K Cognitive and Social Processes in Early

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 17

The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

The Leadership Quarterly


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/leaqua

Full length article

“I want to be the line leader!” Cognitive and social processes in early


leader development
Jessie A. Cannon *, Stephen J. Zaccaro , Thalia R. Goldstein
Department of Psychology, George Mason University, USA

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: The field of leader development has recently begun to focus more on the role of pre-adult leadership experiences
Agency in shaping leader development. However, research has largely neglected to account for children’s and adoles­
Leader development cents’ agency in shaping their own leader development, instead focusing on external drivers of such development
Children
(e.g., parents, schools). This integrative conceptual article provides a model for leader development from
Adolescents
childhood through adolescence, drawing on insights from the cognitive and social child development literature.
Leader mindset
Socio-cognitive skills This model focuses on the reciprocal influences of agency, early leadership experiences, and foundational socio-
cognitive skills, including theory of mind, metacognition, self-regulation, and autobiographical reasoning, to
foster growth and complexity in leadership skills and mindsets. In addition, the enabling forces that influence the
early development and expression of agency, socio-cognitive skills, and leader mindsets are described.

Introduction Child leader development is undoubtedly shaped by external forces.


However, children can and do make conscious decisions to claim lead­
Leader development is a relatively new and growing area of research ership (DeRue & Ashford, 2010b) as well as to actively reflect upon and
(Day et al., 2014). Regarding leader development across the lifespan, therefore grow from such experiences (Nesbit, 2012). While this process
several theoretical and empirical studies have highlighted the impor­ has been described for adult leaders (Day et al., 2009; DeRue & Ashford,
tance of childhood and adolescent leadership experiences and early 2010a), we argue that it begins earlier in one’s lifespan as the claiming
leader skill development. For example, Murphy and Johnson (2011), Liu of leadership roles can appear even in preschoolers (“I want to be the
et al. (2021) and Eva et al. (2021) described how particular child and line leader!”). Indeed, evidence has shown that preschool-age children
adolescent leadership experiences can lead to the growth of early leader recognize (Margoni et al., 2018) and claim leadership roles at least as
identities and leadership skills. In addition, longitudinal studies have early as age 4 (Chen & Kacerek, 2022). Thus, just as agency is a core
linked adult leadership outcomes to childhood experiences that tenet of adult leader development, it is a central aspect of children’s
contribute to leadership-relevant skill development (e.g., Reitan & leader development.
Stenberg, 2019; Strain & Webber, 2017). These studies suggest that to Current models of youth leader development have briefly alluded to
deepen our understanding of leader development, researchers should agency. For example, Eva et al. (2021) noted that, “…by exercising
focus more closely on experiences and processes occurring in childhood agency in programs, adolescent girls can develop a deeper level of skills
that can fuel such growth. While these and other important empirical and abilities than those who attend a prescribed leader development
and theoretical contributions have furthered our understanding of program” (p. 12). Liu et al. (2021) argued that individuals only express
childhood leader development (e.g., Kudo et al., 2012; Liu et al., 2019; agency in their own development once they reach adulthood (age
Meyer & Rinn, 2021), this literature tends to portray children as passive 18–30), noting that “at this stage, individuals start to make decisions by
recipients of leadership learning – they “are given” potential leadership themselves in the face of various opportunities” (p. 8). We argue that
development experiences by others (e.g., parents, teacher, coaches), or this process begins much earlier in development, as early as preschool.
they engage in such experiences only as prescribed by others. Accord­ In developing this argument, we build on prior work on leadership
ingly, the extant literature has not fully captured the role of a child’s development. However, we add to this work in several ways to describe
agency in personal leader development. agency expression and its connection to leadership development

* Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, George Mason University, 4400 University Drive, 3F5, Fairfax, VA 22030, USA.
E-mail address: jcannon5@gmu.edu (J.A. Cannon).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2023.101757
Received 3 June 2022; Received in revised form 21 November 2023; Accepted 3 December 2023
1048-9843/© 2023 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article as: Jessie A. Cannon et al., The Leadership Quarterly, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2023.101757
J.A. Cannon et al. The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx

experiences, socio-cognitive skills and critical enablers and inhibitors of attributes that interact with early experiences to foster growth in more
child leader growth. First, as noted, we emphasize the role of a child’s proximal leadership capacities and mindsets. Many studies from the
agency more so, and in different ways, than in previous conceptual child development literature reviewed in later sections of this article
frameworks. Specifically, agency in the context of early leader devel­ show how these skills derive from the child’s early social experiences
opment refers to the child’s act of choosing what leadership develop­ and in turn help shape and enrich future social and cognitive develop­
ment experiences in which to engage, how to engage in leadership mental experiences. We apply these insights from this literature in our
within a role, and/or how to frame leadership development experiences model of child leader development.
when reflecting on one’s leadership identity. Thus, this agency is critical Finally, we contribute to the literature on child leader development
not only for children accruing early leadership development experi­ by applying and building upon existing models of leader developmental
ences, but also for prompting initiative to reflect, however naively, on complexity (Day & Lance, 2004; Lord & Hall, 2005). Lord and Hall
these experiences and seek meaning about them from others (e.g., par­ (2005) described the developmental trajectory of leaders as moving
ents, teachers, etc.). Further, the child development literature has long from novice to expert skill levels, along with concomitant changes in
recognized children’s agency in their own development and learning. leader identity. These authors also noted that while the leadership skills
For example, “In most kindergarten contexts, young children are of novices reflect surface-level generic behaviors and individual leader
perceived as active agents who learn about themselves and the social identities, intermediate-level skills entail aligning leadership behaviors
world through their participation in social situations in different insti­ to domain and situational cues. Also, leader identities become more
tutional contexts” (Hedegaard, 2020, p. 48). Other well-known child relational. At the expert level, leaders rely even more on situational
development researchers have historically taken similar approaches (e. understanding and alignment, more underlying principles, and more
g., Hedegaard, 2020; Piaget, 1950; Sameroff, 1975; Vygotsky, 1998). We value-based collective identities (Lord & Hall, 2005). In this article we
note that while early expressions of agency are not as well-informed, this argue that such development is cyclical and occurs throughout child­
foundational ability has been found to emerge at a young age (e.g., hood and adolescence, where children evolve the complexity of their
Recchia, 2011) and to develop into progressively more sophisticated leader skillsets and mindsets, by proactively engaging in leadership
expressions throughout childhood and adolescence (e.g., Schwartz et al., development experiences (agency) and leveraging the aforementioned
2005). socio-cognitive skills as well as external enablers to foster growth in such
Second, in line with other researchers (Liu et al., 2021; McCauley complexity. We utilize research from the child and adult development
et al., 2014), we emphasize the critical role of early leadership experi­ literature, particularly regarding constructivism (Carey et al., 2015;
ences in fostering learning and development. As children claim the Kegan, 1980) and the development of executive function complexity
leadership role, their early struggles, successes, and failures foster the (Jacques & Marcovitch, 2010; Zelazo, 2015) to support this argument.
emergence of socio-cognitive skills and self-beliefs that in turn drive
engagement in future leadership experiences. These experiences can be
Model overview
characterized as early leadership emergence, enactment, effectiveness
(or ineffectiveness), and/or leader development activities. Given a
Our conceptual model of child leader development is shown in Fig. 1.
young child’s lack of experience with leadership, we would argue that
While we utilize themes from prior models of child leader development
any expression of early leadership claiming is developmental in nature.
(Eva et al., 2021; Liu et al., 2021; Murphy & Johnson, 2011), our key
Subsequent leadership development experiences, even after children
point of departure is that our model is grounded in children’s agency in
have gained some rudimentary leadership skills, serve to “stamp-in” or
(a) claiming and enacting leadership, and (b) reflecting on these
routinize these skills (Zaccaro & Banks, 2004). Moreover, the child’s
enactment experiences to foster subsequent growth in core leader socio-
agency should push them to seek more challenging experiences that can
cognitive skills and mindsets. Our theoretical model builds on this
foster further growth (McCauley, et al., 1994; Van Velsor & McCauley,
concept of agency to define a co-evolutionary process of youth leader
2004). Thus, in our framework, we include early leader enactment and
development.
emergence in our references to leadership development experiences.
As children engage in leadership development experiences, they
Third, we contribute to the literature of early leader development by
develop and refine socio-cognitive skills (e.g., Deutsch et al., 2017;
incorporating key insights from the child development literature
Hansen et al., 2018). These skills provide a foundation for learning from
regarding the role of agency in child leader development and the key
future leadership development experiences, for the emergence of more
socio-cognitive skills that facilitate growth in leadership skillsets and
specific and proximal leadership capacities (Zaccaro, 2007; Zaccaro
mindsets. While research on leadership development has been useful in
et al., 2018), and for the evolution of leader mindsets. Our specification
understanding some of the processes that drive child leadership learning
of these particular skills is driven both by studies in the child develop­
(Liu et al., 2021; Murphy & Johnson, 2011), the literature on child
ment literature and on a definition of leadership as goal-directed social
development in general, and particularly on the growth of socio-
influence to instigate collective action (c.f. Chemers, 1997). When acting as
cognitive skills and self-identity, should greatly enhance this under­
leaders, children engage in goal-directed activity (a form of agency,
standing. Indeed, Murphy and Johnson (2011, p. 468) called for such an
Hannah et al., 2012) such that choice is exercised through setting and
integration of child development and leader development frameworks,
pursuing collective goals. Also, leadership is a social influence process.
noting “developmental psychologists, and others who research children,
Leader agency is functionally directed toward motivating and influ­
could offer valuable insights” to the study of leader development.
encing others, generating endorsement of and commitment to goal-
Fourth, we also add to the child leader development literature by
directed activities (Day & Dragoni, 2015). Moreover, to develop as a
focusing on four core socio-cognitive skills that reflect key precursor
leader and effectively practice leadership requires children to have at
processes (Parkhill et al., 2018) that contribute to early leader devel­
least a fundamental understanding1 of their own and others’ agency, and
opment: theory of mind, metacognition, self-regulation, and autobio­
the ability to interact with and influence others in social settings.
graphical reasoning. Parkhill et al. described as precursors to adolescent
leadership certain skills and assets such as “sense of identity, belonging,
self-efficacy, and social skills [as well as] an ability to adapt flexibly to 1
When researchers refer to children’s “understanding” of concepts, this can
one’s environment and circumstances” (p. 376). Here, we extend this
have various meanings. For example, 5-year-olds communicate their expecta­
concept to early child leader development, arguing that the four socio- tions about leaders through expressing expectations of what a leader should do
cognitive skills are critical to the development of more specific leader­ in a given situation (Stavans & Diesendruck, 2021). See Carey (2009) for an in-
ship skills and mindsets. Like the developmental framework proposed by depth review of how concepts are understood from infancy through early
Zaccaro et al., (2018; see Fig. 1), we define these as foundational childhood.

2
J.A. Cannon et al. The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx

Fig. 1. Conceptual Model of the Pre-Adult Leadership Development Process


Note: This model depicts the reciprocal relationships between socio-cognitive skills, experiences, and leader mindsets and how they contribute to early leadership
development. Actively seeking out and participating in leadership experiences enables skill development, while these experiences also help youth expand their leader
mindset, which in turn motivates further engagement in experiences. These relationships are driven both by the individual’s agency and by outside influences.

Four socio-cognitive skills are critical to (a) the growth of such un­ Hannah et al., 2013).
derstanding, (b) the effective expression of leadership agency and (c) the Kegan’s (1980) constructive-developmental theory provides an
growth of more complex leader skillsets and mindsets. Theory of mind explanation of the role of agency, leadership development experiences,
enables an understanding of others’ mental states and how they can lead and socio-cognitive skills in facilitating growth in cognitive complexity.
to actions. This capacity has been linked to the ability to influence and He suggested that people actively construct meaning to understand
have good relationships with peers (Peterson et al., 2016), which relates themselves and the world. As people develop, he suggested that each
to both the goal-directed and social influence aspects of leadership. subsequent “level” transcends and includes the previous level. Because
Metacognitive skill enables reflection and understanding about how of this inclusion, the later-stage “cognitive frames” are more complex
mental states lead to intentional actions and has been closely linked to than previous ones. Similarly, theories of cognitive development have
perceptions of one’s own agency (Marulis et al., 2020). Self-regulation is proposed that growth in executive functioning and cognitive complexity
critical for monitoring and regulating one’s own behavior and emotions occurs through reflection, involving iterative reprocessing and consid­
in pursuit of a goal (Montroy et al., 2016). Finally, autobiographical eration of context (Zelazo, 2015). This reflection process and resultant
reasoning enables youth to express agency in their own leadership growth in cognitive complexity enables more effective learning, adap­
development, pursuing and shaping their life trajectories in intentional tation, and goal-directed behavior (Karr et al., 2022).
ways (Van Doeselaar et al., 2020). Such reasoning is critical for the Finally, our model specifies several developmental enablers and in­
evolution of leader mindsets, particularly leader identity. These socio- hibitors that influence each element driving the growth of complex
cognitive skills emerge and grow over time in a dynamic, interdepen­ leader skillsets and mindsets. For example, particular individual differ­
dent process (Immordino-Yang et al., 2019). This maturation is ences in temperament as well parental patterns of behavior can deter­
demonstrated by increasing complexity in leader skillsets and mindsets. mine the degree and direction of children’s agency expression.
The core of the model reflects a co-evolutional developmental spiral Socioeconomic status, cultural norms, and non-parental role models can
driven and sustained by the child’s and adolescent’s agency. Leadership also influence such expression. Similarly, interactions with parents,
researchers have also utilized the idea of co-evolutionary growth spirals peers, and mentors can influence socio-cognitive skill development.
to describe leader development over time. For example, Day et al. These interactions, occurring through leadership development experi­
(2009) proposed a mutually reinforcing spiral for leader development, ences in which children and adolescents can practice leadership be­
asserting that the development of leadership competencies strengthens haviors, also contribute to increased complexity of leadership skillsets
leader identity. This in turn motivates an individual to participate in and mindsets.
leadership development experiences, thus fostering competency devel­ In the remaining sections of this article, we elucidate each element of
opment and further leader identity growth. Similarly, we propose that our model, including its key developmental enablers and inhibitors. We
the child’s expression of agency leads to engagement of early leadership review and cite research from both the leadership and child develop­
development experiences; these experiences provide the basis for the ment literature to support the role of each element in our model in the
emergence and practice of the four socio-cognitive skills and mindsets; growth of children’s complex leader skillsets and mindsets.
these skills and mindsets foster more experience-seeking and thus, the
evolution of (a) more complex understandings of leadership skills and Method of review
their application across different situations, and (b) more complex,
differentiated, and principle-based leader mindsets (Lord & Hall, 2005; Our intent in this article is not to provide a comprehensive literature

