddedness of the research process) is aligned with the
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 14

analytic process of “abduction” that Clarke, Friese, and Washburn colleagues (2018) characterize

as central to SA (see also Timmermans & Tavory, 2012). As opposed to the strict induction of

classic or Glaserian GT (i.e., driven exclusively by data, from the ground up), SA encourages

consistent analytic pivoting between concrete empirical data and more abstract, conceptual ideas,

including mapping and memoing. The abductive gestalt of SA may help researches avoid getting

stuck in description and failing to connect empirical, manifest evidence to conceptual

frameworks that help researchers understand what [this given situation] means.

Situational Maps

Typically, the first kind of map created in a SA project, a situational map is an attempt to

descriptively identify all of the human and nonhuman elements related to the situation of inquiry

(Clarke et al., 2018). The situational mapping process helps researchers purposefully and self-

reflexively construct the study’s content and boundaries based on empirical evidence and critical

thinking. Though usually extended from messy (i.e., free form) to ordered stages, situational

mapping should not be confused with free association or brainstorming. This key stage in the SA

process is guided by preliminary empirical evidence, review of the existing literature (as relevant

and necessary), and the researcher’s and any relevant collaborators’ standpoint and perspective-

taking on the situation of inquiry. Situational mapping in the WRAP project began prior to the

dissertation proposal stage and extended through data collection and writing, moving toward

analytic connection-making between concepts and constructs that Clarke and colleagues (2018)

have more recently termed “relational” analysis.

For counseling psychologists familiar with Bronfenbrenner’s (1979) ecological systems

theory, it might be tempting to compare or even equate situational mapping with his

conceptualization of the micro-, meso-, and macro-levels of influence on the individual.


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 15

However, Clarke (2005) expressly rejected Strauss’s similar multi-level theoretical framing; she

developed SA in order to better reflect postmodern and interpretive principles (Foucault, 1978;

Gergen, 1973). As such, situational mapping rejects a hierarchical (e.g., from lower to higher

order) or binary (e.g., micro vs. macro, agency vs. structure) ethos by considering relevant

elements in total relativity and empirical specificity. Situational maps do not presuppose, for

example, a priori categories of “micro” or “macro” elements, which impose a theoretical

framework on the situation of inquiry rather than ground the organization of the data on the

data’s terms. Instead, the situational mapping framework (1) helps researchers identify potential

kinds of empirical elements that might—or might not—be encountered in the situation and (2)

insists that the SA process itself should clarify the nature of those relationships. In this sense,

situational mapping is always structural, even when identifying implicated actors or objects that

seem individual or tangential to the situation, such as an individual person not directly involved

in the study (e.g., Barack Obama in the WRAP).

The situational mapping process is designed to illuminate how things that might not

initially seem “macro” at all, such as a powerful individual, a new technology, or a new

bureaucratic policy, are indeed functioning in structural terms that are influencing multiple sites,

individuals, and dynamics in the situation of inquiry. Moreover, by challenging researchers to

consider nonhuman actants (i.e., objects that play a role in a situation), situational mapping may

be especially hard and beneficial to counseling psychologists less attuned to consider the role of

nonhumans in research. For example, in the WRAP, Hurricane Katrina was a key actant in the

study through which ideas about race, class, and social inequality were co-constituted. The

hurricane was not a participant, but the storm was implicated in processes germane to the study,

including participants’ lay theories of race, assignment of blame for the storm’s consequences,
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 16

and manifestations or denials of White guilt over systemic racism. Further, by foregrounding

cultural, organizational, and popular discourses, this component of the mapping process may

further resist psychological tendencies to detach research from its social embeddedness or to

neglect the historicity of observed social phenomena (Gergen, 1973; Orford, 2006).

Clarke (2005; Clarke et al., 2018) describes the first stage of the situational mapping

process as “messy.” But messy does not mean useless or bad. Making messy situational maps is

a generative process uninhibited by the precision and eventual taxonomy of the GT process.

Messy situational maps require nothing more than the researcher and a note-taking device.

Guided by an interest in capturing “the situation” as defined by Clarke, this messy process helps

the researcher experiment with ideas and poke at the boundaries of relevancy to the situation.

The first messy maps will likely be sketched before formal analysis has commenced, because

data collection may have yet to begin or may be in preliminary stages. As such, messy maps can

help researchers articulate their assumptions and predispositions vis a vis the data. Messy maps,

at least at the onset of the project, can expose what investigators are thinking about a project,

including expectations about what will or should become relevant. Messy map-making

incentivizes questions such as: “Does this matter? Does it not? Why or why not?”

This stage of the mapping process is key to identifying discursive elements that may be

outside of the immediate scene of inquiry but that should inform theoretical and purposive

sampling efforts in the research design process. Similarly, this process can mark what is

otherwise unmarked, including those elements of the situation that have become so naturalized

that they are taken for granted or invisible, including power and privilege. These messy

situational maps become ordered situational maps (Figure 1) and then eventually, relational maps

(Figure 2), as the process pivots from identification (i.e., what matters here?) to analysis (how
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 17

does this matter?). In relational mapping, which occurs literally on top of messy situational

maps, relationships between elements in the situational maps are uncovered via empirical

evidence and analysis. The lines that connect ideas should not be hypothetical; they should

represent empirically substantiated dynamics or connections that have been uncovered in the

research process, such as the relationship between guilt and Hurricane Katrina in Figure 2. In the

actual writing process, these lines might first be memos that become fleshed out paragraphs that

finally become sections of a manuscript. Relational mapping is one of the reasons to retain messy

situation maps throughout the research process; there is no such thing as a “bad” map.

Messy situational maps are never final, but they are always iterative and sometimes quite

valuable. Like memo-writing in the GT process, messy situational maps will almost certainly not

wind up in a final manuscript but may eventually generate key analytic insights. Clarke and

colleagues (2018) note that old messy maps should be revisited throughout the research process,

providing much needed perspective when, for example, the researcher finds themselves stuck

with a developing theory or wrestling with an idea that is not fully accounting for important parts

of the situation. Returning to earlier drafts that detailed pre-data collection expectations can also

reveal if something is missing from the data, which may potentially represent a site of silence.

Particularly germane to psychological research, I suggest situational maps catalyze anti-variable

thinking in qualitative research design. Often, qualitative research in psychology merely applies

qualitative methods to quantitative questions, such as a qualitative evaluation of a construct (e.g.,

disordered eating, depression) that is otherwise typically assessed quantitatively or via an

established assessment tool. This kind of thinking—what I have referred to elsewhere as

psychology’s epistemic riptide (Grzanka, 2017)—roots even qualitative research in individual

processes and constructs that makes it difficult to conceptualize and even “see” structural
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 18

processes, including intersectionality or overlapping systems of oppression (Bowleg, 2008).

Situational maps are not merely the recitation of relevant constructs that would otherwise be

identified as dependent or independent variables. This kind of mapping unmoors variable-

centered thinking (Grzanka, 2016; Zeiders et al., 2013) from the center of the qualitative scene,

instead foregrounding environmental factors, discursive elements, and nonhuman actants, in

addition to key individuals, groups, historical issues, and spatial factors in the situation.

