Alegre, Benson - 2019 - Family Warmth, Self-Perception, Social

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

The Educational and

Developmental Psychologist
Family warmth, self-perception, social
competence, and friendships in emerging
www.cambridge.org/edp adulthood
Albert Alegre1 and Mark J. Benson2
Original Article 1
East Stroudsburg University, Pennsylvania, USA and 2Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University,
Blacksburg, Virginia, USA
Cite this article: Alegre A and Benson MJ.
(2019) Family warmth, self-perception, social
competence, and friendships in emerging Abstract
adulthood. The Educational and Developmental
Psychologist 36: 75–81. doi: 10.1017/
This study examines the factors influencing friendship quality during emergent adulthood.
edp.2019.10 Data were collected on a sample of 393 college students (age range 18–22, M = 20.01). Nine
multi-item measures were used as indicators in this study. Seven scales were drawn from
Received: 25 March 2019 the Battery of Adolescent Measures (Benson & Faas, 2014). The other two were the pragmatic
Revised: 4 September 2019
(social) language and the aloof scales, which provided sensitised assessment of interpersonal
Accepted: 10 September 2019
First published online: 4 November 2019 deficits (Hurley, Losh, Parlier, Reznick, & Piven, 2007). The article hypothesises that family
warmth predicts friendship quality during emergent adulthood. It also hypothesises that this
Keywords: relationship is mediated by emergent adults’ self-perceptions and by their social competence.
friendship quality; family warmth; Structural equation models supported the first hypothesis and also showed that self-perceptions
self-perceptions; social competence;
emerging adulthood and social competence partially mediate the main relationship. We also examined each media-
tor separately for evidence of partial mediation, but the full model remained a significantly
Author for correspondence: better fit to the data compared to both single mediator models.
Albert Alegre, Email: merchalbert@yahoo.es

Family contexts and friendship relations occupy central roles in human development. The
contemporary developmental theory emphasises family interactions as an enduring, central
influence for the developing person (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 1998). During development
from adolescence toward adulthood, choice in friendships carries increasing importance.
Emerging adulthood theory characterises these years after adolescence as the most volitional
stage of development (Arnett, 2000, 2012). The conscious choices of emerging adults to cultivate
positive friendships and build social support during these years have far-reaching effects. Prior
research on social support in the adult years has shown links to a wide range of benefits such as
positive coping (Henly, Danziger, & Offer, 2005), positive parenting (Lee, Anderson, Horowitz,
& August, 2009), less depression (Lee et al., 2009), and less risk of living in poverty (Henly et al.,
2005). The enduring influence of family, combined with the ongoing value of friendships in
emerging adulthood warrants investigating potential linkages across these domains.
Among family features that could link these domains is the dimension of parental warmth,
characterised by acceptance, support and caring (Rohner, 1986). Prior research provides indi-
rect support for the potential association between family warmth and friendship quality. For
example, in adolescence, parental warmth has been linked to adolescent friendship intimacy
(Rodríguez, Perez-Brena, Updegraff, & Umaña-Taylor, 2014). Also, separate effects for mother
or father warmth have been found in relation to peer acceptance (Davidov & Grusec, 2006), peer
likeability (Kam et al., 2011), and friendship behaviours (Eiden, Colder, Edwards, & Leonard,
2009; Padilla-Walker, Nielson, & Day, 2016). This empirical evidence for links between family
and friendship domains throughout childhood and adolescence, together with the salience of
friendship in emerging adulthood, support the first hypothesis of this current study that warmth
in the family of origin will be associated with friendship quality among emerging adults.
If the assumption that family warmth is associated with friendship quality during emergent
adulthood holds, then how does this connection occur? One explanation that connects family
warmth to development derives from interpersonal acceptance-rejection theory (IPARTheory;
Rohner, 2016). The theory proposes that interpersonal warmth serves an evolutionary role for
the human species and provides a psychological foundation for individual adjustment.
IPARTheory has evolved from PARTheory (Rohner, 1986, 2008; Rohner & Khaleque, 2010),
which focused on parent-child relationships. Although expanded to include interpersonal
relationships of all types, Rohner’s theoretical update (2016) clarifies that despite the name
© Australian Psychological Society Ltd 2019. change and broadened applicability, the IPARTheory maintains its valuable, original influence
as a conceptualisation for advancing research and understanding of parent-child relationships.
Within parent-child dyads, the theory suggests that parental warmth affects the overall psycho-
logical adjustment of their children through both internal and behavioural dispositions. The
current study tests the theory and examines the internal disposition of self-perception and a

