Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 26

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/365806194

Why We Gather: A New Look, Empirically Documented, at Émile Durkheim's


Theory of Collective Assemblies and Collective Effervescence

Article in Perspectives on Psychological Science · November 2022


DOI: 10.1177/17456916221146388

CITATIONS READS
3 441

2 authors:

Bernard Rimé Darío Páez


Université Catholique de Louvain - UCLouvain Universidad Andrés Bello
198 PUBLICATIONS 10,683 CITATIONS 592 PUBLICATIONS 9,335 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Rescate de los 33 mineros de Atacama View project

Afrontamiento y Regulación emocional en víctimas de violencia de Género/ Coping and affect regulation with victims of gender violence View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Darío Páez on 27 March 2023.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


1146388
research-article2023
PPSXXX10.1177/17456916221146388Rimé and PáezPerspectives on Psychological Science

ASSOCIATION FOR
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Perspectives on Psychological Science

Why We Gather: A New Look, Empirically 1­–25


© The Author(s) 2023
Article reuse guidelines:
Documented, at Émile Durkheim’s Theory sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/17456916221146388
https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916221146388

of Collective Assemblies and Collective www.psychologicalscience.org/PPS

Effervescence

Bernard Rimé1 and Dario Páez2,3


1
Institut de Recherches en Sciences Psychologiques, Université catholique de Louvain; 2Department of
Social Psychology, University of the Basque Country; and 3Faculty of Education and Social Sciences,
Universidad Andrés Bello, Chile

Abstract
For Durkheim, individuals’ survival and well-being rest on cultural resources and social belonging that must be
revived periodically in collective assemblies. Durkheim’s concern was to clarify how these assemblies achieve this
revitalization. An intensive examination of primitive religions led him to identify successive levels of engagement
experienced by participants and to develop explanatory principles relevant to all types of collective gatherings.
Durkheim’s conception is widely referred to nowadays. However, the question of its empirical status remains open.
We extracted from his text his main statements and translated them into research questions. We then examined each
question in relation to current theories and findings. In particular, we relied on the plethora of recent cognitive and
social-psychology studies that document conditions of reduced self-other differentiation. Abundant data support that
each successive moment of collective assemblies contributes to blurring this differentiation. Ample support also exists
that because shared emotions are increasingly amplified in collective context, they can fuel high-intensity experiences.
Moreover, recent studies of self-transcendent emotions can account for the self-transformative effects described by
Durkheim at the climax of collective assemblies. In conclusion, this century-old model is remarkably supported by
recent results, mostly collected in experimental settings.

Keywords
collective gatherings, crowd, collective effervescence, rituals, synchrony, collective emotions

A century ago, in The Elementary Forms of Religious during which their members may revivify their
Life, Emile Durkheim (1912/1915) stated that to ensure common faith by manifesting it in common. To
their survival and well-being, individuals must periodi- strengthen those sentiments which, if left to them-
cally come together in collective assemblies. Durkheim selves, would soon weaken, it is sufficient to bring
also pointed that the capacity of individuals to deal those who hold them together and to put them
with existence rests on collective representations accu- into closer and more active relations with one
mulated over generations. When they partake in the another. (Durkheim, 1912/1915, p. 210)
societal ensemble and share these representations, indi-
viduals face the world with confidence and vital energy. From these premises, Durkheim (1912/1915) devel-
But given that daily life is spent in relative isolation, oped a theory of how periodic assemblies revive com-
people’s collective resources erode progressively. They mon faith. In short, individuals gather in the presence
need to revitalize their faith by manifesting it anew in of symbols representing their group. They focus on
communion with others because it is the sharing of the
common faith that makes this faith a reality:
Corresponding Author:
Bernard Rimé, Institut de Recherches en Sciences Psychologiques,
This is why all parties, political, economic or con- Université catholique de Louvain
fessional, are careful to have periodical reunions Email: bernard.rime@uclouvain.be
2 Rimé and Páez

common objects, emblems, and themes. They act in relationships affect health and well-being (e.g., Jetten
unison, synchronizing their gestures, actions, and et al., 2012). Ignored by empirical research during the
expressions. Their coordinated manifestations generate 20th century, collective processes and collective emo-
an atmosphere of emotion and fervor. Their mutual tions have been receiving considerable attention (e.g.,
stimulation yields a climate of emotional fusion. It cul- Barsade & Gibson, 2012; Chung et al., 2022; Elfenbein,
minates in a state of “effervescence,” in which partici- 2014; Goldenberg et al., 2021; Krueger & Szanto, 2016;
pants experience the “we” in place of the “self.” Their Parkinson, 2011, 2020, 2021; Peters & Kashima, 2015;
feeling of group belonging and their faith in collective Salmela, 2012; Thonhauser, 2022; von Scheve & Ismer,
representations being revitalized, participants can 2013; von Scheve & Salmela, 2014). “Effervescence,” the
return to their individual occupations and face daily concept adopted by Durkheim to capture the core of
life with a sense of power and meaning. collective gatherings, raises an unprecedented interest
Durkheim’s (1912/1915) idea that the survival and (see Fig. 1).
well-being of individuals rest on their social integration After a century of neglect, researchers have realized
remained devoid of influence on research for most of that Durkheim was a visionary and that his analysis of
the 20th century. In this period, psychological science collective processes holds great promise. However, his
predominantly viewed the healthy individual as “self- views need to be extracted from the realm of hypoth-
contained, independent and self-reliant, capable of eses and brought into empirical verification. The French
asserting himself and influencing his environment” sociologist did not offer a linear exposition of a theory,
(Riger, 1993, p. 280). The contribution of collective and dealing with The Elementary Forms (Durkheim
processes to health was simply ignored. More recently, 1912/1915) is not easy. Essential ideas emerge scattered
however, the discipline has undergone a considerable throughout lengthy descriptions of anthropological
evolution in this regard. There was a growing aware- observations. Consequently, abundant references are
ness that collective processes are not inherently irra- currently made to Durkheim’s views from secondhand
tional and destructive (e.g., Ehrenreich, 2007; McNeil, or thirdhand sources. The purpose of this article is to
1995; Reicher & Hopkins, 2001). At the same time, return to what Durkheim actually proposed and to
evidence has begun to emerge that social isolation relate it to the empirical evidence available currently. 1
poses a threat to health and to survival. In a landmark More specifically, we (a) extract from his text the essen-
article titled “From Social Integration to Health: tial components of his conception of collective pro-
Durkheim in the New Millennium,” Berkman et al. cesses and (b) translate these components into research
(2000) highlighted that since the 1970s, studies have questions that we then examine alongside contempo-
consistently shown lack of social ties to predict mortal- rary theories and findings. In doing so, we aim to pro-
ity from almost every cause. Subsequently, empirical vide an updated and empirically supported view of
support for these observations has grown steadily. Durkheim’s proposals. Beforehand, we need to specify
Meta-analytic data from 70 studies revealed an average Durkheim’s framework.
increased likelihood of mortality of 29% for social isola-
tion, 26% for loneliness, and 32% for living alone
Durkheim’s Framework
(Holt-Lunstad et al., 2015). At the opposite end of the
spectrum, meta-analyses of studies conducted on the Durkheim (1912/1915) sought to clarify the periodic
general population (Schwarzer & Leppin, 1989, 1991), revival of collective beliefs and representations as
on the elderly (Pinquart & Sörensen, 2000), and on exemplified in religious faiths. To this end, he sought
children and adolescents (Chu et al., 2010) evidenced to examine the most primitive and simplest religion that
effect sizes of r = .20, r = .15, and r = .17, respectively, could be accessed. He found such sources in the
between social support and well-being. A meta-analytic totemic religions of clan-based societies as described
review of 148 studies recorded a 50% increased likeli- by anthropologists among Australian aborigines and
hood of survival for participants with stronger social so-called “North American Indians.” Resting on such
relationships (Holt-Lunstad et al., 2010). observations, Durkheim developed a theory of the pro-
Taken together, the strong links observed between cess unfolding in collective assemblies. Note that his
lack of social connections and mortality and the abun- analysis was in no way directed at crowds. Whereas
dant evidence that social integration improves health “crowd” refers to accidental gathering of individuals
and well-being provide powerful support for Durkheim’s (e.g., in a public square at a busy hour), the collective
(1912/1915) theory that survival, well-being, and social processes addressed by Durkheim involve intentional-
integration are tightly interrelated. Given the theory’s ity, or a purpose shared between the copresent persons.
support, there is then a concern to clarify how social He spoke of rassemblement (gathering) or of assemblée
Perspectives on Psychological Science XX(X) 3

0.00035

0.0003 1580
1750

Relative Google Scholar Hits 0.00025

0.0002 1550
1310 1340
0.00015
816 825
0.0001 657
399 426 573
239 259
0.00005 144 143

20 013

20 015
19 993

19 995

19 997

20 999

20 001

20 003

20 007

20 009

20 011

20 017

20 019

1
20 005

02
-1

-2

-2
-1

-2

-2
-2

-2

-2

-2

-2
-1

-1

-2
-2
94

12

14
92

06

18
00

02

08

10

16
96

98

20
04
19

Fig. 1. Evolution of Scientific Interest for Durkheim’s Effervescence.


Note: Evolution over time of Google Scholar hits for the search of publications mentioning both
terms, “Durkheim” and “effervescence.” The y-axis displays the percentage of hits relative to the total
number of entries (all types, excluding patents and citations) in Google Scholar (June 14, 2022) for the
respective period. The absolute number of hits is displayed above each bar.

(assembly). In the English translation of his book, the Every feast, even when it has purely lay origins, has
word “crowd” occurs only six times, whereas the root certain characteristics of the religious ceremony, for
“assembl-” (for “assembly” or “assembled”) is met 61 in every case its effect is to bring men together, to
times. put the masses into movement and thus to excite a
Although Durkheim (1912/1915) continually referred state of effervescence. (pp. 382–383)
to religious ceremonies of remote societies, his concern
was to reach the essence of collective assemblies and Authors who have addressed his work all agree that
to formulate principles of universal relevance: Durkheim’s (1912/1915) analysis extends to all forms
of collective gathering (e.g., Collins, 2004; Moscovici,
There can be no society which does not feel the 1993; Pickering, 1984). This analysis therefore has rel-
need of upholding and reaffirming at regular evance to the many forms of collective assembly
intervals the collective sentiments and the collec- encountered today, whether in sports, music, culture,
tive ideas which make its unity and its personality. folklore, feasts, religious ceremonies, political gather-
Now this moral remaking cannot be achieved ings, social events, or others.
except by the means of reunions, assemblies and Our task will now consist in extracting from Durkheim’s
meetings where the individuals, being closely (1912/1915) book the essential components of his con-
united to one another, reaffirm in common their ception of collective assemblies. Participants in such
common sentiments; hence come ceremonies events were described as evolving through successive
which do not differ from regular religious ceremo- levels of engagement of rising intensity: (a) common
nies, either in their object, the results which they state of mind, (b) homogeneous manifestations, (c) group
produce, or the processes employed to attain consciousness, and (d) emotional transmission and
these results. (p. 427) reciprocal emotional amplification. They then reach (e)
a high-intensity state called “effervescence.” We examine
Durkheim’s (1912/1915) project was to shed light on each of these moments successively. For each, we con-
the entire spectrum of collective manifestations. He sider how far Durkheim’s propositions are supported
recurrently extended his reflections to sociopolitical by current theory and research. At the end of this review,
assemblies, to revolutionary movements, or to festive we draw up a picture of how collective assemblies
gatherings: appear today given current empirical data.
4 Rimé and Páez

Moment 1: Copresence and Interactions attention to each other. . . . This bodily inter-
orientation is the starting point for what happens
Durkheim’s theory next. (Collins, 2004, p. 34)
As soon as the participants come together, they undergo
a transformation. The contents of their consciousnesses Collins (2004) thus emphasized the role of the mutual
change. The utilitarian and individual preoccupations attention of the copresent participants and the physical
that dominate consciousness in daily life are eclipsed attunement that it involves. In this way, coparticipants
and substituted by collective concerns, shared beliefs, mutually intensify their shared experience, and a state
traditions, and collective ideals: of heightened intersubjectivity develops in the assembly.
On their part, social psychologists working within the
So, their first effect (ceremonies) is to bring indi- framework of social-identity theory (SIT; Tajfel & Turner,
viduals together, to multiply the relations between 1979) and its development through self-categorization
them and to make them more intimate with one theory ( J. C. Turner et al., 1987) have established a
another. By this very fact, the contents of their research tradition grounded in the key role of social
consciousness is changed. . . . At this time, their identity in collective processes. They pointed out that
thoughts are centred upon their common beliefs, exposure to other members of a group allows people
their common traditions, the memory of their infer the identity they share. Drury and Reicher (2005,
great ancestors, the collective ideal of which they 2009) posited that when people take part in a group
are the incarnation, in a word, upon social things. with whom they share features, purposes, or fate, they
(Durkheim, 1912/1915, pp. 348–349) engage in self-categorization. A cognitive transforma-
tion from individual-level identity to group-level iden-
Participants’ evolution toward a collective mindset tification thus takes place. Participants redefine their
is channeled by displayed emblems (e.g., flags, ban- identity and categorize themselves with the other par-
ners, signs, slogans, specific clothing): “That an emblem ticipants in terms of context-relevant features (e.g., “We
is useful as a rallying-centre for any sort of a group it are all drummers” or “We are all antiracist demonstra-
is superfluous to point out. By expressing the social tors”). This transformation affects dimensions such as
unity in a material form, it makes this more obvious to identity content (“who we are”), identity boundaries
all” (Durkheim, 1912/1915, p. 230) (who counts as “one of us”), and definitions of legiti-
The question arising here is thus whether empirical mate behavior (Drury & Reicher, 2000). Once actively
support exists for Durkheim’s (1912/1915) description of identified with a group, participants then act in function
the initial phase of collective assemblies. It involves the of this group’s norms. Shared identification also leads
following sequence: copresence, interactions, modifica- to effective coordination of action, to expectations of
tion of the contents of consciousness toward common mutual goals, and hence to mutual support in reaching
contents, and emergence of a common state of mind. these goals. Altogether, these largely documented
effects (for reviews see, Drury, 2018, 2020) revealed the
contribution of self-categorization to the emergence of
Current theory and research a shared mindset among copresent participants. These
studies thus provide a strong conceptual and empirical
Durkheim (1912/1915) pointed out a number of factors
basis for Durkheim’s (1912/1915) observation that as
that contribute to the evolution of the simple copres-
soon as individuals come together, their copresence
ence to a shared state of mind among participants. They
evolves toward a common state of mind.
involve the interactions attendees develop, the focus of
their consciousness on shared contents, and the indices
of the deployed emblems conveying what connects Moment 2: Homogeneous Manifestations
them. In a theory of ritual interactions drawing on
Durkheim, Randall Collins (2004) stressed that a shared Durkheim’s theory
reality becomes effective in an assembly as soon as Durkheim (1912/1915) noted that once they are assem-
coparticipants’ copresence is converted into focused bled, participants develop homogeneous behavioral mani-
interactions: festations: “Homogeneous manifestations then develop in
the assembly. By emitting same cries, words and gestures,
Society is above all an embodied activity. When participants nourish the group feeling” (p. 212):
human bodies are together in the same place,
there is a physical attunement: currents of feel- And since a collective sentiment cannot express
ings, a sense of wariness or interest, a palpable itself collectively except on the condition of
change in atmosphere. The bodies are paying observing a certain order permitting co-operation
Perspectives on Psychological Science XX(X) 5

