A Comparison of Attitudes and Emotions As Predicto

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/24098714

A Comparison of Attitudes and Emotions as Predictors of Behavior at Diverse


Levels of Behavioral Experience

Article in Journal of Consumer Research · March 1992


DOI: 10.1086/209276 · Source: RePEc

CITATIONS READS

351 4,268

3 authors:

Chris Allen Karen A. Machleit


University of Cincinnati University of Cincinnati
46 PUBLICATIONS 4,427 CITATIONS 43 PUBLICATIONS 6,948 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Susan Schultz Kleine


Bowling Green State University
20 PUBLICATIONS 2,753 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Food studies View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Karen A. Machleit on 29 September 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


A Comparison of Attitudes and Emotions
as Predictors of Behavior at Diverse Levels
of Behavioral Experience
CHRIS T. ALLEN
KAREN A. MACHLEIT
SUSAN SCHULTZ KLEINE*

This study examines individuals' reports about emotive experience vis-a-vis their
attitudinal judgments as predictors of subsequent behavior. Hypotheses are de-
veloped around the general premise that emotional reports tap information with
motivational implications that need not be integrated into attitude judgments and
thus should supplement attitude in prediction. The hypotheses are tested with a
unique data base that allows categorization of subjects on the basis of diverse
levels of prior experience and features naturally occurring behavior over a 12-month
period as the criterion variable. The data support the premise that emotional variables
can serve as incremental predictors in instances in which situational pressures may
inhibit formation of meaningful attitudes. Also, the data suggest that emotional reports
may furnish some unique information about what perpetuates a behavior. Implications
for further integration of emotional experience into consumer research are discussed.

I n two widely cited articles, Hirschman and Holbrook


(1982) and Holbrook and Hirschman (1982) chal-
situationally induced (usually by advertising) moods
rather than more enduring motivational factors and
lenged consumer researchers to break with convention stronger emotional states." It is interesting that the
in search of a richer understanding of the experiential business press is filled with examples (see, e.g., Blu-
aspects of consumption. Central to this challenge was menthal1989; Miller 1990; Woodruffet al. 1990) that
an explicit appeal to incorporate more emotional con- suggest that it is marketing practitioners who have be-
siderations into research. As Holbrook (1986, p. 17) come more intrigued with and informed about complex,
observes, "We all recognize emotional phenomena as emotional motives for consumption.
pervasive components of human behavior in general Although Hirschman and Holbrook have offered
and consumer behavior in particular. Yet, like the way many suggestions for enriching the field, our research
in which weather reporters treat problematic news about was motivated by their specific concern about how af-
hurricanes and tornadoes, we dutifully note the key role fective responses have been incorporated into consumer
played by emotion in consumers' lives without doing research. As they note, to find affect represented in con-
very much about it." sumer research, one can look to the substantial body
To date, consumer research explicitly featuring emo- of work involving the attitude construct. Attitudes may
tion has maintained a narrow focus. As Cohen (1990, be shaped by hedonic responses involving simple pos-
p. 163) notes, "Most of the attention . . . has been to itive or negative feeling states; however, by Holbrook
and Hirschman's estimate (1982, p. 136), this affect
"represents only a tiny subset of the emotions and feel-
'Chris T. Allen is professor of marketing and Karen A. Machleit
is assistant professor of marketing in the College of Business Admin-
ings of interest to the experiential view."
istration, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH 45221. Susan Given the common belief that emotions serve as pri-
Schultz Kleine is assistant professor of marketing, Department of mary motivators of behavior (see, e.g., Abelson et al.
Marketing, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287. Data col- 1982; Ahtola 1985; Izard 1977; Tomkins 1970), Hol-
lection in this project was funded by the Direct Marketing Policy
Center at the University of Cincinnati. The authors wish to express
brook and Hirschman's (1982) concern over the absence
their gratitude to the staff of the Hoxworth Blood Center at the Uni- of emotion in attitude research raises a major question
versity of Cincinnati for their cooperation and support in this project about behavioral explanation and prediction. If the at-
and would like to thank Peter Dickson, Jim Ginter, Frank Kardes, titude construct reflects only a tiny subset of emotive
Paul Miniard, and Esther Thorson for their helpful comments on experience, then behavioral prediction should suffer.
prior drafts of the manuscript.
Any construct or research stream that incompletely re-
493
© 1992 by JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH, Inc.• Vol. 18. March 1992
All rights reserved. 0093-5301/92/1804-0007$2.00
494 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

