Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 4

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/259081888

Phonology: Overview

Chapter · December 2006


DOI: 10.1016/B0-08-044854-2/00035-3

CITATION READS

1 13,662

1 author:

Richard Wiese
Philipps University of Marburg
125 PUBLICATIONS 2,982 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Richard Wiese on 13 April 2023.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


562 Phonology: Overview

Phonology: Overview
R Wiese, Philipps University, Marburg, Germany phoneme, the smallest unit of sound which causes a
ß 2006 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. form to differ in meaning from other forms, can justly
be identified as the origin of phonology. While the
groundwork for phonology was laid in the analysis
Phonology – What Is It About? of many ancient and modern languages, around the
Phonology is that part of language which comprises the beginning of the 20th century the work by Ferdinand
systematic and functional properties of sound in lan- de Saussure (Saussure, 1916), Jan Baudouin de
guage. The term ‘phonology’ is also used, with the Courtenay (Baudouin de Courtenay, 1972), and Franz
ambiguity also found with other terms used for the Boas (Boas, 1889) was crucial for the formulation
description of languages, for the study of those system- of the phonemic analysis in a stricter sense. This was
atic features of sound in language. In this sense, it refers followed in the first half of the 20th century by sever-
to a subdiscipline of linguistics. It was the first such al important formulations of phonological theory,
subdiscipline in which the view of language as an object notably by Edward Sapir (Sapir, 1921), Leonard
with particular structural properties was developed Bloomfield (Bloomfield, 1933), Roman Jakobson
successfully. Phonology seeks to discover those system- (Jakobson, 1939) and Nikolay Trubetzkoy (Trubetzkoy,
atic properties in the domain of sound structure, and 1967). A unit of sound is a phoneme if it func-
find the regularities and principles behind it both tions to distinguish lexical items from each other
for individual languages and for language in general. in terms of meaning, and if it cannot be broken up
More recently, phonology has become considerably any further in a way that other lexical units emerge.
diversified and has found a number of applications. The German (Standard German) word mein ([maI:n]
The emphasis on systematicity in the definition ‘my’), for example, clearly is a different word from
above derives from the observation that behind the nein ([naI:n] ‘no’). At the same time, neither [m] nor
infinitely varying properties of each token of speech [n] can be split up in such a way that other words of
there is an identifiable set of invariant, recurring, German appear. Together with similar comparisons
more abstract properties. The hypothesis that such a relating [m] and [n] to other segments of German,
phonological system exists is largely due to Saussure the so-called minimal pair just presented constitutes
(see Saussure, 1916) and to the phonologists of the evidence that /m/ and /n/ are phonemes of this
early structuralist school, both in Europe (the Prague language.
school and the British school) and in the United States A phoneme therefore is a contrastive structural unit
(American structuralism); see the survey by Anderson of language, and is related to, but not identical with, a
(1985). concrete sound. Phonemes within a language form
Phonology, from its beginnings, has stood in a close, phonemic systems, by means of the contrasts between
but sometimes strained, relation to the other science of phonemes. The nasals /m, n, N/, for example, form
linguistic sounds, phonetics. Phonetics studies the con- part of the phoneme system of German, and contrast
crete, physical features of sound in language, often systematically with an otherwise similar subsystem,
called speech. As the function of phonology is to that of the voiceless consonants /p, t, k/. Seen ab-
make linguistic items, which are represented by rather stractly, a phoneme is nothing but the set of such
abstract symbols, pronounceable and understandable, contrasts; seen more concretely, a phoneme is a class
it is intimately related to phonetics. But while phonet- of related sounds. Phonology also aims at the study
ics is interested in the concrete, continuously varying of the properties of phonemic systems, and has estab-
features of articulation, sound transmission (acous- lished many patterns and principles which hold for
tics), and auditory perception, the subject of phonolo- such systems; see, in particular, Maddieson (1984).
gy is thought to be a set of discrete, symbolic categories The conception of phonemes as objects in human cog-
which belong to the cognitive, and not the physical, nition is largely due to Sapir (1933), who argued that
domain. This distinction can be interpreted either as a speakers must have mental representations of sounds,
rather strict and principled one, or as one which is and that these mental objects cannot be identical to the
gradual and of less importance. concrete realizations of such phonemes.