3
J.A. Cannon et al. The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx

review or a description of the “state of the field” of childhood leadership Table 1


development. Others have recently provided such reviews (Eva et al., List of articles found across all literature searches.3
2021; Liu et al., 2021). Instead, we conducted focused literature reviews Topic Number of
around the key constructs in our model and their proposed relationships. articles
We next describe our methods for conducting these reviews. Socio-cognitive skills and cognitive development 50
The premise and starting point of this article is that children are Play 19
active agents in their own leadership development. Based on our initial Attachment and parents 17
review of key papers on this subject (e.g., Liu et al., 2021; Murphy & Mentors and role models 15
Extracurricular activities and leadership programs 45
Johnson, 2011) we identified agency as an important conceptual gap in Identity: leader identity and autobiographical reasoning 38
the literature. Our iterative process of searching for related and relevant Sports 7
articles began with a review of existing conceptual articles on leadership Children’s views of leadership, implicit leadership views, 18
and leader development in children to determine the degree to which schemas
Social intelligence and theory of mind 43
agency was represented in these frameworks (e.g., articles from lead­
Agency 16
ership and development literatures). Then, we conducted a deeper Brain, genetics, and other individual differences 6
search for research on leader development before adulthood in the Leadership self-efficacy and motivation to lead 6
EBSCO and Google Scholar databases, using the search terms (leader OR Negative life events 6
leadership) AND (child OR children OR youth OR adolescent OR teen OR Gender 4
Leader behavior in childhood/adolescence 15
toddler OR “young adult” OR preschool OR kindergarten OR elementary) as Media and technology 6
well as conducting forward and backward searches of key articles and Classrooms 2
using the “related articles” tool in Google Scholar. Essentialism 6
We then reviewed key articles and books on the nature of agency in Leadership in adulthood 52
Retrospective studies 12
young children to (a) extract definitions of this construct, and (b) to
Undergraduate student leadership 18
identify its nomological network as it would relate to leadership Lit reviews and theoretical papers 26
development (e.g., Carey, 2009; Choi, 2018; Schwartz et al., 2005). Books and book chapters 20
From this review, we identified the four socio-cognitive skills (theory of Articles excluded based on relevancy 39
mind, metacognition, self-regulation, and autobiographical reasoning) Total 486

we argue are particularly relevant to enacting leadership. We then 3


Due to the iterative nature of the literature search process, we determined
conducted searches to explore the relationship between agency and that some of our initial search topics were too tangential to include in our
early skill development and expression. We searched the same databases conceptual model. We thank the reviewers for helping us narrow down and
using agency and each of the four skills as keywords. We also searched refine our conceptual framework.
for longitudinal research on each skill, looking for evidence for a tra­
jectory of how it develops and is expressed over the lifespan. The child development literature began to move from an interpre­
The next step was to review broader influences for each of these tation of children as passive respondents to active agents in their social
constructs. Again, we searched databases using the same search terms interactions in the 1980s and 90s (e.g., Alanen, 1988; James & Prout,
with and without the addition of the word “leadership.” Based on these 1990). This movement forwarded several new perspectives of child
searches, we were able to identify the most prominent influences (e.g., agency. A full review of this literature is beyond the scope of this article
parents, individual differences, SES, team projects in the classroom, (see Baader, 2016; Esser et al., 2016; Oswell, 2013; Varpanen, 2019).
sports) on early leadership, skill development, and agency expression. Instead, we briefly note a few key themes related to the child expression
We then searched and reviewed research pertaining to each of the in­ of leader agency. Fundamentally, agency refers to the child’s intentional
fluences and experiences denoted in our model. and purposive action (Schlosser, 2015; Varpanen, 2019). Such actions
Finally, in our review of the leadership development literature we include exploration activities designed to acquire new knowledge,
identified that many papers had discussed a relationship between skill competencies, and values (Hedegaard, 2020). These activities are
development and identity development (e.g., Day et al., 2009; Day & motivated by the presence of uncertainty and novelty in the child’s
Dragoni, 2015; Liu et al., 2021). We conducted a literature review for environment (Meder, et al., 2021; Schulz, 2015; Schulz et al., 2019).
leader identity and related terms, as well as pulled out all related terms Two perspectives on children’s agency suggest that (a) it derives from
from the articles we had collected in previous searches, referring to this the child’s motive and value orientations, or (b) it is socially con­
collection of related concepts as the “leader mindset.” We then con­ structed, derived from the child’s network of social relationships that
ducted searches in the same databases using the leader mindset terms bound and direct its expression (Sugarman & Sokol, 2012). In our
and keywords related to children/adolescents, and we reviewed articles framework, we reflect recent arguments to integrate both aspects of
from previous searches, to find evidence of how the leader mindset agency (Rissanen, 2017; Varpanen, 2019) by emphasizing both the
develops before adulthood. child’s inclination toward proactivity and the role of social enablers (or
Due to the complex nature of the topic, information learned from inhibitors) of this orientation.
previous searches helped to inform subsequent searches. This iterative Studies have documented children displaying agency from an early
search process enabled us to find a broader and more comprehensive age. For example, research has shown that 6-month-old infants
body of literature than would otherwise have been possible with a single demonstrate that they understand themselves and others as active
search. As shown in Table 1, across all searches we identified 486 agents who have goals (Carey, 2009). Preschool-aged children express
potentially relevant articles. Table 1 shows the number of articles agency through displayed helping behavior (Chernyak & Kushnir, 2018;
identified based on the topic area. Recchia, 2011). Mawson (2011) observed preschool children expressing
agency in a pretend play scene, by introducing new plotlines or identi­
Agency fying a new role for themselves. Elementary-age children expressed
agency through organizing and encouraging teammates to work
A key element of our model is that children can and do exert agency together on a group project (Yamaguchi & Maehr, 2003). Children have
over their own leadership expression and development. Such leadership choices over how they act and react within contexts and can also choose
agency is expressed in young children by their active exploration of which contexts to enter. Holden (2010) noted there are multiple ways
opportunities to influence peers and through regulating their behavior that children actively influence their own developmental trajectories (e.
for the sake of pursuing a collective goal. g., learning an instrument, succeeding academically, or attaining social

4
J.A. Cannon et al. The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx

competence). He further explained that children can accept, reject, grant children more autonomy to foster personal responsibility, allow­
negotiate, or resist their parents-initiated trajectories; they can change ing children to take on challenges (e.g., pursue leadership roles).
their level of effort and cognitive constructions of experiences once on a
trajectory; and they can propose their own trajectories. Accordingly, this Socioeconomic status
agency expressed in many non-leadership situations can extend to Socioeconomic status affects childhood leadership agency through
leadership situations as well. For example, children can accept, resist, or the modeling and social influence of parents. Barling and Weatherhead
reject leadership roles thrust upon them by parents and teachers, and (2016) reported that family poverty status had a significant negative
can negotiate or advocate for taking on different leadership roles and indirect effect on leader role occupancy, mediated by school quality and
trajectories. personal mastery. These authors noted that family poverty may hinder
Importantly, there is no “universal” or automatic progression in the development of agency, as the belief that individuals are unable to
agency expression from infancy to adulthood. Much like individual control or influence their environment is reinforced through observa­
differences in leadership and agency expression in adulthood, develop­ tions of parents’ financial struggles. Such beliefs may in turn limit the
mental trajectories of agentic behavior show individual differences child’s engagement in leadership development experiences. In contrast,
based on personality traits, temperament, and other factors. We will these authors articulated that economically advantaged families provide
expand on this concept of the variability of agency below by reviewing psychological as well as physical resources to children, which help
various enablers and inhibitors of agency development and expressions. children develop self-efficacy, mastery, and a sense of personal control.
Similar conclusions were reported by Yuan et al. (2022), who found that
Enablers/inhibitors of agency family SES and positive parenting positively predicted leadership po­
tential, mediated by children’s self-esteem, and Duan et al. (2022) who
The child’s expressions of agency from infancy through young found that parental SES was associated with transformational leadership
adulthood can be influenced by particular individual differences, behavior in adults.
influential relationships, and contextual factors. For example, some Interestingly, parental behavior and agency also explained excep­
children will differ by temperament in how much they will actively seek tional cases of children experiencing poverty while achieving academic
– or minimize personal exposure to – leadership development experi­ success. Cheang and Goh (2018) studied such “positive deviant” cases
ences (Guerin et al., 2011). Also, several relationship and contextual and found that these children cited caring and supportive relationships
factors can offer more enriched opportunities that provide a wider range with their mothers as a source of motivation. They used their agency to
of choices in such experiences (Hansen et al., 2018; Larson et al., 2019). develop creative ways of helping their families financially, such as
saving their allowances and saving free meal coupons from their schools.
Individual differences Although faced with poverty, the children believed in their own capacity
Certain predispositions influence the degree to which children are to influence outcomes and make a difference in the financial circum­
inclined to express agency around leadership. For example, Guerin et al. stances of their families. Similarly, Gozali and Paik (2023) reported that
(2011) found that temperament (approach/withdrawal) at age two young leaders in Indonesia cited negative personal events such as
predicted extraversion at age 17, which in turn was significantly related financial hardship and the death of a parent as a source of learning,
to leadership potential in adulthood. They reported those with an motivation, and transformation. Indeed, having a sense of agency
“approach” temperament are more inclined to agentically seek out social buffers against challenging experiences.
interactions and experiences, thus gaining more opportunities to
develop social and leadership skills. Narcissism is another individual Culture
difference that fosters such agency. Narcissists typically possess an Culture represents another influence on early leader development
exaggerated self-view that needs continued reinforcement (Cragun and the expression of leadership agency. Wilson and Yip (2010) found
et al., 2020). To gain such reinforcement they seek personal recognition preliminary evidence for a range of differences in how leaders learn and
in the form of social achievement (Elliot & Thrash, 2001). Accordingly, develop across cultures. Further, the attributes of an effective leader
narcissistic children are more likely to seek leadership roles. Along this were found to be in part culturally dependent (Dorfman et al., 2012). Of
line, Brummelman et al. (2021) found that children ages 7–14 who particular relevance to leader agency, researchers have posited that
scored higher on a childhood narcissism scale tended to behave more agency is expressed differently in Eastern versus Western cultures
dominantly and present themselves as role models, which in turn led to (Hernandez & Iyengar, 2001; Trommsdorff, 2012). While Western cul­
peers recognizing them as a leader. Taken together, these studies sup­ tures typically view agency at the level of the individual (i.e., self-
port certain individual differences as enabling child leader agency determination and independent goal pursuit), Eastern cultures concep­
expression. tualize agency at the level of the group (Hernandez & Iyengar, 2001).
Much of the research on leadership emergence in children and adoles­
Parents/caregivers cents reviewed in this paper has been conducted in Western contexts,
Baumrind’s (1971) dimensions of parenting behavior have important although there are notable exceptions (e.g., Liu et al., 2019), and more
implications for childhood agency and leader development. The first research is needed to compare early leader development and agency
dimension, demandingness, is the level of expectations parents have of across cultures. One potential implication of these differences is that
children. The second, responsiveness, is the degree of emotional warmth culture matters for leadership beliefs and expression, such as the way
and support expressed by parents. Liu et al. (2019) found that over­ agency may translate to leadership engagement for Eastern versus
parenting (a combination of high responsiveness and low demanding­ Western cultures.
ness) related negatively to Chinese adolescents choosing leader roles or
displaying leader emergence. Parents who are overly involved in their Socio-cognitive skills
child’s activities limit the child’s ability to apply their own agency,
which in turn hampers their leader development. Our model indicates that four fundamental socio-cognitive skills
In contrast, particular parental behaviors can enable greater agency derive from agency-driven early leadership experiences, and in turn
expression in children. In a U.S. study of adolescent boys ages 14–18, enrich engagement in later experiences. In this section we describe these
Kudo et al. (2012) found that authoritative parenting (high respon­ four skills, along with their enabling conditions. We then follow this
siveness and high demandingness) significantly and positively corre­ with a section on specific developmental experiences linked to these
lated with children’s mastery beliefs, which in turn predicted skills.
transformational leadership behavior. Authoritative parents tend to