Not all GT studies are multi-sited (Clarke, 2005); that is, not all projects that use GT or

SA involve multiple study locations or multiple methods (see Guetterman et al. [2019] for an

overview of mixed-methods GT approaches). However, by focusing on the situation rather than

strictly participants or an activity, SA facilitates both imagining implicated environments not

directly under investigation and, when appropriate, managing these multiple sites in the research

process. For example, in the WRAP, the university from which survey respondents and eventual

interview participants were drawn was a key spatial element in the study. Figure 1 shows how

the university as a space was connected to or at least implicated in participants’ meaning making

around the public controversies that did not immediately seem to have anything to do with the

university. So even though empirical elements of one part of the study—the discourse analysis of

media texts described in the next section on positional maps—were not obviously connected to

other parts of the study, the situational mapping process helps to see and make connections that

are not otherwise evident. In this case, issues on campus and in the immediate region influenced

how respondents made sense of the public controversies; this meaning making was actually vital

to the study. Finally, as Clarke et al. (2018) underscore, situational mapping is about embracing

uncertainty rather than foreclosing upon potential dynamics that are obscured by otherwise

variable-centered or post-positivist analyses. Because this process is so iterative, situational


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 19

mapping builds revision into the process, effectively reframing analytic “mistakes” into

opportunities for reconceptualizing taken-for-granted ideas in the research process.

Positional Maps

It is tempting sometimes to think of qualitative inquiry as devoid of the epistemic

consequences of quantitative research, as if qualification is a fundamentally less potentially

violent (Dotson, 2011) process than quantification (Grzanka, 2016; Bowleg, 2008). But

qualification in SA is a data reduction strategy and therefore involves some degree of reduction.

Reduction does not have to be reductionist, however, and SA’s ambitions are to accurately and

robustly construct the situation of inquiry. Consistent with Levitt et al.’s (2017a) description of

fidelity as an intimate connection between the researcher and their phenomena under

investigation, SA encourages fidelity through iterative mapping and multiple kinds of mapping—

in addition to all of the procedures (e.g., auditing, member-checking) researchers would normally

use in a GT study to ensure trustworthiness (Levitt et al., 2017a; Morrow, 2005). Accordingly, I

see positional maps as productive reduction: these exercises identify the key axes on which

issues in the situation are enacted. These maps ask the researcher to visualize and specify the

terrain on which issues are negotiated in the situation. I can attest that this is the single most

useful process I have ever engaged when working with my own data and/or a student’s project

when the goal is to push beyond description/summation and into genuine analysis.

The first step in the positional mapping process is to identify axes of position-taking. In

Clarke et al.’s (2018) formulation, the objective here is to consider bipolar continua on which

issues are being debated or contested in the situation. These do not have to be active debates

insomuch as your participants may not be actively disagreeing with each other or even

interacting; rather, these positions should represent some element of ideological difference in the
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 20

sample. In the WRAP study, positional maps played a crucial role in the process of conducting a

critical discourse analysis on print media texts about three loci of racial controversy: Cooper’s

coverage of Hurricane Katrina; the Don Imus scandal; and Washington’s firing from Grey’s

Anatomy. Obviously, this kind of media analysis is not common in counseling psychology,

though it is in other parts of critical psychology (Cole et al., 2012). The objective in the WRAP

was to use coverage of these moments to think about how social actors (i.e., journalists)

articulate and make sense of these moments in which guilt, responsibility, and blame were

negotiated in highly public spaces, which were defined by major news outlets as classified by

circulation numbers and LexisNexus (see Grzanka [2010] for methods and inclusion criteria).

Data gleaned from this component of the project would inform both survey item development

and structure the in-depth interviews to be conducted in the third and final phase of the study.

Recall that SA, like GT, is not driven by extant theory but by inductive, reflexive data

analysis. In the WRAP, I was not interested in testing existing ideas, such as theories of negative

self-conscious affect (Tangney & Dearing, 2002), but in understanding how various social actors

created and assigned blame during moments of racial, sexual, and gendered conflict. I was

concerned with discourses of guilt, responsibility, innocence, and individual versus collective

constructions of emotion, and I was especially interested in the extent to which emotions were

articulated (i.e., rhetorical constructions of affect) and named (i.e., specific invocations of

feelings, such as guilt, anger, remorse) in the data. In addition to memoing, positional maps were

a key data organization tool as I attempted to synthesize over 100 news articles that addressed

each of the three sites of public controversy. The axes on which I ultimately framed these events

emerged from questions about the data, not from existing theories of race, gender, sexuality or

emotion. And these axes came to inform not only conclusions about these data but how I related
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 21

to other aspects of the project. These positional maps exposed substantive currents in the flows

of discourse about these controversies which, I argued, were cases of more pervasive ideological

systems that maintained White supremacy, as well as intersecting heterosexism and patriarchy, in

the language of multiculturalism (Grzanka & Maher, 2012). Figure 3 depicts three positional

maps included in the WRAP. Notice that positional maps are not primarily about placing

individual respondents or data points on the map. This is about the discourse in your situation,

not the numerical quantity of respondents. The aims of positional maps are in stark contrast to

the quantitative impulse in psychology that is recapitulated by reviewers and editors alike when

they ask: “how many?” or the countless other questions that are about quantifying otherwise

qualitative data. Positional mapping asks researchers to consider if “how many?” is not the right

question. What if we need to be asking, “why would someone (not) feel this way?”?

These relatively parsimonious maps in Figure 3 represent 2x2 framings of the positions

taken up in mass media discourse on these three issues. In the case of Anderson Cooper’s

“emotional” coverage of Hurricane Katrina’s aftermath in New Orleans—specifically the

mismanagement of the disaster by federal, state, and local governments that resulted in chaos and

loss of life within New Orleans and throughout the Gulf Coast region—the positional axes

represented evaluations of Cooper’s coverage. Note that a site of silence emerged here quickly

and consequentially: the idea that Cooper’s coverage was bad and unemotional did not enter the

discourse I surveyed. Instead, journalists debated the extent to which Cooper was or was not

overly emotional, and the quality of his coverage was assessed accordingly. In the Grey’s

Anatomy scandal, coverage debated the extent to which Washington’s identity as a Black straight

man may or may not have played a role in his firing. Another axis of position-taking regarded the

relative amount of guilt assigned to Washington for having referred to T. R. Knight with a gay
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 22

slur. Finally, in the Don Imus affair, the axes interestingly mapped onto the same dimensions as

the Grey’s Anatomy controversy: does Don Imus’s identity matter, and is he guilty/worth of

punishment? These ordering principles of the discourse played an essential role in the

conclusions I drew from my analyses and from the other components of the WRAP. Positional

mapping helped me to see how axes of perceived culpability across sites were shaped by

intersecting social systems, including race, gender, and sexuality. As I have suggested above, this

element of SA helps to uncover not only the content or substance of data but the shape of

knowledge, which is essential to tracing the structural dimensions of the situation.