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Tulane University Libraries, on 14 Jan 2020 at 00:03:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/edp.2019.10
76 A. Alegre and M.J. Benson

behavioural disposition of interpersonal competence as potential interaction patterns that signal attention, imply connection, and
mediators between family warmth and friendship quality. facilitate communication. Successful acquisition of these conven-
Prior theory and empirical evidence both support the relevance tional social skills has been linked to healthy developmental
of testing self-perceptions as a potential mediator between family outcomes in multiple contexts of functioning (Oshri, Topple, &
warmth and friendship quality (Booth-Laforce et al., 2006). In Carlson, 2017). Together, the twin elements of social interest
IPARTheory, warmth is related to multiple dispositions that and social skills are examined in the current study using a measure
overlap with self-perceptions, including self-esteem, self-adequacy designed to capture variation across the normal and atypical spec-
and positive worldview. Elsewhere, theorising about the self- trum developed by Hurley and colleagues (2007). As with self-per-
system has echoed the role of acceptance or rejection in the social ceptions, the measured elements of social competence are expected
environment. As advanced by Leary (2000), for example, feelings to link to positive peer relations.
about oneself operate as a feedback gauge in response to acceptance In sum, the theory and supporting research suggest three
or rejection in the social environment. By extension, repeated principal hypotheses tested in the current research. First, family
experiences of acceptance or rejection in primary relationships warmth will predict friendship quality in early adulthood.
become internalised into patterns of self-perception. Similarly, Second, self-perceptions and interpersonal competence will pre-
prior findings have linked parental warmth to confidence in dict friendship quality. And third, the effect of family warmth
personal choices among emerging adults (Zimmer-Gembeck, on friendship quality will be mediated by both self-perceptions
Madsen, & Hanisch, 2011). and interpersonal competence.
In addition to self-perception, theory and evidence also support
the relevance of testing interpersonal competence as a potential Method
mediator between family warmth and friendship quality.
Participants
IPARTheory hypothesises that parental warmth provides a climate
that fosters youth development of responsiveness, emotional The participants were 393 college students within the age range of
maturity and interaction skills (Rohner & Khaleque, 2010). 18–22 years, M = 20.01, SD = 1.23. The participants represented
Prior research further suggests that family warmth is indirectly both women, 75.1%, and men, 24.9%, across self-identified ethnic-
related to interpersonal competence. For example, significant asso- ities that included Caucasians, 81.7%, Asian-Americans, 10.2%,
ciations have been found between maternal sensitivity and peer and additional ethnicities, 8.2%, (African American, Middle
competence (NICHD, 2001), and between expressive parental Eastern, Hispanic, and multiracial).
warmth and peer empathy (Zhou et al., 2002). Similar links have Each participant provided background information and
been found that connect the early experience of low warmth with responses to 10 indicator measures, which assessed the four
low interpersonal interest in emerging adulthood (Yoo, Park, & primary constructs of the study. All 10 subscales were drawn from
Jun, 2014). To summarise so far, prior theory and research support batteries that had shown prior evidence for reliability, factorial
a hypothesised connection of family warmth with both self- validity, and concurrent/predictive validity. Table 1 presents the
perceptions and interpersonal competence. descriptions of the technical features, reliability, and citation for
In turn, prior findings further suggest that self-perceptions each measure.
and interpersonal competence have implications for friendships. The background showed approximately an equal split across
Associations between self-perceptions and friendships are well grade levels, including freshmen or first-year university students
documented in childhood research (Gifford-Smith & Brownell, (28%), sophomores or second-year students (27%), juniors or
2003). Similar links are beginning to appear in emerging adulthood third-year students (25%), and seniors or final-year students
research. Global self-perceptions, for example, have been linked to (20%). Testing of means across the grade levels showed no
both friendship duration and friendship stability among college differences across study variables, with the exception of a weak
students (Shany, Wiener, & Assido, 2013). age effect noted for the indicators of friendship quality. The age
In addition to self-perceptions, elements of social competence effect for friend bonds (r = −.17, p < .05) and friend interactions
are expected to link to positive peer relations. Related research has (r = −.17, p < .05) reflects a trend, noted in prior research, toward
shown links between social competence and relationship quality fewer and more stable relationships during the college years
(Demir, Jaafar, Bilyk, & Ariff, 2012; Morry & Kito, 2009). (Borarai & Carey, 2006). Institutional Review Board approval
Besides, the current study sought to further unpack social compe- and informed consent were obtained for the study.
tence, drawing from the science of its absence in the study of the
autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Procedure
The ASD field has advanced two key dimensions of social
Participants were recruited through advertisements placed around
competence: (a) social interest and (b) social skills. The dimensions
campus, email listservs sent out to various academic departments,
are reflected in both theory (Chevallier, Kohls, Troiani, Brodkin, &
or through an online recruitment system. After receiving the study
Schultz, 2012) and measurement (Hurley et al., 2007). This
website information and providing informed consent, participants
combination of interest and skills, in both typical and atypical
completed basic demographic questionnaires, a battery of mea-
development, places a developing person in a state of readiness
sures that included the scales used in this study. After completing
for future friendships. Social interest reflects an approach orienta-
the study, participants were granted extra credit toward a psychol-
tion, the anticipation of social rewards, and receptiveness to future
ogy class and a small financial compensation.
social interaction. The importance of social interest is evident in
links to both individual mental health (Fung & Webster, 2018)
Measures
and interpersonal relationship quality (Rennebohm, Seebeck, &
Thoburn, 2017). If social interest inspires future friendships, social As noted in the method and Table 1, the nine multi-item measures
skills facilitate its enactments. Through experiences across devel- that were used as indicators in this study all showed sufficient
opment, youth acquire social skills, which are the conventional reliability for use in research, α > .77.