and movements in unison, these gestures and that led participants to self-categorize and adopt a salient
cries naturally tend to become rhythmic and regu- social identity opens on an additional and conceptually
lar; hence come songs and dances. (p. 216) distinct step. In the latter, participants perceive copresent
people as sharing this social identity (Neville & Reicher,
Durkheim (1912/1915) viewed these visible and 2011). It is specified that knowledge of one’s shared iden-
homogeneous manifestations as the necessary condi- tity with others results from the interpretation of various
tion for the emergence of a collective feeling in the information sources such as “embodied emotion (e.g.,
assembly. In absence of visible displaying, the partici- facial expressions), ingroup symbols (e.g., wearing team
pants’ transformation noted at the initial stage of the colors), shared action (e.g., chanting), and shared fate
assembly would remain private and thus inconsequen- (e.g., vulnerability to rent increases)” (Neville & Reicher,
tial to the collective process: 2011, p. 387). The perception of a shared social identity
entails a marked relational transformation in the level of
If left to themselves, individual consciousnesses intimacy with coparticipants. Shared in-group membership
are closed to each other; they can communicate was indeed shown to facilitate intragroup trust and coop-
only by means of signs which express their inter- eration, a decrease in stress, comfort in close physical
nal states. If the communication established proximity, and helping behaviors (Haslam & Reicher, 2006;
between them is to become a real communion, Novelli et al., 2010; Reicher & Haslam, 2006). Accordingly,
that is to say, a fusion of all particular sentiments self-categorization (“I am a member of this group”) and
into one common sentiment, the signs expressing shared identity (“We are members of this group”) should
them must themselves be fused into one single be treated as conceptually distinct. If one identifies with a
and unique resultant. It is the appearance of this social category but does not perceive copresent others to
that informs individuals that they are in harmony share this membership, then one’s relations with these
and makes them conscious of their moral unity. It others are unlikely to be transformed toward intimacy
is by uttering the same cry, pronouncing the same (Neville & Reicher, 2011). The relational transformation
word, or performing the same gesture in regard resulting from the perception of a shared social identity is
to some object that they become and feel them- supported by empirical evidence from a variety of collec-
selves to be in unison. . . . Individual minds can- tive assemblies (e.g., Alnabulsi & Drury, 2014; Khan et al.,
not come in contact and communicate with each 2015; Neville et al., 2022; Novelli et al., 2013).
other except by coming out of themselves but they
cannot do this except by movements. So it is the Psychosocial outcomes of homogeneous manifesta-
homogeneity of these movements that gives the tions. An important scientific interest has emerged for the
group consciousness of itself and consequently study of interpersonal coordination, that is, behaviors in
makes it exist. (pp. 230–231) social interactions that are either similar in form or occur
at the same time (Bernieri & Rosenthal, 1991; Chartrand &
In this way, assembly participants inform one another Lakin, 2013). Two forms of interpersonal coordination are
that they share the same internal states, and according investigated: behavioral mimicry and interpersonal syn-
to Durkheim (1912/1915), the feelings of each one chronization. We briefly review their effects for cognitive
would merge into a common feeling and thus into a and affective variables related to social behavior and for
“communion.” social variables.
In behavioral mimicry, one person unintentionally
and effortlessly copies another person’s posture or body
Current theory and research
movements without either one being aware (Chartrand
According to Durkheim (1912/1915), the evolution of & Lakin, 2013; Chartrand & van Baaren, 2009). Chartrand
participants toward a “common feeling” or “commu- and Bargh (1999) found imitation to promote ease of
nion” would result from their perception of visible social interaction and mutual attraction between part-
manifestations, of homogeneous movements, and of ners. Compared with participants who had not been
shared internal states. Nowadays, there are three imitated, partners who had been imitated were also
sources of empirical documentation in this regard. found to be quicker to help and more generous to oth-
These include the psychosocial effects resulting (a) ers (e.g., van Baaren et al., 2003, 2004). Being mimicked
from the perception of a shared identity, (b) from increased the willingness to cooperate (Maddux et al.,
homogeneous behavioral manifestations, and (c) from 2008) and to comply with persuasion attempts (Tanner
the perception of shared feelings. et al., 2008). From a narrative review, Chartrand and
Lakin (2013) concluded that mimicry elicits liking and
Psychosocial effects of the perception of a shared empathy, helping behavior, interdependence, and
identity. According to SIT, the cognitive transformation feelings of closeness between the interactants. From a
6 Rimé and Páez

meta-analytic review, Hale and Hamilton (2016) con- effects of sharing a same experience. They have all evi-
cluded that mimicry changes participants’ self-construal denced remarkably similar outcomes. People who are
in a more interdependent direction. Among others, mim- aware of sharing beliefs with others increase their identi-
icked participants provided more interdependent state- fication, mutual appreciation, cohesion, and perceived
ments, felt closer to others, and increased their prosocial homogeneity with those others (Bar-Tal, 2000). Likewise,
behavior. In a nutshell, it can thus be concluded that the realization that attention is shared increases affiliation,
being mimicked enhances openness to others. cooperation, and social cohesion between coattending
In interpersonal synchrony, the actions of two or persons (Shteynberg, 2015, 2018; Shteynberg et al., 2020).
more people overlap in time (Bernieri et al., 1988; People who believe that they are having the same in-the-
Rennung & Göritz, 2016), whether with same actions moment experience as another person (“I-sharing”) man-
(e.g., a marching band walking in stride) or different ifest enhanced felt closeness, liking, generosity, and
ones (e.g., players in an orchestra; Hove & Risen, 2009). cooperation regarding this person (Pinel, 2018). Among
Early studies have shown that movements exercised in previously unrelated persons, eating shared food from a
synchrony increased affiliation for the coparticipant single plate leads them to behave more cooperatively
(Hove & Risen, 2009); enhanced cooperative behaviors, and less competitively toward each other compared with
feelings of trust, feelings of similarity, and shared eating the same food from separate plates (Woolley &
belonging (Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009); and fostered Fishbach, 2017, 2019). Sharing a same emotional-stimulus
compassion and altruistic behavior (Valdesolo & condition suffices to enhance feelings of closeness and
DeSteno, 2011). In a meta-analytic review, Rennung and integration between coparticipants (Rennung & Göritz,
Göritz (2016) examined 60 experiments that compared 2015). These various situations of shared experience find
an interpersonal synchrony condition (motor movements a niche under the broader label of “shared reality,” or the
or sensory stimulation) with at least one control condi- perceived commonality of inner states (feelings, beliefs,
tion. It yielded significant effects on prosocial attitudes or concerns) with another person about a target referent
(r = .24) and prosocial behaviors (r = .22). Mogan et al. (event, object, or third person; Echterhoff et al., 2009;
(2017) conducted a meta-analytic review of 42 studies in Hardin & Higgins, 1996; Higgins, 2019). Recently, shared
which experimentally manipulated synchronous actions reality was extended to the study of generalized shared
(i.e., exact rhythmic matching of actions in time and in reality (SR-G), or the subjective experience of shared
phase) were compared with control conditions. Syn- reality about multiple topics and domains with a particu-
chrony yielded positive effects on prosocial behaviors lar interaction partner (Rossignac-Milon et al., 2021;
(r = .24), perceived social bonding (r = .17), and positive Rossignac-Milon & Higgins, 2018). Across studies, SR-G
affect (r = .11). Note that positive affect, in turn, enhances predicted “clicking” between strangers as well as close-
altruism, prosocial behavior, and cooperation (Eisenberg, ness, rapport, and the desire to interact again. On days
1991; Fredrickson, 2013). Tarr et al. (2018) used an when partners experienced greater SR-G, they felt more
immersive virtual reality environment to test the role of connected to their partners. People with greater SR-G
synchrony in bonding effects when controlling for con- also tended to be closer and more committed to their
founds of suggestion, competence, and shared intention. romantic partners and more likely to have experienced
Participants in the synchrony condition reported greater the feeling of having “merged minds.”
social closeness on two behavioral measures and greater
prosociality on self-reported measures.
Conclusion
Considerable similarities thus exist between the psy-
chosocial effects of being behaviorally mimicked by an The three research areas we have examined support
interaction partner and those of being in synchrony with Durkheim’s (1912/1915) observations that common
one (e.g., Chartrand & Lakin, 2013; Hale & Hamilton, feelings develop in collective assemblies as a result of
2016). Vicaria and Dickens (2016) conducted joined the perception of visible manifestations, of homoge-
meta-analytic assessments of interpersonal and intrap- neous movements, and of shared internal states. Thus,
ersonal outcomes of 19 interactional-synchrony studies SIT studies have established that the perception of a
and 30 behavioral-mimicry investigations. These two shared social identity entails a marked transformation
modes of interpersonal coordination were consistently in the level of intimacy with coparticipants. The abun-
found to increase harmonious feelings among interact- dant research conducted on behavioral mimicry and
ing partners and to promote prosocial behaviors. interpersonal synchronization has demonstrated that
the practice of homogeneous movements can indeed
Psychosocial outcomes of shared experiences. Many modify the participants’ social rapport by bringing them
recent experimental studies conducted outside the field closer together. Dependent variables for which sig-
of collective processes have examined the psychosocial nificant effects in this direction have been observed
Perspectives on Psychological Science XX(X) 7

abound. They can be grouped into three main catego- more differentiated, each having a proper sphere of
ries: (a) enhanced attraction for partners (e.g., liking, action. However, wrote Durkheim, in this evolved soci-
positive affect, affiliation, empathy, compassion), (b) ety, many social events bring individuals together and
strengthened social ties (e.g., closeness, integration, make them again “commune”: festivals, public ceremo-
interdependence, cohesion, rapport, connection, com- nies, religious or secular, dramatic performances, and
mitment, cooperation, desire to interact), and (c) artistic events. “This is a world not only more intense
enhanced prosociality and/or altruism. Thus, in various but also qualitatively different. Following the collectivity,
ways, participants in conditions involving mimicry or the individual forgets himself for the common end and
synchronization are evidenced to temporarily develop his conduct is orientated in terms of a standard outside
a social openness. The same conclusion results from the himself.” (Durkheim, 1911/1953, p. 48).
study of situations in which participants perceive that What Durkheim meant by group consciousness thus
they share a same experience. Given similar dependent corresponds to conditions under which those elements
variables, such situations also yielded a higher closeness differentiating individuals from one another are blurred:
and integration among participants. There is thus no
longer any doubt that homogeneous movements and There are in each of us . . . two consciences: one
the community of experience have the effect of bridging which is common to our group in its entirety which,
the gap between participants. However, Durkheim’s consequently, is not our self, but society living and
propositions went one step further. They evoked effects acting within us; the other, on the contrary, repre-
such as “a fusion of particular sentiments into one com- sents that in us which is personal and distinct, that
mon sentiment,” becoming “conscious of their moral which makes us an individual. Solidarity which
unity,” or accessing to “the group consciousness of comes from likeness is at its maximum when the
itself.” Such effects go beyond mere bonding and imply collective conscience completely envelops our
an awareness of group unity. Some of the findings in whole conscience and coincides in all points with
our review support such outcomes. Thus, in the section it. But at that moment, our individuality is nil.
on synchrony, significant effects are mentioned for “feel- (Durkheim, 1893/1933, pp. 129–130)
ings of similarity” and “shared belonging” (Wiltermuth
& Heath, 2009) and for “perceived social bonding” In sum, what Durkheim (1893/1933) described is a
(meta-analytic review of Mogan et al., 2017), three vari- temporary evolution of individuals toward a moment
ables that come close to awareness of group unity. Like- when they cease to act as beings differentiated from
wise, in the section on shared experiences, people with one another, abandon their personal goals and con-
greater SR-G were reported as more likely to have expe- cerns, and begin to act according to common goals and
rienced “the feeling of having merged minds.” Such norms. Under these conditions, each individual can
results are in minority, but this may be because research- perceive only his or her similarity to others, which
ers did not consider theoretical reasons for carrying out favors the emergence of a sense of “we-ness” in all.
measurements of this type. Future research should be
attentive to these distinctions.
Current theory and research
The conditions viewed by Durkheim (1912/1915) as the
Moment 3: Group Consciousness substratum of “group consciousness” are far from being
alien in contemporary social sciences. Anthropologists,
Durkheim’s theory sociologists, and social psychologists currently examine
According to Durkheim (1912/1915), participants’ homo- conditions under which differences between individuals
geneous movements and community of experience con- may be blurred. We briefly review some of the promi-
tribute to generate “the fusion of all the particular nent concepts in this area.
feelings into one common sentiment” (p. 230), which, The idea put forward by Tönnies (1887) and by
in his view, equates to “group consciousness.” To prop- Durkheim (1893/1933) of an ancient form of social orga-
erly understand this view, we must go back to its source. nization consisting of an undifferentiated community of
In his book On the Division of Labor in Society, inspired equal individuals is echoed in the notion of “communi-
by Ferdinand Tönnies (1887), Durkheim (1893/1933) tas” developed in the anthropological work of V. W.
described two forms of social organization that suc- Turner (1969, 1974). In communitas, individuals are
ceeded in history. In the primitive one, the division of detached from their differences of class, status, and
labor was rudimentary. People were similar, and indi- social roles, and they meet as integral human beings
viduality was null. They moved together without move- who share the same humanity. The spirit of communitas
ment of their own. Subsequently, societies evolved is periodically revived in pilgrimages, initiation rituals,
toward greater divisions of labor, and individuals became religious events, or countercultural movements, in which
8 Rimé and Páez