Later iterations of maps might actually specify the number of people who took each

position (Grzanka et al., 2015) and axes might be imagined beyond bipolar continua (Grzanka &

Schuch, 2020), even though this diverges from Clarke’s (2003, 2005; Clarke et al., 2018)

original specifications. I have personally found that identifying the number of participants who

took up specific positions relative to others can advance rather than minimize the goals of SA,

because identifying outliers in qualitative inquiry can be about privileging these unusual

positions, rather than eliminating or controlling for them as we might do in quantitative analysis

(i.e., what Clarke [2005] calls treatment of the “negative” case). But the analytic thrust of this

positional mapping process remains on identifying positions, not individuals. Accordingly, this

work amplifies the feminist and critical theory-driven concepts of standpoints and positionality

(Haraway, 1988), rather than frequencies or correlations. However, one can imagine using this

exercise even in quantitative work, particularly the murky mixed-methodological realm of survey

design where focus group and/or interview data may be used to generate items that reflect

bipolar continua of agree vs. disagree (Worthington & Whittaker, 2006).

Social Worlds/Arenas Maps


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 23

The last kind of SA maps encourage researchers to pursue Strauss’s (1978) original

vision of social worlds/arenas as constructed, contested, and overlapping terrains of social life

beyond individual or small group processes. Because of their sociological orientation, these may

be the most uncomfortable maps for counseling psychologists to create. Nevertheless, that is one

reason why I think social worlds/arenas maps may be the most important. Clarke and colleagues

(2018) underscore that social worlds/arenas maps are about organizational processes, which are

distinct from organizations, such as a university, hospital, or company. They assert that

organizational analyses reveal how “meaning making and commitments are organized and

reorganized again and again over time” through porous and highly dynamic interactions between

and among collectivities (p. 150). Because the focus in these maps is on social action rather than

variables, these maps (like all of SA) require honing one’s skills in sociological thinking, which

is invaluable for understanding how individual subjectivities shape and are shaped by social

ecologies. Though a full overview of Strauss’s (1978) social worlds theory warrants a book-

length treatment, I introduce key concepts and examples here that can guide counseling

psychologists in this unique analytic exercise.

Social worlds are groupings of varying sizes that are collective and organized around a

shared activity and one of more shared perspectives. Social worlds may be very small, such as a

therapy group, or very large, such as an entire academic field (e.g., counseling psychology).

Counseling psychology is actually a great example of a social world: while an organization (i.e.,

Division 17 of the American Psychological Association, Society for Counseling Psychology)

does represent and is constituted by counseling psychologists, counseling psychology as a field is

not equivalent to Division 17. In Strauss’s (1978) terms, then, we can conceptualize counseling

psychology as a social world defined by shared training experiences, epistemic and political
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 24

commitments, and areas of practice or social action (e.g., science, clinical work, teaching,

advocacy), even as there may be great diversity within the social world of counseling

psychology. Further, the boundaries of the group may change rapidly or very slowly, and its

membership may fluctuate. Similar to positional mapping described earlier, however, social

worlds are not simply aggregates of individuals who identify the same way; social worlds

represent collectivities with shared interests and action. So, while heterosexuals may not

represent a social group, straight allies might (Grzanka et al., 2015).

Arenas are made up of multiple social worlds and represent areas of debate and

contestation. Arenas are discursive sites insomuch as social worlds produce ideas about the arena

and about other social worlds. To continue the counseling psychology example, we might

consider master’s level credentialing and licensure to be an arena constituted by multiple

overlapping social worlds (e.g., counseling center staff, private practice clinicians, university

faculty, division leadership) and organizations, including the APA, Division 17, etc. These

various stakeholders both produce the terrain of the arena—debates over how to provide master’s

level psychology degrees that would enable licensure to practice therapy—and are influenced by

the arena. Clarke et al. (2018) provide extensive guidance on how to identify additional elements

of social worlds/arenas, but I will focus on here social worlds, arenas, and organizations.

Figure 4 represents one social worlds/arenas map I made in the WRAP to manage the

complex relationship between discourse about Hurricane Katrina and the artificially constructed

study environment in which I recruited survey respondents to participate in two in-depth

structured interviews about various topics related to race, gender, and sexuality. Recall that I

analyzed media texts in part 1 of the study; connecting the ideas in those texts to my participants

became a key part of my SA process, because I asked my respondents to respond to controversy


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 25

in these arenas and then explain to me how it is that they arrived at their conclusions about, for

example, guilt, collective responsibility, and ethics in the wake of Hurricane Katrina. My

participants did not represent a social world, because they were not a working together around a

shared commitment—they did not even know each other or interact like they would in a focus

group study. But that did not mean that other social worlds were not acting upon them. Based on

the data gathered as part of the mass media discourse analyses, I mapped “The Katrina Arena” as

constituted by seven social worlds: news media, activists, federal government (including

Congress and FEMA as organizations), local/state government, New Orleans residents, Gulf

Coast residents, and the audience (i.e., those outside the region not directly affected by the storm

but who watched its coverage). The participants (P1, P2, etc.) are located barely outside the

arena in the university, which is another organization. Note that all boundaries on the map are

purposefully permeable, because the actual boundaries of these worlds, arenas, and organizations

are mutable, fluid, and historically contingent.

A number of insights can be gleaned from this process, in which one should ask: who

makes up these worlds, and what do they want? The answers to these questions should be driven

by the data, not by normative assumptions or existing theory. That is why, for example, my

Katina map (Figure 4) does not exhaustively include all possibly relevant social worlds that

could have anything to do with Katrina. This Katrina map represents the data and boundaries of

my research project or, in SA terms, the situation. Surely, the historic disaster of Hurricane

Katrina cannot be contained in one map! But this map helps me to see social worlds relationally.

For example, the news media are positioned orthogonally to all the other social worlds and most

directly opposite the governmental social worlds whose failure to act after the storm exacerbated

the crisis. Earlier drafts of this map included a single world, “Area residents,” that was later
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 26

divided into two overlapping social worlds to better reflect racialized constructions of Black

New Orleans residents and White greater Gulf Coast region residents. Memoing is once again

essential to this process, which should involve memoing about individual social worlds’ work,

interactions between worlds, and the boundaries of the arena. Naming and articulating social

worlds’ various commitments helps specify what is actually happening in a given situation, as

opposed to tacit assumptions about social groups and organizations. This iterative process of

mapping, memoing, and re-mapping can play a key role in identifying gaps in the data, as well as

when saturation has been reached. A social worlds/arena map can help researchers think through

questions such as, “Do I need to know more about this social world? What kinds of data will

help me understand this social world(s) and its commitment(s)?” In a longitudinal or historical

project, multiple maps may be drawn to illustrate how the arena has changed over time, which

can facilitate comparative analysis. In any project, revisions to a map are essential as more data

is gathered and iterative analyses reveal new dynamics in the arena.

Finally, Clarke et al. (2018) note that not every SA project lends itself to social

worlds/arenas mapping, because not every arena has organized social worlds; they even offer the

example of a project in which individuals were acting, though no collectivity with a shared

commitment had yet formed. But I would caution readers from taking this caveat too seriously. I

certainly did not “see” these maps as evident at the start of the WRAP. Like all researchers, my

subjective assumptions, preliminary data collection, and prior research experiences informed my

initial understanding of the situation when I designed the WRAP, which originally focused on

White individuals’ intersectional experience of racial emotions. Rather than use established

theory to connect these emotional experiences to structural dynamics, social worlds/arenas

mapping encouraged me to yoke subjectivities to structures by way of empirical evidence.


