Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Affective Disorders 165 (2014) 24–30

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Affective Disorders


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jad

Research report

The effect of exercise on hippocampal volume and neurotrophines


in patients with major depression—A randomized clinical trial
Jesper Krogh a,n, Egill Rostrup b, Carsten Thomsen c, Betina Elfving d, Poul Videbech e,
Merete Nordentoft a
a
Mental Health Centre Copenhagen, Mental Health Services in the Capital Region of Denmark, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
b
Functional Imaging Unit, Department of Diagnostics, Copenhagen University Hospital, Glostrup Hospital, Denmark
c
Department of Radiology, Rigshospitalet, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
d
Translational Neuropsychiatry Unit, Department of Clinical Medicine, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
e
Department for Depression and Anxiety Q, Aarhus University Hospital, Risskov, Denmark

art ic l e i nf o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Background: The hippocampal volume is reduced in patients with major depression. Exercise leads to an
Received 4 January 2014 increased hippocampal volume in schizophrenia and in healthy old adults. The effect of exercise on
Received in revised form hippocampal volume is potentially mediated by brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), vascular
15 April 2014
endothelial growth factor (VEGF), and insulin like growth factor 1 (IGF-1). The aim of this trial was to
Accepted 15 April 2014
Available online 23 April 2014
assess the effect of an aerobic exercise intervention on hippocampal volume and serum BDNF, VEGF, and
IGF-1 in patients with major depression.
Keywords: Methods: Patients were randomized to an aerobic exercise intervention (n ¼ 41) or a control condi-
Hippocampus tion (n ¼38). Both interventions consisted of three supervised sessions per week during a three months
Exercise
period.
Depression
Results: Post-intervention the increase in maximal oxygen uptake was 3.90 ml/kg/min (SD 5.1) in the
Neurogenesis
Memory aerobic exercise group and 0.95 ml/kg/min (SD 6.2) in the control group (p¼0.03). The hippocampal
BDNF volume, BDNF, VEGF, or IGF-1 did not differ between the two groups. Post-hoc we found a positive
association between change in hippocampal volume and verbal memory (Rho ¼0.27; p¼ 0.05) and
change in hippocampal volume and depressive symptoms (Rho ¼ 0.30; p¼0.03).
Limitations: Participation was low in both groups corresponding to an average participation of one
session per week.
Conclusion: Despite a significant increase in maximal oxygen uptake, a pragmatic exercise intervention
did not increase hippocampal volume or resting levels of neurotrophines in out-patients with mild to
moderate major depression. Trial identifier: ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT00695552)
& 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction the observed reduction in hippocampal volume in patients with


depression is due to decreased neurogenesis.
The hippocampal region is thought to play a pivotal role in major Randomised clinical trials suggest that exercise leads to an
depression. This temporal structure is involved in cognitive skills increased hippocampal volume in healthy old adults (Erickson
such as memory, learning, regulation of the hypothalamic–pituitary– et al., 2011) while the evidence for schizophrenia is conflicting
adrenal (HPA) axis, and mood (Eisch and Petrik, 2012). Meta-analysis (Pajonk et al., 2010; Scheewe et al., 2013). Furthermore, in two of
suggests that the bilateral hippocampal volume is reduced 0.3–0.4 these trials the increase in hippocampal volume was positively
standard deviations in patients with depression compared to healthy associated to increased maximal oxygen uptake, brain derived
controls (Videbech and Ravnkilde, 2004). It has been speculated that neurotrophic factor (BDNF), spatial and verbal memory (Erickson
et al. 2011; Pajonk et al., 2010).
Studies on rodents have found that the exercise related
n increase in memory and learning is mediated through an up-
Correspondence to: Mental Health Centre Copenhagen, Bispebjerg Bakke 23,
Opg. 13a, DK-2400, Denmark. Tel.: þ45 2553 54 85. regulation of BDNF, which has the ability to stimulate synaptic-
E-mail address: Jesper.krogh@regionh.dk (J. Krogh). plasticity (Tapia-Arancibia et al., 2004). The exercise induced cell

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.04.041
0165-0327/& 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
J. Krogh et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 165 (2014) 24–30 25

proliferation is inhibited by peripheral blockade of either insulin 2.4. Blinding