homogeneity and equality are temporarily restored. & Cialdini, 2007). IOS consists of adopting and sharing
“Communal sharing” (CS), described by Alan Fiske the perspectives and identities of another person in
(1992), is very close to communitas. In CS, individuals such a way that the cognitive construction of the self
focus on what they have in common—nature, ancestry, overlaps the cognitive construction of the other. The
origins, or substance—and disregard their distinct indi- needs of the other person become perceived as needs
vidual identities. CS develops in close kinship, in intense of the self, and meeting these needs becomes norma-
love, and to a lesser degree, in ethnic and national identi- tive. Swann et al. (2009, 2012) described “identity
ties. CS can be activated in collective events, such as fusion” (IF), which is typical of close relationships but
initiation, rites of passage, religious worship, and cere- can also occur with larger groups, such as a country or
monial meals. The sudden realization that one is expe- political party. IF results from the fusion of the personal
riencing CS can trigger a strong emotion labeled kama self and the social self, that is to say, of the individual’s
muta—a Sanskrit concept meaning “being moved.” It is own characteristics—or personal self—and the charac-
accompanied by positive affect, tears and chills, and teristics the individual shares with the group—or social
action tendencies, such as approach behavior, affiliation, self. The self-other barrier is then blurred, and a visceral
prosocial behavior, and social bonding (Zickfeld, sense of oneness with the group ensues, with, in par-
Schubert, Seibt, Blomster, et al., 2019; Zickfeld, Schubert, ticular, a readiness to sacrifice oneself for the group.
Seibt, & Fiske, 2019). Communitas and CS share elements Thus, IOS and IF both accent a process of cognitive
with the notion of “social identity,” which has been overlapping as the source of the feeling of nondiffer-
extensively documented in social psychology. It refers entiation or union with others.
to the individual’s awareness of belonging to the same Yaden et al. (2017) highlighted that experiences
social category as a set of other people (e.g., Drury, 2018; called “self-transcendent” (ST) can momentarily blur
Neville et al., 2022; Reicher et al., 2010). From this moment, the subjective sense of one’s self and trigger the experi-
because they see others as fellow group members, they ence of unity with other people or one’s surrounding.
feel intimate with them. Because they assume others see This is observed, in particular, in mindfulness experi-
them as a fellow group member, they assume they will be ences, in flow, in peak experiences, and in mystical-
intimate with them. This prompts a multilevel sense of type experiences. As for the underlying process, the
we-ness that leads people to mutual support and to act authors suggested the interaction of an annihilation
together as a group (Neville et al., 2022). component that could reduce negative aspects of exces-
Researchers thus have shown that certain types of sive self-focus and a relational component, linked to
social relationships can bring out the feeling of nondif- perceived social connection. Recent research has paid
ferentiation and thus of union between individuals. The particular attention to a subcategory of emotions having
notion of “perceived emotional synchrony” (PES) devel- the property of triggering self-transcendent states
oped by Páez et al. (2015) is in the same vein. Here, (Algoe & Haidt, 2009; Haidt, 2003a, 2003b; Haidt &
close to Durkheim (1912/1915), the experience of col- Morris, 2009; Van Cappellen & Rimé, 2014). Called “self-
lective assembly is viewed as eliciting feelings of we- transcendent positive emotions,” they involve elevation,
ness or unity. PES denotes such a feeling involving a compassion, admiration, gratitude, love, and awe. We
sense of togetherness or unison. Items assessing PES come back to these emotions later and show that they
strongly predicted effects expected to result from a trigger behaviors oriented toward others.
collective experience, including social integration,
empowerment, positive emotions, and endorsement of
Conclusion
socially shared beliefs and values (e.g., Bouchat et al.,
2020; Páez et al., 2015). A meta-analysis (Pizarro et al., Durkheim’s (1912/1915) idea that a sense of unity can
2022) found PES to predict longitudinally these out- result from the perception of undifferentiation between
comes (for social integration, r = .31; for empowerment, self and others is thus widely encountered and sup-
r = 42; for positive emotions, r = .53; for endorsement ported by a variety of present-day research currents.
of socially shared beliefs and values: r = 27). According to these works, the effect described by
Other phenomena involving experiences of oneness Durkheim can result from a perception of equality (com-
with peers have been described and documented by munitas, CS), of shared identity (social identity), or of
social psychologists. Thus, “inclusion of others in the shared conditions, experience, and action (PES). It can
self” (IOS; Aron & Aron, 1986; Aron et al., 2013) occurs also occur when characteristics of the self and of others
in conditions such as falling in love (Aron & Aron, overlap (IOS) or when proper traits and shared traits
2006), emotional intimacy (e.g., Brown et al., 2009), are seen as overlapping (IF). Furthermore, ST experi-
physical intimacy (Sagarin et al., 2009), and believing ences and ST emotions can also elicit a blurring of the
that one “shares brain waves” with the other (Goldstein self–other distinction and an experience of unity with
Perspectives on Psychological Science XX(X) 9

other people. It will be the task of future research to which has fallen upon it. Of course, they have only
better organize the various concepts that were evoked sad emotions in common, but communicating in
in this section. In this regard, we stress that in their sorrow is still communicating. (Durkheim, 1912/1915,
meta-analysis, Pizarro et al. (2022) found kama muta, p. 401)
social identification, PES, fusion of identity, and ST emo-
tions (STEs) to be strongly intercorrelated, suggesting He wrote, “It matters little that this exaltation was pro-
that they belong to a family, or a global latent process voked by a sad event, for it is real, notwithstanding,
of reduction of self-other differentiation. and does not differ specifically from what is observed
But does a reduction of the self-other differentiation in the happy feasts” (Durkheim, 1912/1915, p. 407).
allow to conclude to a group consciousness or collective Concerning the collective expression of affects, Dur-
feeling, as was advanced by Durkheim (1912/1915)? A kheim (1912/1915) thus advanced the following three
positive answer to this question would presume that a statements: (a) Expressed emotions are transmitted
group consciousness is possible beyond individual con- from mind to mind, (b) collective expression has the
sciousnesses and that a collective as such can be a locus effect of amplifying emotions, and (c) outward expres-
of subjective experience (for discussion, see Pacherie, sion leads to expressive manifestations of increased
2017). In the current theoretical context, it seems rea- intensity.
sonable to limit oneself to considering that each indi-
vidual can, under some conditions, experience a sense
of oneness with others and be aware at the same time
Current theory and research
that others around them are most likely having a similar We address the following three questions. First, does
experience. emotion expression in a shared context generate trans-
mission of emotions? Second, are emotional experi-
ences amplified when emotions occur in a shared
Moment 4: Emotions Transmission context? Third, is emotional expression amplified when
and Amplification emotions occur in a shared context?
Durkheim’s theory Does emotion expression in shared context gener-
According to Durkheim (1912/1915), the collective ate transmission of emotions? Much attention is paid
expression of affects generates a transmission of emo- nowadays to the various processes by which emotions
tions from mind to mind. As in an echo chamber, the might be transmitted in collective situations (for reviews
emotions expressed are amplified: and discussion, see Chung et al., 2022; Parkinson, 2011,
2020, 2021; Salmela, 2012; Thonhauser, 2022; von Scheve
Every sentiment expressed finds a place without & Ismer, 2013). Many of these processes are noninter-
resistance in all the minds, which are very open active (e.g., shared sources of emotional appraisal).
to outside impressions; each re-echoes the others, However, Durkheim (1912/1915) specifically invoked
and is re-echoed by the others. The initial impulse interactive processes (i.e., reciprocal adjustments between
thus proceeds, growing as it goes, as an avalanche the participants; “re-echo of affects from mind to mind”).
grows in its advance. (pp. 215–216) Relevant processes currently discussed are of three types.
In the mimicry-based emotion-contagion model (Hatfield
He continued, “Human sentiments are intensified when et al., 1994; Söderkvist et al., 2018), senders’ facial
affirmed collectively. Sorrow, like joy, becomes exalted emotional displays would be automatically imitated by
and amplified when leaping from mind to mind, and receivers, and a facial feedback process would elicit
therefore expresses itself outwardly in the form of exu- the corresponding emotional state in the receiver. This
berant and violent movements” (p. 400). model was poorly supported in empirical testing (for
Durkheim (1912/1915) stressed that this emotional reviews, see Hess & Fischer, 2013, 2014; Parkinson, 2020),
amplification takes place independently of the type of but more recent findings have offered better promises
emotion involved. Whatever the emotion, the key point (Olszanowski et al., 2019). An alternative account lies in
is that it be shared. This was made particularly clear in social appraisal (Manstead & Fischer, 2001), which states
the context of the author’s descriptions of mourning that assembly participants would appraise events relying
rituals: on the emotional manifestations they observe in copar-
ticipants. In a review, Parkinson (2020) concluded that if
Since they weep together, they hold to one another this inferential process is indeed observed in laboratory
and the group is not weakened, in spite of the blow conditions, it might intervene only to a reduced extent in
10 Rimé and Páez

real-life interactions. According to a third model (Parkin- group members. Thus, greater fear, gloom, and glee can
son, 2019) named “orientational calibration,” convergent result from group attention to scary, sad, and happy events.
emotional orientation would develop from lower-level Emotional amplification effects in shared conditions were
cuing process rather than from the registration of others’ also found in the context of exposure to stressful condi-
emotions. For instance, people calibrate their gaze orien- tions. Martin et al. (2015) had pairs of participants under-
tation in function of another person’s shifting gaze; they going a cold-pressor task (immersing a hand in cold water
adapt their bodily movements to those of other people. to induce pain and stress) once alone and once facing
This model meets particularly well Durkheim’s notion of their partner, who observed or also immersed a hand
re-echo of affects. (shared condition). A higher pain was reported when shar-
But we point out that beside a bottom-up process of ing the task versus alone, but only when the partner was
transmission through nonverbal interaction evoked by a friend of the participant, or was a stranger the participant
Durkheim (1912/1915), many other routes exist for bonded with before the task. Nahleen et al. (2019) repli-
inducing convergence of affects, including more elabo- cated these findings in conditions involving more stringent
rate verbal top-down processes. Merely belonging to a experimental control. Several studies have shown shared
same collective already opens a whole lot of paths. flow, or collective optimal experience, to evoke higher levels
Thus, for collective emotions to emerge, participants of positive affect or joy than solitary flow (Magyaródi &
must appraise an event in similar ways (Frijda, 1986), Oláh’s, 2015; Walker, 2010). Finally, in a meta-analytic review
and members of a same collective have numerous of the so-called experiential advantage, or the fact that con-
sources of emotional appraisal in common, such as sumers yield greater happiness from purchasing experiences
collective memories (Pennebaker et al., 1997), group- compared with material possessions, this advantage was
based emotions (Smith, 1993), group-based sentiments found superior when purchasing experiences were social
(von Scheve & Ismer, 2013), emotional attitudes (Oatley, as opposed to solitary (Weingarten & Goodman, 2021).
2000), emotional climate (de Rivera & Páez, 2007), and
norms, values, and conventions (Parkinson et al., 2005). Is emotional expression amplified when emotions
In addition, studies of verbal exchanges have abun- occur in shared context? Evidence exists that experi-
dantly documented the transmissibility of emotions and encing together has the effect of intensifying participants’
emotional content. Emotion indeed constitutes a major emotional expression. Group size, a proxy for the inten-
incentive for communication (Rimé, 2009; Rimé et al., sity of shared experiencing, has been found to be associ-
1991), and participants in collective assemblies can thus ated with greater intensity of negatively and positively
be expected to express their emotions verbally to one valenced emotional responses. Thus, studies that ana-
another. In sum, there is hardly any doubt that in a lyzed archival records of lynching in the United States
collective assembly, emotions are transmitted from per- have found that the larger the size of the crowd, the more
son to person, whether by one of the ways just men- heinous and brutal the aggression was (Leader et al.,
tioned or by still others (Chung et al., 2022; Parkinson, 2007). A multilevel study confirmed that greater crowd
2011, 2020, 2021; Salmela, 2012; Thonhauser, 2022; von density was associated with more intense emotional
Scheve & Ismer, 2013). responses (emotionally exalted dancing, animated move-
ment, and noise-making) at musical concerts (Liebst,
Are emotional experiences amplified when emotions 2019). An observational study showed that smile and
occur in shared context? Many studies have demon- laughter rates increased as a function of group size (Mehu
strated that experiencing affective conditions together with & Dunbar, 2008). Levy and Fenley (1979) examined the
others, even without communication, has the effect of relationship between audience size and laughter at 15
amplifying the experience. Participants liked pleasant preselected scenes during different showings of a satiri-
chocolate more and unpleasant chocolate less when they cal movie in the same theatre. They observed that the
tasted it simultaneously with another person than when larger the audience, the greater was the mean level of
that other person was present but doing something else laughter. Various experimental studies have confirmed
(Boothby et al., 2014). However, this amplification effect that the presence of another person who laughs or is
occurred only when coexperiencers were acquainted laughing at a joke encourages others to laugh and to
(Boothby et al., 2016). In studies conducted by Shteynberg perceive the stimulus as more humorous (Chapman,
et al. (2014), participants attended to a scary advertise- 2017; Fuller & Sheehy-Skeffington, 1974; Smyth & Fuller,
ment, to negative and positive images, and to sad and 1972). People laugh with higher intensity in high-density
happy videos in various group settings. Simultaneous coat- groups (Freedman & Perlick, 1979) and when hearing
tention with one’s group members increased emotional canned laughs that belongs to in-group members but not
intensity relative to attending alone, to coattending with when they originate from out-group members (Platow
strangers, or to attending nonsimultaneously with one’s et al., 2005).
Perspectives on Psychological Science XX(X) 11