flects emotions may be overlooking a major component Compared with the unidimensionalist's conception
of explainable variance. of attitude, emotion represents a richer and more di-
Of course, for consumer researchers, attitude is not verse domain of phenomenological experience (Hol-
just any construct: in terms of attracting and sustaining brook 1986). Several typologies (e.g., Izard 1977; Plut-
research attention, attitude has had no rival. Cohen chik 1980) have been proposed to categorize this
(1990, p. 153, emphasis added) represents the motive diversity in subjective experience into exhaustive sets
underlying this ongoing interest by noting that "the of primary emotions. Izard's (1977) differential-emo-
major purpose in most of this activity was to locate an tions theory postulates 10 primary emotions: interest,
intervening variable that would convincingly 'sum up' joy, surprise, sadness, anger, disgust, contempt, fear,
all the personal and marketplace influences at work and shame, and guilt. His theorizing has been used as a
that would stand in a direct line causally to behavior." starting point for considering emotive experience in di-
But does the attitude construct-as it is conventionally verse contexts (e.g., Allen et al. 1988; Holbrook 1986;
conceptualized and measured-capture or sum up the Westbrook 1987). The demonstrated flexibility and
influence of all complex emotional experience? More comprehensiveness ofIzard's framework made it a log-
than two decades of multiattribute modeling has estab- ical choice for our research.
lished that attitude does a good job of mediating the
influence of product-feature knowledge (Engel, Black- Should Attitudinal Judgments Always
well, and Miniard 1990) or so-called utilitarian beliefs Represent Emotional Experience?
(Zanna and Rempel 1988) in the causal sequence to
behavior. Whether the same can be said for emotional Theorizing that attempts to integrate the disparate
experience is simply not known (cf. Ahtola 1985; Hol- literatures that deal with attitudes and emotions has
brook and Hirschman 1982; Zanna and Rempel 1988); recently begun to appear (Ahtola 1985; Breckler and
however, current thinking in the practitioner literature Wiggins 1989; Cohen 1990; Zanna and Rempel 1988).
suggests that overlooking emotional response in favor One approach has been to treat emotional experience
of cognitive beliefs and evaluations can impede man- as simply another antecedent (along with utilitarian be-
agers' insights about consumers' preferences (e.g., liefs) of one's evaluation of the attitude object (Zanna
Mowen 1988; Woodruff et al. 1990). and Rempel 1988). Simple pleasant (or unpleasant)
In this article we investigate whether emotion's in- feelings experienced through product use could yield a
fluence on behavior is accounted for by attitude or is more (or less) favorable attitude via instrumental learn-
better represented as unique and unmediated. Specifi- ing. However, a central theme of the Holbrook and
cally, we furnish data regarding three questions. First, Hirschman (1982) argument was their appeal to begin
when individuals report emotions experienced in a par- considering behavioral domains typified by richer emo-
ticular domain, will these retrospections predict future tive experience than pleasant or unpleasant affect. Al-
behavior? Second, will these retrospections add sub- though Holbrook and Hirschman stopped short of ac-
stantive explained variance to that which is accounted tually saying it, it follows from their reasoning that, for
for by conventional attitude variables? Finally, does at- behaviors accompanied by complex and conflicting
titude serve to mediate the effect of emotion, or is the emotions, it may be difficult for an individual to inte-
link between emotion and behavior direct? Before pre- grate diverse feelings into a meaningful, global evalu-
senting specific hypotheses, a general case will be made ation of the behavior. In such instances, individuals'
for why and when emotions might be expected to sup- retrospections about specific emotional episodes should
plement attitudes as predictors. improve prediction.
The case against relying on attitude to sum up emo-
WHY AND WHEN SHOULD tion's influence is strengthened if one accepts Tulving's
EMOTIONS ENHANCE (1983) distinction between semantic and episodic
memory. Episodic memory stores information in the
PREDICTION? form of specific events as they were experienced. Se-
To avoid the potential confusion that has plagued mantic memory stores knowledge and abstractions de-
research on attitude and affect (cf. Cohen 1990; Mad- rived from experience. Abstractions like attitudinal
den, Allen, and Twible 1988; Zanna and Rempel 1988), judgments should thus be found in semantic memory,
explication of attitude and emotion terminology is re- whereas emotional experience would be stored initially
quired. We employ the traditional unidimensional view as part of autobiographical accounts in episodic mem-
of an attitude as the categorization of an object on an ory (Friestad and Thorson 1986; Tulving 1983). Al-
evaluative continuum (Cohen 1990; Engel et al. 1990; though there is no contention that semantic and epi-
Fazio 1986; Zanna and Rempel 1988). This conception sodic memory exist in total isolation from one another,
of attitude as an enduring evaluation has dominated there is also no requirement that episodic memory
work with the construct in psychological and consumer traces containing information about emotion must be
research (cf. Batra 1986; Breckler and Wiggins 1989; integrated with the abstractions in semantic memory
Fazio 1986). (Breckler and Wiggins 1989; Cohen 1990; Friestad and
ATTITUDES AND EMOTIONS AS PREDICTORS 495