growth factor 1 (IGF-1) or vascular endothelial growth factor
(VEGF) (Fabel et al., 2003; Trejo et al., 2008), which indicate that Neither participants nor the physiotherapist conducting the
the exercise induced neurogenesis is dependent on not only BDNF intervention were blinded to the allocation. The outcome assessors
but peripheral IGF-I and VEGF. Furthermore, it has been shown (the study nurse, the laboratory and the MRI technicians) were all
that antidepressant treatment up-regulates BDNF in humans blinded to participant allocation. Prior to the follow-up interview,
(Shimizu et al., 2003) and a relationship between genetic variants participants were instructed not to reveal their allocation to the
of BDNF locus and depression has been implied (Schumacher et al., outcome assessors. The statistical analysis and preparation of the
2005). On this background, it could be argued that low BDNF levels first draft was carried out blinded to group assignments.
contribute to the pathophysiology of depression, and that exercise
has a beneficial effect on mood and cognitive function in patients 2.5. Physical examination
with depression through an up-regulation of BDNF, VEGF, and
IGF-1. For the physical examination, the participants were requested to
The aim of the present study was to determine if a pragmatic report at the research department between 8:00 and 10:00 a.m. The
exercise intervention compared to an exercise control condition participants were instructed not to take any food or liquids except for
would increase hippocampal volume and increase serum levels of water beginning from midnight prior to the examination and abstain
neurotrophines (BDNF, VEGF, and IGF-1) in patients with major from strenuous physical activity prior to the examination. Height and
depression. weight was measured using an electronic weight (Soehnle Medicals,
Type 7700, Backnang, Germany). Blood pressure was measured after
5 min of rest in a sitting position. The mean of three consecutive
measurements is reported. To estimate the cardiovascular fitness of
2. Methods
the participants a bicycle cardiopulmonary exercise test (Ergomedic
839 e, Monark, Vansbro, Sweden) was used based on Andersen's
2.1. Design
cycle exercise protocol (Andersen, 1997).
The study was designed as a randomised, parallel-group,
2.6. Intervention
superiority trial with blinded outcome assessment. The trial proto-
col was approved by the local ethics committee (H-A-2008-046),
After baseline assessment the patients were randomised to
the Danish Data Protection Agency (J.nr. 2008-41-2354), and
either aerobic exercise or a control group as described in detail
registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT00695552). Verbal and written
elsewhere(Krogh et al., 2012). In short, both groups were sched-
informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in
uled to participate in 45 min supervised sessions three times per
the trial.
week in a 3 months programme. In the aerobic training group the
participants exercised on stationary bikes at approximately 80% of
2.2. Study population their maximal heart rate, while the training in the exercise control
group was predominantly stretching exercises and low impact
All patients were recruited for participation from the DEMO-II exercise such as throwing and catching balls.
trial, a randomized clinical trial evaluating the antidepressant
effect of aerobic exercise (Krogh et al., 2012). The initial 90 2.7. Outcomes
consecutively enroled participants were asked to participate in
the current sub-study. Eligible participants were men and women 2.7.1. Hippocampal volume
between 18 and 60 years of age, referred from a clinical setting by The primary outcome for this sub-study was the left and right
a physician or a psychologist, a diagnose of major depression hippocampal volumes measured post-intervention. Hippocampal
(DSM-IV) based on the Danish version of the Mini International volume was estimated using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI).
Neuropsychiatric Interview (Bech et al., 1999). The participants all MRI volumetric images were acquired on a 3.0T Siemens system
scored above 12 on the HAM-D17 and were able to comprehend using a T1-weighted 3D gradient MPR sequence with a resolution
the informed consent statement. Exclusion criteria were current of 1  1  1 mm with the following parameters: TR ¼2250 ms;
drug abuse, any antidepressant medication within the last two TE 3 ms; FoV read ¼256 mm; FoV phase ¼100.0%; and flip angle
months, current psychotherapeutic treatment, contraindications 91. All images were performed in the coronal plane at an angle
to physical exercise, regular recreational exercise over 1 h per perpendicular to the hippocampal long axis. All tracings were
week, suicidal behaviour according to the 17-item Hamilton manually performed by the same rater (JK) who was instructed by
Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D17, item 3 over 2), pregnancy, or a specialist in neuroimaging (ER). After an initial training period
current/previous psychotic or manic symptoms. the intra-rater reliability was obtained by estimating the left
and right hippocampus of the same 10 patients with at least one
day between the first and second estimations. The intraclass-
2.3. Randomisation correlation-coefficient of the first and second assessments was
0.98 (95% CI. 0.91–0.99) and 0.95 (95% CI. 0.82–0.99) for the right
Participants were randomized with a 1:1 ratio to either aerobic and left hippocampal regions. In absolute numbers the median
exercise or an attention control group. Randomisation was strati- difference between first and second assessments was  12.5 mm3
fied according to severity of depression (high or low depression (IQR  121 to 96) and 129 mm3 (IQR 25–178) for right and left
score; high 417 HAM-D17) and blood pressure (high or low blood hippocampal volumes.
pressure; high 4 140/95 mmHg). The randomisation was centra- Hippocampus was defined as the cornu ammonis, dentate
lised and carried out by the Copenhagen Trial Unit (CTU) using gyrus, and subiculum. Regions of interest were traced in the
a computerised randomisation sequence with alternating block coronal plane. Sagittal and axial planes were used continuously
sizes (alternating 8, 10, and 12) unknown to the investigators. Prior to assist tracing in the coronal plane. All estimations of volumes
to the first training session the trial physiotherapist did contact the were acquired by tracing regions of interest on each coronal image
CTU by phone for participant allocation. and finally all slices were added in order to determine the volume.
26 J. Krogh et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 165 (2014) 24–30