Conclusion very first occurrence in the book, the word “efferves-


cence” is linked to two components:
Models of the transmission of emotions via interactive
routes are still under discussion at present. But there That general effervescence results which is char-
are many other well-documented routes that can acteristic of revolutionary or creative epochs. Now
account for the convergence of emotions among par- this greater activity results in a general stimulation
ticipants in a collective assembly. Moreover, there is of individual forces. Men see more and differently
ample empirical evidence to support Durkheim’s now than in normal times. Changes are not merely
(1912/1915) assertion that in a shared context, both of shades and degrees; men become different. The
subjective emotional experience and outward expres- passions moving them are of such an intensity
sion of emotion are undergoing an amplification effect. that they cannot be satisfied—except by violent
Note that this issue is of crucial importance in the and unrestrained actions, actions of super-human
evolution of the process analyzed by Durkheim. Indeed, heroism or of bloody barbarism. (pp. 210–211)
if collective expression has the effect of transmitting
emotions and of intensifying both their experience and Effervescence thus involves, on the one hand, a sub-
their expression, it can lead only to further amplifica- jective experience of transformation and, on the other
tion. The collective sharing of emotions is thus the hand, unrestrained behaviors qualified as violent and
source of an upward spiral that, if sustained, can fuel barbaric. Of the 15 subsequent occurrences of “effer-
only a high-intensity emotional condition. vescence” in the book (Durkheim, 1912/1915), their
context evokes violent and unrestrained behavior in
Moment 5: Effervescence six cases (pp. 215, 218, 394, 395, 405, and 407), the
experience of transformation in five cases (pp. 218,
Durkheim’s theory 226, 422, and 427), and both themes at a time in two
In Durkheim’s (1912/1915) view, the emotional exalta- cases (pp. 210 and 382; see the Supplemental Material
tion manifested by assembled participants leads to the available online). Thus, the contexts confirm these two
high-intensity manifestations he labeled “effervescence.” components to constitute the core of effervescence in
Given the considerable interest in this concept today, Durkheim’s eyes. We examine each of them in more
a careful attention to Durkheim’s use of the term is details.
required. “Effervescence” occurs 16 times in the body
of the 450-page English translation of The Elementary Effervescence as violent and unrestrained behav-
Forms, either in isolation (three times) or qualified as ior. Durkheim (1912/1915) repeatedly stressed that the
“state of effervescence” (five times), “collective effer- mere gathering of individuals has exceptionally energiz-
vescence” (three times), and “general effervescence” ing effects: “The very fact of the concentration acts as an
(two times), whereas “great mental effervescence,” exceptionally powerful stimulant. When they are once
“great effervescence,” and “creative effervescence” each come together, a sort of electricity is formed by their col-
occurred only once. Of Latin origin (ex = out of, fervere = lecting which quickly transports them to an extraordinary
to be hot), “effervescence” is used both in French and degree of exaltation” (p. 212).
in English to denote the state of a bubbling liquid. Overpowered by these unusual collective forces, par-
Surprisingly, at no point did Durkheim (1912/1915) ticipants are said to develop violent or promiscuous
provide a formal definition of his figurative use of the behavior and an exaltation that can come close to
term. What he meant by effervescence should thus be delirium:
deduced. Glossing over the elements surrounding the
word “effervescence” in Durkheim’s book provides the This effervescence often reaches such a point that
following approximation of a definition. Assemblies it causes unheard-of actions. The passions released
lead to “a general stimulation of the individual forces” are of such an impetuosity that they can be
(p. 211), a state that is “constantly increasing” (p. 218) restrained by nothing. They are so far removed
in “a psychical exaltation not far removed from delir- from their ordinary conditions of life, and they are
ium” (p. 226); it is “translated outwardly by exuberant so thoroughly conscious of it, that they feel that
movements which are not easily subjected to too care- they must set themselves outside of and above
fully defined ends” (p. 381), and “vital energies are their ordinary morals. The sexes unite contrarily
over-excited, passions more active, sensations stronger” to the rules governing sexual relations. Men
(p. 422). Effervescence thus concerns a high-intensity exchange wives with each other. Sometimes even
excitation combining exaltation and exuberance. At its incestuous unions, which in normal times are
12 Rimé and Páez

thought abominable and are severely punished, exaltation resulting from the amplification of emotions,
are now contracted openly and with impunity. individuals feel driven by external powers that put them
(Durkheim, 1912/1915, p. 216) in an unusual condition. Participants are thus lifted out of
themselves. Around them, their companions undergo the
He also wrote, “They utter terrible cries, fly into a pas- same experience of transfiguration. Durkheim insisted
sion and feel that they must tear and destroy; it is to that they are not fooled by an illusion. The exaltation is
satisfy this need that they beat themselves, wound real. It is the product of forces external and superior to
themselves, and make their blood flow” (Durkheim, the individual:
1912/1915, p. 407).
Thus, the first component of effervescence described It seems to him that he has become a new being:
by Durkheim (1912/1915) is marked by excesses of all the decorations he puts on and the masks that
types, including violent and promiscuous behaviors, cover his face figure materially in this interior
exaltation close to delirium, sexual relations and inces- transformation, and to a still greater extent, they
tuous unions, and destruction and blood flow. Note that aid in determining its nature. And as at the same
by giving this importance to violence and debauchery time all his companions feel themselves trans-
in his treatment of collective assemblies, Durkheim formed in the same way and express this senti-
introduced a severe contradiction in his theory. The ment by their cries, their gestures and their general
essence of this theory revolves around the view that attitude, everything is just as though he really
collective assemblies play an eminent role in the con- were transported into a special world, entirely
stitution of societies. This was particularly explicitly different from the one where he ordinarily lives,
stated in the following text: and into an environment filled with exceptionally
intense forces that take hold of him and metamor-
It is, in fact, at such moments of collective ferment phose him. How could such experiences as these,
[he original French text says: “dans des moments especially when they are repeated every day for
d’effervescence de ce genre”] that are born the weeks, fail to leave in him the conviction that
great ideals upon which civilizations rest. The there really exist two heterogeneous and mutually
periods of creation or renewal occur when men incomparable worlds? One is that where his daily
for various reasons are led into a closer relation- life drags wearily along; but he cannot penetrate
ship with each other, when reunions and assem- into the other without at once entering into rela-
blies are most frequent, relationships better tions with extra- ordinary powers that excite him
maintained and the exchange of ideas most active. to the point of frenzy. The first is the profane world,
Such was the great crisis of Christendom, the the second, that of sacred things. (Durkheim, 1912/
movement of collective enthusiasm which, in the 1915, pp. 218–219)
twelfth and thirteenth centuries, bringing together
in Paris the scholars of Europe, gave birth to In sum, according to Durkheim (1912/1915), the
Scholasticism. Such were the Reformation and emotional amplification developing in the assembly
Renaissance, the revolutionary epoch and the generates a surge of power that participants experience
Socialist upheavals of the nineteenth century. At as a transportation out of oneself and an accession to
such moments this higher form of life is lived with the “world of sacred things.” It is in such a framework,
such intensity and exclusiveness that it monopo- wrote Durkheim, that the feeling of the sacred estab-
lizes all minds to the more or less complete exclu- lishes itself and that the religious idea would be born.
sion of egoism and the commonplace. At such
times the ideal tends to become one with the real,
Current theory and research
and for this reason men have the impression that
the time is close when the ideal will in fact be Effervescence as violent and unrestrained behav-
realized and the Kingdom of God established on ior. The scenes described by Durkheim (1912/1915) are
earth. (Durkheim, 1911/1953, p. 48) transcribed from reports of field anthropologists of his time.
Still today, field anthropologists emphasize extreme mani-
How could this noble constitutive function of effer- festations by which collective phenomena differ from every-
vescence—qualified in this text as “higher form of life”!— day life. Recently, a senior anthropology scholar stated,
be achieved if collective assemblies would culminate in
violence, destruction, and sexual debauchery? Now we know that the phenomenon of collective
effervescence is an altered state of consciousness
Effervescence as experience of transformation. (ASC), commonly called a dissociative state or
According to Durkheim (1912/1915), in the state of trance that occurs in some rituals. . . . Durkheim
Perspectives on Psychological Science XX(X) 13

talked about physiological phenomena that were responsiveness to the situation, a meta-analytic review
not typical in “normal social life” that we now call indicated that under deindividuating conditions, par-
driving mechanisms which can produce ASCs, for ticipants behaved more in accord with situational norms
example, repetitive drum beats, sensory deprivation (Postmes & Spears, 1998). Such findings matched the
and fasting, ingestion of mind-altering substances, alternative model of crowd behavior advanced in the
and communal rituals. (Buehler, 2012, p. 79) context of SIT (e.g., Reicher, 1984, 1987, 2001; Reicher
et al., 1995). It stressed that collective situations do not
In contrast, in sociology and psychology, definitions lead to a loss of personal identity at all. Rather, partici-
of effervescence generally ignore the aspects of vio- pants move from a personal identity to a more social
lence and unbridled behavior evoked by Durkheim or collective identity. In situations in which the preva-
(1912/1915). For Collins (2014), a successful ritual lence of the personal self is reduced, participants
“transmutes any shared emotions into a new emotion: become likely to act according to local norms (i.e., any
the collective effervescence of solidarity” (p. 300). Páez norm that is salient in that specific situation). To an
et al. (2015) defined effervescence as a multifaceted outside observer, such acts may seem senseless or unin-
process of social synchronization. Haidt et al. (2008) hibited, whereas to the group, they are rational and
referred to Durkheim’s effervescence as the intense normative (Reicher, 1987). Social-psychology studies
passion and joy generated by periodic events. Hopkins have confirmed that violent crowd actions are not unre-
et al. (2016) considered that the strong positive emo- strained, are targeted at specific individuals and institu-
tions they recorded in collective assemblies are similar tions (Allen, 1970), and are governed by the sense of
to Durkheim’s effervescence. Fiske (2020) argued that social identity that individuals share as members of that
kama muta, the surge of positive affect elicited by the community (Drury et al., 2020; Stott et al., 2018). Alto-
sudden salience of communal sharing, has all of the gether thus, contemporary evidence diverges radically
features of Durkheim’s collective effervescence. For from the olde representation of crowds as sources of
Gabriel et al. (2020), effervescence involves both a irrational explosions of emotion.
sense of connection to others and a sensation of sacred- A further explanation is to be considered for the dif-
ness, and among respondents to their enquiry, three ference in views on effervescence between Durkheim
quarters reported experiencing effervescence at least (1912/1915) and contemporary scholars. A gap exists
once a week, and one third experienced it daily. between Durkheim’s documentary sources and those
In sum, the violent and unrestrained component of available today. In his concern for the roots of collective
effervescence highlighted by Durkheim (1912/1915) is phenomena, Durkheim relied entirely on field anthro-
absent from current works on collective processes. pologists’ descriptions of exotic rituals. By contrast,
Instead, these works promote a conception of efferves- nowadays, the media expose countless images of col-
cence that is at the antipodes of the barbaric manifesta- lective assemblies of all types: religious and civil ceremo-
tions evoked in Les Formes Elémentaires. Effervescence nies, commemorations, sports, musical events, folklore
is currently described in a positive tone. The question events, collective festivities, sociopolitical events, and so
then arises as to why there is such a radical difference forth. Although extreme manifestations may occasionally
in perspective on the same phenomenon. occur at such events, everyone can witness that the vast
First, recall that views linking collective processes majority of collective assemblies are free of violence,
with unbridled behavior were widely promoted in destruction, and sexual debauchery.
Durkheim’s time by the writings of Gustave Le Bon In conclusion, the violent and unrestrained compo-
(1895/1997). For the latter, when in crowds, people lose nent of effervescence highlighted by Durkheim
their sense of self and their ability to reason to such an (1912/1915) is not supported by the current evidence.
extent that only emotional contagion and exacerbated
emotions are left. Participants are thus brought back to Effervescence as experience of transformation. If
the barbaric time preceding civilization. Representa- the first component of effervescence described by
tions linking crowds and norm violations were wide- Durkheim (1912/1915) is not supported by contempo-
spread at the time (for a comprehensive historical rary views, the opposite goes for the second one. Recent
review, see Moscovici, 1981, 1993). Later, similar views studies of STEs provide increasing support for Dur-
were supported by deindividuation theory (Diener, kheim’s claims that assembly participants experience a
1980; Zimbardo, 1969), according to which in collective transportation out of oneself. STEs gather a subset of
situations, variables such as anonymity, large groups, positive emotions—elevation, compassion, admiration,
and reduced self-awareness cause antinormative behav- gratitude, love, and awe—that were found to transform
ior. However, opposite to the hypothesis that deindi- temporarily those who experience them (Haidt, 2003a,
viduation decreases self-control and diminishes 2003b). Participants exposed to STE-eliciting conditions
14 Rimé and Páez

shifted from self-absorption and selfish concerns to out- experience increased over time at mass gatherings and
ward concerns such as other peoples’ interests, environ- persisted at least 6 months after attendance. Overall,
mental concerns, or group concerns (Algoe & Haidt, 63.2% of participants reported being at least “somewhat”
2009; Haidt, 2003a, 2003b; Haidt & Morris, 2009; Hanley transformed, and 19.5% said they were “absolutely”
& Garland, 2019; Stellar et al., 2017; Van Cappellen & transformed. The most frequently reported qualities of
Rimé, 2014). participants’ experiences involved feeling socially con-
Among STEs, awe is particularly likely to be experi- nected to something larger than oneself. Thus, these
enced in a collective assembly. In his classic text, findings suggest that the most prevalent attributes of
Edmund Burke (1757) emphasized how the experience transformative experiences are socially oriented (i.e.,
of power can trigger an awe with its double feeling of oriented toward others and the community).
terror and amazement. In line with this view, awe was
empirically defined as resulting from the experience of
Conclusion
something larger than oneself (Keltner & Haidt, 2003).
The experience of power viewed by Durkheim Whereas the “violence and unbridled behavior” com-
(1912/1915) at the peak of a collective assembly is thus ponent that Durkheim (1912/1915) evoked about effer-
a prototypical awe-triggering condition. There is empiri- vescence appears irrelevant in the current scientific
cal evidence of the transformative effects of experienc- context outside anthropology, the “self-transformation
ing awe. Shiota et al. (2007) found it associated with a and access to the sacred” component of effervescence
sense of the smallness of the self as well as some dis- is more and more supported. According to laboratory
engagement from awareness of the self. They also findings, emotions that are most likely aroused in col-
observed awe to affect the content of the self-concept lective participation produce a transformation of the
by de-emphasizing the individual self and increasing person involving lessened self-focus and higher out-
one’s sense of the self as part of a greater whole. Experi- ward orientation. These emotions are also capable of
mental studies supported the transformative potential stimulating feelings of a religious nature. Recent inves-
of awe toward outward social concerns. Thus, compared tigation of collective assemblies confirmed that partici-
with control participants, participants who were exposed pation stimulates socially oriented self-transformative
to awe-inducing videos expressed stronger feelings of experiences in the form of feeling connected to some-
oneness with friends and with people in general (Van thing larger than oneself.
Cappellen & Saroglou, 2012). Along the same lines,
Pizarro et al. (2021) found video-induced experiences
of STEs involving awe, love, and kama muta to be asso-
Discussion
ciated to increased identification with all humanity (i.e., So far, we have examined the successive moments in
increased concern for all other human beings) and to the unfolding of collective assemblies as described by
motivations to help distant others. Finally, experimental Durkheim (1912/1915). For each of these moments, we
evidence exists that awe produces not only a transpor- identified corresponding processes that are documented
tation out of oneself but also an accession to the by current empirical research. The data from this
“world of sacred things.” Participants who watched awe- research thus provide a new look, empirically validated,
inducing videos (awe of nature; awe at childbirth) sub- of the processes at work in collective assemblies. We
sequently reported feeling themselves more religious now briefly summarize the key elements of this renewed
and more spiritual than participants who viewed control reading of Durkheim’s theory:
videos (Saroglou et al., 2008). In a recent review,
Monroy & Keltner, 2022 concluded that awe engages a •• When they meet, participants realize that they
diminished focus on the self, increased prosocial rela- share features, purposes, or fate. They thus rede-
tionality, greater social integration, and a heightened fine their identity and categorize themselves with
sense of meaning. coparticipants in terms of context-relevant fea-
Durkheim’s (1912/1915) view that involvement in a tures. As a result of this identity shift, participants’
collective assembly culminates in an experience of per- behaviors align with the norms and values of the
sonal transformation was directly supported by a large- assembly.
scale longitudinal study of participants at immersive •• Next, the perception of a shared social identity,
secular mass gatherings lasting 3 or more days. Yudkin the development of homogeneous movements
et al. (2022) assessed the degree to which participants and motor mimicry, and the perception of shared
in these gatherings had had an experience they consid- experience all contribute to participants’ feeling
ered transformative (“Have you had a transformative of having merged minds. A multitude of psycho-
experience at [field site]?”). Self-reported transformative social effects follows, including prosocial attitudes
Perspectives on Psychological Science XX(X) 15