Thorson 1986). Lacking complete integration, individ- flicting emotional states may be difficult to integrate
uals' retrospections about emotions experienced during into overall evaluations of a behavior. Integration is
a particular event should furnish unique information complicated further by the possibility that emotional
vis-a-vis attitudes for predictive purposes (Abelson et experience may be stored initially in episodic memory
al. 1982; Breckler and Wiggins 1989). and need not be transferred to abstractions stored in
semantic memory. The "when" question revolves
The Moderating Role of Prior Experience around the nature of one's prior experience. When past
behavior is manded, emotion's unique influence on fu-
It would be overly simplistic to propose that emo- ture behavior should be more pronounced. Also, when
tional reports will always supplement attitudinal judg- prior experience has been extensive, emotional factors
ments. Obviously, no incremental prediction would be may again emerge as a dominant influence on behavior.
expected for domains that are devoid of emotion. These arguments are formalized as research hypotheses
Moreover, traditional moderators of the attitude-be- below.
havior relationship, such as direct experience, should
affect relative predictiveness. The generalization that
attitudes serve as more robust predictors when they are
RESEARCH CONTEXT AND
formed via direct experience is widely accepted (e.g., HYPOTHESES
Engel et al. 1990; Zanna and Rempel 1988). Two ex- Our research context involves predicting voluntary
ceptions to this generalization motivate our reasoning blood donation. This context has been used frequently
about when emotional reports might be expected to to study attitude-behavior consistency (e.g., Bagozzi
supplement attitudes. 1981; Charng et al. 1988) and has several desirable fea-
The first exception derives from Bern's (1972) self- tures. One reason for its popularity is that it can provide
perception theory. As Fazio (1986, p. 221) notes, "Only an unobtrusive measure of actual behavior. Addition-
freely chosen, unmanded behavior can serve this pur- ally, although attitudes predict donation, they are weak
pose. Behavior that is manded (i.e., obviously attrib- predictors: Bagozzi (1981, p. 625) concluded, "Attitude
utable to a situational cause) is not considered to reflect does appear to influence (donation) behavior, but its
internal dispositions." However, there is no reason to impact is a relatively small one." Finally, complex and
contend that manded behaviors would be devoid of conflicting emotions (e.g., joy vs. fear vs. shame) often
emotion; furthermore, the nature of the emotions ac- accompany donation.
companying manded behavior may be an important
determinant offuture performance. Hence, in contexts
in which situational factors inhibit the development of Manded Behavior and the Predictiveness
causal attitudes, recollection of emotion should be the of Attitude versus Emotion
superior predictor.
A second exception to the generalization that atti- Two hypotheses dealing with experience as a mod-
tudes predict better when formed through experience erator of predictiveness are tested. The first is based on
comes from recent theorizing about habits (Charng, Pi- the argument that attitudes will be useful predictors
liavin, and Callero 1988; Ronis, Yates, and Kirscht when they are formed through direct experience but
1989). These authors draw a distinction between be- only when behavior is unmanded. When prior behavior
haviors that result from conscious decisions and be- is manded, it would be unlikely that diverse and con-
haviors that have become habits. They argue that once flicting emotive experiences would be integrated into a
behavior is habituated, it is no longer under volitional coherent attitude. Conversely, the spontaneity and pre-
control and may become relatively autonomous of at- dictiveness of emotions should not be suppressed by a
titudes. The question then becomes, Are recurrent be- behavior's manded status. Thus we posit:
haviors also likely to be devoid of emotion? Although H1: Where prior behavioral experience is heavily
the answer would certainly vary with the context, manded, emotion will provide a substantive
there are domains in which emotional experiences are contribution to attitude in predicting behav-
important in perpetuating behavior (Cohen 1990; ior. As behavioral experience becomes less
O'Guinn and Faber 1989; Piliavin et al. 1982). In such manded, attitude will improve as a predictor.
instances, emotional reports may again improve pre-
diction. It is common to find that an individual's first few
experiences as a donor are compelled by situational
Summary factors (i.e., are manded). New donors are recruited ag-
gressively through systematic campaigns in which mo-
Arguments are presented above for why and when bile units are sent to cooperating organizations, and
attitude may not be expected to capture and mediate donors are solicited by members of those organizations
emotion's influence on behavior. The "why" question (Condie, Warner, and Gillman 1976; Piliavin et al.
revolves around an integration issue. Complex or con- 1984). Piliavin et al. (1984) conclude that at least three
496 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