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 27

Again, such data-driven analyses can enhance the fidelity of a given project by maintaining an

intimate relationship between researchers and their data (Levitt et al., 2017a). In sum,

challenging oneself to think through social worlds theory (Strauss, 1978) may help actualize

counseling psychologists’ ongoing aspirations to structural and systems-level analysis and

intervention (Mallinckrodt et al., 2014; Moradi & Grzanka, 2017; Shin et al., 2017), a point to

which I return in the conclusion.

A Critical-Cartographic Turn in Counseling Psychology?

In this paper, I offered a primer on SA as a potentially valuable tool for critical

qualitative inquiry in counseling psychology. I used the WRAP to demonstrate how three forms

of mapping—situational, positional, and social worlds/arenas—can facilitate data analysis and

theory-generation above and beyond traditional GT analysis, though not as a replacement for

GT. Explicit in this elaboration of SA is an argument that these analyses could help counseling

psychology researchers achieve goals that have been elaborated in the pages of this journal

(Moradi & Grzanka, 2017) and other leading venues (Hargons et al., 2017; Nadal, 2018), at

conferences (Jackson, 2019), and in other professional discourse about the future of the field

(Goodman et al., 2004). Borrowing from Clarke’s original framing, I conclude by discussing

how SA might usher a critical-cartographic turn in counseling psychology along at least four

axes of work that reflect central commitments of the field. I link the terms critical and

cartographic here to invoke a sensitivity to relations of power and inequality (i.e., critical) that is

informed by mapping the shape and structure of those power relations (i.e., cartographic).

Further, I suggest these axes were at least implicit in Haverkamp and colleagues’ 2005 special

issue, and I offer them as a provocations—not critiques—of that earlier, foundational thinking on

the qualitative question in counseling psychology.


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 28

First, the questions of systems-level analyses and systems-level advocacy in the interest

of social justice are hardly new concerns in counseling psychology (Speight & Vera, 2008; Vera

& Speight, 2003). But there has been much handwringing about how to do so, and at times it has

felt like we—including myself—have been guilty of making more “calls to action” than plans of

action when it comes to systems-level advocacy (Grzanka & Frantell, 2017). It has been over 10

years since the first APA accredited counseling psychology program adopted a scientist-

practitioner-advocate model (Mallinckrodt et al., 2014), and yet social justice training still

connotes a kind of vanguard in the field. SA is not an advocacy tool in and of itself, but I have

argued that it incites structural thinking at all stages of the research design process that

destabilize normative, “best” (but not unproblematic) research practices. By emphasizing the

situation rather than variables, SA encourages research design in structural relief. Clarke (2003,

2005) and colleagues (Clarke et al., 2018) have extensively detailed SA’s origins in critical

inquiry and its many political ambitions and influences, including feminist and antiracist

scholarship and social movements. Setting traditionally trained social scientists off to conduct

GT analyses can result in reductionist codes, over-simplifications, and a reliance on conceptual

parsimony and construct discreteness over and above accuracy, validity, and complex

representation of the empirical world (Clarke, 2005; Fassinger, 2005). Surely, consciousness-

raising and education in critical theories and perspectives is a prerequisite of actually conducting

critical research (see Levitt et al., in press). Though much GT research has produced critical

scholarly interventions, the mere practice of GT analysis will not, in and of itself, produce

structural analysis. SA will not automatically do so, either, but it can help researchers get there.

Sociologists, social workers, and community psychologists have been attending to science and

advocacy at the structural level for generations (e.g., Morris, 2015); drawing on these rich,
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 29

interdisciplinary knowledges and integrating structurally oriented methods will only help

counseling psychologists continue to turn attention toward justice, power, and inequity as matters

of social—rather than merely psychological—concern.

Second, based on my use of SA in projects that are informed by intersectionality theory, I

suggest that SA is a tool for “using intersectionality responsibly” as described by Moradi and

Grzanka (2017). Moradi and Grzanka called for a moratorium on the phrase “intersecting

identities” in counseling psychology, and they join a chorus of scholars who are concerned with

how some psychologists have implemented intersectionality as a theory of identities, rather than

a way of understanding and resisting systems of interlocking inequality (Carbado, 2013; Warner,

Settles, & Shields, 2018). SA resists identitarian thinking in all mapping stages by instead

sensitizing analyses to action in the situation and the organizational forms that are manifesting in

the data. Like SA, intersectional analyses are those that think relationally about categories and

situate social categories historically and contextually within systems of domination (Dill &

Kohlman, 2012; Shin et al., 2017). SA emphasizes co-constitution and co-production, two

related sociological concepts that describe how different things come to make each other up (i.e.,

social worlds and arenas) (Clarke, 2005). As a substitution for multiple identities, a phrase that

merely signals that individuals identify as more than one thing at the same time, co-constitution

can help researchers better understand how systems of domination are co-creating each other.

SA mapping procedures expose the social embeddedness and inter-connectedness of

psychological processes, power relationships, and systems of inequality. Whereas multiple

identities or intersectionality-lite (Grzanka & Miles, 2016) rhetoric (1) implies discrete identities

that come together in some individuals (but not others) and (2) denotes no specific critique of

power, SA maps visualize the co-creation of systems and subjects as various positions are taken
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 30

up in the data and various social worlds materialize. For example, in the WRAP, Cooper’s

gender performance and sexuality were implicated in the evaluation of his “emotional” coverage

of Katrina (e.g., he was “hysterical”), even though his then-undisclosed sexual orientation was

not named in the data. In other words, his social identity (as a not publicly out gay man) was not

particularly germane to the situation, insomuch as his critics did not invoke his sexual

orientation. Nonetheless, sexuality, gender, and race intersected to co-create the positions taken

up by powerful social actors to evaluate the legitimacy of his coverage. Understanding this

intersectional dynamic via positional mapping helped later in the project to see how my

interview participants articulated gendered and sexualized narratives of guilt and shame often

without ever saying anything about gender and sexual identities, per se.

Third, qualitative research in psychology (not just counseling) continues to wrestle with

the legacy of positivism and post-positivism (Bowleg, 2008; Ponterotto, 2005), and SA can help

reframe the conversation. Against accusations of methodological sloppiness, imprecision, non-

representative samples, and generally unscientific research practice, qualitative researchers have

spent a disproportionate amount of their time defending qualitative research. A defensive posture

has epistemic consequences, such as the ones detailed by Nash (2019) in her analyses of

defensiveness when it comes to the uses and perceived abuses of intersectionality. In the context

of qualitative inquiry in the social sciences, researchers have sometimes sacrificed some of the

most advantageous parts of qualitative design to conform to quantitative methodologies and

epistemic priorities, including quantifying data even within the context of qualitative research.

Limitations sections in qualitative manuscripts often practically apologize for the qualitative

nature of the study by positioning lack of generalizability—which is not a goal of most

qualitative research (Luker, 2008)—as a failure of the study. In the midst of ongoing professional
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 31

concerns about the reproducibility of findings produced by psychological science, open science

practices (e.g., preregistration of hypotheses and planned analyses, making datasets publicly

available) have been positioned as potential ways to enhance scientific rigor and accountability

in psychology (Nosek et al., 2015). While the open science movement has already produced

exciting developments across the discipline, open science practices have almost exclusively been

normed on quantitative methods practiced by exclusively quantitative researchers. Accordingly,

the open science movement will present major challenges to qualitative research in psychology,

as prominent qualitative methodologists have already elaborated (e.g., Syed, 2019).