The Phonological Features


The Categories of Phonology
The phoneme turns out to be an important, but not
The Phoneme
the only, category needed for the phonological de-
The first invariant of the categories identified in scription of languages. Most importantly, phonemes
phonology was the phoneme. The discovery of the can be shown to function in groups which share
Phonology: Overview 563

identifiable properties. This observation, largely due items can be described as the result of a set of phono-
to Trubetzkoy and Jakobson, led to the formulation logical processes, in this case the so-called Great
of a theory of distinctive features. Such features de- Vowel Shift.
scribe the classes of phonemes by assigning the same Generative phonology in the version proposed by
feature to all members of a class. At the same time, Chomsky and Halle (1968) and in other works was
features define sounds (phonemes or not) and express strongly process oriented in giving rules a central
their composite nature. Thus, a sound [p] may be place in the model. Phonological rules are based on
assigned the feature [labial], on the grounds that clo- features in the sense introduced above and apply
sure of the lips is a crucial ingredient of this sound. to underlying forms (abstract phonemes) to derive
The description of this sound then includes the fea- surface forms. More recent theories, in particular
ture [labial], and the class of labial consonants (and declarative phonology and optimality theory, diverge
perhaps vowels such as [u]) is defined as the set of from this view. Processes are now modeled not by
sounds bearing this feature. means of feature-changing rules, but as the surface
Finally, the features can also be seen as the set of result of static constraints which put different
atomic units from which all larger units of sound are demands on sounds under different conditions.
constructed, in all languages. In this function, the set
Prosody and Its Categories
of phonological features is part of the cognitive equip-
ment which allows human beings to use language. In Phonology in many recent conceptions also is a theo-
phonological theory, since the work by Jakobson et al. ry of phonological grouping, that is, of units of a size
(1952), the hypothesis has been proposed that there is larger than the phoneme. The size of these groupings
indeed a small set of features from which all sound ranges from the small subphonemic unit (such as
segments can be built up. This feature set thus con- the atomic features) to the most comprehensive one,
stitutes something like the set of atoms from which often called the utterance. These units are usually
all larger units derive. Several sets of features have assumed to be hierarchically related to each other.
been proposed since then, most influentially that in The most widely used of the prosodic units is the
Chomsky and Halle (1968). syllable. It usually consists of a vowel and some flank-
ing consonants, which may or may not be present.
Phonological Processes
The principles of syllable structure largely determine
Sounds, whether phonemes or not, often relate what sequences of sounds are permissible in a lan-
to each other in systematic ways. A first type of guage. Furthermore, the syllable is an important
relation is that between a phoneme and its (possibly domain for phonological processes. Syllables often
several) realizations. This relation can be described come in two types, stressed and unstressed, as in the
by means of rules which specify precisely in which English word Piccadilly [%pIk@"dIlI], which consists of
way a phoneme is modified under specific circum- two stressed and two unstressed syllables, resulting in
stances, such as influence from its right-hand or left- an alternating sequence of stressed and unstressed
hand neighbors. For example, a vowel will often syllables. Groupings consisting of a stressed syllable
be nasalized if standing before a nasal consonant. plus any unstressed syllables are identified as
This approach was most thoroughly proposed by members of a further prosodic category, called the
Chomsky and Halle (1968) in a rule-based phonolog- foot. (Other languages display other types of feet;
ical theory. In this theory, processes are not only those see Hayes, 1995 for a typology of feet.)
which describe the relation between a phoneme The example just given also illustrates that stress
and its realizations, but also those which relate pho- (also called accent) is a phonological phenomenon. It
nemes of several connected word forms. In this view, can even be phonemic in the sense of being contras-
a rule is also called upon to describe the relation tive, as in English ı́mport (n.) with initial stress versus
between the sounds in, for example, relate with final impórt (v.) with final stress. Such phenomena of stress
/t/ and relation with final /s/ in the same morpheme, can be observed in units of several sizes. The domain
but before the suffix -ion. In other theories, processes of word stress is the unit called the prosodic word
of this type (often riddled by exceptions in one direc- or phonological word, and the domain of stress in
tion or other) are placed in a separate component of larger units is the phonological or prosodic phrase.
grammar called morphophonology. Thirdly, phono- Thus, prosodic words and prosodic phrases are
logical processes can also be observed if language is other units of phonology. This hierarchy extends up-
looked at diachronically. Thus, the changes leading wards to the intonational phrase, the domain of into-
from the Middle English vowel system including /i:/, national patterns. These units also serve as the
as in wife or mice, to the Modern English vowel domains of phonological restrictions or processes.
system with the diphthong /VI/ in the corresponding The phonological word, for example, is the
564 Phonology: Overview