donations are required before the behavior begins to of affect that has dominated consumer research; they,
lose its manded status. too, seek to incorporate a full gamut of emotional re-
sponses into their work. However, the studies diverge
Habituated Behavior and the Predictive ness in terms of focal mediators and dependent measures.
Holbrook and Batra assess emotion as a mediator of
of Attitude versus Emotion advertising's influence on brand and ad evaluations. In
Recent theorizing (Ronis et al. 1989) about habits our study, attitude is examined as a potential mediator
also presents an exception to the generalization that of emotion's influence on naturally occurring behavior.
direct experience always produces more predictive at- Holbrook and Batra (1987) furnish fundamental sup-
titudes. To extend Ronis et al.'s position, we have sug- port for Zanna and Rempel's (1988) position that emo-
gested that emotional influences may serve to perpet- tion serves as a unique antecedent of attitude. Our study
uat: a habit after attitude loses its effect. However, investigates the possibility that emotive experience is
testmg such a proposition is problematic from the not mediated by attitude and thus influences behavior
standpoint that Ronis et al. (1989) supply no criteria directly.
for determining when a behavior has actually become
a habit. Thus, this second hypothesis should be viewed METHOD
as exploratory and is stated in the null form.
Sample
H2: Increasingly high levels of experience with a
behavior will have no effect on the relative Data in this project consist of mailed questionnaire
contribution of attitude and emotion as pre- responses and donation records of 361 donors from the
dictors of future behavior. Hoxworth Blood Center. At the time of the study, Hox-
worth had nearly 120,000 names on file; however, a
majority of these were inexperienced donors who had
Does Attitude Mediate the Influence of only donated at mobile units as part of institutional
Complex Emotions on Behavior? blood drives. A stratified random sample was drawn to
The above hypotheses assert that emotional variables yield subjects with diverse experience: 200 names were
will provide incremental prediction in certain situa- drawn from the one-time donors, 500 names were
tions. Our third hypothesis emphasizes the issue of me- drawn from those who had accumulated between 2 and
diation and is motivated by the theorizing of Zanna 10 donations, and 200 names were drawn from II-plus
and Rempel (1988). Zanna and Rempel (1988, p. 319) donors. This sampling plan also assured adequate rep-
define attitude "as the categorization of a stimulus ob- resentation of donors above and below the gallon-
ject along an evaluative dimension." Their novel thesis donation level. The importance of the gallon demar-
is the proposition that these evaluations can have three cation is explained below.
unique antecedents: cognitions or beliefs, past behavior, A cover letter, questionnaire, and return envelope
and emotional experience. Hence, they posit that emo- were mailed to these 900 donors. Excluding 43 cases
tion's influence on behavior will not be direct but in- that were not delivered because of bad addresses, the
stead will be mediated by an overall evaluation or at- response rate for this single mailing was 42.1 percent. 1
titude. Prior information concerning the donors' gender, age,
Zanna and Rempel (1988) provide an excellent start- and donation experience provides for a limited com-
ing point for research featuring both attitudinal and parison of respondents and nonrespondents. The av-
emotional variables. However, their thesis is contra- erage age of respondents was 40.33 years versus 35.85
dicted by Ahtola (1985), who proposes that the influence years for nonrespondents; 60.4 percent of respondents
of hedonic experience on behavior will not be totally versus 64.8 percent ofnonrespondents were males. The
mediated by an overall evaluation. Like Holbrook and age difference is related to the fact that more experienced
Hirschman (1982), we expect attitudinal judgments to donors were more likely to respond. Mean prior do-
undervalue or misrepresent complex emotional expe- nations for respondents was 9.14 versus 4.66 for non-
rience: respondents.

H3: Retrospections about emotive experience will Measures


serve as direct predictors of behavior; that is,
their effect will not be mediated by overall Attitude toward the Act. Subjects reported their
evaluations or attitudes. evaluations of the act of blood donation at the beginning

A brief comparison of this hypothesis with the agenda


in Holbrook and Batra's (1987) research on emotional IOfthe 361 persons who responded, 58 were one-time donors, 173
had accumulated between 2 and 10 donations, and 130 had accu-
mediators highlights the uniqueness of our study. As mulated more than 10 donations at the time of the survey. Response
with our own, Holbrook and Batra's (1987) work was rates by sample strata were thus 29 percent, 34.6 percent, and 65
motivated by concern over the restrictive conception percent, respectively.
ATTITUDES AND EMOTIONS AS PREDICTORS 497