For tracing and volume estimation an in house programme was (Buschke and Fuld, 1974). A list of 10 different unrelated words was
used (RIP, written for Matlab). Border definitions were based on read aloud to the participant. The participant is then asked to recall the
common segmentation protocols (Konrad et al., 2009). The ante- list. The interviewer repeats the words that the patient misses and the
rior border was located in the sagittal plane. Differentiation of participant is asked to try again until all ten words can be said, or until
amygdala and hippocampus was based on the alveus. The superior ten attempts. The score is the total number of blanks or mistakes; thus,
border was identified using alveus as an internal landmark, or the a high score indicates poorer performance. Visuospatial memory was
temporal horn of the lateral ventricle as an external landmark. The assessed by the Rey's Complex Figure Test (Meyers et al., 1996). In this
inferior border was defined as the white matter of the parahippo- test the participant was shown a geometrically complex figure on a
campal gyrus. The inferomedial border was drawn from the tip of sheet of paper and asked to copy it to another sheet of paper. When
white matter from the parahippocampal gyrus medially to cisterna this is done the drawings and the original are put away and after three
ambiens. The lateral border was defined by CSF of the lateral minutes the participant is asked to draw as much of the figure they
ventricle as an external landmark. The posterior border was can recall. The score is calculated based on the tree-minute recall
located where an ovoid grey matter appear inferiomedially to drawing. A high score reflects better performance.
the trigone of the lateral ventricle. Head of the hippocampus was
anteriorly identified as described above and posteriorly as the last 2.8. Statistics
coronal image where the uncal apex was visible (Malykhin et al.,
2007). The total volume of the brain tissue and grey matter The statistical analysis was based on the intention-to-treat princi-
was estimated with SIENAX (Smith et al., 2002), part of the FSL ple including all randomised patients regardless of subsequent with-
software package (2013) (FMRIB Software Library). drawal or deviation from the protocol. Assessment of variables
measured at baseline and post-intervention was reported using mean
and standard deviation. Post-intervention values were compared using
2.7.2. Neurotrophic factors
student's t-test for unpaired data. Missing values were imputed using
Previously, we have shown the BDNF levels in plasma samples,
the multiple imputation technique available in SPSS, with 100 impu-
but not in serum or whole blood samples, to be affected by the
tations and 20 iterations having allocation and baseline values as the
preanalytical conditions (Elfving et al., 2010). BDNF, VEGF, and IGF-
predicting variables. Per protocol analysis was undertaken including
1 were all estimated using serum samples collected from an
participants who attended Z50% of the scheduled sessions.
antecubital vein after five minutes of rest and stored at 801
All significance tests were 2-tailed and p-values r0.05 were
until analysis. Quantification of serum BDNF, VEGF, and IGF-1
considered significant. Statistical analysis was conducted using
levels was performed with the enzyme-linked immunosorbent
SPSS version 19.0 (SPSS, Inc.: Chicago, USA).
assay (ELISA) kits. The same batch number was used for the entire
experiments of BDNF, VEGF, and IGF-1. The standard curves and
the samples were run in duplicate, blinded for case and control. 3. Results
Serum BDNF levels were measured using the Quantikine Human
BDNF Immunoassay from R&D Systems (USA). The determination The initial 90 patients included in the DEMO-II trial were
was processed according to the manufacturer's specifications and considered for enrolment in the current sub-study, as displayed in
the absorbance was immediately measured at 450 nm with wave- Fig. 1. The inclusion of patients occurred between September 2008
length correction set to 540 nm (EL 800 Universal Microplate reader, and April 2010. Eleven patients were excluded from MRI due to either
Bio-Tek instruments, INC). For the BDNF measurements the samples severe claustrophobia (n¼10) or metallic implants (n¼1), leaving 79
were diluted 1:20 to be within the range of the standard curve. participants to constitute the current sample. Forty-one were allo-
The standard curves ranged from 62.5 to 2000 pg/ml BDNF. Three cated to the aerobic exercise group and 38 were allocated to the
internal BDNF controls (high: 1959–3315 pg/ml, medium: 1099– control group. The mean age in the included group was 41.3 (SD 12.1)
1525 pg/ml, low: 344–506 pg/ml), commercial available from R&D years and consisted of 53/79 (67.1%) female participants. The mean
Systems (USA), were included on each plate. HAM-D17 was 19.0 (SD 4.3), 32/79 (40.5%) had recurrent depression,
The serum VEGF levels were measured using the Quantikine and 43/79 (54.4%) generalised anxiety disorder comorbid with
Human VEGF Immunoassay from R&D Systems (USA). The deter- their depression. The aerobic exercise group and control group
mination was processed according to the manufacturer's specifica- were comparable with respect to baseline demographic and clinical
tions and the absorbance was immediately measured at 450 nm characteristics as displayed in Table 1.
with wavelength correction set to 540 nm (EL 800 Universal
Microplate reader, Bio-Tek instruments, INC). The standard curves
3.1. Compliance
ranged from 31.2 to 1000 pg/ml VEGF and the samples were
run undiluted to be within the range of the standard curve.
The mean attendance was 12.9 (SD 9.6) sessions in the aerobic
Three internal VEGF controls (high: 682–1354 pg/ml, medium:
exercise group compared to 12.4 (SD 8.6) sessions in the control
350–686 pg/ml, low: 115–259 pg/ml), commercially available from
group (p¼0.54) of a planned total of 36 sessions. This corresponds to
R&D Systems (USA), were included on each plate.
an average participation of one session per week. The per-protocol
The serum IGF-1 levels were measured using RayBios Human
population (Z18 sessions) consisted of 14 participants from the
IGF-1 ELISA kit from RayBiotech, Inc (USA). The determination was
aerobic exercise group with a mean number of attended sessions of
processed according to the manufacturer's specifications and the
24.1 (SD 4.7) and 13 participants from the control group with a mean
absorbance was immediately measured at 450 nm (EL 800 Uni-
number of attended sessions of 22.4 (SD 2.8) (p¼0.10).
versal Microplate reader, Bio-Tek instruments, INC). The standard
curves ranged from 0.25 to 60 ng/ml IGF-1 and the samples were
3.2. Follow-up
run undiluted to be within the range of the standard curve.
Post-intervention, 33/41 (80.5%) in the aerobic exercise group
2.7.3. Assessment of mood and cognitive function participated in a MRI scan and 22/38 (57.9%) in the control group
Depression severity was assessed using the Hamilton Depression (p¼ 0.03) suggesting that follow-up was skewed. The pre-inter-
Rating Scale with 17-items (Hamilton, 1960). Assessment of verbal vention right hippocampal volume was 182 mm3 lower (95% CI
memory was assessed by the Buschke's Selective Reminding test 0.1–364; p¼0.05), diastolic blood pressure was 5.3 mmHg higher
J. Krogh et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 165 (2014) 24–30 27