5
J.A. Cannon et al. The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx

Theory of mind metacognitive representations to others (Sodian et al., 2012). This


declarative form of metacognition continues to develop throughout
Theory of mind (ToM), defined as “the understanding of others’ childhood and adolescence, as knowledge about metacognitive strate­
thoughts and feelings and their behavioral consequences” (Peterson gies is gained (Schneider, 2008).
et al., 2016, p. 147), is dependent on a child’s rudimentary under­ Leadership research has also identified metacognition as critical for
standing of agency. After understanding that agents can have goals, leadership development and practice (e.g., Black et al., 2016; Day et al.,
children develop an awareness around age 4 that agents can also act 2004, Zaccaro, 2007). In children, metacognitive knowledge and skill
according to true or false beliefs (Carey, 2009), building on earlier un­ promotes goal regulation and planning in collaborative activities.
derstanding of desires and knowledge (Wellman & Liu, 2004). ToM in­ Indeed, Daher et al. (2018) found that middle school children who
cludes an awareness of how others express their own agency. Such engaged in metacognitive processes were more likely to succeed in
awareness in turn enables a more accurate prediction of how they will claiming leadership roles on a collaborative mathematics solution task.
act, an ability associated with ToM. Longitudinal growth in ToM un­ Moreover, children’s reflection on early leadership development expe­
derstanding in children is reflected in mastery of a series of progressively riences fosters growth in functional leadership and problem-solving
more difficult tasks. According to Peterson et al., (2016, p. 150), these skills (Marshall-Mies et al., 2000) and greater developmental readiness
tasks include diverse desires (“awareness that different people may want (Black et al., 2016; Hannah & Avolio, 2010).
different things”), diverse beliefs (“awareness that different people may
hold different potentially true beliefs or opinions”), knowledge access Self-regulation
(“awareness that not seeing leads to ignorance”), false beliefs (ability to
“infer the behavior, speech, or thoughts of protagonists with false be­ Self-regulation involves active efforts to direct one’s own thoughts
liefs,” p. 147), hidden emotion (“awareness that people’s facial expres­ and actions using cognitive and emotional strategies (Slot et al., 2017).
sions might not match their true feelings”), and sarcasm understanding According to Slot et al., cognitive self-regulation includes the use of
(“awareness that the literal meanings of people’s words might not match metacognitive knowledge and strategies to regulate task behavior like
their intended meanings”). In most cases, “typically developing” chil­ planning, monitoring, and control of cognition, and motivational factors
dren pass the first four tasks by age 7, but the latter two tasks remain like persistence and attention. Slot et al. defined emotional self-regulation
highly challenging for children from ages 7 to 12 (Peterson et al., 2016; as involving the use of explicit knowledge about emotions, controlling
see Wellman & Liu, 2004 for a detailed account of ToM development and modulating emotional expression, meeting the social expectations
before age 7). Longitudinal research has indicated that around age 7, of a situation, and getting along and resolving conflicts with peers.
children achieve the ability to understand that mental states can be Frazier et al. (2021) proposed that agency and metacognition were
recursive (e.g., someone can hold a belief about someone else’s belief) jointly foundational to the exercise of self-regulation. They argued that
which is referred to as advanced or second-order theory of mind having (a) a sense of personal responsibility and control over one’s
(Osterhaus & Koerber, 2021). Theory of mind also continues to develop behavior and, (b) the ability to monitor goal-directed strategies and
throughout adulthood (Derksen et al., 2018; Klindt et al., 2017). actions, motivate behavior toward regulating emotions and persisting in
In a leadership context, an understanding that someone can hold the face of challenges.
beliefs about other’s beliefs can help with sensemaking and problem- Self-regulation skill begins to develop in infancy, as parents and
solving. This understanding can aid discussions aimed at achieving children co-regulate, with children learning from the explicit and im­
consensus on and working together to solve a problem, thus facilitating plicit modeling of regulation from their parents (Montroy et al., 2016).
collective action toward a shared goal. In addition, accurate social According to Sameroff (2010), the growth trajectory of self-regulation
perception involves understanding others’ reactions to one’s own lead­ begins with regulation of biological needs (temperature, hunger), to
ership attempts (Zaccaro et al., 1991). Accordingly, research has found psychological and social regulation involving greater agentic decision-
that children who possessed a more sophisticated ToM were also more making (e.g., to regulate behavior, attention, and social interactions).
likely to be perceived as leaders (Peterson et al., 2016). Montroy et al. (2016) noted that the developmental trajectories of self-
regulation can vary widely in response to life experiences. Similarly,
Sameroff (2010) proposed that the self-regulation system interacts and
Metacognition
develops in the context of one’s family, school, community, and other
social contexts. A number of early experiences have been found to play a
Metacognition has been defined as “the knowledge, monitoring, and
role in the development of self-regulation, such as pretend play (Slot
control of one’s cognition” (Marulis et al., 2020, p. 47).2 It reflects
et al., 2017; Whitebread & O’Sullivan, 2012), supportive educational
children’s self-knowledge, awareness of other children’s cognitive pro­
settings (Immordino-Yang et al., 2019), and extracurricular leadership
cessing, and “skill in regulating one’s learning activities including
programs involving mentoring and teaching self-regulation skills
problem solving” (Daher et al., 2018, p. 293; See also Flavell, 1999).
(Annesi, 2020).
Like ToM, children’s sense of agency is also closely intertwined with
Self-regulation is inextricably bound with effective leadership (Moss
metacognitive skill (Larkin, 2010). Early understandings of agency, such
et al., 2009). As children gain skill in self-regulation, they improve their
that one’s actions can influence the external environment (Kloo and
abilities to engage in goal planning and regulation, persist in goal
Rohwer, 2012), help children engage in metacognitive processing by
achievement, and to adapt goal plans in response to shifting events
motivating intentional thinking about one’s actions. Rudimentary forms
(Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2009). Such abilities in turn foster leadership
of metacognition emerge in the first few years of life. Indeed, skills such
emergence and development (Avolio &Vogelgesang, 2011; Reitan &
as error monitoring were shown to emerge in nascent form as early as 12
Stenberg, 2019).
months (Goupil & Koiuder, 2016). A system of explicit metacognition
So far, we have outlined how these socio-cognitive skills cited in the
develops around ages 3–4, where children become able to report
developmental literature correspond with skills that have been docu­
mented as necessary for leadership in the adult leadership literature.
2 Although current developmental research no longer supports the
Kuhn (2008) noted that while Piaget’s stage theory of development has
generally not held up in subsequent empirical research, his identification of concept of strong and separable developmental “stages” (Bjorklund,
second-order thinking (thinking about thinking) is still largely correct and is 2018), there is evidence for a qualitative shift in socio-cognitive skills
now known as metacognition. While Piaget theorized that this skill does not and behaviors which begins around early adolescence (Janacsek et al.,
begin to emerge until late childhood or adolescence, research has found that 2012). These changes correspond with more sophisticated abilities to
metacognition emerges in rudimentary forms much earlier in life. reflect on one’s own identity and conceptualize the self in more complex

6
J.A. Cannon et al. The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx

ways (van Doeselaar et al., 2020). The following section will elaborate cognitive skills referred to in our model. For example, Ohtani and
on the fourth skill in our model, autobiographical reasoning, which Hisasaka (2018) noted that metacognition and intelligence are corre­
emerges during this time in development and is crucial to the develop­ lated, possibly because higher intelligence affords greater cognitive re­
ment of leader identity and exercising of agency as an emerging leader. sources to devote to metacognitive activities while performing a task.
Further, intelligence has been associated with ToM in preschool-age
Autobiographical reasoning children (Frye et al., 1995), secondary school students (Ibanez et al.,
2013) and in college students (Coyle et al., 2018). Ibanez et al. (2013)
Autobiographical reasoning is the ability to construct a coherent explained this link, noting that fluid intelligence is associated with
narrative of one’s life and how it contributes to a self-identity (van greater facial processing ability and adaptation in social contexts, which
Doeselaar et al., 2020). Many developmental researchers concur that would lead to improved understanding and response to others’ mental
autobiographical reasoning is the main process through which one’s life and emotional states. Additionally, Coyle et al. (2018) explained that the
story and thereafter self and leadership identity is formed (Banks & ability to make complex inferences is associated with both intelligence
Salmon, 2013; Lilgendahl & McAdams, 2011; McLean & Breen, 2009). and ToM ability (specifically, the ability to make complex inferences
Autobiographical reasoning has been conceptualized as having two about others’ mental states). Likewise, Hughes and Ensor (2007) found
subcomponents, self-event connections and sophistication of meaning in a longitudinal study of children ages 2–4 that stronger executive
(Banks & Salmon, 2013). According to Banks & Salmon, the former are functioning predicted ToM use and growth. These studies suggest that
statements linking individual experiences to personal traits, beliefs, and intelligence may correspond with the practice and development of the
attitudes, and the latter is the level of complexity or insight of knowl­ four socio-cognitive skills in our model.
edge especially about the self, resulting from self-reflections. These
concepts link information about the self to life events and have been Parents
studied in connection with agency expression. A longitudinal study of Parental relationships also provide a context for children to practice
Dutch adolescents found that those who engaged in autobiographical socio-cognitive skills. For example, parents can encourage self-
reasoning (i.e., made self-event connections) in more agentic ways regulation development through warmth and responsiveness (Grolnick
subsequently engaged in more adaptive exploration of their identities in & Farkas, 2002) and autonomy support (Vasquez et al., 2016). In
breadth and depth (van Doeselaar et al., 2020). Feeling a greater sense of addition, when parents allow children space to negotiate, this behavior
agency over how one experiences life events may empower adolescents affirms their autonomy and can foster early growth in ToM through
to make active choices in exploring and committing to chosen identities. perspective-taking, and metacognitive development through setting and
Autobiographical reasoning ability continues to progress and build communicating personal goals. “By using negotiation, children not only
through adulthood. The narratives of adults in late middle age were assertively communicate their disagreement with some aspect of
found to have the greatest complexity, individuation, and integration parental expectations but also engage with their parents relationally by
with the self-concept (Pasupathi & Mansour, 2006). These findings align considering their perspectives while pursuing their own autonomous
with theoretical propositions about what it takes to become an expert goals” (Kuczynskiy et al., 2018, p. 8).
leader (e.g., Lord & Hall, 2005). According to Lord and Hall, expert
leaders possess a deep and elaborated sense of identity and values which Mentors and role models
can be flexibly applied according to the particular demands of the When working with mentors and role models who encourage agency
situation. expression, youth have opportunities to learn socio-cognitive skills
Taken together, these four socio-cognitive skills provide the foun­ including theory of mind, metacognition, and self-regulation. For
dation for the expression of leadership and the development of a leader example, early adolescent girls (age 12) reported that participating in a
mindset. Cognitive development researchers have proposed that socio- one-on-one mentoring program helped them develop self-regulation
cognitive skills mutually influence the development of one another – skills and self-understanding skills by practicing goal setting and study
advancement in one skill enables more successful enactment of other skills (Deutsch et al., 2017). Ridell (2017) noted that leadership op­
skills. Along this line, some research has hypothesized that ToM in turn portunities where adolescents are required to lead by example (an op­
facilitates the acquisition of metacognitive skills (Schneider, 2008). portunity to practice agency) can help increase self-regulation skills.
Metacognitive skill also facilitates growth in autobiographical reasoning Actively choosing which leader role models to follow can be a way of
(Hiver et al., 2020). The mutual growth in these socio-cognitive skills practicing autobiographical reasoning – making sense of and shaping
enables early leader development. Self-regulation and metacognition one’s growing leader identity by focusing on role models to emulate.
foster the ability to understand and enact leader goal achievement be­ This choice of leadership role models can also extend to fictional media,
haviors. Theory of mind helps developing leaders understand, influence, such as leaders on television (Harms & Spain, 2016; Maskell et al.,
and motivate others to work collectively in pursuit of goals. Finally, 2022).
autobiographical reasoning enables the crystallization of a leader Interaction with mentors, role models, and peers often occurs
identity, which can further motivate pursuit of leadership roles and self- through leadership development experiences, the next component in our
development goals. model which we expand upon below.

Enablers/inhibitors of socio-cognitive skills Leadership development experiences

A number of individual differences and external influences have been Children express agency in part by seeking out particular leadership
found to impact the development of socio-cognitive skills, which in turn development experiences. These experiences in turn enable growth in
influence early leader development. socio-cognitive and leadership skills and, as competence grows, a
heightened sense of agency. In this section, we suggest that early lead­
Individual differences ership development experiences tend to share key features regarding
Intelligence has been linked to skill development as well as leader­ what makes them growth opportunities for young leaders. These fea­
ship potential. Longitudinal research on leadership in children indicated tures can include freedom to exert agency (i.e., freedom of decision-
that childhood IQ scores at age 12–13 had a positive relationship with making); working with others to accomplish a shared goal (e.g., Oli­
leadership potential, as measured by officer suitability in the military vares et al., 2007), engagement in complex and/or reflective thinking
draft at age 18–20 (Reitan & Stenberg, 2019). Some of the mechanisms and planning (e.g., DeRue & Ashford, 2010a); and experiencing an
linking intelligence to growth in leadership ability include the socio- optimal degree of challenge (e.g., Ohlott, 2004). This section will