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Tulane University Libraries, on 14 Jan 2020 at 00:03:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/edp.2019.10
The Educational and Developmental Psychologist 77

Note: All scales have five response choices, except for positive affect which has four. aNumber of items on scale. bSubscales from the Battery of Adolescent Measures (Benson & Faas, 2014). cSubscales (reversed) from Hurley and colleagues (2007). dNot at all
Seven scales were drawn from an assessment of adolescent

.86
.94
.96

.88
.89

.91
.78

.85
.82
α
assets and risks that yielded evidence for reliability and validity
in previous research (Benson & Faas, 2014). These seven previously
validated scales included both intact and modified scales from the
Battery of Adolescent Measures (Benson & Faas, 2014), which was
B&F α

developed and validated using a nationally representative U.S.


.80
.84
.84

.85
.71

.68
.68
NA
NA
sample of adolescents who were 12–18 years of age. Whereas
the reliability of the scale items was established with adolescents
(Benson & Faas, 2014), the technical adequacy of the scale for
use with young adults is evidenced in the current study. Table 1
shows both the original coefficient alphas from Benson and Faas
Possible range

and alphas from the current study. As seen in the table, the internal
3–15d
e

6–30e
e

3–15
6–30
1–6e
e
5–25
5–25

4–20

1–6
consistency estimates for each of the scales in the current study
with young adults is higher than the earlier study based on
responses from adolescents. These seven scales comprised the
indicators for family warmth (3), self-perception (2), and friend-
ship quality (2). Examples of the item content for each scale appear
in Table 1 of the brief report.
Indicators of social competence were drawn from two scales
3
5
5

6
4

3
6
12
12
Na

designed to assess social competence deficits in the ASD field


across a range from normal to atypical (Hurley et al., 2007). For
ease of readability and interpretation, subtest data (aloof and
pragmatic) were reverse coded to yield social interest and social
to very much. eStrongly disagree to strongly agree. fNever to most of the time. gVery rarely to very often. B&F = Benson and Fass (2014). NA = not applicable.
Degree of interaction with friends, including shared time together, the frequency of interaction

skills scores. The social interest scale items assess enjoyment


of social situations and connecting with people. The social
Openness to interaction, enjoyment of communication, and friendliness in interactions

skills items assess attention to conversation, speech fluency, and


Skills in conversational fluency, turn-taking, reading cues, and conversation tracking

interpretation of social cues. The reliability estimates are in the


acceptable range for both social interest (.91) and social skills
Pride in self, liking oneself, feeling loved, and confidence in personal qualities
Family members understood, had fun, and paid attention to the adolescent

(.78). Examples of the item content for each of these two scales
appear in Table 1. In addition, Table 1 shows the Cronbach’s alpha,
Attention from best friends, being understood, and having fun together

item range, response format, and the number of items for all nine
indicator scales.
Warm, loving, satisfied, communicate well, closeness, and caring
Warm, loving, satisfied, communicate well, closeness, and caring