Nevertheless, SA circumvents the quantitative/qualitative binary and its associated

dualisms (e.g., objective/subjective, essentialist/constructionist) by rejecting notions of

unmitigated scientific discovery (Clarke et al., 2018). As Clarke (2005) explained, the feminist

debates of the 1980s and 90s about science and empiricism buttress SA’s epistemic assumptions,

including the assertion that there are multiple, simultaneous truths about a given situation and

that it is researcher’s burden and opportunity to understand how these truths are made and

remade in relations of power. The debates over whether objectivity or truth exist come to seem a

bit tedious when positivism is thoroughly jettisoned for alternative, critical epistemologies

(Cauce, 2011; Collins, 2000; Haraway, 1988). Though unquestionably driven by data and

evidence, SA is rooted in a belief in the constructed nature of all empirical inquiry (cf. Gergen,

1973). Rather than view this a weakness or strength, SA takes social constructionism as self-

evident and then uses cartography as a heuristic to trace the shape of knowledge. Following

longstanding feminist reconfigurations of objectivity, Clarke (2005) reframed rigor as the extent

to which the researcher is accountable to their data, rather than the extent to which a finding is

able to be reproduced. Indeed, questions of reproducibility are complicated by SA (and other


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 32

forms of critical qualitative inquiry; see Levitt [2021]) because the researcher and/or research

team’s standpoint is so fundamental to the knowledge produced in the study. Accordingly, in

designing SA, Clarke (2005) implicitly challenged reproducibility’s status as a scientific virtue

by rejecting positivist fixations on objectivity and the stability of knowledge. As counseling

psychologists look back at the influence of Haverkamp, Morrow, and Ponterotto’s (2005) work

and forward to the next 15 years of qualitative inquiry, I suspect the intertwined methodological

and epistemic implications of SA are at least as promising and generative as SA’s specific

methods/procedures.

Finally, I suggest that SA has specific utility in the context of counseling psychology for

facilitating innovative research on clinical work as a situation of inquiry. The dominant

framework of “process and outcome” has produced decades of important research on clinical

interventions (Kivlighan et al., 2000; Heppner et al., 2015). But SA has the potential to animate

clinical inquiry with research questions including and beyond process and outcome, inputs and

outputs. SA can help map psychotherapy as a situation. In the context of the rise of evidence-

based practice and the implementation of newer multicultural approaches such as cultural

humility (Hook et al., 2017) and structural competency (Metzl & Hansen, 2014), how might SA

facilitate understanding the ways that training directors, staff psychologists, trainees, faculty, and

clients navigate novel approaches in institutional contexts? What positions are psychologists

taking up in these debates, and how are they influencing how we train future psychologists?

Counseling psychologists have stressed the importance of studying the shape of change using

advanced quantitative methods (Frankfurt et al., 2016; Miles & Paquin, 2014), and this work is

vital. I have offered an introduction of SA to invite counseling psychologists to consider what

other questions we might ask about psychotherapy, mental health, inequality, and social justice.
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 33

Fassinger (2005) wrote that constructivist GT might push the field closer than any other

method—qualitative or otherwise (e.g., Fassinger & Morrow, 2013)—toward the scientist-

practitioner-advocacy model (Fassinger & O’Brien, 2000; Mallinckrodt et al., 2014), because of

GT’s emphasis on the integration of theory and practice, or praxis. I recommend SA as another

tool with which to achieve this goal.

Certainly, SA is not appropriate for all studies and is not without its limitations. One

major concern of researchers may be the additional time and resources SA mapping adds to the

already arduous process of qualitative data analyses, which are often un- or under-funded.

However, Levitt et al.’s (2017a) recommendations for ensuring fidelity and utility in qualitative

research do not suggest that time in and of itself should be a criterion that influences which

analyses should be adopted. In quantitative research, statistical software and advances in

computing power have undoubtedly made some analyses faster to conduct, whereas these same

technological enhancements have added new, sometimes time-consuming steps that have

become increasingly expected and which help ensure the validity of statistical claims (e.g.,

advanced structural equation modeling, missing data imputation). On the other hand, just

because one has the ability to conduct a complex statistical procedure does not mean that a

simple ANOVA might not produce similar or identical conclusions with much greater parsimony

and speed. Likewise, the question of whether to use SA in a GT study should depend upon the

research question driving the study and the resources available to investigators, among a variety

of other serious practical concerns.

As I have stressed above, SA is well suited to research questions that concern a process

and for which explanatory frameworks and contextually driven theory are the study aims.

Researchers will need to weigh the benefits of these analyses when determining the aims of their
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 34

study, familiarity with the given data set or population, and the researchers’ own self-efficacy

when it comes to qualitative inquiry. For example, not all projects may be understood as

situational or particularly contextual, and the philosophical presumptions of SA may contradict

the epistemic orientation of a given project or group of researchers. While a novice or seasoned

qualitative researcher starting a new research project might find these procedures to be

particularly beneficial for critically orienting their analyses, others might find SA to have

diminishing returns or to be somewhat duplicative of other analytic procedures. In these cases,

SA may not be appropriate.

SA maps themselves bring with them a set of epistemic assumptions: that positions can

be located on bipolar continua in positional maps, and that empirical data in the situation can

generally be categorized into a set of prescribed categories that include nonhuman actants and

temporal elements, among others. As researchers adopt SA in fields and projects that take these

procedures farther away from their disciplinary origins in medical sociology and science and

technology studies, researchers will inevitably contest certain precepts in Clarke’s formulation of

SA and create new ways of thinking about and conducting SA. As researchers routinely invoke

having taken a “modified grounded theory” approach (see Levitt et al., 2017b), we might see

counseling psychologists take a “modified situational analysis approach.” I view this as an

opportunity for greater methodological innovation, rather than a limitation. As optimistic as

Fassinger was in 2005 about GT, I am equally excited about what 15 years of critical-

cartographic work might look like in counseling psychology, because of how this work might

create forms of science and justice that truly exceed our current imaginations.
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 35

References

Bowleg, L. (2008). When Black + lesbian + woman ≠ Black lesbian woman: The methodological

challenges of qualitative and quantitative intersectionality research. Sex Roles: A Journal

of Research, 59(5-6), 312–325. http://doi.org/10.1007/s11199- 008-9400-z

Burawoy, M. (1998). The extended case method. Sociological Theory, 16(1), 4–33.

https://doi.org/10.1111/0735-2751.00040

Burnes, T. R., Wood, J. M., Inman, J. L., & Welikson, G. A. (2013). An investigation of process

variables in feminist group clinical supervision. The Counseling Psychologist, 41(1), 86-

109. http://doi.org/10.1177/0011000012442653

Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006) Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in

Psychology, 3(2), 77-101. https://doi.org/10.1191/1478088706qp063oa

Bryant, A., & Charmaz, K. (Eds.) (2019). The SAGE handbook of current developments in

grounded theory (2nd ed.). SAGE.