7
J.A. Cannon et al. The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx

describe four experiences in which socio-cognitive skills can be devel­ Veen et al., 2017).
oped: pretend play, classroom projects, sports, and extracurricular ac­
tivities. Our intention is not to represent the full array of experiences Team projects in the classroom
that afford child leader development. Liu et al., (2021) provided a
broader summary of such experiences. Our focus is on how particular Classroom activities and group projects provide significant oppor­
kinds of experiences promote growth in these four socio-cognitive skills. tunities for children to express agency and practice leadership (Liu et al.,
2021). Different affordances in such activities can influence the form of
Pretend play leadership and functionality of particular leadership behaviors that
emerge in such contexts. For example, in a U.S. study of 4th-6th grade
Child development researchers have defined pretend play as a spe­ students, Yamaguchi (2001) found that student groups assigned a task
cific form of imaginary play which includes fantasy and sociodramatic under a “performance” condition (i.e. try to do better than the other
play and argue that it is one of the most important activities for socio- groups) tended to be dominated by one leader, whereas student groups
cognitive development during the preschool age (3–6 years; White­ assigned the same task under a “mastery” condition (i.e. focus on
bread & O’Sullivan, 2012). In a pretend play setting, children have learning and improving rather than competition) tended to exhibit more
opportunities to choose what activities to engage in, allowing free shared leadership. In addition, the mastery groups’ leadership behavior
expression and practice (i.e., agency) of leader behavior. This form of focused not only on the task but also on building group cohesion. These
play can involve several different components related to representa­ groups were more effective at completing the task. Thus, the mastery
tional imagination, such as object substitution, role enactment, or the task condition afforded a different form of leadership, allowing greater
creation of an imaginary storyline, with children playing different roles opportunities for students to learn how to practice collective leadership.
or characters. Several studies have documented leadership behavior (i. These tasks also afford opportunities to practice and develop metacog­
e., a child leading a group of peers) emerging spontaneously in a group nition and ToM, as students share their thought processes and learn
play setting (Howes et al., 1989; Kim, 2021; Mawson, 2011). about others’ ideas and perspectives.
The situational affordances in the context of pretend play can offer Classroom activities which specify the goals but do not specify how
opportunities to practice agency and develop socio-cognitive skills. to complete them also encourage greater expressions of leader agency,
Research has found that a natural outdoor environment affords a greater and opportunities to practice leadership skills. For example, in an open
degree of autonomy than a manufactured outdoor environment (Kim, group discussion activity with 4th grade students, Li et al. (2007) found
2021; Morrissey et al., 2017). In a recent study of sociodramatic play in that some students spontaneously emerged as leaders, and child leaders
preschoolers, when children had access to a yard in a more naturalized displayed a range of leadership styles (e.g., turn management, argument
environment with open-ended materials such as sand and logs, they development, planning and organizing, and topic control). They
engaged in longer and more complex episodes of pretend play than concluded that the differences in styles were influenced by the person­
when given access to a plastic cubby house and fewer open-ended ma­ alities of leaders, and by contextual factors such as the characteristics of
terials (Morrissey et al., 2017). The format of pretend play enables group members, the group dynamics, and the circumstances of the dis­
children to practice other skills related to leadership, including negoti­ cussion. In a study of an extracurricular activity of a “court” created by
ation (though deciding what each person’s character will do, or dis­ students ages 6–12 and teachers, children displayed varying levels of
cussing the plot of the imaginary scene), symbolic thinking (through leadership behaviors based on the roles they chose to take and the
object substitutions, such as sand representing flour for a cake). This situational features. For example, children playing the judge role dis­
format can also foster problem-solving (of problems that arise through played more leadership than witnesses/observers, and judges displayed
enactment of the imaginary scene) through communication with peers more leadership towards the end of the trials (Wilcox, 2004). Group
as well as reflective thinking (imagining how their character should act) activities such as these that involve performing a fictional role, such as a
and taking others’ perspectives (imagining what peers’ characters’ judge or lawyer in this study, encourage children to practice self-
motivations are; Kim, 2021; Morrissey et al., 2017). regulation, through the effortful control of maintaining a character,
Pretend play also enables skill development through the mechanism and ToM, through imagining what the character’s beliefs and perspec­
of metacommunication, a form of communication that is focused on the tives might be within the fictional scenario. Overall, these studies
roles and rules of imaginary situations (Whitebread & O’Sullivan, 2012). demonstrate the nature and structure of classroom activities can have
An example of such metacommunication is when a one child says to significant effects on the extent to which children can express agency
another, “This is your tractor and you have to drive it into the field” and practice socio-cognitive skills that give rise to leadership capacities.
(Whitebread & O’Sullivan, 2012, p. 205), or “Pretend I was your kitty”
(Kim, 2021, p. 49). According to Whitebread and O’Sullivan (2012), by Sports
verbally discussing their intentions with one another, meta­
communication enables children to create an understanding of the In a recent review of research on adolescent girls’ leader develop­
shared goal and tasks assigned to each of the players to accomplish that ment, Eva et al. (2021) concluded that while it is widely acknowledged
goal. These conversations foster a greater understanding of peers’ that sport participation is important for leader development, there is
mental states and thought processes, which in turn contributes to ToM little analysis about why sports are strong modes of such development.
understanding (Schwebel et al., 1999; Wolf, 2022). In addition, the We propose that the experience of working together with a team and the
process of planning and regulating behavior in group pretend play inherent challenges of the games provide opportunities to practice
provides an opportunity to practice metacognitive strategies (Slot et al., agency, leadership, and socio-cognitive skills.
2017; Whitebread & O’Sullivan, 2012). Research has found that suc­ Studies of youth currently involved in sports have provided support
cessful child leaders were more likely to use complex patterns of meta­ for this assertion (Pierce et al., 2020). Sports offer opportunities to
communication to extend pretend play and were also more effective practice self-regulation through regulating physical, mental, and
negotiators and compromisers (Elias & Berk, 2002). Children who emotional efforts toward improving sports-related skills and winning
practice taking on both follower and leader roles also develop a better games. Surveys of high school student athletes revealed that sports
awareness of their peers’ perspectives (Kim, 2021; Shin et al., 2004), participation was significantly associated with higher levels of
enabling greater ToM understanding. While most of the research on emotional regulation, a component of self-regulation (Hansen et al.,
metacommunication has focused on pretend play, practicing meta­ 2003; 2010; Larson et al., 2006). In particular, Hansen et al. (2010)
communication, or being exposed to others’ metacommunication in found that team sports participation fostered significantly higher levels
other contexts, can also foster growth in socio-cognitive skills (van der of development in several key leadership areas (identity work, initiative,

8
J.A. Cannon et al. The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx

emotional regulation, teamwork and social skills, adult networks and experiences. We focus specifically on the role of parents/caregivers,
social capital, and interpersonal relationships), compared to individual teachers and coaches as developmental partners.
sports. They noted that the requirement of interdependency to perform
tasks with team sports likely explain differences in outcomes. Parents/caregivers
Additionally, participation in team sports offers an opportunity to Parents and caregivers can offer a supporting role to enhance, or
express agency through claiming informal leadership roles. For example, detract from, leader development through experiences. Parents can
in a study of athletes aged 15 and above, Fransen et al. (2014) proposed provide support for leader development by having conversations and
that there are multiple leadership roles within a sports team (task leader, encouraging youth to reflect about themselves as leaders (Komives et al.,
social leader, motivational leader, and external leader), and found evi­ 2005). These conversations can aid in metacognitive development and
dence that different team members took on these various leadership help children develop a leader mindset. Conversely, parents who
roles. Taken together, these studies demonstrate that the features of micromanage their children’s lives (“helicopter” parents) may
sports activities offer clear affordances for the expression of leadership discourage agency expression and leader development by hampering
agency and the development of socio-cognitive skills. children’s role in (a) choosing which leader development activities to
engage in, and (b) choosing how they express agency and leadership
Extracurricular activities while engaging in such activities (Liu et al., 2019).

Other leader development opportunities are afforded by participa­ Teachers and coaches
tion in extracurricular activities such as church groups, leadership The behaviors of teachers in the classroom foster growth in socio-
programs, student government, community groups, and internships cognitive skills and child leadership in several ways. First, they can
(Murphy & Johnson, 2011). Given the range and variety of such extra­ “permit” autonomy and child proactivity in the classroom in ways that
curricular activities, we outline the elements across them that can lead afford expression of the child’s agency (Baker, et al., 2023; Hedegaard,
to growth in leadership skillsets and mindsets. 2020). Second. they can provide playful experiences and classroom tasks
One element is the social novelty of extracurricular activities. We that are specifically designed to promote such agency and foster practice
propose that activities in which youth work with groups or individuals of self-regulation skills (Baker et al., 2023; Rimm-Kaufman, et al., 2009).
who are unfamiliar to them may prompt greater growth in ToM. For Finally, they can provide prompts to children in their classroom to
example, Larson et al. (2019) found that after participating in a lead­ reflect on their leadership experiences in ways that enhances their un­
ership exercise, teenagers developed an understanding of how children derstanding of others (ToM), increases self-awareness of their own
differed in what they needed from their leaders. They also found that thinking processes (metacognition), and integrates them into their
holding formal and informal leadership roles in youth organizations growing leader identity (autobiographical reasoning) (Davis, 2003;
provided opportunities to exercise agency and helped teenage leaders to English & Kitsantas, 2013).
develop a sense of responsibility for the needs of others. Other studies Coaches support child leader development by modeling and cueing
have shown how extracurricular activities, particularly in service or the expression of self-regulation behaviors in particular contexts. For
faith-based organizations, fostered growth in emotional regulation and example, Gould and Carson (2010) found that sports coaches’ behaviors
identity reflection (Hansen et al., 2003). of goal setting, positive rapport, discussing competition strategies and
Crucially, participation in developmental activities must also be sports lessons were associated with high school youth development of
accompanied by conscious reflection (i.e., metacognition) and choice to emotional regulation (a component of self-regulation), prosocial
become a leader or continue to lead others. For example, Harris and behavior (i.e., theory of mind), and cognitive skills. In a study of athletes
Beckert (2019) noted that participants in youth leadership projects used aged 10–19, Cronin and Allen (2015) found that coaches’ autonomy
agentic language, claiming ownership over their work. In their study, support (i.e., encouraging agentic behavior) played a positive role in the
one interviewee noted, development of youth skills including goal setting, personal and social
skills, and cognitive skills. In these two studies, we suggest that coaches
“And by the end of the project I realized that this was something that
likely modeled metacognitive skills through discussing strategies for
I started and I needed to take ownership for it and this is not the
games and teaching youth how to monitor their performance. If these
adults’ project. This is my project and I need to stop asking for
conversations occur in a team setting, coaches may also model ToM skill
permission and just do and take ownership of that” (Harris & Beck­
through addressing teammates’ roles and perspectives.
ert, 2019, p. 117).
The process of reflecting on leadership development experiences The leader mindset
helps youth develop a leader mindset and recognize themselves as
leaders. Other recent conceptual models of pre-adult leader development
In summary, participation in a range of activities can accelerate have noted that early leadership development experiences and influ­
youth leader development through practicing agency and the socio- ential relationships (e.g., parents, teachers) not only foster growth in
cognitive skills, paired with self-reflection to integrate the experiences leadership and socio-cognitive skills but also help to shape young
into one’s emerging sense of self as a leader. We emphasize that other leaders’ mindsets (e.g., Liu et al., 2021; Eva et al., 2021). The leader
experiences not mentioned in this article may also be developmental, if mindset can be conceptualized as including three mutually reinforcing
they share some of the same features as the ones described above. elements: leader knowledge structures (Hannah et al., 2010), leader iden­
tity (Hiller, 2005), and leadership self-efficacy (Dwyer, 2019). All three
Enablers/inhibitors of leader development through experiences elements of the mindset interact to shape choices in whether and how to
engage in leadership in various settings.
The research on leadership development experiences has indicated
that key enablers of leader development are relational interactions Leader knowledge structures
during such experiences, such as with teachers, coaches, mentors, and
peers. Such developmental relationships have been critical for adult Leader knowledge structures have been defined as “…the in­
leader development because of their utility in providing feedback, dividual’s mental organization of information in a particular perceptual
coaching, and support for leadership growth (McCauley & Douglas, domain” (Hannah, et al., 2010, p. 414). Such structures encode infor­
1998). We argue that such relationships are even more critical for mation about leader attributes and behaviors, with more elaborated
helping child leaders derive meaning from their leader development structures linking behaviors to situational contingencies (Dinh & Lord,

9
J.A. Cannon et al. The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx

2012). Hannah et al. (2013) suggested that knowledge structures per­ Influences of leader development experiences on leader mindset
taining to leadership help individuals create self-concepts around lead­ development
ership roles. These structures have been theorized to increase in
complexity according to the level of experience and deliberate self- The content of leadership knowledge structures is influenced
reflection and development. Indeed, children’s understanding of what strongly by childhood experiences. Hannah et al. (2013) noted that the
a leader is, and what leadership entails, has been found to emerge at an knowledge structures pertaining to the leader’s self-concept are formed
early age and increase in complexity throughout development. Stavans through social learning over time. Youth who seek out and engage in
and Baillargeon (2019) found that children’s mental models for lead­ more leadership development experiences would likely be exposed to a
ership begin to emerge around age two. Vega (2020) reported that im­ wider range of leadership role models, which, combined with ToM
plicit leadership theories (ILTs) of children in kindergarten and early ability, would likely expand their understanding of how leadership can
primary school tended to emphasize physical and spatio-temporal fea­ be expressed across different situations.
tures (e.g., physical power, being in the front of the line), whereas ILTs Evidence from youth leader development literature has shown that
of children in late primary school (grade 6) began to focus more on early leadership development experiences often trigger a shift in leader
humanitarian and environmentally concerned images of leaders. By identity. Active involvement in the community, such as in student
high school, ILTs resembled those found in adults in the workplace government and youth organizations, can help young leaders’ identities
(Frost et al., 2016). These studies indicate both the early emergence of increase in breadth and depth (Murphy & Johnson, 2011; Komives et al.,
leader knowledge structures and their increasing complexity as children 2005; Liu et al., 2021). However, Meyer and Rinn (2021) noted that the
gain more leadership experiences. direction of the relationship between youth involvement and leader
identity is not clear, and more research in this area is necessary. In
addition, these authors noted that more research is needed to investigate
Leader identity the role of youth programs in developing motivation to lead, of which
leadership self-efficacy is a precursor (Chan & Drasgow, 2001).
Leader identity has been defined as the self-identity of an individual
in the leadership domain (Hiller, 2005). Such identity is built upon the Enablers/inhibitors of leader mindset development
foundations developed during childhood and continues to grow and
expand throughout adulthood (Day et al., 2009; Liu et al., 2021). Research has found that the leader mindset is formed not just
Identity formation more generally begins early in life – by age six, through early exposure to leaders and leaderlike figures outside of one’s
children have a rudimentary self-concept, self-history and narrative control, but also through agency in seeking out role models and inten­
understanding (Dunlop & Walker, 2013). The process of incorporating tional reflection (Fitzsimmons et al., 2013; Komives et al., 2005; Yeager
“leader” into one’s self-concept fundamentally involves agency – it de­ & Callahan, 2016). Early experiences of leaders provide a frame of
pends on an individual’s conscious choice to “claim” a leadership role reference to draw from as the child matures. During adolescence, young
(DeRue & Ashford, 2010b), which then motivates further development leaders can gain social feedback that reaffirms or changes their leader
(Day et al., 2009). Also, leaders must actively choose to persist in identity. For example, when others do not respond to the adolescent’s
development (DeRue & Ashford, 2010a). leadership style, they could either blame others or could revise their
identity to incorporate the feedback (Harrell-Levy & Kerpelman, 2010).