Optimism, hopefulness, happiness, and personal enjoyment


Table 1. Study Measures by Construct, Items, Response Choice, Range, and Internal Consistency

Results
Full model tests (self-perception and interpersonal
competence)
The hypotheses were tested with structural equation modelling
(SEM) procedures (Jöreskog, 1993; Kline, 2005) with MPlus soft-
ware, Version 5.1 (Little & Rubin, 2002; Muthén & Muthén,
2007). We followed the standard procedures, which begin with
examining modification indices for significance and meaning
(Byrne, 2012). The modification indices revealed a significant,
meaningful coefficient for the path between two indicators, social
orientation and interaction frequency. This parameter reflects the
substantive link between interest and behaviour, which has been
Item Content

previously hypothesised and empirically documented (Yoo et al.,


2014). Because of this significant and ‘substantively meaningful’ link
(Byrne, 2012, p. 88), we estimated the parameter in subsequent
models. This subsequent measurement model evidenced a good fit
to the data, χ2(21) = 27.05, root mean square error or approximation
(RMSEA) .03, standardised root mean square residual (SRMR) .03.
The goodness of fit provided indirect support for the shared
Interpersonal competence

Interactions with friendsb

variance among related indicators and discriminative validity


Family understandingb

across unrelated indicators. Examination of the correlation coeffi-


Bonds with friendsb
Friendship quality

cients added support for convergence among related indicators


b

b
Mother support

Self-perception

b
Family warmth

Social interestc
Father support

Positive affect

and divergence across unrelated indicators. The median correla-


Self-esteemb

c
Social skills

tion among related indicators significantly exceeded those across


Subscale

unrelated indicators, r = .54 vs. 32, p < .001. The 36 individual


correlation coefficients among the nine study indicators further
evidenced discriminant validity. After correcting for measurement

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Tulane University Libraries, on 14 Jan 2020 at 00:03:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/edp.2019.10
78 A. Alegre and M.J. Benson

Table 2. Fit Statistics from Structural Equation Models Testing Mediation Hypotheses (N = 393)

Model (Step) Latent Variables χ2 df RMSEA SRMR


1 Step 1. IV→DV Family warmth → Friendship quality 4.20 4 .01 .02
2 Step 2. Family warmth → Self-perception, interpersonal competence 13.45 11 .02 .02
3 Step 3. Self-perception, interpersonal competence → Friendship quality 7.53 5 .04 .02
4 Step 4. Full model 24.29 20 .02 .03
5 Step 5. Mediation model 27.05 21 .03 .03
Note: The mediation model (step 5) shows no decrement in model fit compared to the full model (step 4), Δχ (1) = 2.76, p = ns, providing the basis for inferring mediation.
2

RMSEA = root mean square error or approximation, SRMR = standardised root mean square residual.

S-E PA
. .
.87 .85
FU Self-Perceptions BF
.87
.55 .37
.97 . .63
Family 09 (direct: .50) Friendship
.68 Warmth Quality
MS .15 .58

.54 Interpersonal .40


Competence IF
FS
Fig 1. Full model (Model 4). Indicators are: Family
Understanding (FU), Mother Support (MS), Father Support .65 .86
(FS), Self-Esteem (S-E), Positive Affect (PA), Interaction
Skills (IS), Social Engagement (SE), Bonds with Friends IS SE
(BF), Interactions with Friends (IF).