and end of the questionnaire. Initially, they responded (1977) recommends, subjects respond to each of the 30
to an II-item semantic differential scale built around phrases on five-point scales anchored by "never" and
five items taken from Bagozzi (1981). Confirmatory "very often." Coefficient alphas for 9 of the 10 subscales
factor analysis revealed that this was not a unidimen- are in the .80-.90 range for our sample. For the con-
sional scale; hence, conventional procedures were em- tempt subscale, alpha is .56.
ployed (cf. Madden et al. 1988) in identifying the prin-
Perceived Self-Motivation. Although donation often
cipal evaluative factor. This procedure led back to
Bagozzi's five items as the best unidimensional repre- begins as a manded behavior, intrinsic motives may
evolve. A measure of subjects' perceptions of their mo-
sentation of the evaluative factor.2 Attitude toward the
tives was taken to check whether those with more ex-
act (A act) is the sum of these five items. Following the
perience did perceive their behavior as more self-mo-
semantic differential, subjects reported confidence in
tivated. Perceived self-motivation was measured on a
their evaluations on a single seven-point scale anchored
four-item scale: subjects responded to such statements
by "very confident" and "not confident at all."
as "The last time I gave blood, someone had pressured
The second attitude measure came at the end of the
me into it," on a seven-point scale anchored by
questionnaire at which point subjects evaluated the act
"strongly agree" and "strongly disagree.,,3
of donation on a seven-point scale anchored by "very
favorable" and "very unfavorable." The label Aact2 refers Donation Behavior. Respondents' donation histo-
to this single-item measure. ries, including whether their last donation came on a
Emotions. Blood donation is a domain in which mobile unit or at a fixed-site location, and their dona-
diverse and conflicting emotions are common. While a tion activity for 12 months after the survey were drawn
variety of emotions can affect helping (see, e.g., Rosen- from the computerized records of the collection center.
han et al. 1981), it was not our goal to furnish a detailed In the analyses that follow, 24 subjects were excluded
analysis of emotional motivators of donation. Our em- because of their responses to the final question in the
phasis involves the relative predictive ness of attitude survey: it asked, "What would make you stop donating
versus emotion. blood?" Those excluded volunteered that they could
In operationalizing emotion it was deemed important no longer donate because of medical reasons or because
to draw on a conventional taxonomy that could accom- they had left the area.
modate the full gamut of feelings associated with do-
nation. Izard's (1977) differential-emotions theory fur- RESULTS
nishes a comprehensive taxonomy that has influenced
research in diverse contexts (e.g., Kotsch, Gerbing, and Does Experience Alter the Manded Status
Schwartz 1982; Westbrook and Oliver 1991). Having of Donation?
adopted this taxonomy, we applied the attendant mea- To test the hypotheses, subjects were assigned to one
sure following Izard's (1977) direction. The measure of four groups on the basis of number of donations ac-
asks respondents to report the frequency with which cumulated prior to the survey. The cutoffs were: 1 or
various emotions were experienced in a specified time 2,3-7,8-15, and 16 or more accumulated donations.
period (cf. Westbrook 1987).lzard(1977,p.125)main- Observed new donation activity is shown in Table 1.
tains that "the time period to be considered by the in- The rationale for these cutoffs is presented below, fol-
dividual completing the scale can be varied to suit the lowed by empirical evidence that the manded nature
purpose." We directed subjects to retrospect about do- of donation erodes with experience.
nation and report how often they had felt each ofIzard's Aggressive recruiting tactics used by collection cen-
10 emotions either before, during, or after giving blood. ters produce new donors with weak intrinsic motives.
To capture participants' recollections of specific Although self-perception theory (Bem 1972) suggests
emotional experiences associated with donation, we that repeated behaviors can foster internalization, it has
used the instrument referred to as the Differential Emo- drawn criticism for not specifying the conditions under
tions Scale III (cf. Allen et al. 1988; Kotsch et al. 1982). which performance prompts self-reflection to yield in-
In this scale, a set of three phrases describes each of the trinsic motives (e.g., Allen and Dillon 1983; Tybout
10 primary emotions in Izard's (1977) taxonomy. For and Yalch 1980). Working with blood donors, Piliavin
example, for the emotion sadness, the three phrases are: et al. (1984) have tried to identify the number of do-
"feel unhappy, blue, downhearted"; "feel sad and
gloomy, almost like crying"; and "feel discouraged, like
you can't make it, nothing is going right." As Izard 3This scale was derived from a larger set of items that gauged re-
spondents' perceptions about many different aspects of blood do-
nation. Exploratory factor analysis was used to identify the dimensions
2The five items in the A.ct scale are good/bad, foolish/wise, pleasant/ of response; confirmatory factor analysis was used to establish uni-
unpleasant, unsafe/safe, and punishing/rewarding. The scale's uni- dimensionality. All fit indicators from LISREL VI confirmed the
dimensionality was confirmed through LISREL VI: all indicators unidimensionality of the four-item scale (e.g., x2(2) = 3.23, p < .20,
suggested an excellent fit (e.g., x2(5) = 2.53, p < .77, and adjusted and adjusted goodness-of-fit index = .975). Coefficient alpha for per-
goodness-of-fit index = .991). Coefficient alpha for A.ct was. 72. ceived self-motivation was .75.
498 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

TABLE 1 TABLE 2
OBSERVED DONATION FREQUENCIES AFTER 12 CROSS CLASSIFICATION OF A SUBJECT'S EXPERIENCE
MONTHS, BY EXPERIENCE GROUP GROUPING, BY SITE OF LAST DONATION MADE

Experience groupings based on accumulated Experience groupings based on


donations before the survey accumulated donations before the survey
Number of
donations One or two 3-7 8-15 16 or more Site of last One or two 3-7 8-15 16 or more
after donations donations donations donations donation donations donations donations donations
12 months (n = 87) (n = 99) (n = 87) (n = 64)
Fixed site:
0 55 46 14 8 Frequency 22 52 50 45
1 19 28 21 6 Percentage
2 10 22 27 19 of column 27.2 54.7 58.1 71.4
3 2 1 20 17 Mobile unit:
4 1 1 5 7 Frequency 59 43 36 18
5 3 Percentage
6 2 of column 72.8 45.3 41.9 28.6
7 1
8 1 NOTE.-Data on site of last donation were missing for 12 cases; X2 = 31.134,
P < .00001. Kendall's TC = - .323, p < .00001.