Leadership self-efficacy Individual differences


Research on leader schemas in the workplace has demonstrated the
Leadership self-efficacy provides individuals with the desire and existence of implicit gender (e.g., Koenig et al., 2011) and racial biases
ability to persist in taking on leadership roles, enacting leadership (e.g., Ubaka et al., 2022) that can impair leader self-efficacy and one’s
behavior, and actively developing as a leader (Day et al., 2009). This leader identity before entering the workplace. Eva et al. (2021) found
type of self-efficacy refers to confidence in one’s leadership skills adolescent girls faced barriers to leader emergence due to masculine
(Dwyer, 2019), belief in one’s ability to apply relevant skills to influence leader prototypes. They noted, “adolescent girls’ internal perceptions of
leadership outcomes, and ability to regulate one’s thinking and behavior how women should lead influence the types of behaviors they are
to move towards accomplishing desired leadership outcomes (Hannah comfortable engaging in (i.e., not wanting to seem ‘too bossy’ or ‘too
et al., 2012). soft’)” (p. 11). Turman et al. (2018) emphasized the importance of youth
having leadership role models who share identity characteristics such as
gender or race, as these models tend to be more influential (Bandura,
Influences of socio-cognitive skills on leader mindset development 1997). Depending on the presence of similar role models, youth may
perceive having a greater or lesser capacity to express agency in various
We propose that advanced ToM and metacognitive ability can enable leadership situations regardless of what skills or abilities they possess.
children to more easily understand social nuances in leadership contexts
and reflect on what these mean for decisions about how to lead across Parents, mentors, and teachers
different situations. Accordingly, these skills foster growth in leader Developmental relationships are critical drivers of leader mindset
knowledge structures, particularly in terms of how leadership may growth. Bandura’s (1997) social learning model describes how different
change in different situations and when relating to different followers aspects of such relationships, such as vicarious learning, verbal
(Black et al., 2016). The goal regulation aspects of self-regulation skill persuasion, and physiological arousal, can foster growth in leader self-
promote greater engagement and persistence in leader development efficacy. Vicarious learning experiences include observing the suc­
activities (Murphy & Johnson, 2016). As success occurs through such cesses of peer leaders or leader role models. Individuals can build self-
regulation, child leaders strengthen their leadership self-efficacy. efficacy by comparing their own leadership behavior to the behavior
Finally, the exercise of autobiographical reasoning by reflecting on of role models (Turman et al., 2018) or peers (Kodama & Laylo, 2017).
such experiences enables greater leader identity development. Engaging Verbal persuasion can include nomination for a leader development
in personal reflection and biography results in the production of more program, encouragement from others (Murphy & Johnson, 2016), or
complex narratives of the self (Dunlop & Walker, 2013), and fosters developmental feedback (Machida-Kosuga, 2017). Mentoring relation­
growth of the leader identity if this reflection is focused on one’s own ships that offer such persuasion have been found to increase leader self-
leadership experiences. efficacy (Crisp & Alvarado-Young, 2018). Finally, physiological arousal

10
J.A. Cannon et al.
Table 2
Levels of Leader Complexity.
Level Description Examples from the literature

Level 1: Individual Leaders are authority figures with power, often holding a formal position. Leaders’ and followers’ • Preference for dominance in young children: Children ages 3–4 preferred dominant peers, 5-year-olds showed
leadership roles are distinct and non-overlapping. Leaders make demands and set objectives that others follow. conflicting preferences, and 8-year-olds strongly preferred the subordinate individual (Charafeddine et al.,
2016).
• Children ages 4–7 perceived authority figures as less likely to help others (Terrizzi et al., 2020).
• In children ages 7–14 narcissism associated with leader emergence and incorrect perceptions of being a more
effective leader compared to peers (Brummelman et al., 2021).
• Adolescents raised in high-conflict family environment tended to endorse tyrannical implicit leadership the­
ories 20 years later (Walker et al., 2020).
• Children ages 6–8 were found to prefer the more dominant and less trustworthy (i.e., authoritarian) face as
captain for their team if they experienced more stressors in life such as deprivation and poverty (Safra et al.,
2017).
• Researchers interviewed kindergarten through 4th grade students, asking “what is a leader?” Younger students
tended to provide leader-centric answers including, “A leader is someone who is at the front of the line.” and “A
leader is in charge.” (Bailey et al., 2017, p. 497-498).
• Physical and spatio-temporal implicit leadership theories were more common in kindergarten and early pri­
mary school compared to later ages (Vega, 2020).
Level 2: Relational Leaders exist in relationship with followers. Leaders’ and followers’ roles are fluid and can be shared. • Children’s leadership schemas tended to be more complex and abstract at older ages (Matthews et al., 1990).
leadership Leaders work to help and facilitate the accomplishment of collective goals. • Interviews of youth who participated in student government and youth organizations reveal this changing
mindset, “I can be a leader even when not being the leader.” (Komives et al., 2005, p. 605).
• Emergent leadership in group projects and group discussions enables children to practice shared leadership (Li
et al., 2007; Yamaguchi, 2001).
• Adolescent girls in a sports context focused on psychosocial aspects of leadership, such as friendship quality, in
their assessments of leadership (Moran & Weiss, 2006).
11

• When rating attributes of the self and attributes of an ideal leader, 4th grade students rated the leadership
attributes of setting and accomplishing goals, accepting criticisms and suggestions from others as lower for an
ideal leader than older students (grades 5 and 6) (Chauvin & Karnes, 1984). Older children showed a greater
prioritization of leadership traits of working with others than younger students.
• In a 6-month community-based program for developing leadership skills, all high school participants empha­
sized the importance of facilitation as a leadership attribute rather than instruction (such as “being the boss”)
(Parkhill et al., 2018). However, half the participants commented that they wanted to be in leadership roles but
had not had the opportunity (i.e., seeing leadership only as a position).
• Researchers interviewed kindergarten through 4th grade students, asking “what is a leader?” Older students
tended to provide more relational answers including, “Leaders stand up for themselves and others.” and
“Leadership means being kind, not mean, and [means] including others, and if you are kind to others it will
spread” (Bailey et al., 2017, p. 498).
• Socio-emotional and relationship-oriented implicit leadership theories were more common in children grades
3–6 compared to earlier grades. Humanitarian and environmentally-concerned ILTs became more common
starting in grade 6 (Vega, 2020).
Level 3: Collective Leaders’ purpose is to serve the needs of the organization or community. Leaders work to help their • A survey of a cross-cultural sample of high school students revealed that students believed good leaders cared
leadership team meet the needs of the broader community. Leaders facilitate the growth and leadership about others’ feelings, and honesty, social responsibility, and morality were the most important attributes of
development of others. leaders (Lee et al., 2020).

The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx


• In response to a high school leadership curriculum, a student expanded her views of what leadership was:
Before the program, a student described leadership as “somebody who has enough power and the motivation to
influence other people to follow them.” After the program, the student said, “Leadership is about…keeping the
balance between community and business. You have to think about who your decisions will benefit and fighting
for what you think is right.” (Hoyt & Kennedy, 2008, p. 211).
• In the “generativity” stage of leader identity development, students recognized the larger purposes of leadership
and sought to develop others, regenerate and sustain organizations (Komives et al., 2005).
• College students who mentored younger students exhibited a higher level of generativity compared to college
students who did not (Hastings et al., 2015).
J.A. Cannon et al. The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx

involves getting future leaders excited about becoming a leader. power or authority, and leaders and followers have distinct roles (Barnes
Observing leader characteristics possessed by mentors can highlight the et al., 2018; Uhl-Bien et al., 2014). Thus, if someone with this under­
gap between the type of leader one wants to be and one’s current status, standing of leadership does not hold a formal leadership position, they
motivating and catalyzing further development (Priest & Middleton, would not see themselves as a leader. A large body of literature has
2016). Positional role models can also provide energy and momentum to documented a relationship between attachment styles during childhood
grow as a leader (Bowers et al., 2016). and leadership views in adulthood (e.g., Berson et al., 2006; Fein et al.,
Parents shape children’s leader mindsets directly and indirectly in 2020; Keller, 2003). One specific example of such relationship is the
various ways. First, by modeling leadership behavior through the finding that adolescents raised in high-conflict family environment
parent–child relationship, parents can influence the development of tended to endorse tyrannical implicit leadership theories 20 years later
children’s leader knowledge structures (Keller, 1999; Walker et al., (Walker et al., 2020). These findings suggest that such negative envi­
2020). They can also influence children’s views of their own leader ronments can hinder growth in leader complexity. Similarly, children
identity through narratives. Even as early as preschool age, parents tell experiencing deprivation and poverty preferred a more dominant and
children stories about the family narrative (Fiese et al., 1995). Indeed, less trustworthy face as captain for their sports team (Safra et al., 2017).
leadership stories can be passed along from one generation to the next This leader-centric view has been observed in research on children’s
(Zaccaro, 2014), and such stories may inspire greater motivation to lead, views of leaders. For example, in a study of children in kindergarten
including leadership self-efficacy. Parents can also encourage adoles­ through 4th grade who were asked “what is a leader?”, younger children
cent’s leader identity formation through offering support and feedback tended to provide leader-centric answers including, “A leader is some­
(Harrell-Levy & Kerpelman, 2010). one who is at the front of the line,” and, “A leader is in charge” (Bailey
Mentors and teachers play a critical role in students’ identity et al., 2017, p. 497-498). These descriptions are similar to the physical
development through shaping their environment and experiences and and spatio-temporal ILTs observed in young children (Vega, 2020).
through providing feedback (Harrell-Levy & Kerpelman, 2010). One Another example of such views comes from an interview of a college
leader reflected on positive feedback from her teacher that can act to student about her changing views of leadership: “When I was a girl, I
increase leadership self-efficacy, “I hope you grow into the beautiful thought leadership was the person who could boss everyone around and
person that I see inside of you” (Jones-Morales & Konrad, 2018, p. 252). make them do what they wanted to do” (Komives et al., 2005, p. 605).
Deutsch et al. (2017) noted how observing mentors spurred changes in These level-1 views of leaders can develop into more complex forms,
adolescent girls’ self-understanding and helped them consider new depending on leadership development experiences, exposure to men­
possible future selves. In addition to direct mentoring relationships, tors, role models, and other influencers, and individual agency in
influential figures can also shape children’s and adolescents’ leader seeking out leadership opportunities and reflecting on the meaning of
knowledge structures, leadership self-efficacy, and leader identity. leadership.
Studies on children’s drawings of leaders reveal that children’s own
identity (e.g., their gender and religious identity) influences their Level 2
choices in which leaders to view as role models (Liu et al., 2012; Oli­
veira, 2016). In addition, positional role models such as community and At the second level of leader complexity, the understanding of
world leaders can be a source of motivation to lead (Bowers et al., 2016), leadership expands to include views of leadership as a relationship (Uhl-
in part by influencing leadership self-efficacy (Chan & Drasgow, 2001). Bien et al., 2014). Leaders do not necessarily exert absolute power, and
care about followers’ and team members’ needs and opinions. Leaders
Increasing complexity of leadership skillsets and mindsets can also contribute to accomplishing group outcomes rather than
unilaterally directing followers to complete tasks (Zaccaro, 2007). Also
As children continue to express leadership agency and pursue leader at this level, an understanding emerges that leadership can be shared.
development experiences, their skillsets and mindsets continue to grow This understanding of leadership can be observed in children’s and
in complexity across childhood and adolescence. Growth in cognitive adolescents’ behavior and views of leadership in situations involving
complexity enables an increasing capacity to understand and exercise collaboration. For example, Bailey et al. (2017) found that after
more complex forms of leadership. We propose that this leader devel­ participation in a leadership development program focused on teaching
opment process involves a progression across three levels of cognitive self-efficacy and leadership skills, older students tended to provide more
complexity. Table 2 indicates these levels, along with supporting relational answers to the question of “what is a leader?” including,
research from the child development literature. These levels progress “Leaders stand up for themselves and others.” and “Leadership means
from a simple, to a more complex and elaborated conception of what being kind, not mean, and [means] including others, and if you are kind
leadership means: individual leadership, relational leadership, and to others it will spread.” (Bailey et al., 2017, p. 498). This view aligns
collective leadership. A similar progression has been proposed in the with the humanitarian ILTs observed in late-primary-school children
extant leadership literature (Day & Harrison, 2007; Hammond et al., (Vega, 2020). Additionally, in a 6-month community-based program for
2017; Lord & Hall, 2005). Later levels of understanding include, rather developing leadership skills, all high school participants emphasized the
than abandon, previous levels (McCauley et al., 2006), enabling a wider importance of facilitation as a leadership attribute. One noted that
repertoire of potential forms of leadership to exercise and apply to meet leadership, rather than being “the boss,” was defined by guiding others
the specific needs of a situation (Hannah et al., 2013). The progression and giving others a chance to think through ideas (Parkhill et al., 2018).
from one level to another depends on the interplay of skill development, Other experiences that promote the development of this view of lead­
relevant leadership development experiences (including exposure to ership include collaborative team projects in the classroom (e.g., Li
role models and mentors), and individual agency (e.g., intentional et al., 2007; Yamaguchi, 2001) and team sports (e.g., Moran & Weiss,
reflection on experiences leading to an elaborated understanding of 2006).
leadership). As such, it is not tied to specific ages or age ranges. Below,
we describe each level with examples of how it can be manifested at Level 3
various ages.
The highest level of leader complexity includes an understanding of
Level 1 leadership at the level of the community. This level of complexity re­
sembles the concept of “generativity” introduced in the leader identity
The first level of leader complexity refers to an understanding of model proposed by Komives et al. (2005). Komives et al. described this
leadership as a hierarchical relationship. Leadership is associated with stage of leader identity as a commitment to a larger purpose and a focus