and behaviors; increased interdependence; feel- realization that the core of what happens in collective
ings of closeness, liking, and empathy; perceived assemblies is quite simple. Indeed, at each successive
social bonding; increased affiliation for copartici- moment, one and the same effect is repeated. It consists
pants; enhanced cooperative behaviors; feelings in the ever-growing openness of participants to one
of trust; feelings of similarity and shared belong- another. Identity shifts, homogeneous movements,
ing; compassion; altruistic behavior; and greater shared experience, perception of equivalence, ampli-
social closeness. fied emotions, and self-transcendent emotions all fuel
•• As documented in sociology, anthropology, and the feeling that the other is an alter ego. Moment after
social psychology, the perception of equality, of moment, this same effect goes on deepening. Conse-
shared identity, or of shared conditions, experi- quently, perceived differences between self and others
ence, and action can entail a reduced self-other are steadily narrowing. Reinforced by the upward spiral
differentiation. The same effect can also occur of amplified emotions, this evolution necessarily leads
when characteristics of the self and of others to a high-intensity state. Everyone experiences a pro-
overlap or when proper traits and shared traits found self-transformation on two levels with, on the
are seen as overlapping. Thus, the perception of one hand, a considerably heightened energy that gener-
self-others equivalence (equality/similarity/over- ates an experience of empowerment and, on the other
lap) emerges as a critical factor driving a reduced hand, a self that is profoundly “socialized” and thus
differentiation. In fact, the perception of equiva- radically different from its daily life, self-centered coun-
lence among the participants is already implicit terpart. The simplified trajectory of collective assem-
in the previous steps, and a self-others overlap blies revealed here demonstrates that the successive
is thus continually deepening in the process of a moments of these assemblies all pursue a same goal.
collective assembly. It consists in blurring the differences between individu-
•• In the assembly, emotions are transmitted and als in such a way that they will experience their equiva-
shared by a multitude of ways. Experiencing lence—in identity, in thoughts, in motives, in behaviors,
emotions together with others has the effect of in expression, and in emotions. This experience of
amplifying these emotions and their outward equivalence prompts a sense of unity among assembled
manifestations. This effect seems to depend in participants—e pluribus unum.
part on the social ties binding participants. Of
course, all the preceding processes are setting up
such ties.
The link to Durkheim’s broader project
•• In the assembly, the combination of emotional This trajectory, which highlights a same effect through-
transmission and emotional amplification is out the different moments of a collective assembly,
bound to feed up an upward spiral leading to an should be regarded in relation to Durkheim’s (1912/
ever-increasing intensification of emotions. Thus, 1915) broader project. His idea was that in everyday
the emotional state of the participants is expected life, individuals are disconnected from their group.
to evolve to a climax. They are focused on their own business, their utilitarian
•• The emotional amplification prompts sensations activities, and their personal concerns. In doing so,
of heightened energy and empowerment among people lose contact with the socially shared representa-
participants in such a way that they experience tions that bind them to their community. By the same
self-transformation. Experiencing the power of token, they lose the tools that give meaning to life and
the assembly is a likely trigger for the self- enable them to face human existence. They thus
transcendent emotion of awe. Awe reduces the become vulnerable. From his analysis of elementary
sense of individual self, increases the sense of religious practices, Durkheim concluded that the func-
self as part of a larger whole, orients participants tion of a collective assembly is to revitalize the partici-
toward outwards social concerns, and induces pants by opening them up to their community again.
feelings of oneness with others. It also feeds up The collective process has to transform individuals and
feelings of the religious or sacred. to replace in their consciousness what is personal and
distinct by what is collective and shared. At that
moment, individuals will forget themselves in favor of
what is common. Their conduct will be geared by the
The core effect: blurring the differences standards of the community, and their solidarity will be
This renewed reading of Durkheim’s (1912/1915) model maximal. The least we can say is that the view devel-
in the light of current empirical evidence leads to the oped by Durkheim on how collective assemblies
16 Rimé and Páez

achieve this goal is remarkably supported by the empir- of collective phenomena, the studies we have reviewed
ical data available a century later. are well appropriate for testing the specific hypotheses
we considered. They most often rest on experimental
designs of great precision, and their results were gener-
What exactly is collective effervescence?
ally replicated by several teams and under various con-
We have emphasized that nowhere in his writings does ditions. Methodologies based on the collective level
Durkheim provide a definition of effervescence. Our are now progressively emerging. For instance, Draper
analysis of his work leads us to propose what follows. (2014) examined data from 434 religious congrega-
Collective effervescence refers to the considerably tions. Using the congregation as the unit of analysis,
amplified emotional state manifested by the participants the study showed that collective effervescence corre-
of a collective assembly because of their experience of lates strongly with social solidarity. Likewise, in a sur-
shared emotions and the ensuing reciprocal emotional vey of a large music festival at which tens of thousands
activation. It thus denotes a shared emotional state of of visitors built tent camps and engaged in extensive
high intensity. Effervescence does not necessarily imply drinking and partying activities, Liebst (2019) adopted
high arousal. The coexperience and emotional coactiva- tent camps’ locations (N = 50) as the unit of analysis.
tion that fuel effervescence may rest on low-arousal Results demonstrated that crowding density was mark-
emotions highly saturated in negative or positive edly associated with the level of effervescence reported
valence, such as sadness (e.g., at a funeral) or deep joy by participants.
(e.g., at a Buddhist ceremony). The coexperience and One may wonder whether there is any counterevidence
coactivation enhance the intensity of the emotion, that to Durkheim’s (1912/1915) model, such as negative rather
is, the magnitude of its hold on a person’s psycho- than positive effects of collective assemblies? Some studies
physiological condition. Collective effervescence can on the effect of religious rituals in distressed individuals
involve both routine emotions (e.g., joy, sadness, anger, did record negative effects of participation, such as
fear) and more complex emotional states (e.g., kama increased anxiety and higher somatization or bodily symp-
muta, awe, collective pride). In all cases, the same effect toms (e.g., Thompson & Vardaman, 1997; Zeidner &
follows, that of the transformation of the participants Hammer, 1992; see also Hopkins & Reicher, 2016). How-
who feel carried above themselves by the power of the ever, similar studies that paid attention to measuring
collective experience. effects in both directions have frequently observed at once
Note that physical copresence might not be a neces- both positive and negative effects for participants (e.g.,
sary condition in the elicitation of the process described Pargament et al., 1994; Park & Cohen, 1993). This has also
by Durkheim (1912/1915). The coexperience and emo- been observed in the study of collective processes set up
tional coactivation that fuel effervescence might also in aftermath of periods of collective violence. Thus, vic-
develop between participants who experience only a tims of collective violence in Guatemala who participated
virtual copresence. This was suggested by the findings in mourning and funeral ceremonies reported enhanced
of a large-scale analysis of the content of tweets emitted negative affect, sense of injustice, and mourning, but their
by French users before and after the November 2015 participation was also associated with a greater recon-
terrorist attacks in Paris (García & Rimé, 2019). More struction of social support and a higher willingness to
negative emotional exchanges at the time of the attacks help other people (Beristaín et al., 2000). After the 1994
were followed by effects suggesting that participants genocide in Rwanda, participation in the Gacaca truth and
involved in such exchanges had been carried above reconciliation commissions reactivated negative affect and
themselves. They evidenced a marked long-term posttraumatic-stress symptoms among survivors, but it
increase in the use of lexical indicators of solidarity, also improved indicators of their social integration
prosocial behavior, and positive affect. (Kanyangara et al., 2007; Rimé et al., 2011). Such findings
were replicated in a study of community-level reconcili-
ation forums in Sierra Leone (Cilliers et al., 2016). These
About the collected evidence
results support the general principle that collective
Most of the empirical evidence we have gathered on encounters heighten the emotions involved. It is therefore
Durkheim’s (1912/1916) model of collective assemblies consistent that a mourning ritual heightens sadness.
comes from studies involving dyads or other inter­ Whatever the emotion, the key to the positive effects
personal contexts, not collectivities. This was to be of a collective process is that the emotion gets shared.
expected because most research on social behavior In the introduction of this article, we insisted that
takes the individual as the unit of analysis. Although for Durkheim (1912/1915), survival, well-being, and
conducted in research areas totally foreign to the study social integration are tightly interrelated. It could then
Perspectives on Psychological Science XX(X) 17

be argued that the strongest test of Durkheim’s model Fredrickson (1998, 2013) gathered all such findings
would consist of an examination of effects of collectiv- together with studies from her own lab in a broaden-
ity on survival. Actually, evidence from meta-analytic and-build theory of positive emotions. According to this
investigation already supported the effect of collective theory, the evolutionary function of positive emotions
gatherings on survival. McCullough et al. (2000) exam- is to build an individual’s resources for survival. This
ined 41 research reports in which a measure of religious build function is achieved by a momentarily broadened
involvement was assessed as a predictor of all-cause scope of awareness. The theory states that positive
mortality. A positive relationship (r = .10) between reli- emotions, relative to negative emotions and neutral
gious activity and survival or longer life expectancy states, widen the array of thoughts, action urges, and
was found. Results showed that this relationship was percepts that spontaneously come to mind (Fredrickson,
explained by the frequency of participation in public 2013). Obviously, there is a huge overlap between the
religious rituals. broadening effects thus documented as following
a positive emotional experience and those we have
listed as resulting or supposed to result from collective
Alternative perspectives
participation.
Is a collective participation in an assembly a necessary Positive emotional experiences are far from being
condition to yield the personal transformation that, alone in eliciting effects reminding the blurring of self-
according to Durkheim’s (1912/1915) model, opens other differentiation described in Durkheim’s (1912/1915)
health and well-being benefits? The investigation of the model of collective assemblies. For reasons that the
effects of positive emotional experiences opens an impor- history of science might profitably address, there is cur-
tant avenue for reflection on this question. Alice Isen’s rently a plethora of studies documenting phenomena
research has abundantly demonstrated that positive emo- of reduced self-other differentiation and rapprochement
tional experiences modify individuals both cognitively between individuals. As was shown in our review of
and socially (for a review, see Isen, 2000) in a direction current theory and research related to Durkheim’s
suggesting that these individuals are, as in the collective Moment 2 (homogeneous manifestations), the depen-
effervescence, carried above themselves. Cognitively, dent variables of these studies included prosocial atti-
compared with individuals whose emotional state was tudes, altruistic behavior, interdependence, feeling of
negative or neutral, those in a positive emotional state closeness, liking, empathy, compassion, affiliation, simi-
were found more creative, open to information, willing larity, belonging, cooperation, trust, integration, “click-
to explore, and successful in synthesis and problem- ing,” rapport, desire to interact, and connectedness,
solving tasks. Socially, these individuals took into account among others. In addition, in our review of current
the point of view of others more and were more sociable, theory and research relevant to Durkheim’s Moment 3
cooperative, generous, socially responsible, and apt to (group consciousness), we encountered a plethora of
negotiate. Since Isen’s (2000) review, new data largely concepts denoting social processes involving a sense of
confirmed her observations. At the cognitive level, com- nondifferentiation, union between individuals, together-
pared with participants in control conditions, participants ness, we-ness, oneness with one’s peers, unison, and so
in a positive-emotion setting evidenced a more global forth. They included communitas, CS, social identity,
visual processing and a broader repertoire of action PES, IOS, IF, and self-transcendence.
(Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005), a more extended visual If so many conditions can produce effects of broad-
attention (Wadlinger & Isaacowitz, 2006), a broadened ening of the self or of blurring of the self-other differ-
semantic access and visuospatial attention (Rowe et al., entiation, what then remains proper to the process of
2007), and a higher creativity (Baas et al., 2008; Davis, collective assemblies described by Durkheim
2009). At the social level, relative to control participants, (1912/1915)? As our review showed, Durkheim’s pro-
individuals who experienced positive emotions increased cess has the particularity of encompassing almost all
their trust (Dunn & Schweitzer, 2005), formed more inclu- the others. Moreover, it carries out this integration of
sive social categories (Dovidio et al., 1995; Isen et al., the operative variables in a cumulative manner. Moment
1992), formed more common in-group identities (Dovidio after moment, the variables that produce the central
et al., 1998), reduced the own-race bias in face perception effect succeed one another and intensify this effect
( Johnson & Fredrickson, 2005), manifested greater further, whereas it was already largely fed in the pre-
perspective-taking and compassion for a person from a ceding moments. The unfolding of the successive
dissimilar cultural background (Nelson, 2009), and evi- moments takes place in an almost inexorable way
denced higher levels of prosocial or helping behaviors because of, in particular, the volatility of the shared
(e.g., Carlson et al., 1988). emotions. Being immersed in the group and in close
18 Rimé and Páez