error, all fell well below the .85 threshold for inferring discriminant As evident in Table 2, all four models showed a good fit to the
validity (Henseler, Ringle, & Sarastedt, 2015; Kline, 2011). Two data, as indicated by the conservative tests of RMSEA and SRMR,
indicators, self-esteem and positive affect, showed substantial over- which were below the .05 threshold (Rigdon, 1996). A good fit was
lap, r = .73, and .91 corrected. Because of the distinctly different item also evidenced by the indices of the confirmatory factor index
content, the disparate origins, and separate purposes for these two (CFI) and Tucker Lewis index (TLI), which exceeded .98.
scales, subsequent models maintained these as separate indicators. Because robust relationships among latent variables have been
shown to inflate CFI and TLI coefficients (Kenny, 2015), we fol-
lowed the recommended conservative procedure of using
Mediation testing RMSEA and SRMR (Rigdon, 1996).
If these first four tests are met, Holmbeck (1997) then recom-
Next, the mediation testing examined associations among predictor, mends testing for fit change between the full model and the
mediator and outcome according to the sequence recommended by hypothesised mediation model, which omits the direct path from
Holmbeck (1997) for testing mediation using SEM. The sequence predictor to outcome. Together with adequate models in the first
follows the classic mediation testing procedure, which involves a four steps, the lack of significant decline in fit between the media-
prescribed series of steps (Baron & Kenny, 1986). In addition to test- tion model relative to the full model provides the final basis for
ing the significance and significant changes in path coefficients, SEM inferring mediation. As shown in Table 2, when compared to
enables testing the model fits at each step in the testing procedure. the full model, the mediation model in step 5 showed no significant
Evidence of significance and good model fit tests the baseline decrement in model fit relative to the full model in step 4,
assumption of a relationship between the predictor and outcome. Δχ2(1) = 2.76, p = ns. This provides the basis for inferring media-
If this structural path is significant, the model fit is good, and the tion. As noted in Figure 1, the structural path between the predictor
researcher proceeds to steps 2 and 3. These steps test the relationship and outcome, family warmth to friendship quality, declined from
between the predictor and mediator, and the mediator and outcome, a robust significant effect in Model 1, β = .50, p < .001, to non-
respectively. Evidence for a significant path and good model fit significance in the full model, Model 4, β = .09, p = ns. In sum-
documents the relevance of the mediator to its predictor and out- mary, self-perceptions and interpersonal competence mediated
come. In step 4, all variables are included. In step 5, the reduction the relationship between family warmth and friendship quality.
in the magnitude of the structural path without compromising over- This finding confirms the second hypothesis of the study and sup-
all fit provides evidence of mediation. ports the underlying theory.
As shown in Table 2, the series tests the relationships between
the predictor and outcome (step 1), the predictor and mediators
Partial model tests
(step 2), and the mediators and outcome (step 3). In step 4, the full
model includes all variables including the path from the predictor In addition to the twin mediators of self-perception and interper-
to the outcome (step 4). sonal competence as advanced by the theory (Rohner, 1986), we

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Tulane University Libraries, on 14 Jan 2020 at 00:03:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/edp.2019.10
The Educational and Developmental Psychologist 79