12
J.A. Cannon et al. The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx

on serving and developing others. One example of this level of indicators of each level of development. Taken together, these ideas
complexity comes from a student who participated in a high school offer new and important directions for child leader development prac­
leadership development program. Before the program, a student tices and theories.
described leadership as “somebody who has enough power and the
motivation to influence other people to follow them.” After the program, Practical implications
the student said, “Leadership is about…keeping the balance between
community and business. You have to think about who your decisions Our conclusions suggest several recommendations for anyone who
will benefit and fighting for what you think is right.” (Hoyt & Kennedy, works with children and adolescents. These learning partners (Rob­
2008, p. 211). Other such opportunities to develop this complex un­ ertson, 2009) can include parents, teachers and administrators who
derstanding of leadership are through mentoring younger students (e.g., work in the education system, and coaches and mentors who work with
Hastings et al., 2015) and contributing to social causes (e.g., Turman children in extracurricular contexts. First, given that children are active
et al., 2018). agents in their own leader development, we recommend fostering and
These three levels reflect increasing complexity of what leadership encouraging their agency expression, rather than managing and
means in terms of oneself, one’s relationships in collaborative activities dictating how children should behave in all situations (e.g., through
calling for leadership, and how one can act effectively as a leader in “helicopter parenting,” Liu et al., 2019, p. 1227). We also recommend
different situations. It is based on a well-developed ToM that reflects that children be provided access to the developmental experiences that
deep social understanding of different social nuances and stakeholders particularly afford the expression of leadership agency (e.g., team
across variable leadership contexts. They indicate increasing sophisti­ sports, leading team projects in the classroom, volunteering for a social
cation in leader identity (Lord & Hall, 2005) driven by the application of cause). Advice for adult developmental partnerships (e.g., mentors,
autobiographical reasoning to the expression of different leadership coaches) can also apply to aspiring child leaders. For example, the
roles. The three levels also reflect increasing differentiation and literature on mentoring and coaching recommends that learning part­
principle-based higher order integration (Lord & Hall, 2005) as growing ners can provide meaning-making (Merriam & Heuer, 1996; Smith,
leaders learn how to align behavioral strategies to contextual contin­ 2015) and feedback, especially after developmentally challenging ex­
gencies. This growth in complexity derives from children and adoles­ periences (DeRue & Wellman, 2009). These forms of support can help
cents pursuing multiple and varied leadership experiences. Many children and youth reflect upon and integrate leadership development
children (and indeed many adults) do not reach level 3 (or even level 2 experiences into their identity as leaders.
complexity) because a) they do not express agency toward continually We also offer recommendations to practitioners seeking ways to
pursuing leadership development experiences (Wallace & Zaccaro, accelerate child leader development through formal programs. Such
2022); b) they limit the range and scope of their pursued experiences; c) programs thrive when they include an assessment of strengths and op­
they do not engage in metacognitive reflection and autobiographical portunities for growth (Noe, 2017). Thus, the instructional design can
reasoning to derive deeper meaning from these experiences, and/or d) feature a targeted approach to leader development through first
they do not build a social network of parents, teachers, coaches, and assessing the four socio-cognitive skills and the three levels of
other role models who can enable and promote these meaning making complexity. Second, based on these assessments, learning can be
processes. Programs of child leader development that address these four directed to help nascent leaders improve in specific areas. For example,
sets of neutralizing elements are likely to be more successful in growing if assessments show a need to foster metacognitive skills, programs
early leaders with complex leader skillsets and mindsets. designed to target those skills (Marulis et al., 2020) can be integrated
into an individual child leader development plan. Further, the socio-
Conclusions and implications cognitive skills and levels of complexity could be used as metrics to
measure and evaluate the success of formal leadership programs. Wal­
Summary of major contributions lace et al. (2021) recommended that evaluators of leader and leadership
development programs be more specific about what outcomes are being
In this article, we argued for the importance of agency in the process measured, and our model of early leader development suggests specific
of early leader development and enactment, beginning in childhood and target areas for such evaluation.
progressing through adolescence. We described how children express
agency by actively shaping their own development as nascent leaders, Theoretical implications
through (a) seeking opportunities to lead and influence peers, and (b)
reflecting on and integrating past experiences into one’s leader mindset. While we have some knowledge about how external influences in
In addition, we articulated four socio-cognitive skills – theory of mind, childhood shape leadership outcomes later in life (e.g., Barling &
metacognition, self-regulation, and autobiographical reasoning – that begin Weatherhead, 2016; Fitzsimmons et al., 2013), less is known about the
to develop at an early age and provide a foundation for the enactment of influence of child and adolescent decisions on their own leader devel­
early leadership behavior. The expression of such skills also contributes opment. Our framework suggests that future theoretical and empirical
to continued growth in leader identity and leadership skill development. work should account for child and adolescent agency in shaping the
We described how leadership skills and leader identity can be honed trajectory of leader development. Such research could involve an
through leadership development experiences, especially if such experi­ exploration of how children actively seek out, respond to, reflect on, and
ences include elements of challenge, freedom to exert agency, working grow from opportunities to take on leadership roles. For example, little
collaboratively toward a shared goal, and engagement in complex or understanding exists about how children’s and adolescents’ active
reflective thinking or planning. pursuits of leadership experiences shape long-term leadership outcomes.
While these processes are, in part, driven by individual leaders’ In addition, the current findings on the role of active agency in leader
agency and socio-cognitive skills, we also described how these processes development provide an opportunity to examine and compare the
are shaped and influenced by various enablers and inhibitors: one’s impact of youth development programs that take a youth-driven versus
socioeconomic circumstances, individual differences, culture, and sig­ instructor-driven approach. As noted by Karagianni and Montgomery
nificant relationships such as parents, mentors, and coaches. Finally, we (2018), more rigorous empirical research is needed to investigate the
built upon previous theoretical work on leader complexity to offer ways effectiveness of youth leadership programs. Using the perspective of
that it develops and is expressed in childhood and adolescence. We how much the program encourages or enables leadership agency may be
proposed a trajectory of development in which individuals advance a fruitful avenue for research on its effectiveness.
through three levels of complexity, and we suggest potential behavioral Secondly, given the current gaps in the literature, our model can

13
J.A. Cannon et al. The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx

offer an organizing framework for future empirical and theoretical work Chauvin, J. C., & Karnes, F. A. (1984). Perceptions of leadership characteristics by gifted
elementary students. Roeper Review, 6(4), 238–240.
on child and adolescent leadership. In particular, interdisciplinary
Cheang, C. J., & Goh, E. C. (2018). Why some children from poor families do well—an in-
research is needed, incorporating research from developmental psy­ depth analysis of positive deviance cases in Singapore. International Journal of
chology with education and leadership research. We hope our theoret­ Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-being, 13(sup1), 1563431.
ical model will inspire future empirical work to test the relationships Chemers, M. M. (1997). Leadership, change, and organizational effectiveness (Vol. 6). Santa
Cruz, CA: University of California.
among the key elements, such as the influence of agency and develop­ Chen, J. J., & Kacerek, C. R. (2022). Leaders and followers during sociodramatic play: A
mental activities on socio-cognitive skills. We also hope that this work study of racial/ethnic minority preschool children from socioeconomically
will inspire further collaboration between child development and lead­ disadvantaged backgrounds. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 36(3),
517–540.
ership development researchers. Chernyak, N., & Kushnir, T. (2018). The influence of understanding and having choice on
children’s prosocial behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 107–110.
CRediT authorship contribution statement Choi, Y. (2018). A preschooler’s agency: Why relational types of agency emerge in peer
interactions? Early Child Development and Care, 190(10), 1525–1536.
Coyle, T. R., Elpers, K. E., Gonzalez, M. C., Freeman, J., & Baggio, J. A. (2018). General
Jessie A. Cannon: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, intelligence (g), ACT scores, and theory of mind:(ACT) g predicts limited variance
Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & among theory of mind tests. Intelligence, 71, 85–91.
Cragun, O. R., Olsen, K. J., & Wright, P. M. (2020). Making CEO narcissism research
editing. Stephen J. Zaccaro: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – great: A review and meta-analysis of CEO narcissism. Journal of Management, 46(6),
original draft, Writing – review & editing. Thalia R. Goldstein: Re­ 908–936.
sources, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & Crisp, G., & Alvarado-Young, K. (2018). The role of mentoring in leadership
development. New directions for student leadership, 2018(158), 37–47.
editing.
Cronin, L. D., & Allen, J. B. (2015). Developmental experiences and well-being in sport:
The importance of the coaching climate. The Sport Psychologist, 29(1), 62–71.
Declaration of competing interest Daher, W., Anabousy, A., & Jabarin, R. (2018). Metacognition, Positioning and Emotions
in Mathematical Activities. International Journal of Research in Education and Science,
4(1), 292–303.
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial Davis, E. A. (2003). Prompting middle school science students for productive reflection:
interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence Generic and directed prompts. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 12(1), 91–142.
Day, D. V., & Dragoni, L. (2015). Leadership development: An outcome-oriented review
the work reported in this paper. based on time and levels of analyses. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and
Organizational Behavior, 2(1), 133–156.
References Day, D. V., & Harrison, M. M. (2007). A multilevel, identity-based approach to leadership
development. Human Resource Management Review, 17(4), 360–373.
Day, D. V., & Lance, C. E. (2004). Understanding the development of leadership
Alanen, L. (1988). Rethinking childhood. Acta Sociologica, 31(1), 53–67.
complexity through latent growth modeling. Leader Development for Transforming
Annesi, J. J. (2020). Effects of a teen resident camp leadership program on changes in
Organizations, 41–69.
dimensions of self-control and interpersonal functioning, and their theory-based
Day, D. V., Fleenor, J. W., Atwater, L. E., Sturm, R. E., & McKee, R. A. (2014). Advances
relationships. Evaluation and Program Planning, 78, Article 101745.
in leader and leadership development: A review of 25 years of research and theory.
Avolio, B., & Vogelgesang, G. (2011). Beginnings matter in genuine leadership
The Leadership Quarterly, 25, 63–82.
development. In S. E. Murphy, & R. J. Reichard (Eds.), Early development and
Day, D. V., Gronn, P., & Salas, E. (2004). Leadership capacity in teams. The Leadership
leadership: Building the next generation of leaders (pp. 179–204). New York, NY:
Quarterly, 15(6), 857–880.
Psychology Press/Routledge.
Day, D. V., Harrison, M. M., & Halpin, S. M. (2009). An integrative approach to leader
Baader, M. (2016). Tracing and contextualizing childhood agency from historical and
development: Connecting adult development, identity, and expertise. Routledge.
systematic perspectives. In F. Esser, M. Baader, T. Betz, & B. Hungerland (Eds.),
Derksen, D. G., Hunsche, M. C., Giroux, M. E., Connolly, D. A., & Bernstein, D. M. (2018).
Reconceptualising agency and childhood: New perspectives in childhood studies (pp.
A systematic review of theory of mind’s precursors and functions. Zeitschrift für
135–149). London/New York: Routledge.
Psychologie, 226, 87–97. https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000325
Bailey, D., Hufford, M. M., Emmerson, M. S., & Eckert, S. A. (2017). Identifying and
DeRue, D. S., & Ashford, S. J. (2010a). Power to the people: Where has personal agency
living leadership in the lives of prekindergarten through 4th-grade girls: The story of
gone in leadership development? Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 3(1),
one intentional leadership identity development program. Journal of Research in
24–27.
Childhood Education, 31(4), 487–507.
DeRue, D. S., & Ashford, S. J. (2010b). Who will lead and who will follow? A social
Baker, S. T., Le Courtois, S., & Eberhart, J. (2023). Making space for children’s agency
process of leadership identity construction in organizations. Academy of Management
with playful learning. International Journal of Early Years Education, 31(2), 372–384.
Review, 35, 627–647.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. Macmillan.
DeRue, D. S., & Wellman, N. (2009). Developing leaders via experience: The role of
Banks, M. V., & Salmon, K. (2013). Reasoning about the self in positive and negative
developmental challenge, learning orientation, and feedback availability. Journal of
ways: Relationship to psychological functioning in young adulthood. Memory, 21(1),
Applied Psychology, 94(4), 859–875.
10–26.
Deutsch, N. L., Reitz-Krueger, C. L., Henneberger, A. K., Futch Ehrlich, V. A., &
Barling, J., & Weatherhead, J. G. (2016). Persistent exposure to poverty during childhood
Lawrence, E. C. (2017). “It gave me ways to solve problems and ways to talk to
limits later leader emergence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101(9), 1305–1318.
people” Outcomes from a combined group and one-on-one mentoring program for
Barnes, A. C., Olson, T. H., & Reynolds, D. J. (2018). Teaching power as an inconvenient
early adolescent girls. Journal of Adolescent Research, 32(3), 291–322.
but imperative dimension of critical leadership development. New Directions for
Dinh, J. E., & Lord, R. G. (2012). Implications of dispositional and process views of traits
Student Leadership, 2018(159), 77–90.
for individual difference research in leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 23(4),
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology, 4
651–669.
(1, Pt.2), 1–103. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0030372
Dorfman, P., Javidan, M., Hanges, P., Dastmalchian, A., & House, R. (2012). GLOBE: A
Berson, Y., Dan, O., & Yammarino, F. J. (2006). Attachment style and individual
twenty-year journey into the intriguing world of culture and leadership. Journal of
differences in leadership perceptions and emergence. The Journal of Social
World Business, 47(4), 504–518.
Psychology, 146(2), 165–182.
Duan, J., Ren, X., Liu, Z., & Riggio, R. E. (2022). Connecting the dots: How parental and
Bjorklund, D. F. (2018). A metatheory for cognitive development (or “Piaget is dead”
current socioeconomic status shape individuals’ transformational leadership. Journal
revisited). Child Development, 89, 2288–2302.
of Business Research, 150, 51–58.
Black, H., Soto, L., & Spurlin, S. (2016). Thinking about thinking about leadership:
Dunlop, W. L., & Walker, L. J. (2013). The life story: Its development and relation to
Metacognitive ability and leader developmental readiness. New Directions for Student
narration and personal identity. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 37
Leadership, 2016(149), 85–95.
(3), 235–247.
Bowers, J. R., Rosch, D. M., & Collier, D. A. (2016). Examining the relationship between
Dwyer, L. P. (2019). Leadership self-efficacy: Review and leader development
role models and leadership growth during the transition to adulthood. Journal of
implications. Journal of Management Development, 38(8), 637–650.
Adolescent Research, 31, 96–118.
Elias, C. L., & Berk, L. E. (2002). Self-regulation in young children: Is there a role for
Brummelman, E., Nevicka, B., & O’Brien, J. M. (2021). Narcissism and leadership in
sociodramatic play? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 17(2), 216–238.
children. Psychological Science, 32(3), 354–363.
Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2001). Achievement goals and the hierarchical model of
Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. Oxford University Press.
achievement motivation. Educational Psychology Review, 13, 139–156.
Carey, S., Zaitchik, D., & Bascandziev, I. (2015). Theories of development: In dialog with
English, M. C., & Kitsantas, A. (2013). Supporting student self-regulated learning in
Jean Piaget. Developmental Review, 38, 36–54.
́ problem-and project-based learning. Interdisciplinary journal of problem-based
Charafeddine, R., Mercier, H., Clement, F., Kaufmann, L., Reboul, A., & Van der
learning, 7(2), 128–150.
Henst, J. B. (2016). Children’s allocation of resources in social dominance situations.
Esser, F., Baader, M., Betz, T., & Hungerland, B. (Eds.). (2016). Reconceptualising agency
Developmental Psychology, 52, 1843–1857. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000164
and childhood: New perspectives in childhood studies. London/New York: Routledge.
Chan, K.-Y., & Drasgow, F. (2001). Toward a theory of individual differences and
leadership: Understanding the motivation to lead. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86
(3), 481–498. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.481