Fig. 1. Flow-diagram of participant flow.

Table 1 3.4. Hippocampal volume


Clinical and demographical characteristics.
Post-intervention, there were no differences in right or left
Aerobic exercise Exercise control P
N ¼ 41 N¼ 38 value hippocampal volume as illustrated in Fig. 2. Neither did the
intervention result in any changes of total hippocampal volume
Age, years 38.9 (11.7) 43.8 (12.2) 0.07 or grey matter volume as displayed in Table 2.
Female, n (%) 30 (73.2) 23 (60.5) 0.34
Post-hoc we adjusted for age and gender as main effects. This
Smoking, daily, n (%) 13 (31.7) 15 (39.5) 0.54
did not change the results. Neither did inclusion of age as an
Psychometrics interaction term (age  allocation) influence post-intervention
HAM-D17 19.0 (3.9) 18.9 (4.6) 0.92
hippocampal volumes.
HAM-A14 18.1 (5.0) 16.3 (6.0) 0.17
Generalised anxiety, n (%) 23 (56.1) 20 (52.6) 0.82
Recurrent depression, n (%) 17 (41.4) 20 (52.6) 0.37 3.5. Neurotrophines and cognitive skills
Age at first depression 33.9 (11.6) 38.8 (13.1) 0.09

Somatic Post-intervention, we found no differences in serum BDNF,


Systolic blood pressure, 121.8 (15.5) 124.3 (17.7) 0.52 VEGF, and IGF-1 levels or any of the assessed cognitive skills
mmHg between the two groups.
Diastolic blood pressure, 80.1 (9.2) 82.6 (13.0) 0.34
mmHg
Weight, kg (SD) 76.0 (22.5) 75.6 (17.5) 0.96 3.6. Per-protocol analysis
Body mass index (SD) 25.8 (6.4) 25.2 (5.1) 0.70
Cholesterol (SD) 5.2 (1.1) 5.3 (1.4) 0.72 The per-protocol population (Z18 sessions) consisted of 14
Maximal oxygen uptake 26.5 (7.2) 27.2 (5.9) 0.65
participants from the aerobic exercise group with a mean atten-
Mean and standard deviation are reported unless otherwise noted. dance of 24.1 (SD 4.7) sessions per week and 13 participants from
Abbreviations: HAM-D17—Hamilton Depression Scale, 17 items; HAM-A14—Hamil- the control group with a mean attendance of 22.4 (SD 2.8) sessions
ton Anxiety Scale, 14 items. per week (p ¼0.10). Also in this restricted sample we did not find
any differences in right hippocampal volume (p ¼0.31), left hippo-
(95% CI 0.1–10.7; p¼0.05), and number of attended sessions was on campal volume (p¼ 0.35), total hippocampal volume (p¼ 0.85), or
average 10.5 higher (95% CI 7.5–13.5; po0.001) in participants grey matter volume (p ¼0.37).
attending the post-intervention MRI compared to non-attenders.
Otherwise the attenders and non-attenders to post-intervention 3.7. Post-hoc analysis
MRI were not significantly different on any other reported variable.
As illustrated in Fig. 3, change in maximal oxygen uptake was
3.3. Maximal oxygen uptake not associated to change in total hippocampal volume in this study
(p ¼0.85). A positive association was found between an increase in
Post-intervention the mean maximal oxygen uptake increased right hippocampal volume and a decrease in HAM-D17 score and
with 3.90 ml/kg/min (SD 5.1) in the aerobic exercise group and an increase in left hippocampal volume and an increase in verbal
0.95 ml/kg/min (SD 6.2) in the control group (p ¼0.03). performance of the Buschke selective reminding test as illustrated
28 J. Krogh et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 165 (2014) 24–30