Cauce, A. R. (2011). Is multicultural psychology a-scientific? Diverse methods for diversity

research. Cultural diversity and ethnic minority psychology, 17, 228-233.

https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023880

Charmaz, K. (2017). The power of constructivist grounded theory for critical inquiry. Qualitative

Inquiry, 23(1), 34-45. https://doi.org/ 10.1177/1077800416657105

Charmaz, K. (2014). Constructing grounded theory (2nd ed.). SAGE Publications.

Clarke, A. E. (2003). Situational analyses: Grounded theory mapping after the postmodern turn.

Symbolic Interaction, 26(4), 553-576. https://doi.org/10.1525/si.2003.26.4.553

Clarke, A. E. (2005). Situational analysis: Grounded theory after the postmodern turn. SAGE

Publications.
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 36

Clarke, A. E. (2012). Feminism, grounded theory, and situational analysis revisited. In S. N.

Hesse-Biber (Ed.), Handbook of Feminist Research: Theory & Praxis (2nd ed., pp. 388-

412). SAGE Publications.

Clarke, A. E., & Charmaz, K. (Eds.) (2014). Grounded Theory & Situational Analysis. SAGE.

Clarke, A. E., Friese, C., & Washburn, R. (Eds.). (2015). Situational analysis in practice:

Mapping research with grounded theory. Left Coast Press.

Clarke, A. E., Friese, C., & Washburn, R. S. (2018). Situational analysis: Grounded theory after

the interpretive turn (2nd ed.). SAGE Publications.

Cole, E. R., Avery, L. R., Dodson, C., & Goodman, K. D. (2012). Against nature: How

arguments about the naturalness of marriage privilege heterosexuality. Journal of Social

Issues, 68(1), 46–62. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2012.01735.x

Collins, P. H. (2000). Black feminist thought: Knowledge, consciousness, and the politics of

empowerment (2nd ed.). Routledge.

Corbin, J. M., & Strauss, A. (2014). Basic of qualitative research: Techniques and procedures

for developing grounded theory (4th ed.). SAGE.

Creswell, J. W., Hanson, W. E., Clark, V. L. P., & Morales, A. (2007). Qualitative research

designs: Selection and implementation. The Counseling Psychologist, 35(2), 236-264.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000006287390

Denzin, N. (2015). What is critical qualitative inquiry? In G. Cannella, M. Pérez, & P. Pasque

(Eds.), Critical qualitative inquiry: Foundations and futures (pp. 31-50). Left Coast

Press.

Denzin, N., & Lincoln, Y. S. (Eds.) (2011). The SAGE Handbook of Qualitative Research (4th

ed.). SAGE.
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 37

Espín, O. M. (1993). Giving voice to silence: The psychologist as witness. American

Psychologist, 44, 408-414.

Fassinger, R. E. (2005). Paradigms, praxis, problems, and promise: Grounded theory in

counseling psychology research. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52(2), 156-166.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.52.2.156

Fassinger, R. E., & Morrow, S. L. (2013). Toward best practices in quantitative, qualitative, and

mixed- method research: A social justice perspective. Journal for Social Action in

Counseling and Psychology, 5(2), 69-83.

Fassinger, R. E., & O’Brien, K. M. (2000). Career counseling with college women: A scientist-

practitioner-advocate model of intervention. In D. Luzzo (Ed.), Career development of

college students: Translating theory and research into practice (pp. 253–265).

Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Fine, M. (2012). Resuscitating critical psychology for “revolting” times. Journal of Social

Issues, 68(2), 416-438. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2012.01756.x

Fine, M. (2018). Just research in contentious times: Widening the methodological imagination.

Teachers College Press.

Frankfurt, S., Frazier, P., Syed, M., & Jung, K. R. (2016). Using group-based trajectory and

growth mixture modeling to identify change trajectories. The Counseling Psychologist,

44(5), 622-660. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000016658097

Fuks, N., Smith, N. G., Peláez, S., De Stefano, J., & Brown, T. L. (2018). Acculturation

experiences among lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender immigrants in Canada. The

Counseling Psychologist, 46(3), 296-332. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000018768538


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 38

Gergen, K. J. (1973). Social psychology as history. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 26(2), 309-320.

Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1965). Awareness of dying. AldineTransaction.

Glaser, B. G. & A. L. Strauss. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory: Strategies for

qualitative research. AldineTransaction.

Giorgi, A. (Ed.). (1985). Phenomenology and psychological research. Duquesne University

Press.

Goodman, L.A., Liang, B., Helms, J.E., Latta, R.E., Sparks, E., & Weintraub, S.R. (2004).

Training counseling psychologists as social justice agents: Feminist and multicultural

principles in action. The Counseling Psychologist, 32(6), 793-837.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000004268802

Grzanka, P. R. (2010). White guilt: Race, gender, sexuality, and emergent racisms in the

contemporary United States (Publication No. 3409581) [Doctoral dissertation, University

of Maryland]. ProQuest Dissertations Publishing.

Grzanka, P. R. (2016). Queer survey research and the ontological dimensions of heterosexism.

WSQ: Women’s Studies Quarterly, 44(3-4), 131-149. http://doi:10.1353/wsq.2016.0039

Grzanka, P. R. (2017). Undoing the psychology of gender: Intersectional feminism and social

science pedagogy. In K. A. Case (Ed.), Intersectional pedagogy: A model for

complicating identity and social justice (pp. 61-79). Routledge.

Grzanka, P. R., Adler, J., & Blazer, J. (2015). Making up allies: The identity choreography of

straight LGBT activism. Sexuality Research and Social Policy, 12(3), 165–181.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s13178-014-0179-0
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 39

Grzanka, P. R., DeVore, E. N., Frantell, K. A., Miles, J. R., & Spengler, E. S. (2019).

Conscience clauses and sexual and gender minority mental health care: A case study.

Journal of Counseling Psychology. Advance online publication.

https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000396

Grzanka, P. R., & Maher, J. T. (2012). Different, like everyone else: Stuff White People Like

and the marketplace of diversity. Symbolic Interaction, 35, 368-393.

http://doi.org/10.1002/symb.24

Grzanka, P. R., & Miles, J. R. (2016). The problem with the phrase “intersecting identities”:

LGBT affirmative therapy, intersectionality, and neoliberalism. Sexuality Research and

Social Policy, 13(4), 371–389. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13178-016-0240-2

Grzanka, P. R., & Frantell, K. A. (2017). Counseling psychology and reproductive justice: A call

to action. The Counseling Psychologist, 45, 326-352.

http://doi.org/10.1177/0011000017699871

Grzanka, P. R., Frantell, K. A., & Fassinger, R. E. (2020). The White Racial Affect Scale

(WRAS): A measure of White guilt, shame, and negation. The Counseling Psychologist,

48(1), 47-77. http://doi.org/10.1177/0011000019878808

Grzanka, P. R., & Schuch, E. (2020). Reproductive anxiety and conditional agency at the

intersections of privilege: A focus group study of emerging adults’ perception of long-

acting reversible contraception. Journal of Social Issues. Advance online publication.

http://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12363

Guetterman, T. C., Babchuk, W. A., Howell Smith, M. C., & Stevens, J. (2019). Contemporary

approaches to mixed methods-grounded theory research: A field-based analysis. Journal

of Mixed Methods Research, 13(2), 179-195. https://doi.org/10.1177/1558689817710877


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 40

Haraway, D. (1988). Situated knowledges: The science question in feminism and the privilege of

partial perspective. Feminist Studies, 14, 575-599.