14
J.A. Cannon et al. The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx

Eva, N., De Cieri, H., Murphy, S. E., & Lowe, K. B. (2021). Leader development for Howes, C., Unger, O., & Seidner, L. B. (1989). Social pretend play in toddlers: Parallels
adolescent girls: State of the field and a framework for moving forward.The. with social play and with solitary pretend. Child Development, 60, 77–84.
Leadership Quarterly, Article 101457. Hoyt, M. A., & Kennedy, C. L. (2008). Leadership and adolescent girls: A qualitative study
Fein, E. C., Benea, D., Idzadikhah, Z., & Tziner, A. (2020). The security to lead: A of leadership development. American Journal of Community Psychology, 42(3–4),
systematic review of leader and follower attachment styles and leader–member 203–219.
exchange. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 29(1), 106–125. Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2007). Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive
Fiese, B. H., Hooker, K. A., Kotary, L., Schwagler, J., & Rimmer, M. (1995). Family stories relations from ages 2 to 4. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1447–1459.
in the early stages of parenthood. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 763–770. Ibanez, A., Huepe, D., Gempp, R., Gutiérrez, V., Rivera-Rei, A., & Toledo, M. I. (2013).
Fitzsimmons, T. W., Callan, V. J., & Paulsen, N. (2013). Gender disparity in the C-suite: Empathy, sex and fluid intelligence as predictors of theory of mind. Personality and
Do male and female CEOs differ in how they reached the top? The Leadership Individual Differences, 54(5), 616–621.
Quarterly, 25, 245–266. Immordino-Yang, M. H., Darling-Hammond, L., & Krone, C. R. (2019). Nurturing nature:
Flavell, J. H. (1999). Cognitive development: Children’s knowledge about the mind. How brain development is inherently social and emotional, and what this means for
Annual Review of Psychology, 50(1), 21–45. education. Educational Psychologist, 54(3), 185–204.
Fransen, K., Vanbeselaere, N., De Cuyper, B., Vande Broek, G., & Boen, F. (2014). The Jacques, S., & Marcovitch, S. (2010). Executive function. In W. Overton (Ed.), Handbook
myth of the team captain as principal leader: Extending the athlete leadership of lifespan development (pp. 431–466). New York: Wiley.
classification within sport teams. Journal of Sports Sciences, 32(14), 1389–1397. James, A., & Prout, A. (1990). Constructing and reconstructing childhood. Lewes: Falmer
Frazier, L. D., Schwartz, B. L., & Metcalfe, J. (2021). The MAPS model of self-regulation: Press.
Integrating metacognition, agency, and possible selves. Metacognition and Learning, Janacsek, K., Fiser, J., & Nemeth, D. (2012). The best time to acquire new skills: Age-
16(2), 297–318. related differences in implicit sequence learning across the human lifespan.
Frost, D. E. (2016). Development of implicit leadership theories prior to training or Developmental Science, 15(4), 496–505.
employment. Journal of Applied Business Research (JABR), 32(2), 555–568. Jones-Morales, J., & Konrad, A. M. (2018). Attaining elite leadership: Career
Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Palfai, T. (1995). Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. development and childhood socioeconomic status. Career Development International,
Cognitive Development, 10, 483–527. 23(3), 246–260. https://doi.org/10.1108/CDI-03-2017-0047
Gould, D., & Carson, S. (2010). The relationship between perceived coaching behaviors Karr, J. E., Rodriguez, J. E., Goh, P. K., Martel, M. M., & Rast, P. (2022). The unity and
and developmental benefits of high school sports participation. Hellenic Journal of diversity of executive functions: A network approach to life span development.
Psychology, 7(1), 298–314. Developmental Psychology, 58(4), 751–767.
Goupil, L., & Kouider, S. (2016). Behavioral and neural indices of metacognitive Kegan, R. (1980). Making meaning: The constructive-developmental approach to persons
sensitivity in preverbal infants. Current Biology, 26(22), 3038–3045. and practice. The Personnel and Guidance Journal, 58(5), 373–380.
Gozali, C., & Paik, S. J. (2023). Nurturing focused motivation: Leadership development Keller, T. (1999). Images of the familiar: Individual differences and implicit leadership
in Indonesia. European Journal of Training and Development, 47(3/4), 348–368. theories. The Leadership Quarterly, 10(4), 589–607. https://doi-rg.mutex.gmu.
Grolnick, W. S., & Farkas, M. (2002). Parenting and the development of children’s self- edu/10.1016/S1048-9843(99)00033-8.
regulation. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 5. Practical issues in Keller, T. (2003). Parental images as a guide to leadership sensemaking: An attachment
parenting (2nd ed., pp. 89–110). Erlbaum. perspective on implicit leadership theories. The Leadership Quarterly, 14(2), 141–160.
Guerin, D. W., Oliver, P. H., Gottfried, A. W., Gottfried, A. E., Reichard, R. J., & Kim, S. (2021). Preschoolers’ leadership-followership communication in outdoor pretend play
Riggio, R. E. (2011). Childhood and adolescent antecedents of social skills and on a child care playground. Kansas State University). Doctoral dissertation.
leadership potential in adulthood: Temperamental approach/withdrawal and Klindt, D., Devaine, M., & Daunizeau, J. (2017). Does the way we read others’ mind
extraversion. The Leadership Quarterly, 22, 482–494. change over the lifespan? Insights from a massive web poll of cognitive skills from
Hammond, M., Clapp-Smith, R., & Palanski, M. (2017). Beyond (just) the workplace: A childhood to late adulthood. Cortex, 86, 205–215.
theory of leader development across multiple domains. The Academy of Management Kloo, D., & Rohwer, M. (2012). The development of earlier and later forms of
Review, 42(3), 481–498. metacognitive abilities: Reflections on agency and ignorance. In M. J. Beran,
Hannah, S. T., & Avolio, B. J. (2010). Ready or not: How do we accelerate the J. L. Brandl, J. Perner, & J. Proust (Eds.), Foundations of metacognition (pp. 167–180).
developmental readiness of leaders? Journal of Organizational Behavior, 31(8), Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
1181–1187. Kodama, C. M., & Laylo, R. (2017). The unique context of identity-based student
Hannah, S. T., Avolio, B. J., Walumbwa, F. O., & Chan, A. (2012). Leader self and means organizations in developing leadership. New Directions for Student Leadership, 2017
efficacy: A multi-component approach. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision (155), 71–81.
Processes, 118(2), 143–161. Koenig, A. M., Eagly, A. H., Mitchell, A. A., & Ristikari, T. (2011). Are leader stereotypes
Hannah, S. T., Balthazard, P. A., Waldman, D. A., Jennings, P. L., & Thatcher, R. W. masculine? A meta-analysis of three research paradigms. Psychological Bulletin, 137
(2013). The psychological and neurological bases of leader self-complexity and (4), 616–642. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023557
effects on adaptive decision-making. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98(3), 393–411. Komives, S. R., Owen, J. E., Longerbeam, S. D., Mainella, F. C., & Osteen, L. (2005).
Hannah, S. T., Jennings, P. L., & Nobel, O. B. Y. (2010). Tactical military leader requisite Developing a leadership identity: A grounded theory. Journal of College Student
complexity: Toward a referent structure. Military Psychology, 22(4), 412–449. Development, 46, 593–611.
Hansen, D. M., Larson, R. W., & Dworkin, J. B. (2003). What adolescents learn in Kuczynski, L., Pitman, R., & Twigger, K. (2018). Flirting with resistance: Children’s
organized youth activities: A survey of self-reported developmental experiences. expressions of autonomy during middle childhood. International Journal of Qualitative
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13(1), 25–55. Studies on Health and Well-being, 13(sup1), 1564519.
Hansen, D. M., Moore, E. W., & Jessop, N. (2018). Youth Program Adult Leader’s Kudo, F. T., Longhofer, J. L., & Floersch, J. E. (2012). On the origins of early leadership:
Directive Assistance and Autonomy Support and Development of Adolescents’ The role of authoritative parenting practices and mastery orientation. Leadership, 8,
Agency Capacity. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 28(2), 505–519. 345–375.
Hansen, D. M., Skorupski, W. P., & Arrington, T. L. (2010). Differences in developmental Larson, R. W., Hansen, D. M., & Moneta, G. (2006). Differing profiles of developmental
experiences for commonly used categories of organized youth activities. Journal of experiences across types of organized youth activities. Developmental Psychology, 42
Applied Developmental Psychology, 31(6), 413–421. (5), 849–863. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.849
Harms, P. D., & Spain, S. M. (2016). Children’s stories as a foundation for leadership Larson, R. W., Raffaelli, M., Guzman, S., Salusky, I., Orson, C. N., & Kenzer, A. (2019).
schemas: more than meets the eye. In C. Peus, S. Braun, & B. Schyns (Eds.), The important (but neglected) developmental value of roles: Findings from youth
Monographs in leadership and management: leadership Lessons from compelling contexts programs. Developmental Psychology, 55(5), 1019–1033. https://doi.org/10.1037/
(Vol. 8, pp. 301–325). Emerald. dev0000674
Harrell-Levy, M. K., & Kerpelman, J. L. (2010). Identity process and transformative Larkin, S. (2010). Metacognition in young children. Routledge.
pedagogy: Teachers as agents of identity formation.Identity: An international journal of Lee, S. Y., Matthews, M., Boo, E., & Kim, Y. K. (2020). Gifted students’ perceptions about
theory and research, 10(2), 76–91. leadership and leadership development. High Ability Studies, 32(2), 219–259.
Harris, A., & Beckert, T. (2019). Leadership emergence through volunteerism: A case Li, Y., Anderson, R. C., Nguyen-Jahiel, K., Dong, T., Archodidou, A., Kim, I. H., …
study of late adolescent exemplars. Journal of Leadership Education, 18(2), 110–125. Miller, B. (2007). Emergent leadership in children’s discussion groups. Cognition and
Hastings, L. J., Griesen, J. V., Hoover, R. E., Creswell, J. W., & Dlugosh, L. L. (2015). Instruction, 25(1), 1–2.
Generativity in college students: Comparing and explaining the impact of mentoring. Lilgendahl, J. P., & McAdams, D. P. (2011). Constructing stories of self-growth: How
Journal of College Student Development, 56(7), 651–669. individual differences in patterns of autobiographical reasoning relate to well-being
Hedegaard, M. (2020). Children’s Exploration as a Key in Children’s Play and Learning in midlife. Journal of Personality, 79(2), 391–428.
Activity in Social and Cultural Formation. In M. Hedegaard, & E. Ødegaard (Eds.), Liu, L., Ayman, R., & Ayman-Nolley, S. (2012). Children’s image of leadership in China.
Children’s Exploration and Cultural Formation: International Perspectives on Early In Worldly leadership (pp. 105–134). London: Palgrave Macmillan.
Childhood Education and Development (Vol. 29, pp. 11–27). Switzerland: Springer. Liu, Z., Riggio, R. E., Day, D. V., Zheng, C., Dai, S., & Bian, Y. (2019). Leader
Hernandez, M., & Iyengar, S. S. (2001). What drives whom? A cultural perspective on development begins at home: Overparenting harms adolescent leader emergence.
human agency. Social cognition, 19(3: Special issue), 269–294. Journal of Applied Psychology, 104, 1226–1242.
Hiller, N. J. (2005). An examination of leadership beliefs and leadership self-identity: Liu, Z., Venkatesh, S., Murphy, S. E., & Riggio, R. E. (2021). Leader development across
Constructs, correlates. and outcomes. The Pennsylvania State University. the lifespan: A dynamic experiences-grounded approach. The Leadership Quarterly, 32
Hiver, P., Zhou, S. A., Tahmouresi, S., Sang, Y., & Papi, M. (2020). Why stories matter: (5), Article 101382.
Exploring learner engagement and metacognition through narratives of the L2 Lord, R. G., & Hall, R. J. (2005). Identity, deep structure and the development of
learning experience. System, 91, Article 102260. leadership skill. The Leadership Quarterly, 16, 591–615.
Holden, G. W. (2010). Childrearing and developmental trajectories: Positive pathways, Machida-Kosuga, M. (2017). The interaction of efficacy and leadership competency
off-ramps, and dynamic processes. Child Development Perspectives, 4, 197–204. development. New Directions for Student Leadership, 2017(156), 19–30.