also examined each mediator separately for evidence of partial (Chung et al., 2014) and that teaching interpersonal skills has
mediation. With self-perception as the sole mediator, the full benefits for subsequent relationships (Licciardello, Harchick, &
model remained a significantly better fit to the data compared Luiselli, 2008). Such changes are consistent with a view of self-
to the single mediator model, Δχ2(1) = 5.83, p < .05. Similarly, perception and interpersonal competence as not merely passive
for interpersonal competence, the full model also proved to be a outcomes, but as active processes undertaken by emerging adults.
better fit than the single mediator model, Δχ2(1) = 12.05, Prior research with emerging adults supports the view that
p < .01. Although neither mediator independently evidenced com- feelings about oneself derive from a dynamic process involving
plete mediation, each remained a partial mediator. Compared to selective perception, active interpretation, and selective memory
the direct effect between family warmth and friendship quality, (Tafarodi, Tam, & Milne, 2001). The active-processes view is also
β = .50, the effect reduced to β = .14, with self-perception as a implied in prior research showing that interpersonal competencies
mediator. Similarly, the direct effect reduced when interpersonal in early adulthood buffered the effects of adverse family conditions
competence was added to the model, β = .21. In summary, both (Brown, Oudekerk, Szwedo, & Allen, 2013).
self-perceptions and friendship quality played partial mediating The contributions and findings from this study are constrained
roles, and jointly, they fully mediated the relationship between by several limitations. First, the current sample is from a primarily
family warmth and friendship quality. American residential student population. Emerging adults in other
living situations or other cultures may vary with respect to family
warmth. Prior research suggests that family is important for both
Discussion
college student and worker samples (Crocetti & Meeus, 2014), but
The findings confirm two hypotheses: (a) warmth in the family of the pattern of relations across constructs cannot be assumed for
origin relates to friendship quality during emerging adulthood, and non-residential or working samples. Second, because the data were
(b) self-perceptions and interpersonal competence together medi- self-reported, the observed effect sizes may overestimate the under-
ate the link between family warmth and friendship quality. Overall, lying relationships. Nevertheless, the higher internal consistency
these study findings add specificity to the tenets of IPARTheory estimates observed in this study relative to earlier measures extend
(Rohner, 2016), extend knowledge about emerging adulthood, the potential of these measures for use with young adults. Last,
and imply strategies for enhancing friendship quality. theoretical tenets and time precedence support the logical sequence
The findings add further evidence to the foundational tenant of of the variables, but the contemporaneous data collection pre-
IPARTheory (Rohner, 2016) and advance key mechanisms that cludes conclusions of causality.
link warmth and psychological adjustment. Regarding the central- Though focused on family and friendship contexts, the current
ity of parental warmth for psychological adjustment, the findings study shows potential for educational and developmental psychol-
associate warmth with three important indicators in the lives of ogists’ future research. Besides family, other contexts, such as edu-
emerging adults: self-perceptions, interpersonal competence, and cation, employment and voluntary organisations, could be
friendship quality. The findings further extend IPARTheory by examined in relation to the mediators and outcomes. Moreover,
implicating two overall dispositions that mediate the effects of the prior evidence linking family of origin and romantic relation-
family warmth in emerging adulthood. The internal disposition ships (Anderson et al., 2014; Seiffge-Krenke, Overbeek, &
of self-perception and the behaviours of interpersonal competence Vermulst, 2010) implies the plausibility of testing these specific
join an expanding list of dispositional and behavioural implica- mediators in the narrower, but salient field of romantic
tions advanced in IPARTheory (Khaleque & Rohner, 2012; relationships.
Rohner & Khaleque, 2010). Overall, the current study findings suggest a process model for
Besides these theoretical contributions, the findings here extend further theory, research, and intervention. Family warmth gener-
knowledge about development during emerging adulthood. ates an internal orientation for favourable self-schema and an out-
Warmth in the family of origin continues to play a key role in ward orientation toward interpersonal competence. Concurrently,
emerging adulthood. As evidenced here, even beyond the adoles- these internal and outward orientations provide a foundation for
cent years, family warmth still shows important associations with developing high-quality friendships. These processes provide clues
self-perceptions, interpersonal competence, and friendship quality. for researchers, professionals, and organisations about ways to
Moreover, these family, personal, and friendship qualities have enhance the lives of emerging adults.
continuing relevance for emerging adults in the areas of marriage
and parenthood. Characteristics such as resolved family relations, Financial support. This research received no specific grant from any funding
positive self-perception and interpersonal competencies are agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
among criteria that most young adults view as necessary for
Conflicts of interest. None.
marriage (Carroll et al., 2009) and also reflect themes that predict
positive parenting in the family of creation (Lee et al., 2009).
Ethical standards. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this
In addition to contributing to theory and knowledge, the work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional
current study findings have implications for educational and devel- committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of
opmental psychologists because they suggest strategies for enhanc- 1975, as revised in 2008.
ing development during emerging adulthood. The mediation by
self-perception and interpersonal competence provides clues for
improving friendships in academic, work and community settings. References
Emerging adults who experienced rejection or low warmth in the Anderson, J.R., Johnson, M.D., Liu, W., Zheng, F., Hardy, N.R., &
family of origin, for example, retain the potential to change Lindstrom, R.A. (2014). Young adult romantic relationships in Mainland
self-perceptions and interpersonal competencies during emerging China: Perceptions of family of origin functioning are directly and indirectly
adulthood. Prior research has documented that change in associated with relationship success. Journal of Social and Personal
self-perceptions does indeed occur during emerging adulthood Relationships, 31, 871–887.

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Tulane University Libraries, on 14 Jan 2020 at 00:03:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/edp.2019.10
80 A. Alegre and M.J. Benson