Harding, S. (Ed.) (1987). Feminism and methodology: Social science issues. Indiana University

Press.

Hargons, C., Mosley, D., Falconer, J., Faloughi, R. F., Singh, A., Stevens-Watkins, D., Cokley,

K. (2017). Black lives matter: A call to action for counseling psychology leaders. The

Counseling Psychologist, 45(6), 873-901. https://doi.org/10.1177/2F0011000017733048

Haverkamp, B. E. , Morrow, S. L. , & Ponterotto, J. G. (2005). A time and place for qualitative

and mixed methods in counseling psychology research. Journal of Counseling

Psychology, 52(2), 123-125. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.52.2.123

Hesse-Biber, S., & Piatelli, D. (2012). The feminist practice of holistic reflexivity. In S. N.

Hesse-Biber (Ed.), The handbook of feminist re- search: Theory and praxis (2nd edition,

pp. 575–582). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Hill, C. E., Knox, S., Thompson, B. J., Williams, E. N., Hess, S. A. (2005). Consensual

qualitative research: An update. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52(2): 196-205.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.52.2.19

Inman, A. G., Tummala-Narra, P., Kaduvettoor-Davidson, A., Alvarez, A. N., & Yeh, C. J.

(2015). Perceptions of race-based discrimination among first-generation Asian Indians in

the United States. The Counseling Psychologist, 43(2), 217-247.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000014566992
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 41

Jackson, S. (Chair). (2019, August). Beyond the buzzword: What (really) is "social justice" and

what does it mean for psychology? Symposium presented at the annual convention of the

American Psychological Association, Chicago, IL.

Kenny, M., & Fourie, R. (2014). Tracing the history of grounded theory methodology: From

formation to fragmentation. The Qualitative Report, 19(52), 1-9. Retrieved from

https://nsuworks.nova.edu/tqr/vol19/iss52/16

Kidd, S. A. , & Kral, M. J. (2005). Practicing participatory action research. Journal of

Counseling Psychology, 52(2), 187-195. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.52.2.187

Kivlighan, D. M., Jr., Coleman, M. N., & Anderson, D. C. (2000). Process, outcome, and

methodology in group counseling research. In S. D. Brown & R. W. Lent

(Eds.), Handbook of counseling psychology (p. 767–796). John Wiley & Sons Inc.

Latta, R. E., & Goodman, L. A. (2011). Intervening in partner violence against women: A

grounded theory exploration of informal network members’ experiences. The Counseling

Psychologist, 39(7), 973-1023. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000011398504

Levitt, H. M. (2021). Essentials of critical-constructivist grounded theory methods. In book

series, Introduction to Qualitative Methods (Series Eds., C. E. Hill & S. Knox).

Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Levitt, H. M., Morrill, Z., Collins, K. M., & Rizo, J. L. (In press). The methodological integrity

of critical qualitative research: Principles to support design and research review. Journal

of Counseling Psychology.

Levitt, H. M., Motulsky, S. L., Wertz, F. J., Morrow, S. L., & Ponterotto, J. G. (2017a).

Recommendations for designing and reviewing qualitative research in psychology.


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 42

Promoting methodological integrity. Qualitative Psychology, 4(1), 2-22.

https://doi.org/10.1037/qup0000082

Levitt, H. M., Pomerville, A., Surace, F. I., Grabowski, L. M. (2017b). Metamethod study of

qualitative psychotherapy research on clients’ experiences: Review and

recommendations. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 64(6), 626-644.

https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000222

Luker, K. (2008). Salsa dancing into the social sciences: Research in an age of info-glut.

Harvard University Press.

Mallinckrodt, B., Miles, J. R., & Levy, J. J. (2014). The scientist-practitioner-advocate model:

Addressing contemporary training needs for social justice advocacy. Training and

Education in Professional Psychology, 8(4), 303–311.

https://doi.org/10.1037/tep0000045

Metzl, J. M., & Hansen, H. (2014). Structural competency: Theorizing a new medical

engagement with stigma and inequality. Social Science & Medicine, 103, 126– 133.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.06.032

Miles, J. R., & Paquin, J. D. (2014). Best practices in group counseling and psychotherapy

research. In J. L. Delucia-Waack, C. R. Kalodner, & M. Riva (Eds.), The handbook of

group counseling and psychotherapy (2nd ed.) (pp. 178-192). SAGE.

Mills, J., Bonner, A., & Francis, K. (2006). The development of constructivist grounded theory.

International Journal of Qualitative Methods, 5(1), 25-35.

Moradi, B., & Grzanka, P. R. (2017). Using intersectionality responsibly: Toward critical

epistemology, structural analysis, and social justice activism. Journal of Counseling

Psychology, 64(5), 500–513. https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000203


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 43

Moradi, B., Tebbe, E. A., Connelly, K. E., Lenzen, A. L., & Flores, M. J. (2019). Objectification

of sexual minority women: Responses, consequences, and resistance. Journal of

Counseling Psychology, 66(5), 519–533. https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000348

Morris, A. D. (2015). The scholar denied: W. E. B. DuBois and the birth of modern sociology.

University of California Press.

Morrow, S. L. (2005). Quality and trustworthiness in qualitative research in counseling

psychology. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52(2), 250– 260.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.52.2.250

Nadal, K. L. (2017). “Let’s get in formation”: On becoming a psychologist-activist in the 21st

century. American Psychologist, 72(9), 935-946. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000212

Nash, J. C. (2019). Black feminism reimagined: After intersectionality. Duke University Press.

Nosek, B. A., Alter, G., Banks, G. C., Borsboom, D., Bowman, S. D., Breckler, S.

J., … Yarkoni, T.(2015). Promoting an open research culture. Science, 348, 1422–1425.

https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aab2374

Pérez, M. S., & Cannella, G. S. (2013). Situational analysis as an avenue for critical qualitative

research: Mapping post-Katrina. Qualitative Inquiry, 19(7), 505-517.

https://doi.org/10.1177/1077800413489514

Pope-Davis, D. B., Toporek, R. L., Ortega-Villalobos, L., Ligiéro, D. P., Brittan-Powell, C.

S.,…& Liang, C. T. H. (2002). Client perspectives of multicultural counseling

competence: A qualitative examination. The Counseling Psychologist, 30(3), 355-393.

https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000002303001
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 44

Ponterotto, J. G. (2005). Qualitative research in counseling psychology: A primer on research

paradigms and philosophy of science. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52(2), 126-136.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.52.2.126

Poulin, K. L. (2007). Teaching qualitative research: Lessons from practice. The Counseling

Psychologist, 35(3), 431-458. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000006294813

Prilletensky, I., & Nelson, G. (1997). Community psychology: Reclaiming social justice. In D.

Fox and I. Prilletensky (Eds.), Critical psychology: An introduction (pp. 166-184).

SAGE.

Rennie, D. L., Phillips, J. R., Quartaro, G. K. (1989). Grounded theory: A promising approach to

conceptualization in psychology? Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 29(2),

139-150. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0079765

Shin, R. Q., Welch, J. C., Kaya, A. E., Yeung, J. G., Obana, C., Sharma, R., . . . Yee, S. (2017).