15
J.A. Cannon et al. The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx

Margoni, F., Baillargeon, R., & Surian, L. (2018). Infants distinguish between leaders and Pierce, S., Erickson, K., & Sarkar, M. (2020). High school student-athletes’ perceptions
bullies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(38), E8835–E8843. and experiences of leadership as a life skill. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 51,
Marshall-Mies, J. C., Fleishman, E. A., Martin, J. A., Zaccaro, S. J., Baughman, W. A., & Article 101716.
McGee, M. L. (2000). Development and evaluation of cognitive and metacognitive Priest, K. L., & Middleton, E. (2016). Exploring leader identity and development. New
measures for predicting leadership potential. The Leadership Quarterly, 11(1), Directions for Student Leadership, 2016(149), 37–47.
135–153. Recchia, S. (2011). Preschool leaders in the early childhood classroom. In S. E. Murphy,
Marulis, L. M., Baker, S. T., & Whitebread, D. (2020). Integrating metacognition and & R. J. Reichard (Eds.), Early development and leadership: Building the next generation
executive function to enhance young children’s perception of and agency in their of leaders (pp. 39–58). New York: Psychology Press/Routledge.
learning. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 46–54. Reitan, T., & Stenberg, S.Å. (2019). From classroom to conscription. Leadership
Maskell, S., McCarron, G. P., Cannon, J., Zhou, S., Zacarro, S. J., & Goldstein, T. R. emergence in childhood and early adulthood. The Leadership Quarterly, 30(3),
(2022). The Leadership Stories Our Youth Are Told: Characterizations of Leadership 298–319.
Behaviors and Orientations in Popular Youth TV Shows. Journal of Youth and Ridell, P. M. (2017). Reward and threat in the adolescent brain: Implications for
Adolescence, 51(1), 114–127. leadership development. Leadership & Organizational Development Journal, 38(4),
Mawson, B. (2011). Children’s leadership strategies in early childhood. Journal of 530–548. https://doi.org/10.1108/LODJ-03-2015-0062
Research in Childhood Education, 25(4), 327–338. Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Curby, T. W., Grimm, K. J., Nathanson, L., & Brock, L. L. (2009).
McCauley, C. D., DeRue, D. S., Yost, P. R., & Taylor, S. (2014). Experience-driven leader The contribution of children’s self-regulation and classroom quality to children’s
development: Models, tools, best practices, and advice for on-the-job development. San adaptive behaviors in the kindergarten classroom. Developmental Psychology, 45(4),
Francisco, CA: Wiley. 958–972.
McCauley, C. D., & Douglas, C. A. (1998). Developmental relationships. In Rissanen, M. (2017). “It’s as if…” Preschoolers Encountering Contemporary
C. D. McCauley, R. S. Moxley, & E. Van Velsor (Eds.), The Center for Creative Photography. International Journal of Education & the Arts, 18(28), 1–29.
Leadership handbook of leadership development (pp. 160–193). San Francisco: Jossey- Robertson, J. (2009). Coaching leadership learning through partnership. School
Bass. Leadership and Management, 29(1), 39–49.
McCauley, C. D., Drath, W. H., Palus, C. J., O’Connor, P. M., & Baker, B. A. (2006). The Safra, L., Algan, Y., Tecu, T., Grèzes, J., Baumard, N., & Chevallier, C. (2017). Childhood
use of constructive-developmental theory to advance the understanding of harshness predicts long-lasting leader preferences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38
leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 17(6), 634–653. (5), 645–651.
McCauley, C. D., Ruderman, M. N., Ohlott, P. J., & Morrow, J. E. (1994). Assessing the Sameroff, A. (1975). Transactional models of early social relations. Human Development,
developmental components of managerial jobs. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79(4), 18, 65–79.
544–560. Sameroff, A. (2010). A unified theory of development: A dialectic integration of nature
McLean, K. C., & Breen, A. V. (2009). Processes and content of narrative identity and nurture. Child Development, 81(1), 6–22.
development in adolescence: Gender and well-being. Developmental Psychology, 45 Schlosser, M., 2015. Agency. In: Zalta, E.N. (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of
(3), 702–710. philosophy, Fall 2015 Edition.
Meder, B., Wu, C. M., Schulz, E., & Ruggeri, A. (2021). Development of directed and Schneider, W. (2008). The development of metacognitive knowledge in children and
random exploration in children. Developmental Science, 24(4), e13095. adolescents: Major trends and implications for education. Mind, Brain, and Education,
Merriam, S. B., & Heuer, B. (1996). Meaning-making, adult learning and development: A 2(3), 114–121.
model with implications for practice. International Journal of Lifelong Education, 15 Schulz, E., Wu, C. M., Ruggeri, A., & Meder, B. (2019). Searching for rewards like a child
(4), 243–255. means less generalization and more directed exploration. Psychological Science, 30,
Meyer, M. S., & Rinn, A. N. (2021). Developing leadership talent in adolescents and 1561–1572.
emerging adults: A systematic review. Gifted Child Quarterly, 65(3), 287–313. Schulz, L. E. (2015). Infants explore the unexpected. Science, 348, 42–43.
Montroy, J. J., Bowles, R. P., Skibbe, L. E., McClelland, M. M., & Morrison, F. J. (2016). Schwartz, S. J., Cote, J. E., & Arnett, J. J. (2005). Identity and agency in emerging
The development of self-regulation across early childhood. Developmental Psychology, adulthood: Two developmental routes in the individualization process. Youth and
52(11), 1744–1762. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000159 Society, 37, 201–229.
Moran, M. M., & Weiss, M. R. (2006). Peer leadership in sport: Links with friendship, peer Schwebel, D. C., Rosen, C. S., & Singer, J. L. (1999). Preschoolers’ pretend play and
acceptance, psychological characteristics, and athletic ability. Journal of Applied theory of mind: The role of jointly constructed pretence. British Journal of
Sport Psychology, 18(2), 97–113. Developmental Psychology, 17(3), 333–348.
Morrissey, A. M., Scott, C., & Rahimi, M. (2017). A comparison of sociodramatic play Shin, M. S., Recchia, S. L., Lee, S. Y., Lee, Y. J., & Mullarkey, L. S. (2004). Understanding
processes of preschoolers in a naturalized and a traditional outdoor space. early childhood leadership: Emerging competencies in the context of relationships.
International Journal of Play, 6(2), 177–197. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 2(3), 301–316.
Moss, S. A., Dowling, N., & Callanan, J. (2009). Towards an integrated model of Slot, P. L., Mulder, H., Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2017). Preschoolers’ cognitive
leadership and self-regulation. The Leadership Quarterly, 20(2), 162–176. and emotional self-regulation in pretend play: Relations with executive functions
Murphy, S. E., & Johnson, S. K. (2011). The benefits of a long-lens approach to leader and quality of play. Infant and Child Development, 26(6), e2038.
development: Understanding the seeds of leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 22, Smith, C. L. (2015). How coaching helps leadership resilience: The leadership
459–470. perspective. International Coaching Psychology Review, 10(1), 6–19.
Murphy, S. E., & Johnson, S. K. (2016). Leadership and leader developmental self- Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., Kristen, S., & Perst, H. (2012). Metacognition in infants and
efficacy: Their role in enhancing leader development efforts. New Directions for young children. In M. J. Beran, J. L. Brandl, J. Perner, & J. Proust (Eds.), Foundations
Student Leadership, 2016(149), 73–84. of metacognition (pp. 119–133). Oxford University Press.
Nesbit, P. L. (2012). The role of self-reflection, emotional management of feedback, and Stavans, M., & Baillargeon, R. (2019). Infants expect leaders to right wrongs. Proceedings
self-regulation processes in self-directed leadership development. Human Resource of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(33), 16292–16301.
Development Review, 11(2), 203–226. Stavans, M., & Diesendruck, G. (2021). Children hold leaders primarily responsible, not
Noe, R. A. (2017). Employee training and development (7th ed.). McGraw Hill Education. entitled. Child Development, 92, 308–323.
Ohlott, P. J. (2004). Job assignments. In C. McCauley, & E. V. Velsor (Eds.), The Center for Strain, M. R., & Webber, D. A. (2017). High school experiences, the gender wage gap, and
Creative Leadership handbook of leadership development (2nd ed., pp. 151–182). San the selection of occupation. Applied Economics, 49(49), 5040–5049.
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Sugarman, J., & Sokol, B. (2012). Human agency and development: An introduction and
Ohtani, K., & Hisasaka, T. (2018). Beyond intelligence: A meta-analytic review of the theoretical sketch [Editorial]. New Ideas in Psychology, 30(1), 1–14.
relationship among metacognition, intelligence, and academic performance. Terrizzi, B. F., Woodward, A. M., & Beier, J. S. (2020). Young children and adults
Metacognition and Learning, 13(2), 179–212. associate social power with indifference to others’ needs. Journal of Experimental
Olivares, O. J., Peterson, G., & Hess, K. P. (2007). An existential-phenomenological Child Psychology, 198, Article 104867.
framework for understanding leadership development experiences. Leadership and Trommsdorff, G. (2012). Development of “agentic” regulation in cultural context: The
Organization Development Journal, 28, 76–91. role of self and world views. Child Development Perspectives, 6(1), 19–26.
Oliveira, K. A. C. (2016). Children’s implicit leadership theories in middle childhood: Turman, N. T., Garcia, K. C. A., & Howes, S. (2018). Deepening attention to social
Christian children’s perceptions in the Cavite province. Journal of Research on location in building leader and leadership efficacy. New Directions for Student
Christian Education, 25(3), 251–272. Leadership, 2018(159), 65–76.
Osterhaus, C., & Koerber, S. (2021). The development of advanced theory of mind in Ubaka, A., Lu, X., & Gutierrez, L. (2022). Testing the generalizability of the white
middle childhood: A longitudinal study from age 5 to 10 years. Child Development, 92 leadership standard in the post-Obama era. The Leadership Quarterly, 101591.
(5), 1872–1888. Uhl-Bien, M., Riggio, R. E., Lowe, K. B., & Carsten, M. K. (2014). Followership theory: A
Oswell, D. (2013). The agency of children: From family to global human rights. Cambridge: review and research agenda. The Leadership Quarterly, 25(1), 83–104.
Cambridge University Press. van der Veen, C., de Mey, L., van Kruistum, C., & van Oers, B. (2017). The effect of
Parkhill, A., Deans, C. L., & Chapin, L. A. (2018). Pre-leadership processes in leadership productive classroom talk and metacommunication on young children’s oral
training for adolescents. Children and Youth Services Review, 88, 375–379. communicative competence and subject matter knowledge: An intervention study in
Pasupathi, M., & Mansour, E. (2006). Adult age differences in autobiographical reasoning early childhood education. Learning and Instruction, 48, 14–22.
in narratives. Developmental Psychology, 42(5), 798–808. https://doi.org/10.1037/ van Doeselaar, L., McLean, K. C., Meeus, W., Denissen, J. J., & Klimstra, T. A. (2020).
0012-1649.42.5.798 Adolescents’ identity formation: Linking the narrative and the dual-cycle approach.
Peterson, C. C., O’Reilly, K., & Wellman, H. M. (2016). Deaf and hearing children’s Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 49(4), 818–835.
development of theory of mind, peer popularity, and leadership during middle Van Velsor, E., & McCauley, C. D. (2004). Our view of leadership development. In
childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 149, 146–158. C. D. McCauley, & E. Van Velsor (Eds.), The Center for Creative Leadership Handbook
Piaget, J. (1950). The Psychology of Intelligence. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. of Leadership Development (2nd ed., pp. 1–22). San Francisco: CA7 Jossey-Bass.

16
J.A. Cannon et al. The Leadership Quarterly xxx (xxxx) xxx

Varpanen, J. (2019). What is children’s agency? A review of conceptualizations used in Wolf, J. (2022). Implications of pretend play for Theory of Mind research. Synthese, 200
early childhood education research. Educational Research Review, 28, Article 100288. (6), 1–21.
Vasquez, A. C., Patall, E. A., Fong, C. J., Corrigan, A. S., & Pine, L. (2016). Parent Yamaguchi, R. (2001). Children’s learning groups: A study of emergent leadership,
autonomy support, academic achievement, and psychosocial functioning: A meta- dominance, and group effectiveness. Small Group Research, 32(6), 671–697.
analysis of research. Educational Psychology Review, 28(3), 605–644. Yamaguchi, R., & Maehr, M. L. (2003). A multi-method study of children’s emergent
Vega, C. E. (2020). Understanding the next generation of leaders: An exploratory study of leadership in collaborative learning groups. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social
constructions of leadership during childhood. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Research. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. 476660).
Wollongong). Yeager, K. L., & Callahan, J. L. (2016). Learning to lead: Foundations of emerging leader
Vygotsky, L. S. (1998). Child psychology. The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky (Vol. 5). identity development. Advances in Developing Human Resources, 18, 286–300.
New York: Plenum Press. Yuan, Y., Sun, X., Chen, Q., Liu, Z., & Xue, G. (2022). Family socioeconomic status and
Walker, D. O., Reichard, R. J., Riggio, R. E., & Keller Hansbrough, T. (2020). Who might youth leadership potential: Serial mediating effects of parental rearing behaviors and
support a tyrant? An exploration of links between adolescent family conflict and youth self-esteem. Current Psychology, 41, 2034–2044. https://doi.org/10.1007/
endorsement of tyrannical implicit leadership theories. Journal of Leadership & s12144-020-00728-x
Organizational Studies, 27(4), 340–356. Zaccaro, S. J. (2007). Trait-based perspectives of leadership. American Psychologist, 62,
Wallace, D. M., Torres, E. M., & Zaccaro, S. J. (2021). Just what do we think we are 6–16.
doing? Learning outcomes of leader and leadership development. The Leadership Zaccaro, S. J. (2014). 2 leadership memes: From ancient history and literature to twenty-
Quarterly, 32(5), Article 101494. first century theory and research. In D. Day (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of leadership
Wallace, D., & Zaccaro, S. J. (2022). WHO WANTS TO BE HERE? Empirically and organizations (pp. 13–38). Oxford: University Press.
Investigating Motivation and Engagement During Leader Development. UMBC Zaccaro, S. J., & Banks, D. (2004). Leader visioning and adaptability: Bridging the gap
Faculty Collection. between research and practice on developing the ability to manage change. Human
Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75 Resource Management Journal, 43, 367–380.
(2), 523–541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x Zaccaro, S. J., Gilbert, J. A., Thor, K. K., & Mumford, M. D. (1991). Leadership and social
Whitebread, D., & O’Sullivan, L. (2012). Preschool children’s social pretend play: intelligence: Linking social perceptiveness and behavioral flexibility to leader
Supporting the development of metacommunication, metacognition and self- effectiveness. The Leadership Quarterly, 2(4), 317–342.
regulation. International Journal of Play, 1(2), 197–213. Zaccaro, S. J., Green, J. P., Dubrow, S., & Kolze, M. (2018). Leader individual differences,
Wilcox, C. D. (2004). Order in the Kidsville Court: A study of the longitudinal co- situational parameters, and leadership outcomes: A comprehensive review and
construction of child leader development and group activity development. (Doctoral integration. The Leadership Quarterly, 29, 2–43.
dissertation, Michigan State University). Zelazo, P. D. (2015). Executive function: Reflection, iterative reprocessing, complexity,
Wilson, M. S., & Yip, J. (2010). Grounding leader development: Cultural perspectives. and the developing brain. Developmental Review, 38, 55–68.
Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 3(1), 52–55.

17

You might also like