proximity to the many others who are participating in emotion is experienced (e.g., Gable & Reis, 2010; Rimé,
the same process and experiencing the same effects 2009, 2020). Collective assemblies therefore only refresh
obviously creates optimal conditions for the emergence a relational potential that is existing in individuals since
and intensification of the experience of unity. Positive their most distant origins.
emotions, too, are mobilized by collective participation
(Ehrenreich, 2007) so that their self-broadening effects
Conclusion: an ever-increasing
signaled by Isen and by Fredrickson are added to those
of the many other variables. In sum, the process socio-integrative experience
described by Durkheim is arguably unparalleled in its At the end of this review of Durkheim’s (1912/1915)
power to produce the effects that have been described. text and of the relevant empirical data currently avail-
able, we conclude that participation in a successful
Blurring of self-other differentiation collective assembly progressively leads participants to
a high-intensity experience of self-transformation. This
The notion of a reduction of differences between indi- experience is paved by the socio-integrative effects
viduals, of a blurring of individual boundaries, and of accumulated in the previous phases of the collective
a sense of fusion between individuals and their sur- process. The proximal trigger of the high-intensity state
roundings or their group is not easily conceived for lies in the ever-increasing amplification of emotions
people who are situated in an individualistic culture. in the assembly. Participants then experience a self-
Nevertheless, our literature review showed that this transformation resting on intense emotions, high energy,
notion is well represented in current social sciences. In and empowerment. It is accompanied by reduced self-
addition, the phenomenon of blurring of individual focus, enhanced social orientation, and possibly feeling
boundaries could be evidenced under very simple labo- of sacredness. Following our analysis of Durkheim’s
ratory conditions. In an investigation of the effects of notion of effervescence, it seems reasonable to retain
synchronized multisensory stimulation, Paladino et al. the use of this term to denote conditions involving the
(2010) exposed participants to a video image of a various elements just enumerated. This article showed
stranger. The experiment consisted of brushing the par- that the term “effervescence,” devoid of connotations
ticipant’s cheek synchronously (vs. asynchronously) of violence and barbarism, covers nevertheless excep-
with brushing applied to the face seen on the video. tional moments. At the height of a collective assembly,
Compared with participants in the asynchronous condi- in a state of high emotional intensity, and sustained by
tions, participants in the synchronous one perceived the power of the mass, participants experience a drastic
their face as more similar to the face of the stranger, reduction in their differentiation from their copartici-
manifested a higher overlap with the other in an IOS pants. What they can see around them indicates that a
measurement, felt more attracted to the other, projected multitude of others are undergoing a similar experi-
the self onto the other more in an inference task, and ence. This is a potential source of an experience of
evidenced a higher anchoring of their behavior in that mise en abyme, during which each person feels dupli-
of the other in a conformity task. This study thus cated in the multitude that surrounds them. This is also
showed that the self-other overlap can result from a a potential source of perception of generalized har-
rudimentary sensorial experience. mony, which is capable of eliciting deep joy (e.g.,
We add that the experience of fusion with another is Ehrenreich, 2007). Of course, most collective experi-
far from being an alien experience to humans. In fact, ences will probably not produce such extreme effects.
it corresponds to their initial condition of existence. A But what can be stated at the end of our analysis is that
human being develops in symbiosis with another human as soon as one steps into a collective assembly, the
being during a lengthy intrauterine period. After birth, psychosocial process described in this article is set in
a fusional relationship is perpetuated between parent motion. And the further into this process one goes, the
and child in interactive synchrony—exchanges of sen- more a feeling of oneness is felt.
sory, hormonal, and physiological signals during social
interactions—that spans the period of early gestation to Transparency
weaning (Feldman, 2007). These symbiotic relationships Action Editor: Klaus Fiedler
constitute the grounds on which children can gradually Editor: Klaus Fiedler
undertake their autonomy and develop a growing indi- Declaration of Conflicting Interests
vidualization. Even when individualized, individuals The author(s) declared that there were no conflicts of
continue to be linked to their entourage by attachment interest with respect to the authorship or the publication
ties—ties that are particularly reactivated every time an of this article.
Perspectives on Psychological Science XX(X) 19

ORCID iD relationships (pp. 90–115). Oxford University Press. https://


doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195398694.013.0005
Bernard Rimé https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8487-575X
Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-
Acknowledgments analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic
tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin,
Due to the countless questions, criticisms and suggestions 134(6), 779–806. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012815
received, this article owes a great deal to its anonymous Barsade, S. G., & Gibson, D. E. (2012). Group affect: Its
reviewers, to Adam Cohen who was action editor of an earlier influence on individual and group outcomes. Current
version of the manuscript, and to the action of Klaus Fiedler. Directions in Psychological Science, 21(2), 119–123.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412438352
Note
Bar-Tal, D. (2000). Shared beliefs in a society: Social psychologi-
1. One reviewer suggested that contemporary theories of social cal analysis. Sage. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781452204697
rituals should also be examined here. It is beyond the scope Beristaín, C. M., Páez, D., & González, J. L. (2000). Rituals,
of this article to review these theories and compare them with social sharing, silence, emotions and collective mem-
Durkheim’s (1912/1915) visions because with the exception of ory claims in the case of the Guatemalan genocide.
Collins’s (2004) model, which is an extension of Durkheim’s Psicothema, 12(1), 117–130.
ideas, contemporary theories of rituals are more specific in Berkman, L. F., Glass, T., Brissette, I., & Seeman, T. E. (2000).
scope. For example, Whitehouse’s model of social rituals (e.g., From social integration to health: Durkheim in the new
Whitehouse & Lanman, 2014) is specifically aimed at low- millennium. Social Science & Medicine, 51(6), 843–857.
frequency rituals heavily loaded in negative affect that are https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00065-4
thought to feed into intrapsychic mechanisms of affectively Bernieri, F. J., Reznick, J. S., & Rosenthal, R. (1988). Synchrony,
charged autobiographical memory. Comparatively, Durkheim’s pseudosynchrony, and dissynchrony: Measuring the
theory is unique in that it addresses all types of collective gath- entrainment process in mother-infant interactions. Journal
erings regardless of their degree of ritualization, their positive of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(2), 243–253.
or negative valence, their high or low level of arousal, or their https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.2.243
high or low frequency, and it proposes a sequence of interper- Bernieri, F. J., & Rosenthal, R. (1991). Interpersonal coordi-
sonal and social processes that produce a wide range of effects. nation: Behavior matching and interactional synchrony.
In view of the abundance of references to Durkheim’s theory In R. S. Feldman & B. Rimé (Eds.), Fundamentals of
today and because at the time his model was formulated, it was nonverbal behavior (pp. 401–432). Cambridge University
not yet the norm to conduct empirical tests, we examine the Press & Editions de la Maison des Sciences de l’Homme.
following questions: What is the validity of this model today? Boothby, E. J., Clark, M. S., & Bargh, J. A. (2014). Shared
Do we find empirical arguments that would justify the attention experiences are amplified. Psychological Science, 25(12),
contemporary scholars give it? 2209–2216. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614551162
Boothby, E. J., Smith, L. K., Clark, M. S., & Bargh, J. A.
References (2016). Psychological distance moderates the ampli-
Algoe, S. B., & Haidt, J. (2009). Witnessing excellence in action: fication of shared experience. Personality and Social
The ‘other-praising’ emotions of elevation, gratitude, and Psychology Bulletin, 42(10), 1431–1444. https://doi.org/
admiration. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4(2), 105– 10.1177/0146167216662869
127. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760802650519 Bouchat, P., Rimé, B., Van Eycken, R., & Nils, F. (2020). The
Allen, V. (1970). Toward understanding riots: Some perspec- virtues of collective gatherings: A study on the positive
tives. Journal of Social Issues, 26(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/ effects of a major scouting event. Journal of Applied Social
10.1111/j.1540-4560.1970.tb01278.x Psychology, 50(3), 189–201. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp
Alnabulsi, H., & Drury, J. (2014). Social identification mod- .12649
erates the effect of crowd density on safety at the Brown, S. L., Fredrickson, B. L., Wirth, M. M., Poulin, M. J.,
Hajj. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Meier, E. A., Heaphy, E. D., Cohen, M. D., & Schultheiss,
USA, 111(25), 9091–9096. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas O. C. (2009). Social closeness increases salivary progester-
.1404953111 one in humans. Hormones and Behavior, 56(1), 108–111.
Aron, A., & Aron, E. N. (1986). Love and the expansion of self: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.03.022
Understanding attraction and satisfaction. Harper & Row. Buehler, A. (2012). The twenty-first-century study of col-
Aron, A., & Aron, E. N. (2006). Romantic relationships from the lective effervescence: Expanding the context of field-
perspectives of the self-expansion model and attachment work. Fieldwork in Religion, 7(1), 70–97. https://doi
theory: Partially overlapping circles. In M. Mikulincer & .org/10.1558/firn.v7i1.70
F. S. Goodman (Eds.), The dynamics of romantic love: Burke, E. (1757/2008). A philosophical enquiry into the sub-
Attachment, caregiving, and sex (pp. 359–382). Guilford. lime and beautiful. Routledge Classics.
Aron, A., Lewandowski, G. W., Jr., Mashek, D., & Aron, E. N. Carlson, M., Charlin, V., & Miller, N. (1988). Positive mood
(2013). The self-expansion model of motivation and and helping behavior: A test of six hypotheses. Journal
cognition in close relationships. In J. A. Simpson & of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(2), 211–229.
L. Campbell (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of close https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.55.2.211
20 Rimé and Páez

Chapman, A. J. (2017). Social aspects of humorous laughter. In Drury, J. (2018). The role of social identity processes in mass
A. J. Chapman & H. C. Foot (Eds.), Humor and laughter: emergency behaviour: An integrative review. European
Theory, research, and applications (pp. 155–186). John Review of Social Psychology, 29(1), 38–81. https://doi.org/
Wiley & Sons. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203789469-9 10.1080/10463283.2018.1471948
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon Drury, J. (2020). Recent developments in the psychology of
effect: The perception-behavior link and social interac- crowds and collective behaviour. Current Opinion in
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), Psychology, 35, 12–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc
893–910. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893 .2020.02.005
Chartrand, T. L., & Lakin, J. L. (2013). The antecedents and con- Drury, J., & Reicher, S. D. (2000). Collective action and psy-
sequences of human behavioral mimicry. Annual Review chological change: The emergence of new social identi-
of Psychology, 64, 285–308. https://doi.org/10.1146/ ties. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39(4), 579–604.
annurev-psych-113011-143754 https://doi.org/10.1348/014466600164642
Chartrand, T. L., & van Baaren, R. (2009). Human mimicry. In Drury, J., & Reicher, S. D. (2005). Explaining enduring
M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy- empowerment: A comparative study of collective action
chology (Vol. 41, pp. 219–274). Elsevier Academic Press. and psychological outcomes. European Journal of Social
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)00405-X Psychology, 35(1), 35–58. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp
Chu, P. S., Saucier, D. A., & Hafner, E. (2010). Meta-analysis of .231
the relationships between social support and well-being Drury, J., & Reicher, S. D. (2009). Collective psychological
in children and adolescents. Journal of Social and Clinical empowerment as a model of social change: Researching
Psychology, 29(6), 624–645. https://doi.org/10.1521/ crowds and power. Journal of Social Issues, 65(4), 707–
jscp.2010.29.6.624 725. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2009.01622.x
Chung, V., Grèzes, J., & Pacherie, E. (2022). Collective emo- Drury, J., Stott, C., Ball, R., Reicher, S., Neville, F., Bell, L.,
tion: An experimental framework. PsychArchives. https:// Biddlestone, M., Choudhury, S., Lovell, M., & Ryan, C.
doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/axz2v (2020). A social identity model of riot diffusion: From
Cilliers, J., Dube, O., & Siddiqi, B. (2016). Reconciling after injustice to empowerment in the 2011 London riots.
civil conflict increases social capital but decreases indi- European Journal of Social Psychology, 50(3), 646–661.
vidual well-being. Science, 352(6287), 787–794. https:// https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2650
doi.org/10.1126/science.aad9682 Dunn, J. R., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2005). Feeling and believing:
Collins, R. (2004). Interaction ritual chains. Princeton The influence of emotion on trust. Journal of Personality
University Press. and Social Psychology, 88(5), 736–748. https://doi.org/
Collins, R. (2014). Interaction ritual chains and collective effer- 10.1037/0022-3514.88.5.736
vescence. In C. von Scheve & M. Salmela (Eds.), Collective Durkheim, E. (1915). The elementary forms of religious life
emotions: Perspectives from psychology, philosophy, and (J. D. Swan, Trans.). George Allen & Unwin. (Original
sociology (pp. 299–311). Oxford University Press. https:// work published 1912)
doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199659180.003.0020 Durkheim, É. (1953). Value judgments and judgments of real-
Davis, M. A. (2009). Understanding the relationship between ity. In E. Durkheim (Ed.) & D. F. Pocock (Trans.), Sociology
mood and creativity: A meta-analysis. Organizational and philosophy (pp. 42–51). Routledge. (Original work
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 108(1), 25–38. published 1911)
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2008.04.001 Durkheim, E. (1893/1933). The division of labor in society (W.
de Rivera, J., & Páez, D. (2007). Emotional climate, human Halls, Trans.). The Free Press. (Original work published
security, and cultures of peace. Journal of Social Issues, 1893)
63(2), 233–253. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2007 Echterhoff, G., Higgins, E. T., & Levine, J. M. (2009). Shared
.00506.x reality: Experiencing commonality with others’ inner
Diener, E. (1980). Deindividuation: The absence of self- states about the world. Perspectives on Psychological
awareness and self-regulation in group members. In P. B. Science, 4(5), 496–521. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-
Paulus (Ed.), Psychology of group influence (pp. 209–242). 6924.2009.01161.x
Erlbaum. Ehrenreich, B. (2007). Dancing in the streets: A history of col-
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., Isen, A. M., & Lowrance, lective joy. Macmillan.
R. (1995). Group representations and intergroup bias: Eisenberg, N. (1991). Meta-analytic contributions to the lit-
Positive affect, similarity, and group size. Personality and erature on prosocial behavior. Personality and Social
Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(8), 856–865. https://doi Psychology Bulletin, 17(3), 273–282. https://doi.org/
.org/10.1177/0146167295218009 10.1177/0146167291173007
Dovidio, J. F., Isen, A. M., Guerra, P., Gaertner, S. L., & Rust, Elfenbein, H. A. (2014). The many faces of emotional con-
M. (1998). Positive affect, cognition, and the reduction of tagion: An affective process theory of affective linkage.
intergroup bias. In C. Sedikides, J. Schopler, & C. A. Insko Organizational Psychology Review, 4(4), 326–362. https://
(Eds.), Intergroup cognition and intergroup behavior (pp. doi.org/10.1177/2041386614542889
337–366). Erlbaum. Feldman, R. (2007). Parent-infant synchrony: Biological foun-
Draper, S. (2014). Effervescence and solidarity in religious dations and developmental outcomes. Current Directions
organizations. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, in Psychological Science, 16(6), 340–345. https://doi
53(2), 229–248. https://doi.org/10.1111/jssr.12109 .org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00532.x
Perspectives on Psychological Science XX(X) 21

Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of social- Academy of Sciences, USA, 106(19), 7687–7688. https://
ity: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0903076106
Psychological Review, 99(4), 689–723. https://doi.org/ Haidt, J., Patrick Seder, J., & Kesebir, S. (2008). Hive psy-
10.1037/0033-295X.99.4.689 chology, happiness, and public policy. The Journal of
Fiske, A. P. (2020). Kama muta: Discovering the connecting emo- Legal Studies, 37(Suppl. 2), S133–S156. https://doi.org/
tion. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780367220952 10.1086/529447
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Hale, J., & Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2016). Cognitive mechanisms
Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 300–319. https://doi for responding to mimicry from others. Neuroscience &
.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.300 Biobehavioral Reviews, 63, 106–123. https://doi.org/
Fredrickson, B. L. (2013). Positive emotions broaden and 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.02.006
build. In E. A. Plant & P. G. Devine (Eds.), Advances Hanley, A. W., & Garland, E. L. (2019). Spatial frame of
in experimental social psychology (Vol. 47, pp. 1–53). reference as a phenomenological feature of self-tran-
Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12- scendence: Measurement and manipulation through
407236-7.00001-2 mindfulness meditation. Psychology of Consciousness:
Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(4), 329–345. https://
broaden the scope of attention and thought-action reper- doi.org/10.1037/cns0000204
toires. Cognition & Emotion, 19(3), 313–332. https://doi Hardin, C. D., & Higgins, E. T. (1996). Shared reality: How
.org/10.1080/02699930441000238 social verification makes the subjective objective. In R. M.
Freedman, J. L., & Perlick, D. (1979). Crowding, contagion, and Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation
laughter. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 15(3), and cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 28–84). The Guilford Press.
295–303. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(79)90040-4 Haslam, S. A., & Reicher, S. D. (2006). Stressing the group:
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge University Social identity and the unfolding dynamics of stress.
Press. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(5), 1037–1052. https://
Fuller, R. G., & Sheehy-Skeffington, A. (1974). Effects of group doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.91.5.1037
laughter on responses to humourous material: A replica- Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional
tion and extension. Psychological Reports, 35(1), 531–534. contagion. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/
https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1974.35.1.531 10.1017/CBO9781139174138
Gable, S. L., & Reis, H. T. (2010). Good news! Capitalizing Hess, U., & Fischer, A. (2013). Emotional mimicry as social
on positive events in an interpersonal context. In M. P. regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17,
Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychol- 142–157. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868312472607
ogy (Vol. 42, pp. 195–257). Academic Press. https://doi Hess, U., & Fischer, A. (2014). Emotional mimicry: Why
.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(10)42004-3 and when we mimic emotions. Social and Personality
Gabriel, S., Naidu, E., Paravati, E., Morrison, C. D., & Gainey, K. Psychology Compass, 8(2), 45–57. https://doi.org/10.1111/
(2020). Creating the sacred from the profane: Collective spc3.12083
effervescence and everyday activities. The Journal of Higgins, E. T. (2019). Shared reality: What makes us strong
Positive Psychology, 15(1), 129–154. https://doi.org/10 and tears us apart. Oxford University Press. https://doi
.1080/17439760.2019.1689412 .org/10.1093/oso/9780190948054.001.0001
García, D., & Rimé, B. (2019). Collective emotions and Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., &
social resilience in the digital traces after a terrorist Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness and social isolation
attack. Psychological Science, 30(4), 617–628. https://doi as risk factors for mortality: A meta-analytic review.
.org/10.1177/0956797619831964 Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 227–237.
Goldenberg, A., Weisz, E., Sweeny, T. D., Cikara, M., & https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614568352
Gross, J. J. (2021). The crowd-emotion-amplification Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social
effect. Psychological Science, 32(3), 437–450. https://doi relationships and mortality risk: A meta-analytic review.
.org/10.1177/0956797620970561 PLOS Medicine, 7(7), Article e1000316. https://doi.org/10
Goldstein, N. J., & Cialdini, R. B. (2007). The spyglass self: A .1371/journal.pmed.1000316
model of vicarious self-perception. Journal of Personality Hopkins, N., & Reicher, S. (2016). The psychology of health
and Social Psychology, 92(3), 402–417. https://doi.org/ and well-being in mass gatherings: A review and a research
10.1037/0022-3514.92.3.402 agenda. Journal of Epidemiology and Global Health, 6(2),
Haidt, J. (2003a). Elevation and the positive psychology of 49–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jegh.2015.06.001
morality. In C. L. M. Keyes & J. Haidt (Eds.), Flourishing: Hopkins, N., Reicher, S. D., Khan, S. S., Tewari, S., Srinivasan,
Positive psychology and the life well lived (pp. 275–289). N., & Stevenson, C. (2016). Explaining effervescence:
American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/ Investigating the relationship between shared social
10.1037/10594-012 identity and positive experience in crowds. Cognition &
Haidt, J. (2003b). The moral emotions. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Emotion, 30(1), 20–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/0269993
Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective 1.2015.1015969
sciences (pp. 852–870). Oxford University Press. Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal
Haidt, J., & Morris, J. P. (2009). Finding the self in self- synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27(6),
transcendent emotions. Proceedings of the National 949–960. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949
22 Rimé and Páez

Isen, A. (2000). Positive affect and decision making. In (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods,
M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions research (pp. 221–232). Oxford University Press.
(pp. 261–277). The Guilford Press. Martin, L. J., Hathaway, G., Isbester, K., Mirali, S., Acland,
Isen, A. M., Niedenthal, P. M., & Cantor, N. (1992). An influ- E. L., Niederstrasser, N., Slepian, P. M., Trost, Z., Bartz,
ence of positive affect on social categorization. Motivation J. A., Sapolsky, R. M., Sternberg, W. F., Levitin, D. J., &
and Emotion, 16(1), 65–78. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Mogil, J. S. (2015). Reducing social stress elicits emo-
BF00996487 tional contagion of pain in mouse and human strangers.
Jetten, J., Haslam, C., & Alexander, S. H. (2012). The social Current Biology, 25(3), 326–332. https://doi.org/10.1016/
cure: Identity, health and well-being. Psychology Press. j.cub.2014.11.028
https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203813195 McCullough, M. E., Hoyt, W. T., Larson, D. B., Koenig, H. G., &
Johnson, K. J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2005). “We all look the Thoresen, C. (2000). Religious involvement and mortality:
same to me”: Positive emotions eliminate the own-race bias A meta-analytic review. Health Psychology, 19(3), 211–
in face recognition. Psychological Science, 16(11), 875– 222. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.19.3.211
881. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01631.x McNeil, W. (1995). Keeping together in time: Dance and drill
Kanyangara, P., Rimé, B., Philippot, P., & Yzerbyt, V. (2007). in human history. Harvard University Press.
Collective rituals, emotional climate and intergroup percep- Mehu, M., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2008). Naturalistic observa-
tion: Participation in “Gacaca” tribunals and assimilation tions of smiling and laughter in human group interac-
of the Rwandan genocide. Journal of Social Issues, 63(2), tions. Behaviour, 145(12), 1747–1780. https://doi.org/10
387–403. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2007.00515.x .1163/156853908786279619
Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (2003). Approaching awe, a moral, Mogan, R., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. A. (2017). To be in
spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cognition & Emo­ synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony’s effects
tion, 17(2), 297–314. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930 on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. Journal
302297 of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 13–20. https://doi
Khan, S. S., Hopkins, N., Reicher, S., Tewari, S., Srinivasan, .org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.03.009
N., & Stevenson, C. (2015). Shared identity predicts Monroy, M., & Keltner, D. (2022). Awe as a pathway to mental
enhanced health at a mass gathering. Group Processes & and physical health. Perspectives on Psychological Science.
Intergroup Relations, 18(4), 504–522. https://doi.org/10 Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/
.1177/1368430214556703 17456916221094856
Krueger, J., & Szanto, T. (2016). Extended emotions. Philosophy Moscovici, S. (1981). L’âge des foules [The age of crowds]. Fayard.
Compass, 11(12), 863–878. https://doi.org/10.1111/phc3 Moscovici, S. (1993). The invention of society. Psychological
.12390 explanations for social phenomena. Polity Press.
Leader, T., Mullen, B., & Abrams, D. (2007). Without mercy: Nahleen, S., Dornin, G., & Takarangi, M. K. (2019). When
The immediate impact of group size on lynch mob atroc- more is not merrier: Shared stressful experiences amplify.
ity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(10), Cognition & Emotion, 33(8), 1718–1725. https://doi.org/
1340–1352. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167207303951 10.1080/02699931.2019.1597683
Le Bon, G. (1895/1997). The crowd: A study of the popular mind Nelson, K. (2009). Young minds in social worlds. Harvard
(Ernest Benn & Snedders D. S. Trans.). (Original work University Press.
published in 1895) Neville, F. G., Novelli, D., Drury, J., & Reicher, S. D. (2022).
Levy, S. G., & Fenley, W. F. (1979). Audience size and likeli- Shared social identity transforms social relations in imagi-
hood and intensity of response during a humorous movie. nary crowds. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations,
Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 13(6), 409–412. 25(1), 158–173. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430220
https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03336909 936759
Liebst, L. S. (2019). Exploring the sources of collective effer- Neville, F. G., & Reicher, S. D. (2011). The experience of
vescence: A multilevel study. Sociological Science, 6, collective participation: Shared identity, relatedness and
27–42. https://doi.org/10.15195/v6.a2 emotionality. Contemporary Social Science, 6, 377–396.
Maddux, W. W., Mullen, E., & Galinsky, A. (2008). Chameleons https://doi.org/10.1080/21582041.2012.627277
bake bigger pies: Strategic behavioral mimicry facilitates Novelli, D., Drury, J., & Reicher, S. (2010). Come together:
integrative negotiations outcomes. Journal of Experimen­tal Two studies concerning the impact of group relations on
Social Psychology, 44(2), 461–468. https://doi.org/10.1016/ personal space. British Journal of Social Psychology, 49(2),
j.jesp.2007.02.003 223–236. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466609X449377
Magyaródi, T., & Oláh, A. (2015). A cross-sectional survey Novelli, D., Drury, J., Reicher, S., & Stott, C. (2013).
study about the most common solitary and social flow Crowdedness mediates the effect of social identification
activities to extend the concept of optimal experience. on positive emotion in a crowd: A survey of two crowd
Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 11(4), 632–650. https:// events. PLOS ONE, 8(11), Article e78983. https://doi.org/10
doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v11i4.866 .1371/journal.pone.0078983
Manstead, A. S. R., & Fischer, A. H. (2001). Social appraisal: Oatley, K. (2000). The sentiments and beliefs of distrib-
The social world as object of and influence on appraisal uted cognition. In N. H. Frijda, A. S. R. Manstead, & S.
processes. In K. R. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone Bem (Eds.), Emotions and belief: How feelings influence
Perspectives on Psychological Science XX(X) 23

thoughts (pp. 78–107). Cambridge University Press. Pinel, E. C. (2018). Existential isolation and I-sharing: Inter­
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511659904.004 personal and intergroup implications. Current Opinion
Olszanowski, M., Wróbel, M., & Hess, U. (2019). Mimicking and in Psychology, 23, 84–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc
sharing emotions: A re-examination of the link between .2018.01.002
facial mimicry and emotional contagion. Cognition & Pinquart, M., & Sörensen, S. (2000). Influences of socioeco-
Emotion, 34(2), 367–376. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699 nomic status, social network, and competence on subjec-
931.2019.1611543 tive well-being in later life: A meta-analysis. Psychology
Pacherie, E. (2017). Collective phenomenology. In M. Jankovic and Aging, 15(2), 187–224. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-
& K. Ludwig (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of collec- 7974.15.2.187
tive intentionality (pp. 162–173). Routledge. https://doi Pizarro, J. J., Basabe, N., Fernández, I., Carrera, P., Apodaca, P.,
.org/10.4324/9781315768571-16 Man Ging, C. I., Cusi, O., & Páez, D. (2021). Self-
Páez, D., Rimé, B., Basabe, N., Wlodarczyk, A., & Zumeta, transcendent emotions and their social effects: Awe,
L. N. (2015). Psychosocial effects of perceived emotional elevation and kama muta promote a human identification
synchrony in collective gatherings. Journal of Personality and motivations to help others. Frontiers in Psychology,
and Social Psychology, 108(5), 711–729. https://doi 12, Article 709859. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021
.org/10.1037/pspi0000014 .709859
Paladino, M. P., Mazzurega, M., Pavani, F., & Schubert, T. W. Pizarro, J. J., Zumeta, L. N., Bouchat, P., Włodarczyk, A., Rimé, B.,
(2010). Synchronous multisensory stimulation blurs self- Basabe, N., Amutio, A., & Páez, D. (2022). Emotional
other boundaries. Psychological Science, 21(9), 1202– processes, collective behavior, and social movements: A
1207. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610379234 meta-analytic review of collective effervescence outcomes
Pargament, K. I., Ishler, K., Dubow, E. F., Stanik, P., Rouiller, R., during collective gatherings and demonstrations. Frontiers
Crowe, P., Cullman, E. P., Albert, M., & Royster, B. J. in Psychology, 13, Article 974683. https://doi.org/10.3389/
(1994). Methods of religious coping with the Gulf War: fpsyg.2022.974683
Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Journal for the Platow, M. J., Haslam, S. A., Both, A., Chew, I., Cuddon, M.,
Scientific Study of Religion, 33(4), 347–361. https://doi Goharpey, N., Maurer, J., Rosini, S., Tsekouris, A., &
.org/10.2307/1386494 Grace, D. M. (2005). “It’s not funny if they’re laugh-
Park, C. L., & Cohen, L. H. (1993). Religious and nonreli- ing”: Self-categorization, social influence, and responses
gious coping with the death of a friend. Cognitive Therapy to canned laughter. Journal of Experimental Social
and Research, 17(6), 561–577. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Psychology, 41(5), 542–550. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp
BF01176079 .2004.09.005
Parkinson, B. (2011). Interpersonal emotion transfer: Contagion Postmes, T., & Spears, R. (1998). Deindividuation and anti-
and social appraisal. Social and Personality Psychology normative behavior: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Compass, 5(7), 428–439. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751- Bulletin, 123(3), 238–259. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-
9004.2011.00365.x 2909.123.3.238
Parkinson, B. (2019). Calibrating emotional orientations: Reicher, S., Spears, R., & Haslam, S. A. (2010). The social
Social appraisal and other kinds of relation alignment. identity approach in social psychology. In M. S. Wetherell
In D. Dukes & F. Clément (Eds.), Foundations of affective & C. T. Mohanty (Eds.), Sage identities handbook
social learning: Conceptualising the transmission of social (pp. 45–62). Sage. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446200
value (pp. 117–141). Cambridge University Press. https:// 889.n4
doi.org/10.1017/9781108661362.006 Reicher, S. D. (1984). The St. Pauls’ riot: An explanation of
Parkinson, B. (2020). Intragroup emotion convergence: Beyond the limits of crowd action in terms of a social identity
contagion and social appraisal. Personality and Social model. European Journal of Social Psychology, 14(1),
Psychology Review, 24(2), 121–140. https://doi.org/10 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420140102
.1177/1088868319882596 Reicher, S. D. (1987). Crowd behaviour as social action. In
Parkinson, B. (2021). Heart to heart: A relation-alignment J. Turner, M. Hogg, P. Oakes, S. Reicher, & M. Wetherell
approach to emotion’s social effects. Emotion Review, (Eds.), Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization
13(2), 78–89. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073921990733 theory (pp. 171–202). Blackwell.
Parkinson, B., Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. (2005). Reicher, S. D. (2001). The psychology of crowd dynamics. In
Emotion in social relations: Cultural, group, and inter- M. Hogg & S. Tindale (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social
personal processes. Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10 psychology: Group processes (pp. 182–208). Blackwell.
.4324/9780203644966 https://doi.org/10.1111/b.9781405106535.2002.00010.x
Pennebaker, J. W., Paez, D., & Rimé, B. (Eds.). (1997). Reicher, S. D., & Haslam, S. A. (2006). Rethinking the psy-
Collective memory of political events: Social and psycho- chology of tyranny: The BBC prison experiment. British
logical perspectives. Erlbaum. Journal of Social Psychology, 45(1), 1–40. https://doi.org/
Peters, K., & Kashima, Y. (2015). A multimodal theory of 10.1348/014466605X48998
affect diffusion. Psychological Bulletin, 141(5), 966–992. Reicher, S. D., & Hopkins, N. (2001). Psychology and the end
https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000020 of history: A critique and a proposal for the psychology of
Pickering, W. S. F. (1984). Durkheim’s sociology of religion: social categorization. Political Psychology, 22(2), 383–407.
Themes and theories. Routledge & Kegan Paul. https://doi.org/10.1111/0162-895X.00246
24 Rimé and Páez