The intersectionality framework and identity intersections in the Journal of Counseling

Psychology and The Counseling Psychologist: A content analysis. Journal of Counseling

Psychology, 64(5), 458–474. doi:10.1037/cou0000204

Spanierman, L. B., & Poteat, V. P. (2005). Moving beyond complacency to commitment:

Multicultural research in counseling psychology. The Counseling Psychologist, 33(4),

513–523. doi:10.1177/0011000005276469

Speight, S. L., & Vera, E. M. (2008). Social justice and counseling psychology: A challenge to

the profession. In S. D. Brown, & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Handbook of counseling psychology

(4th ed., pp. 54–67). New York, NY: Wiley.

Stake, R. E. (1995). The art of case study research. SAGE.


SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 45

Strauss, A. L. (1978). A Social Worlds Perspective. In N. K. Denzin (Ed.), Studies in Symbolic

Interaction, 1(1), 119-128.

Syed, M. (2019). The open science movement is for all of us. Pre-print. Retrieved from

https://psyarxiv.com/cteyb

Timmermans, S., & Tavory, S. (2012). Theory construction in qualitative research: From

grounded theory to abductive analysis. Sociological Theory, 30(3), 167-186.

https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0735275112457914

Vera, E. M., & Speight, S. L. (2003). Multicultural competence, social justice, and counseling

psychology: Expanding our roles. The Counseling Psychologist, 31(3), 253–272.

doi:10.1177/0011000003031003001

Warner, L. R., Settles, I. H., & Shields, S. A. (2018). Intersectionality theory in the psychology

of women. In C. B. Travis, J. W. White, A. Rutherford, W. S. Williams, S. L. Cook, & K.

F. Wyche (Eds.), APA handbook of the psychology of women: History, theory, and

battlegrounds (pp. 521–539). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Yeh, C. J., & Inman, A. G. (2007). Qualitative data analysis and interpretation in counseling

psychology: Strategies for best practices. The Counseling Psychologist, 35(3), 369–

403. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000006292596

Zeiders, K. H., Roosa, M. W., Knight, G. P., & Gonzales, N. A. (2013). Mexican American

adolescents’ profiles of risk and mental health: A person-centered longitudinal approach.

Journal of Adolescence, 36(3), 603–612.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2013.03.014
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 46

Figure 1
Situational Map (Ordered), White Racial Affect Project (WRAP)

Individual Human Elements/Actors


Undergraduate students at the University
Me, as researcher
Celebrities and popular culture icons, specifically Anderson Cooper; Don Imus, Al Sharpton,
Rutgers Women’s basketball team; Isaiah Washington, Ellen DeGeneres, T.R. Knight
and the individual characters on Grey’s Anatomy

Collective Human Elements/Actors


Classrooms of sample participants
The University’s student body
White people
Racial minorities, especially Black Americans
Americans (generally defined)
Television consumers

Discursive Constructions of Individual and/or Collective Human Actors


White people as racist, ignorant and defensive – White stereotypes
Black people as angry – Black stereotypes
Racial identity no longer matters
Gendered racial dynamics
Sexualized racial dynamics
Classed racial dynamics
Gendered racism
The University as a predominantly White institution (PWI)
The University as a diverse and multicultural institution
Americans as multicultural melting-pot and/or colorblind society
Americans as racist
Television consumers as uncritical consumers (viewing habits)
Television consumers as active viewers (viewing habits)

Political/Economic Elements
Increased diversification of colleges/universities
Controversy over televisual representations -- the politics of representation
Controversy over affirmative action and race-based public policies
Colorblind ideology/racism and modern racisms
Multiculturalism
Liberal individualism
Neoliberalism
Neoconservatism
White guilt as popular culture concept
White guilt and White shame as psychosocial/academic concepts

Temporal Elements
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 47

History of U.S. racism


Contemporary U.S. racism
Academic calendar
Individual lifespans
Early adult developmental trajectory and identity enactment

Major Issues/Debates (Usually Contested)


Meanings/consequences of race and dimensions of difference (individual and collective)
Melting pot versus salad versus separatist ideologies
Individual and collective feelings of guilt and responsibility for racism
Whiteness as differentially experienced according to gender and sexuality (and other dimensions
of difference, especially class, religion, ability) [Intersectionality]
Race matters (i.e., anti-colorblind racial ideology, racial consciousness)

Nonhuman Elements/Actants
Particular course syllabi
Contemporary accounts/histories of racial politics
The University and its surrounding community
Communities away from campus (“home”)
Skin color – racial phenotype
Sexual orientation identities
Gender identities

Implicated/Silent Actors/Actants
Friends and families of students
Communities of practice and identity-based groups
Psychological and psychosocial constructs (e.g. collective guilt, shame, guilt, collective self-
esteem, social dominance orientation, racial/gender/sexuality identity salience and
valence, White guilt, White shame, perception of racism in TV)
Institutional racism, heterosexism and sexism
Politics of televisual raced, gendered and sexualized representation
Social desirability

Temporal Events in Situation


Survey administration
Interview(s)
2008 Presidential Election

Discursive Constructions of Nonhuman Actants


Concepts of race, gender, sexuality and other dimensions of difference
Constructions of feminism, anti-racism, racism, progressive and conservative politics
Individualism versus collectivism
Neoliberal ideology
Dominant televisual culture
Popular conceptions of guilt, shame, privilege (both individual and collective)
Imus scandal
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 48

Hurricane Katrina
Multicultural television
African American “homophobia”
New Orleans

Sociocultural/Symbolic Elements
Signifiers of race, gender, sexuality (and other dimensions of difference)
Popular mass culture, particular entertainment and television culture
Popular news/journalism
Campus culture – representations of “diversity” and “multiculturalism”

Spatial Elements
Local variations of background (who/where you live)
Regional backgrounds (where you come from)
Access to television and other forms of mass-media

Related Discourses (Historical, Narrative, and/or Visual)


Postracial America
Affirmative action
Post-feminism
Post-gay discourse

Note. Clarke et al. (2018) suggest, when necessary, to add a category for “Other Kinds of
Elements” as found in the situation. For more examples, see
http://clarkessituationalanalysis.blogspot.com.
Running head: SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 49

Figure 2
Situational Map (Messy) with Relational Mapping, White Racial Affect Project (WRAP)

Note. The lines, which are added during relational mapping, specify empirically observed relationships between elements in the
situation. Each of these lines would constitute at least a single memo’s worth of writing and analysis.
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 50

Figure 3
Positional Maps, White Racial Affect Project (WRAP)

Note. Each map represents media coverage of: Anderson Cooper’s coverage of Hurricane Katrina (2005); Isaiah Washington’s firing
from series Grey’s Anatomy (2007); Don Imus’s insulting the Rutgers University women’s basketball team (2007).
SITUATIONAL ANALYSIS 51

Figure 4
Social Worlds/Arenas Map, White Racial Affect Project (WRAP)

Note. This map integrates data from across all the qualitative components in the WRAP, including critical discourse analysis of media
coverage of Hurricane Katrina and interviews with White college students about their political attitudes. P=Participant.

View publication stats

You might also like