Arnett, J.J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from late Hurley, R.S. E., Losh, M., Parlier, M., Reznick, J.S., & Piven, J. (2007). The
teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55, 469–480. Broad Autism Phenotype Questionnaire. Journal of Autism and
Arnett, J.J. (2012). New horizons in research on emerging and young adult- Developmental Disorders, 37, 1679–1690.
hood. In A. Booth, S.L. Brown, N.S. Landale, W.D. Manning, & S.M. Jöreskog, K.G. (1993). Testing structural equation models. In K.A. Bollen, & J.S.
McHale (Eds.), Early adulthood in family context (pp. 231–244). New Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 294–316). Thousand
York, NY: Springer. Oaks, CA: Sage.
Baron, R.M., & Kenny, D.A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinc- Kam, C., Greenberg, M.T., Bierman, K.L., Coie, J.D., Dodge, K.A., Foster,
tion in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical M.E., : : : Pinderhughes, E.E. (2011). Maternal depressive symptoms and
considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, child social preference during the early school years: Mediation by maternal
1173–1182. warmth and child emotion regulation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
Benson, M.J., & Faas, C. (2014). Assessing adolescent contexts: Measures of 39, 365–377.
families, peers, schools and neighborhoods. International Journal of Child Kenny, D.A. (2015). Measuring model fit. Structural equation modeling.
and Adolescent Health, 7, 53–69. Retrieved November 15, 2015, from http://davidkenny.net/cm/fit.htm
Booth-Laforce, C., Oh, W., Kim, A.H., Rubin, K.H., Rose-Krasnor, L., & Khaleque, A., & Rohner, R.P. (2012). Pancultural associations between per-
Burgess, K. (2006). Attachment, self-worth, and peer-group functioning ceived parental acceptance and psychological adjustment of children and
in middle childhood. Attachment and Human Development, 8, 309–325. adults: A meta-analytic review of worldwide research. Journal of Cross-
Borarai, B., & Carey, K.B. (2006). How the quality of peer relationships Cultural Psychology, 43, 784–800.
influences college alcohol use. Drug and Alcohol Review, 25, 361–370. Kline, R.B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P.A. (1998). The ecology of developmental ed.). New York, NY: Guilford.
processes. In W. Damon & R.M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology Kline, R.B. (2011). Methodology in the social sciences. Principles and practice of
(vol. 1): Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 993–1023). structural equation modeling (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
New York, NY: Wiley. Leary, M.R. (2000). Sociometer theory and the pursuit of relational value:
Brown, C.L., Oudekerk, B.A., Szwedo, D.E., & Allen, J.P. (2013). Inter-parent Getting to the root of self-esteem. European Review of Social Psychology,
aggression as a precursor to disengagement coping in emerging adulthood: 16, 75–111.
The buffering role of friendship competence. Social Development, 22, Lee, C.S., Anderson, J.R., Horowitz, J.L., & August, G.J. (2009). Family
683–700. income and parenting: the role of parental depression and social support.
Byrne, B.M. (2012). Structural equation modeling with Mplus: Basic concepts, Family Relations, 58, 417–430.
applications, and programming. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Licciardello, C.C., Harchik, A.E., & Luiselli, J.K. (2008). Social skills interven-
Carroll, J.S., Badger, S., Willoughby, B.J., Nelson, L.J., Madsen, S.D., & tion for children with autism during interactive play at a public elementary
Barry, C.M. (2009). Ready or not?: Criteria for marriage readiness among school. Education and Treatment of Children, 31, 27–37.
emerging adults. Journal of Adolescent Research, 24, 349–375. Little, R.J.A., & Rubin, D.B. (2002). Statistical analysis with missing data (2nd
Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E.S., & Schultz, R.T. (2012). ed.) New York, NY: Wiley.
The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16, Morry, M.M., & Kito, M. (2009). Relational-interdependent self-construal as a
231–239. predictor of relationship quality: The mediating roles of one’s own behaviors
Chung, J.M., Robins, R.W., Trzesniewski, K.H., Noftie, E.E., Roberts, B.W., and perceptions of the fulfillment of friendship functions. Journal of Social
& Widaman, K.F. (2014). Continuity and change in self-esteem during Psychology, 149, 205–222.
emerging adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, Muthén, L.K., & Muthén, B.O. (2007). Mplus user’s guide (5th ed.). Los
469–483. Angeles, CA: Author.
Crocetti, E., & Meeus, W. (2014). ‘Family comes first!’ Relationships with fam- NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2001). Child care and children’s
ily and friends in Italian emerging adults. Journal of Adolescence, 37, peer interaction at 24 and 36 months: The NICHD Study of early child care.
1463–1473. Child Development, 72, 1478–1500.
Davidov, M., & Grusec, J. (2006). Untangling the links of parental responsive- Oshri, A., Topple, T.A., & Carlson, M.W. (2017). Positive youth development
ness to distress and warmth to child outcomes. Child Development, 77, and resilience: Growth patterns of social skills among youth investigated for
44–58. maltreatment. Child Development, 88, 1087–1099.
Demir, M., Jaafar, J., Bilyk, N., & Ariff, M.R. M. (2012). Social skills, friend- Padilla-Walker, L.M., Nielson, M.G., & Day, R.D. (2016). The role of parental
ship and happiness: A cross-cultural investigation. Journal of Social warmth and hostility on adolescents’ prosocial behavior toward multiple tar-
Psychology, 152, 379–385. gets. Journal of Family Psychology, 30, 331–340.
Eiden, R.D., Colder, C., Edwards, E.P., & Leonard, K.E. (2009). A longitudinal Rennebohm, S.B., Seebeck, J., & Thoburn, J.W. (2017). Attachment, dyadic
study of social competence among children of alcoholic and nonalcoholic adjustment, and social interest: An indirect effects model. Journal of
parents: Role of parental psychopathology, parental warmth, and self-regu- Individual Psychology, 73, 208–224.
lation. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 23, 36–46. Rigdon, E.E. (1996). CFI versus RMSEA: A comparison of two fit indexes for
Fung, T.Y., & Webster, R. (2018). Perceived social support, social interest, and structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling: A
mental health: Testing a mediator model. Journal of Individual Psychology, Multidisciplinary Journal, 3, 369–379.
74, 157–171. Rodríguez, S.A., Perez-Brena, N.J., Updegraff, K.A., & Umaña-Taylor, A.
Gifford-Smith, M.E., & Brownell, C.A. (2003). Childhood peer relationships: (2014). Emotional closeness in Mexican-origin adolescents’ relationships
Social acceptance, friendships, and peer networks. Journal of School with mothers, fathers, and same-sex friends. Journal of Youth and
Psychology, 41, 235–284. Adolescence, 43, 1953–1968.
Henly, J.R., Danziger, S.K., & Offer, S. (2005). The contribution of social sup- Rohner, R.P. (1986). The warmth dimension: Foundations of parental accep-
port to the material well-being of low-income families. Journal of Marriage tance-rejection theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
and Family, 67, 122–140. Rohner, R.P. (2008). Introduction to parental acceptance-rejection theory
Henseler, J., Ringle, C.M., & Sarstedt, M. (2015). A new criterion for assessing studies of intimate adult relationships. Cross-Cultural Research, 42, 5–12.
discriminant validity in variance-based structural equation modeling. Rohner, R.P. (2016). Introduction to interpersonal acceptance-rejection theory
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 43, 115–135. (IPARTheory) and evidence. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 6,
Holmbeck, G.N. (1997). Toward terminological, conceptual, and statistical Article 4.
clarity in the study of mediators and moderators: Examples from the Rohner, R.P., & Khaleque, A. (2010). Testing central postulates of parental
child-clinical and pediatric psychology literatures. Journal of Consulting acceptance-rejection theory (PARTheory): A meta-analysis of cross-cultural
and Clinical Psychology, 4, 599–610. studies. Journal of Family Theory and Review, 3, 73–87.