domain for vowel harmony in many languages, as in prosodic units are formed, syntax indirectly influ-
Hungarian or Turkish. ences the placement of phrasal stress and intonation
A further prosodic phenomenon is that of tone. contours. As for semantics, the main connection to
Tone is the lexically contrastive use of pitch move- phonology is again through intonation. Intonation is
ment, as in Standard Chinese (Mandarin Chinese), constrained by phonology (through the principles
where one and the same syllable can be combined governing the assignment of tonal features within
with several (four) such pitch levels and movements, intonational phrases), syntax, and semantics. The
with the result that most of the combinations between result of such constraints deriving from syntax, se-
segmental syllables and tones are different lexical mantics, and phonology is often called information
items. The phenomena of stress and tone and their structure, a particular packaging and ordering of
analysis in phonology were to a large extent respon- information within a sentence.
sible for developments in phonological theory which
led beyond the view that a phonological representa- See also: Autosegmental Phonology; Chinese (Mandarin):
tion is nothing but a string of segments (with segments Phonology; Declarative Approaches to Phonology; Foot;
built up from elementary features). Tone and stress Hungarian: Phonology; Intonation; Kimatuumbi: Phonolo-
turn out to be inherently nonsegmental in the sense gy; Phonemic Analysis; Phonological Phrase; Phonologi-
that they relate to units both larger and smaller than cal Words; Phonology: Optimality Theory; Phonology–
an individual sound segment. There may be more Phonetics Interface; Prosodic Morphology; Structuralist
than one tone per segment, and there may be one Phonology: Prague School; Syllabic Constituents; Sylla-
tone relating to more than one segment, the tone ble: Phonology; Tone: Phonology.
sometimes spanning a domain as large as a whole
word. These observations led to a new theory of
representations in phonology, called autosegmental Bibliography
phonology.
Anderson S R (1985). Phonology in the twentieth century:
theories of rules and theories of representations. Chicago/
Phonology in Its Relation to Other Parts of London: University of Chicago Press.
Language Baudouin de Courtenay J ([1895] 1972). ‘An attempt at a
theory of phonetic alternations.’ In Stankiewicz E (ed.)
The relation of phonology to phonetics has been dis- Selected writings of Baudouin de Courtenay. Bloomington,
cussed in the section ‘Phonology – What Is It About?’ Indiana: Indiana University Press. 144–212.
above. As the phonology of a language is the system- Bloomfield L (1933). Language. New York: H. Holt.
atic use of phonetically given material, the interface Boas F (1889). ‘On alternating sounds.’ American Anthro-
between phonology and phonetics is crucial in any pologist 2, 47–53.
Chomsky N A & Halle M (1968). The sound pattern of
phonological theory. A phonological system being
English. New York: Harper and Row.
part of the structural system of language, which
Hayes B P (1995). Metrical stress theory: principles and
includes at least morphology, syntax, and semantics, case studies. Chicago/London: University of Chicago
its relation to these components of language needs to Press.
be discussed as well. In most structuralist conceptions Jakobson R (1939). ‘Observations sur le classement pho-
of language and grammar, several levels of linguistic nologique des consonnes.’ In Proceedings of the 3rd
analysis must be distinguished. While the phonologi- International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. 34–41.
cal level of analysis contains at least a phonemic and Jakobson R, Fant G & Halle M (1952). Preliminaries to
an allophonic level, phonology is also closely related speech analysis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
to morphology, simply because morphemes are signs Maddieson I (1984). Patterns of sound. Cambridge:
relating a meaning with a phonological form. Further- Cambridge University Press.
Sapir E (1921). Language. New York: Harcourt, Brace &
more, the concatenation of morphemes often causes
World.
the changes identified as morphophonological above.
Sapir E (1933). ‘La réalité psychologique du phonème.’
Finally, there are often prosodic requirements on both Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique 30,
simplex and complex words, requiring a further close 247–265.
interaction between phonology and morphology. Saussure F de (1916). Cours de linguistique generale. Paris:
Phonology relates to syntax mostly through the for- Payot.
mation of phonological phrases and intonational Trubetzkoy N S ([1939] 1967). Grundzüge der Phonologie.
phrases. By determining (at least in part) how such Göttingen: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht.

View publication stats

You might also like