nations required before intrinsic motives develop. Their


data suggest that a minimum of three donations is nec- that between-group contrasts establish the first gallon
essary, which led us to select three as the first cutoff in of accumulated donation as pivotal in terms of statis-
partitioning respondents for analysis. tical significance. Cell means for perceived self-moti-
It is certainly possible that simply performing this vation are higher in both the 8-15 and 16-plus categories
behavior would not be enough to alter its manded status: than in the other two groups (p < .05). Group com-
other cues or events may be needed to prompt the self- parisons above and below the gallon cutoff are not sig-
reflection that fosters internalization. Such an event oc- nificant.
curs when one reaches the gallon level of donation. Further evidence that motives internalize with ex-
When donors reach their first gallon-and for every perience is furnished by data concerning where donors
gallon thereafter-they are recognized with a lapel pin made their last donation. Donations on mobile units
and in a letter from the center's director that labels them stem from institutional blood drives, whereas fixed-site
as continuing supporters of their community's blood donations may occur purely on the individual's initi-
program. This recognition and labeling could be ex- ative (cf. Charng et al. 1988). In support of this con-
pected to prompt the self-reflection that is called for if tention, those whose last donation was at a fixed site
an intrinsic motive is to be inferred from one's behavior expressed higher levels of perceived self-motivation than
(Allen and Dillon 1983; Tybout and Yalch 1980). Ac- those who last donated at a mobile site (X = 25.24 vs.
cordingly, 8 and 16, representing one and two gallons 24.17, F(1,323) = 8.78,p < .003). Moreover, as shown
of accumulated donations, respectively, were chosen as in Table 2, the percent of prior donations made at a
the other cutoffs in partitioning respondents for anal- fixed site rises with experience, indicating a higher level
ysis. 4 of individual initiative among experienced donors.
Direct evidence supporting the premise that the A final point concerning intrinsic motivation is made
manded status of donation declines with experience is in Table 1. Notice in Table 1 how Gallon Club members
furnished by the perceived self-motivation measure. were more likely to make multiple donations in the
Perceived self-motivation differs across the four groups year after the survey. Higher donation rates also indicate
(F(3,336) = 6.29, p < .0004).5 The test for a linear trend individual initiative on the part of experienced donors,
in these data is significant (p < .0001), suggesting that because institutional blood drives typically occur on an
self-motivation rises (and mandedness falls) as a func- annual cycle. One can thus infer that those making
tion of experience. It is important to note, however, multiple donations within a 12-month time frame are
pursuing the behavior beyond the highly manded in-
stitutional context.
4The goal underlying our sampling plan was to produce a sample
with a broad range of prior experience both above and below the 1- These data support a conclusion that the four group-
gallon level: this was achieved. We did not envision the 2-gallon-plus ings do reflect meaningful information about changes
grouping at the time the sample was drawn; however, incorporating in donors' motives. The perceived self-motivation, do-
this group into the data analysis allowed us to explore the issue em- nation-site, and donation-frequency results converge to
bodied in Hypothesis 2 concerning the moderating role of extraor-
dinary experience.
indicate that donation becomes less manded with ex-
5Mean scores on perceived self-motivation across the four groups perience. Achieving Gallon Club membership appears
were 23.84, 24.35, 25.29, and 25.85. to have special significance in the internalization pro-
ATTITUDES AND EMOTIONS AS PREDICTORS 499

TABLE 3 TABLE 4

ATTITUDE VERSUS EMOTION AT LOW EXPERIENCE LEVELS: ATTITUDE VERSUS EMOTION AT HIGH EXPERIENCE LEVELS:
DISCRIMINANT ANALYSES ON DONATION OCCURRENCE REGRESSION ANALYSES ON DONATION FREQUENCY
AFTER 12 MONTHS AFTER 12 MONTHS

One or two prior 3-7 prior donations 8-15 prior donations 16-plus prior donations
donations group group group group

2. Attitude 4. Attitude 2. Attitude 4. Attitude


1. Attitude plus 3. Attitude plus 1. Attitude plus 3. Attitude plus
Predictors only emotion only emotion Predictors only emotion only emotion

Aact .312 .404+ Aact .200+ .267* -.013


Aact2 .815 .718* .553 Aact2 .170 .218
Sadness .864** Disgust .190
Contempt -.724* Contempt -.185
Joy .505+ Shame .402**
Fear -.707* Fear -.230+
Chi-square .514 13.048** 4.737+ 8.447** F 4.772** 2.694* 1.432 4.964 **
Canonical MultipleR .321 .318 .212 .380
correlation .079 .391 .224 .303 Adjusted R2 .082 .063 .014 .115
Classification
rate .640 .702 .547 .596 NOTE.-Coefficients are beta weights. Predictors not entered in the stepwise
equation are designated with ellipses.
NOTE.-Coefficients are from the standardized canonical discriminant function. +p < .10.
Predictors not entered in the stepwise function are designated with ellipses. *p < .05.
+p<.10. **p < .01.
*p < .05.
**p < .01.

1; these data involve inexperienced donors for whom


cess. These four experience groups are maintained donation is more likely to be manded. Function 1 shows
throughout testing of the three research hypotheses. that, for these respondents, attitudes do not predict do-
nation occurrence. However, three emotions-con-
Hypothesis 1: Manded Behavior and tempt, joy, and sadness-emerge as significant predic-
Relative Prediction tors in function 2.
The hypotheses were tested via a series of discrimi- Function 3 in Table 3 demonstrates that attitude be-
nant and regression analyses. As shown in Table 1, over gins to emerge as a significant predictor in the 3-7 prior-
half the subjects in the two lower experience groups did donations group. However, findings from the stepwise
not donate in the year after the survey. In these two procedure again indicate improved prediction through
groups (i.e., 1 or 2, and 3-7 prior donations), discrim- addition of the emotion variables. As illustrated in
inant analysis was used to identify predictors of whether function 4, when fear replaces A act , prediction improves.
a donation occurred during the year. Donation activity A comparison of the classification rates of functions 1
was much more robust among Gallon Club members versus 2, and 3 versus 4, upholds the primary assertion
(i.e., 8-15, and 16 or more prior donations). In these from Hypothesis 1: when past behavior is manded,
groups, regression analysis was used to identify predic- emotion will provide a substantive contribution to at-
tors of donation frequency.6 Analyses were conducted titude in predicting future behavior.
in pairs; the first function or equation included just the Prediction at Higher Experience Levels. It was ar-
two attitude measures, Aact and A act2 ' The second func- gued above that the recognition and labeling that occurs
tion or equation in a pair was derived through a stepwise when one reaches the gallon-donation level should
procedure in which the two attitude indicators were in- stimulate self-reflection, prompting formation of in-
troduced into the function or equation along with the trinsic motives. As reported above, between-group con-
10 subscales from the emotion measure. Results are trasts involving perceived self-motivation indicated that
summarized in Tables 3 and 4. individuals who had attained Gallon Club status per-
Prediction at Low Experience Levels. Functions 1 ceived that they were more self-motivated than those
and 2 in Table 3 furnish clear support for Hypothesis who had not. Table 4 displays findings for Gallon Club
members only: these data offer support for Hypothesis
1's secondary assertion.
6S ubjects in all four experience categories expressed high degrees Equations 1 and 2 in Table 4 represent tests of atti-
of confidence in their attitudinal judgments. Mean scores on the con-
fidence scale item ranged from 1.37 to 1.70 across the four groups tude's improvement as a predictor when behavioral ex-
(where 1 = very confident). The mean values are not different statis- perience is less manded. In both these equations the
tically (F(3,330) = 1.88, p > .13). attitude variables provide a modest level of prediction;
500 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