cm3 ITT analysis PP analysis


Right Left Right Left
P = 0.91 P = 0.80 P = 0.17 P = 0.50 P = 0.31 P = 0.35

Fig. 2. Right and left hippocampal volumes in response to an exercise intervention. Data are presented as mean (SD). Intention-to-treat analysis (ITT; n¼ 41þ 38) was
conducted using multiple imputation adjusting for baseline right hippocampal volume and diastolic blood pressure. Per protocol analysis (PP; n¼ 14þ 13) was conducted and
post-intervention data is shown here.

Table 2
Hippocampus volume, neurotrophic factors and cognitive skill before and after an exercise intervention.

Aerobic exercise Exercise control P valuea


N ¼ 41 N ¼ 38 Between-group

Pre-intervention Post-intervention Pre-intervention Post-intervention Pre Post

Brain volumes
Total hippocampal, cm3 6.353 (0.7) 6.325 (0.7) 6.421 (0.6) 6.380.0 (0.7) 0.54 0.88
Right hippocampal 3.151 (0.4) 3.117 (0.4) 3.136 (0.4) 3.110 (0.4) 0.91 0.80
Anterior part 1.537 (0.3) 1.546 (0.3) 1.599 (0.3) 1.622 (0.3) 0.43 0.35
Left hippocampal 3.202 (0.3) 3.201 (0.3) 3.284 (0.3) 3.267 (0.4) 0.17 0.50
Anterior part 1.644 (0.2) 1.605 (0.2) 1.699 (0.2) 1.651 (0.2) 0.38 0.50
Grey matter 624.337 (60.8) 621.455 (62.0) 605.591 (101.0) 605.960 (103.0) 0.32 0.41
Total brain 1166.187 (108.0) 1163.100 (108.0) 1167.996 (92.7) 1165.000 (96.0) 0.97 0.64

Neurotrophic factors
BDNF, pg/ml 25347.0 (6224) 26005.5 (7184) 25488.0 (7165) 25043.8 (7428) 0.93 0.47
VEGF, pg/ml 309.2 (175.7) 303.8 (182.5) 332.1 (211.3) 312.6 (244.7) 0.62 0.86
IGF-1, ng/ml 86.6 (110) 81.7 (114.6) 88.6 (104) 67.4 (110.3) 0.94b 0.54

Cognitive function
DART 34.0 (9.9) – 34.6 (7.6) – 0.79 –
Buschke, total 16.4 (12.0) 11.4 (10.9) 17.9 (11.8) 12.9 (12.3) 0.57 0.60
Rey recall 20.9 (6.7) 25.5 (6.4) 21.3 (7.2) 23.4 (7.4) 0.79 0.28

Mean and standard deviation are reported.


Missing data post-intervention was handled by multiple imputations.
a
P values are based on independent t-test. All comparison of brain volumes are adjusted for whole brain volume.
b
Confirmed in non-parametric analysis.

in Fig. 4. An association between increase in right hippocampal


volume and improved performance of the Rey memory test
(rho¼ 0.23; p ¼0.10) was borderline significant. No other associa-
tion between change in maximal oxygen uptake, hippocampal
volume, and neurotrophines was observed.
At follow-up, 4/33 (12.1%) patients in the aerobic exercise group
and 4/22 (18.2%) in the control group had started antidepressant
medication (p¼ 0.70). Adjusting for this in the analysis did not
change the results.