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Tulane University Libraries, on 14 Jan 2020 at 00:03:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/edp.2019.10
The Educational and Developmental Psychologist 81

Seiffge-Krenke, I., Overbeek, G., & Vermulst, A. (2010). Parent–child Yoo, G., Park, J.H., & Jun, H.J. (2014). Early maladaptive schemas as predic-
relationship trajectories during adolescence: Longitudinal associations tors of interpersonal orientation and peer connectedness in university
with romantic outcomes in emerging adulthood. Journal of Adolescence, 33, students. Social Behavior and Personality, 42, 1377–1394.
159–171. Zhou, Q., Eisenberg, N., Losoya, S.H., Fabes, R.A., Reiser, M., Guthrie, I.K.,
Shany, M., Wiener, J., & Assido, M. (2013). Friendship predictors of : : : Shepard, S.A. (2002). The relations of parental warmth and positive
global self-worth and domain-specific self-concepts in university students expressiveness to children’s empathy-related responding and social
with and without learning disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46, functioning: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 73, 893–915.
444–452. Zimmer-Gembeck, M.J., Madsen, S.D., & Hanisch, M. (2011). Connecting the
Tafarodi, R.W., Tam, J., & Milne, A.B. (2001). Selective memory and the intrapersonal to the interpersonal: Autonomy, voice, parents, and romantic
persistence of paradoxical self-esteem. Personality and Social Psychology relationships in emerging adulthood. European Journal of Developmental
Bulletin, 27, 1179–1189. Psychology, 8, 509–525.

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. Tulane University Libraries, on 14 Jan 2020 at 00:03:33, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/edp.2019.10

You might also like