thus, when behavioral experience is less manded, atti- EXHIBIT 1


tude does improve as a predictor. Notably, Equation 2 REGRESSION TESTS OF ATTITUDE'S STATUS AS A MEDIATOR
also documents that no emotion variables emerged as OF EMOTION'S INFLUENCE
significant predictors among respondents with 8-15
prior donations. RegreSSion equations

1. Aact with sadness (-.123)


Hypothesis 2: Habituated Behavior and 2. ABct2 with sadness (-.224*)
3. Donation with sadness (.186+)
Relative Prediction 4. Donation with ABet (.042) and sadness (.196+)
5. Donation with ABct2 (.057) and sadness (.199+)
The case has been made that donation's mandedness 6. Aact with fear (-.372 **)
erodes with experience and that reaching the first gallon 7. Aact2 with fear (-.309**)
demarcation has special significance in this process. 8. Donation with fear (-.244*)
Unfortunately, no rationale exists to specify what 9. Donation with ABet (.026) and fear (-.262*)
10. Donation with Aact2 (-.046) and fear (-.258*)
amount of experience is required for a behavior to be
declared habituated. The only guidance provided by NOTE.-Beta weights for each predictor are shown in parentheses.
Ronis et al. (1989) on this issue is their assertion that +p < .10.
'p < .05.
"the longer the behavior has been repeated, the less it "p < .01.
will be correlated with and predicted by attitude" (p.
221). We thus feature our most experienced donor cat-
egory-those with a minimum of two gallons of accu-
As shown in Exhibit 1, Baron and Kenny's (1986)
mulated donations-in exploring the issues raised in
preliminary conditions were met for sadness (see Equa-
Hypothesis 2. However, there is no basis for contending
that the 16-donation cutoff has any special significance tions 1-3) and fear (see Equations 6-8). The crucial test
of mediation then entails how the proposed mediator
in terms of the habituated status of this behavior.
Data in Table 4 uphold Ronis et al.'s (1989) assertion influences the relationship between the emotion and
donation when it is added to the regression equation.
that attitude's predictive ness erodes at extreme expe-
To infer mediational status for attitude, the regression
rience levels. No association between attitude and be-
coefficients for the emotion variables should decrease
havior was found for the highest experience group. On
the other hand, Equation 4 shows a reemergence of significantly when Aact and/or Aact2 are entered. As Ex-
hibit 1 shows, there were no such decreases. Comparing
emotional predictors: two emotional variables, shame
Equation 3 with 4 and 5 shows a slight increase in the
and fear, were significant. Comparing R2 values for
beta weights for sadness. Notice also that the association
Equations 3 and 4, the two emotions account for a 10
between sadness and attitude is negative, whereas the
percent increment in explained variance. Hence, Hy-
relationship between sadness and donation is positive.
pothesis 2 is not upheld: in this highest experience cat-
Thus, greater sadness implies a higher propensity to
egory, attitude's contribution to prediction is clearly
donate and a less favorable attitude. Clearly, such a
transcended by that of emotion.
pattern defies the simple mediational argument that ex-
periencing negative affect produces less favorable atti-
Hypothesis 3: Attitude as a Mediator tudes, which, in turn, deter behavior.
of Emotion's Influence For fear, comparing Equation 8 with 9 and 10 again
shows a slight increase in the magnitude of the beta
Our third hypothesis considers the issue of whether weights for fear when the attitude variables are entered.
emotion's association with behavior is direct or, as pos- Thus, the case for inferring mediational status for at-
ited by Zanna and Rempel (1988), is mediated by at- titude again breaks down at this crucial step. Overall,
titude. It was tested with procedures recommended by these results uphold Hypothesis 3: emotion can have a
Baron and Kenny (1986, p. 1177). The second condition direct influence on behavior that is not captured or
of Baron and Kenny's three-step procedure requires that summed up by attitude judgments.
the independent variable (in our case, an emotion) be
associated with the dependent variable (in our case, be- DISCUSSION
havior). Three instances supporting this condition (see
functions 2 and 4 in Table 3 and Equation 4 in Table In general terms, these results demonstrate that in-
4) were used in testing Hypothesis 3: they involve sad- dividuals' reports about emotive experiences can be
ness in the 1 or 2 prior-donations group; fear in the valuable for predicting behavior. Moreover, such rec-
3-7 group; and shame in the 16 or more group. The ollections need not merely duplicate information de-
analysis involving shame was discontinued because the rived via traditional attitude measures. We report sev-
mediator and independent variable were unrelated (t- eral instances in which emotion variables contribute
values less than one). Complete results for sadness and increments of explained variance that are substantive
fear are summarized in Exhibit 1. by the standards of consumer research (cf. Peterson,
ATTITUDES AND EMOTIONS AS PREDICTORS 501