4. Discussion

The primary aim of the current study was to assess hippocam-


pal volume in response to aerobic exercise. We did not find that a
three month aerobic exercise intervention increased the hippo-
campal volume in a group of mild to moderate depressed out-
patients. Furthermore, the aerobic exercise intervention did not
Fig. 3. Association between change in total hippocampal volume and change in
increase serum levels of BDNF, VEGF or IGF-1. We did, however,
maximal oxygen uptake. Spearmann's rho and p-value is based on all randomized find an association between an increase in hippocampal volume,
participants. Missing data post-intervention were handled by multiple imputations. improved depression, and verbal memory independent of exercise.
J. Krogh et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 165 (2014) 24–30 29

Fig. 4. The association between change in right hippocampal volume and HAM-D17 and change in left hippocampal volume and verbal memory. Spearmann's rho and p-
value is based on all randomized participants. Missing data post-intervention were handled by multiple imputations. 1Busckhes Selective Reminding Test.

To our best knowledge, this is the first study to assess hippo- difference post-intervention as observed in the study by Erickson
campal volume in response to aerobic exercise in patients with et al. (2011). Therefore, we cannot exclude smaller increases in
major depression. Several randomized clinical trials have pre- hippocampal volume in response to an exercise intervention.
viously shown an increase in hippocampal volume in response However, this potential effect on hippocampal volume did not
to aerobic exercise. An increase of 12% was found in a small study produce clinically relevant results. A neurotrophin like BNDF
of patients with schizophrenia in response to a three month increases in response to an acute bout of exercise and it is possible
exercise intervention (Pajonk et al., 2010) and an increase of 2% that the continuous stimulation of BDNF and other neurotrophines
was found in healthy older adults in response to a 6 months to the brain is responsible for hippocampal growth. To our knowl-
exercise programme (Erickson et al., 2011). These findings are edge, only three randomised clinical trials on exercise and hippo-
supported by non-controlled prospective studies in healthy campal growth have been published to date (Erickson et al., 2011;
humans demonstrating an association between increase in hippo- Pajonk et al., 2010;Scheewe et al., 2013) and none of these on
campal volume and increase in aerobic fitness (Parker et al., 2011; patients with depression. Both schizophrenia and increasing age
Pereira et al., 2007). Also, a substantial volume of animal studies are associated with decreased hippocampal volume. Recent meta-
support the association between exercise and hippocampal analysis of hippocampal volume in patients with depression
volume (Voss et al., 2013b). However, a recent semi-large rando- suggests that hippocampal volume is reduced only in a sub-
mized clinical trial of patients with schizophrenia was not able to group of patients minority of patients (McKinnon et al., 2009)
reproduce the previously reported findings in subjects with and it could be argued that a pragmatic exercise intervention is
schizophrenia (Scheewe et al., 2013). Interestingly, the increase unlikely to increase the volume of a non-affected brain structure.
in hippocampal volume in trials with positive findings has been The evidence of hippocampal growth in response to exercise in
linked to increase in maximal oxygen uptake. In the current study humans is therefore still uncertain and with the current study no
the increase in maximal oxygen uptake in the aerobic exercise data from human subjects supports that exercise in patients with
group was 14.7% compared to 11% in the study by Pajonk et al. depression will induce hippocampal growth.
(2010) and 7.8% in the study by Erickson et al. (2011) illustrating The increase in hippocampal volume is thought to be mediated
that increase in maximal oxygen uptake is not sufficient for partly by neurotrophines such as BDNF, VEGF, and IGF-1. BDNF is
hippocampal growth and a frequent stimulation of exercise responsible for neural survival, growth, and synaptic plasticity and
mediated factors on the hippocampus is potentially necessary. BDNF mRNA is upregulated in the dentate gyrus of the hippocam-
Therefore, one potential explanation is the low participation rate pal region in response to exercise (Vaynman et al., 2004). The
in the current study compared to the study by Pajonk et al. (2010) current study did not find that aerobic exercise training increases
who included 16 patients that participated in 75% of the sessions serum levels of BDNF, VEGF, or IGF-1, which is in line with a
or more. In our per protocol analysis (n ¼27), including partici- similar study of healthy older adults (Voss et al., 2013a). In rodents,
pants that participated in more than 50% of the sessions we did peripheral blockade of VEGF and IGF-1 has been shown to inhibit
not find any suggestions of hippocampal growth. It is possible that the exercise induced hippocampal growth. While BDNF increases
the effect of exercise on hippocampal volume was less than the in response to acute aerobic exercise, the effect of chronic aerobic
uncertainty of our measurement and changes of 2% would be exercise training is less clear (Huang et al., 2014). Of particular
smaller than our method would detect. Including patients for this interest is that Erickson et al. (2011) found that the exercise
sub-study ended after approaching patient number 90. This was mediated increase in hippocampal volume was positively asso-
based partly on a rough estimate on expected effect observed in ciated to increase in BDNF in healthy old participants, which was
the study by Pajonk et al. (2010), in which exercise programme not confirmed in the present study. Neither did the study by
was very similar to current study, and partly due to the availability Erickson et al., 2011 find an effect of exercise training on VEGF or
and costs of MRI. Assuming a two-sided type 1 error of 5% the IGF-1. IGF-1 should ideally be analysed in the context of the IGF-1
current study had a 92% chance (power) of finding a 10% difference carrier protein IGFB-3, but that analysis was not undertaken in the
post-intervention in left or right hippocampal volume, however, current study. While it is possible that BDNF in some populations
the current setting allow only a 10% chance (power) of finding a 2% will increase in response to aerobic training there is at present no
30 J. Krogh et al. / Journal of Affective Disorders 165 (2014) 24–30