Reicher, S. D., Spears, R., & Postmes, T. (1995). A social iden- Schwarzer, R., & Leppin, A. (1991). Social support and health:
tity model of deindividuation phenomena. In W. Stroebe A theoretical and empirical overview. Journal of Social
& M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychol- and Personal Relationships, 8(1), 99–127. https://doi
ogy (Vol. 6, pp. 161–198). John Wiley & Sons. https://doi .org/10.1177/0265407591081005
.org/10.1080/14792779443000049 Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., & Mossman, A. (2007). The nature
Rennung, M., & Göritz, A. S. (2015). Facing sorrow as a group of awe: Elicitors, appraisals, and effects on self-concept.
unites. Facing sorrow in a group divides. PLOS ONE, 10(9), Cognition & Emotion, 21(5), 944–963. https://doi.org/
Article e0136750. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone 10.1080/02699930600923668
.0136750 Shteynberg, G. (2015). Shared attention. Perspectives on
Rennung, M., & Göritz, A. S. (2016). Prosocial consequences Psychological Science, 10(5), 579–590. https://doi.org/10
of interpersonal synchrony: A meta-analysis. Zeitschrift .1177/1745691615589104
fur Psychologie, 224(3), 168–189. https://doi.org/10.1027/ Shteynberg, G. (2018). A collective perspective: Shared atten-
2151-2604/a000252 tion and the mind. Current Opinion in Psychology, 23,
Riger, S. (1993). What’s wrong with empowerment? American 93–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.12.007
Journal of Community Psychology, 21, 279–292. https:// Shteynberg, G., Hirsh, J. B., Apfelbaum, E. P., Larsen,
doi.org/10.1007/BF00941504 J. T., Galinsky, A. D., & Roese, N. J. (2014). Feeling
Rimé, B. (2009). Emotion elicits the social sharing of emo- more together: Group attention intensifies emotion.
tion: Theory and empirical review. Emotion Review, 1(1), Emotion, 14(6), 1102–1114. https://doi.org/10.1037/a00
60–85. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073908097189 37697
Rimé, B. (2020). Emotions at the service of cultural con- Shteynberg, G., Hirsh, J. B., Bentley, R. A., & Garthoff, J.
struction. Emotion Review, 12(2), 65–78. https://doi (2020). Shared worlds and shared minds: A theory of
.org/10.1177/1754073919876036 collective learning and a psychology of common knowl-
Rimé, B., Kanyangara, P., Yzerbyt, V., & Paez, D. (2011). The edge. Psychological Review, 127(5), 918–931. https://doi
impact of Gacaca tribunals in Rwanda: Psychosocial effects .org/10.1037/rev0000200
of participation in a truth and reconciliation process after Smith, E. R. (1993). Social identity and social emotions:
a genocide. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41(6), Toward new conceptualizations of prejudice. In D. M.
695–706. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.822 Mackie & D. L. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and
Rimé, B., Mesquita, B., Philippot, P., & Boca, S. (1991). Beyond stereotyping: Interactive processes in group perception (pp.
the emotional event: Six studies on the social sharing of 297–315). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-
emotion. Cognition & Emotion, 5(5/6), 435–465. https:// 0-08-088579-7.50017-X
doi.org/10.1080/02699939108411052 Smyth, M. M., & Fuller, R. G. (1972). Effects of group laugh-
Rossignac-Milon, M., Bolger, N., Zee, K. S., Boothby, E. J., & ter on responses to humorous material. Psychological
Higgins, E. T. (2021). Merged minds: Generalized shared Reports, 30(1), 132–134. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1972
reality in dyadic relationships. Journal of Personality and .30.1.132
Social Psychology, 120(4), 882–911. https://doi.org/10 Söderkvist, S., Ohlén, K., & Dimberg, U. (2018). How the
.1037/pspi0000266 experience of emotion is modulated by facial feedback.
Rossignac-Milon, M., & Higgins, E. T. (2018). Epistemic com- Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 42(1), 129–151. https://
panions: Shared reality development in close relation- doi.org/10.1007/s10919-017-0264-1
ships. Current Opinion in Psychology, 23, 66–71. https:// Stellar, J. E., Gordon, A. M., Piff, P. K., Cordaro, D., Anderson,
doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.01.001 C. L., Bai, Y., Maruskin, L., & Keltner, D. (2017). Self-
Rowe, G., Hirsh, J. B., & Anderson, A. K. (2007). Positive affect transcendent emotions and their social functions: Com­
increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings passion, gratitude, and awe bind us to others through
of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 104(1), 383– prosociality. Emotion Review, 9(3), 200–207. https://doi
388. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0605198104 .org/10.1177/1754073916684557
Sagarin, B. J., Cutler, B., Cutler, N., Lawler-Sagarin, K. A., Stott, C., Ball, R., Drury, J., Neville, F., Reicher, S., Boardman, A.,
& Matuszewich, L. (2009). Hormonal changes and cou- & Choudhury, S. (2018). The evolving normative dimen-
ple bonding in consensual sadomasochistic activity. sions of ‘riot’: Towards an elaborated social identity
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38(2), 186–200. https://doi explanation. European Journal of Social Psychology,
.org/10.1007/s10508-008-9374-5 48(6), 834–849. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2376
Salmela, M. (2012). Shared emotions. Philosophical Explora­ Swann, W. B., Jr., Gómez, A., Seyle, D. C., Morales, J., &
tions, 15(1), 33–46. https://doi.org/10.1080/13869795.2 Huici, C. (2009). Identity fusion: The interplay of personal
012.647355 and social identities in extreme group behavior. Journal
Saroglou, V., Buxant, C., & Tilquin, J. (2008). Positive emo- of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 995–1011.
tions as leading to religion and spirituality. The Journal https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013668
of Positive Psychology, 3(3), 165–173. https://doi.org/10 Swann, W. B., Jr., Jetten, J., Gómez, Á., Whitehouse, H.,
.1080/17439760801998737 & Bastian, B. (2012). When group membership gets
Schwarzer, R., & Leppin, A. (1989). Social support and health: personal: A theory of identity fusion. Psychological
A meta-analysis. Psychology and Health, 3(1), 1–15. Review, 119(3), 441–456. https://doi.org/10.1037/a00
https://doi.org/10.1080/08870448908400361 28589
Perspectives on Psychological Science XX(X) 25

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of sociology. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10
inter-group conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), .1093/acprof:oso/9780199659180.001.0001
The social psychology of inter-group relations (pp. 33–47). Wadlinger, H. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2006). Positive mood
Brooks/Cole. broadens visual attention to positive stimuli. Motivation
Tanner, R., Ferraro, R., Chartrand, T. L., Bettman, J., & van and Emotion, 30(1), 87–99. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-
Baaren, R. (2008). Of chameleons and consumption: The 006-9021-1
impact of mimicry on choices and preferences. Journal Walker, C. J. (2010). Experiencing flow: Is doing it together bet-
of Consumer Research, 34(6), 754–766. https://doi.org/10 ter than doing it alone? The Journal of Positive Psychology,
.1086/522322 5(1), 3–11. https://doi.org/10.1080/174397609032
Tarr, B., Slater, M., & Cohen, E. (2018). Synchrony and social 71116
connection in immersive virtual reality. Scientific Reports, Weingarten, E., & Goodman, J. K. (2021). Re-examining the
8(1), Article 3693. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018- experiential advantage in consumption: A meta-analysis
21765-4 and review. Journal of Consumer Research, 47(6), 855–
Thompson, M. P., & Vardaman, P. J. (1997). The role of religion 877. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucaa047
in coping with the loss of a family member to homicide. Whitehouse, H., & Lanman, J. A. (2014). The ties that bind us:
Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 36(1), 44–51. Ritual, fusion, and identification. Current Anthropology,
https://doi.org/10.2307/1387881 55(6), 674–695. https://doi.org/10.1086/678698
Thonhauser, G. (2022). Towards a taxonomy of collective Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and coop-
emotions. Emotion Review, 14(1), 31–42. https://doi eration. Psychological Science, 20(1), 1–5. https://doi
.org/10.1177/17540739211072469 .org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.x
Tönnies, F. (1887). Gemeinschaft und Gesellschaft [Community Woolley, K., & Fishbach, A. (2017). A recipe for friendship:
and society]. Fues’s Verlag. Similar food consumption promotes trust and coop-
Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & eration. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 27(1), 1–10.
Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2016.06.003
self-categorization theory. Blackwell. Woolley, K., & Fishbach, A. (2019). Shared plates, shared minds:
Turner, V. W. (1969). The ritual process: Structure and anti- Consuming from a shared plate promotes cooperation.
structure. Aldine. Psychological Science, 30(4), 541–552. https://doi.org/
Turner, V. W. (1974). Dramas, fields and metaphors. Cornell 10.1177/0956797619830633
University Press. Yaden, D. B., Haidt, J., Hood, R. W., Vago, D. R., & Newberg,
Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2011). Synchrony and the social A. B. (2017). The varieties of self-transcendent experi-
tuning of compassion. Emotion, 11(2), 262–266. https:// ence. Review of General Psychology, 21(2), 143–160.
doi.org/10.1037/a0021302 https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000102
van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Kawakami, K., & van Yudkin, D. A., Prosser, A., Heller, S. M., McRae, K., Chakroff, A.,
Knippenberg, A. (2004). Mimicry and prosocial behav- & Crockett, M. J. (2022). Prosocial correlates of transfor-
ior. Psychological Science, 15(1), 71–74. https://doi.org/ mative experiences at secular multi-day mass gatherings.
10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01501012.x Nature Communications, 13(1), Article 2600. https://doi
van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Steenaert, B., & Van .org/10.1038/s41467-022-29600-1
Knippenberg, A. (2003). Mimicry for money: Behavioral Zeidner, M., & Hammer, A. L. (1992). Coping with missile
consequences of imitation. Journal of Experimental Social attack: Resources, strategies, and outcomes. Journal of
Psychology, 39(4), 393–398. https://doi.org/10.1016/ Personality, 60(4), 709–746. https://doi.org/10.1111/j
S0022-1031(03)00014-3 .1467-6494.1992.tb00271.x
Van Cappellen, P., & Rimé, B. (2014). Positive emotions and Zickfeld, J. H., Schubert, T. W., Seibt, B., Blomster, J. K., Arriaga, P.,
self-transcendence. In V. Saroglou (Ed.), Religion, personal- Basabe, N., Blaut, A., Caballero, A., Carrera, P., Dalgar, I.,
ity, and social behavior (pp. 123–145). Psychology Press. Ding, Y., Dumont, K., Gaulhofer, V., Gračanin, A., Gyenis, R.,
Van Cappellen, P., & Saroglou, V. (2012). Awe activates reli- Hu, C.-P., Kardum, I., Lazarević, L. B., Mathew, L., . . .
gious and spiritual feelings and behavioral intentions. Fiske, A. P. (2019). Kama muta: Conceptualizing and mea-
Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 4(3), 223–236. suring the experience often labelled being moved across
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025986 19 nations and 15 languages. Emotion, 19(3), 402–424.
Vicaria, I. M., & Dickens, L. (2016). Meta-analyses of the https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000450
intra- and interpersonal outcomes of interpersonal coor- Zickfeld, J. H., Schubert, T. W., Seibt, B., & Fiske, A. P. (2019).
dination. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 40(4), 335–361. Moving through the literature: What is the emotion often
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-016-0238-8 denoted being moved? Emotion Review, 11(2), 123–139.
von Scheve, C., & Ismer, S. (2013). Towards a theory of col- https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073918820126
lective emotions. Emotion Review, 5(4), 406–413. https:// Zimbardo, P. G. (1969). The human choice: Individuation, rea-
doi.org/10.1177/1754073913484170 son, and order vs. deindividuation, impulse, and chaos. In
von Scheve, C., & Salmela, M. (Eds.). (2014). Collective W. J. Arnold & D. Levine (Eds.), Nebraska symposium on
emotions: Perspectives from psychology, philosophy and motivation (pp. 237–307). University of Nebraska Press.

View publication stats

You might also like