evidence in favour of this in psychiatric populations. Neither is Erickson, K., Voss, M., Prakash, R., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L., Kim, J., Heo, S.,
there any support of aerobic exercise training will increase or Alves, H., White, S., Wojcicki, T, Mailey, E, Vieira, V., Martin, S., Pence, B.,
Woods, J., McAuley, E, Kramer, A., 2011. Exercise training increases size of
decrease VEGF or IGF-1 in this patient group. hippocampus and improves memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 108, 3017–3022.
We conducted post-hoc analysis to examine a number of FMRIB Software Library. www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl.
correlations and these results should purely be seen as hypothesis Fabel, K., Fabel, K., Tab, B., Kaufer, D., Baiker, A., Simmons, N., Kuo, C., Palmer, T.,
2003. VEGF is necessary for exercise-induced adult hippocampal neurogenesis.
generating. As illustrated, we found an increase in verbal memory
Eur. J. Neurosci. 18, 2803–2812.
performance (Buschke Selective Reminding Test). This association Hamilton, M., 1960. A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry
between verbal memory and left hippocampal volume is sup- 23, 56–62.
ported by a previous study finding an association between verbal Huang, T., Larsen, K.T., Ried-Larsen, M., Moller, N.C., Andersen, L.B., 2014. The
effects of physical activity and exercise on brain-derived neurotrophic factor in
memory and hippocampal microstructure but these findings were healthy humans: a review. Scand. J. Med. Sci. 24 (1), 1–10.
not specifically related to left or right hippocampal region (van Konrad, C., Ukas, T., Nebel, C., Arolt, V., Toga, A.W., Narr, K.L., 2009. Defining the
Norden et al., 2012). The association between verbal memory human hippocampus in cerebral magnetic resonance images – an overview of
performance and left hippocampal volume should also be inter- current segmentation protocols. NeuroImage 47, 1185–1195.
Krogh, J., Videbech, P., Thomsen, C., Gluud, C., Nordentoft, M., 2012. DEMO-II trial.
preted in the context of speech production predominantly being aerobic exercise versus stretching exercise in patients with major depression –
situated in the left frontal–temporal region. The association a randomised clinical trial. PLoS One 7 (10), e48316.
between hippocampal growth and reduction in depressive symp- Malykhin, N.V., Bouchard, T.P., Ogilve, C.J., Coupland, N.J., Seres, P., Camicioli, R.,
2007. Three-dimensional volumetric analysis and reconstruction of amygdala,
toms supports a role for hippocampus in depression and as hippocampal head, body and tail. Psychiatr. Res. 155 (2), 155–165.
previously shown the volume reduction in depression is poten- McKinnon, M.C., Yucel, K., Nazarov, A., MacQueen, G.M., 2009. A meta-analysis
tially state dependent and will after recovery (Ahdidan et al., 2011) examining clinical predictors of hippocampal volume in patients with major
regain the pre-morbid volume. depressive disorder. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 34 (1), 41–54.
Meyers, J.E., Bayless, J.D., Meyers, K.R., 1996. Rey complex figure: memory error
In summary, an exercise intervention for outpatients with mild patterns and functional abilities. Appl. Neuropsychol. 3, 89–92.
to moderate major depression does not increase hippocampal van Norden, A.G., de Laat, K.F., Fick, I., van Uden, I.W., van Oudheusden, L.J., Gons, R.A.,
volume and does not increase serum levels of BDNF, VEGF, or Norris, D.G., Zwiers, M.P., Kessels, R.P., de Leeuw, F.E., 2012. Diffusion tensor
imaging of the hippocampus and verbal memory performance: the RUN DMC
IGF-1. Furthermore, we found no association between change in
study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33 (3), 542–551.
hippocampal volume and change in maximal oxygen uptake Pajonk, F., Wobrock, T., Gruber, O., Scherk, H., Berner, D., Kaizl, I., Kierer, A., Muller, S.,
Oest, M., Meyer, T., Backens, M., Schneider-Axmann, T., Thornton, A., Honer, W.,
Falkai, P., 2010. Hippocampal plasticity in response to exercise in schizophrenia.
Role of the funding source Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 67, 133–143.
Parker, B.A., Thompson, P.D., Jordan, K.C., Grimaldi, A.S., Assaf, M., Jagannathan, K.,
Pearlson, G.D., 2011. Effect of exercise training on hippocampal volume in
None of the funders had any role in trial design, data collection, humans: a pilot study. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 82 (3), 585–591.
manuscript preparation or analysis of data. Jesper Krogh had full Pereira, A., Huddleston, D., Brickman, A., Sosunov, S., Hen, R., McKhann, G., Sloan, R.,
access to all of the data in the study and takes full responsibility Gage, F., Brown, T., Small, S., 2007. An in vivo correlate of exercise-induced
neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104,
for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. 5638–5643.
Scheewe, T.W., van Haren, N.E., Sarkisyan, G., Schnack, H.G., Brouwer, R.M., de, G.M.,
Hulshoff Pol, H.E., Backx, F.J., Kahn, R.S., Cahn, W., 2013. Exercise therapy,
Conflict of interests cardiorespiratory fitness and their effect on brain volumes: a randomised
controlled trial in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Eur.
Neuropsychopharmacol. 23 (7), 675–685.
None of the authors report any conflicts of interest. Schumacher, J., Jamra, R., Becker, T., Ohlraun, S., Klopp, N., Binder, E.B., et al., 2005.
Evidence for a relationship between genetic variants at the brain-derived
neurotrophic factor (BDNF) locus and Major Depression. Biol. Psychiatry 58,
Acknowledgement 307–314.
We thank all the participants, without their interest in our research this study Shimizu, E., Hashimoto, K., Okamura, N., Koike, K., Komatsu, N., Kumakiri, C., et al.,
would not have been possible. We are also grateful for the assistance of Eivy Olsen 2003. Alterations of serum levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF)
for assessing the participants' cognitive skills and Poul-Henrik Frandsen for in depressed patients with or without antidepressants. Biol. Psychiatry 54,
assistance with MRI, and Pia Hogh Plougmann for analysis of neutrophines. This 70–75.
study was funded by Trygfonden, Nordea-Danmark fonden, Helsefonden, and Aase Smith, S.M., Zhang, Y., Jenkinson, M., Chen, J., Mathews, P., Frederico, A., De Stefano,
and Ejnar Danielsensfond. N., 2002. Accurate, robust and automated longitudinal and cross-sectional brain
change analysis. NeuroImage 17 (1), 479–489.
Tapia-Arancibia, L., Rage, F., Givalois, L., Arancibia, S., 2004. Physiology of BDNF:
References focus on hypothalamic function. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 25, 77–107.
Trejo, J.L., Llorens-Martin, M.V., Torres-Aleman, I., 2008. The effects of exercise on
Ahdidan, J., Hviid, L., Chakravarty, M., Ravnkilde, B., Rosenberg, R., Rodell, A., spatial learning and anxiety-like behavior are mediated by an IGF-I-dependent
Stodkilde-Jorgensen, H., Videbech, P., 2011. Longitudinal MR study of brain mechanism related to hippocampal neurogenesis. Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 37 (2),
structure and hippocampus volume in major depressive disorder. Acta Psy- 402–411.
chiatr. Scand. 123 (3), 211–219. Vaynman, S., Ying, Z., Gomez-Pinilla, F., 2004. Hippocampal BDNF mediates the
Andersen, L.B., 1997. A maximal cycle exercise protocol to predict maximal oxygen efficacy of exercise on synaptic plasticity and cognition. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20,
uptake. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 5, 143–146. 2580–2590.
Bech,P., Bech-Andersen,G., Schütze,T., 1999. Mini Internationalt Neuropsykiatrisk Videbech, P., Ravnkilde, B., 2004. Hippocampal volume and depression: a meta-
Interview. Dansk Version 5.0.0. www. ccmh. dk. analysis of MRI studies. Am. J. Psychiatry 161 (11), 1957–1966.
Buschke, H., Fuld, P.A., 1974. Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in Voss, M.W., Erickson, K.I., Prakash, R.S., Chaddock, L., Kim, J.S., Alves, H., Szabo, A.,
disordered memory and learning. Neurology 24, 1019–1025. Phillips, S.M., Wojcicki, T.R., Mailey, E.L., Olson, E.A., Gothe, N., Vieira-Potter, V.J.,
Eisch, A.J., Petrik, D., 2012. Depression and hippocampal neurogenesis: a road to Martin, S.A., Pence, B.D., Cook, M.D., Woods, J.A., McAuley, E., Kramer, A.F.,
remission? Science 338, 72–75. 2013a. Neurobiological markers of exercise-related brain plasticity in older
Elfving, B., Plougmann, P., Wegener, G., 2010. Detection of brain-derived neuro- adults. Brain Behav. Immun. 28, 90–99.
trophic factor (BDNF) in rat blood and brain preparations using ELISA: pitfalls Voss, M.W., Vivar, C., Kramer, A.F., van, P.H., 2013b. Bridging animal and human
and solutions. J. Neurosci. Methods 187, 73–77. models of exercise-induced brain plasticity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17 (10), 525–544.

You might also like