Albaum, and Beltramini 1985). A dependent measure shows that fear predicts donation at intermediate ex-
involving naturally occurring behavior that transpired perience levels.
over 12 months adds to the results' noteworthiness. A final example of the supplementary diagnostic po-
Findings derived in testing Hypotheses 1 and 2 sug- tential of reports on emotions comes from those with
gest more specific conclusions. First, it was shown that extensive experience. While shame is commonly por-
attitude is an ineffective predictor when prior behavior trayed as a motivator of helping (Izard 1977; Rosenhan
is likely to be manded, upholding Fazio's (1986) con- et al. 1981), its emergence among veteran donors (see
tention that only freely chosen, unmanded behaviors Table 4) is of special interest. This finding on shame
lead to predictive attitudes. In addition, tests of Hy- may reflect a motivational mechanism that is common
pothesis 1 indicate that, in those contexts in which past to many compulsive behaviors. Alleviating negative
behavior is manded, one may expect emotional vari- feelings is a primary motive that drives compulsion,
ables to supplement prediction. and these negative feelings are spawned in part by low
The data also demonstrate that emotions can serve self-esteem (O'Guinn and Faber 1989). The likelihood
as supplementary predictors at very high levels of ex- that low self-esteem will beget specific negative feelings
perience. Given the exploratory nature of our second like shame (Izard 1977) suggests that donation may as-
hypothesis, the results can only serve as a basis for en- sume a compulsive character for some veteran donors.
couraging future research; however, findings concerning
Hypothesis 2 suggest that, after extensive prior expe- Limitations and Future Research
rience, attitude's value as a predictor may be limited. Hypotheses were tested in this research with a unique
This finding is consistent with recent theorizing about data base that facilitated partitioning of respondents
habits (Charng et al. 1988; Ronis et al. 1989). Unex- into diverse prior-experience categories and featured a
pectedly, emotive reports predicted best for those re- year's worth of actual behavior as the criterion variable.
spondents with extensive experience. Additional theo- In this section we wish to acknowledge the unique set
rizing and research about emotion's role in perpetuating of limitations that attend these data. Although nonre-
behavior seem justified by these data. sponse biases are always a matter of concern in survey
research, it is noteworthy that response rates to our sur-
Diagnosing Motives from Emotional vey varied dramatically as a function of prior experi-
Reports ence. For instance, among Gallon Club members, the
response rate was 62.5 percent; for nonmembers, it was
Furnishing specific diagnoses of the emotional mo- 31.1 percent. Although we acknowledge that these dif-
tivators of donation was not a primary goal of this re- ferential response rates represent a potential confound
search. Although the results summarized in Tables 3 when comparing findings between prior-experience
and 4 can be interpreted as reflecting different types of categories, it should be emphasized that the primary
motives, any such interpretation is obviously post hoc. focus of our analyses entailed within-category compar-
We offer some speculative interpretation at this juncture isons. We can see no basis for concern that attitude or
simply to illustrate the potential of reports of emotions emotion would gain a within-category advantage in
for supplementing attitude judgments in diagnosing prediction as a result of higher or lower response rates.
motives. This project's findings also emphasize how much we
Among the least experienced donors (see Table 3), have yet to learn about the factors that perpetuate be-
the positive relationship between sadness and donation havior. As others have suggested (cf. Charng et al. 1988;
indicates that donation may be used for relieving neg- Cohen 1990; Ronis et al. 1989), the attitude construct
ative moods (cf. Rosenhan et al. 1981). Alternatively, can only furnish an incomplete account for what main-
the negative relationship between contempt and do- tains, accelerates, or undermines behavioral perfor-
nation suggests the influence of empathy, another fun- mance over time. Our data suggest that reports on emo-
damental motivator of pro social behavior (Piliavin et tions offer a meaningful supplement to attitude
al. 1982). Those seeking relief from negative moods or judgments for predicting performance; this incremental
those who are compelled by empathetic responses may prediction varied as a function of prior experience.
also be sensitive to the gratification accompanying do- These findings would have been more compelling had
nation, which would account for the joy and donation there been a theoretical basis for hypothesizing which
linkage. specific emotions would be predictive in particular ex-
Piliavin et al. (1984) note that a unique motivational perience categories. Unfortunately, theories that inte-
aspect of donation involves fear about negative con- grate affective, cognitive, evaluative, and behavioral
sequences: fear and anxiety must be mastered if one is domains are rare (cf. Cohen 1990). Yet without such
to become a repeat donor. Fear is so pervasive among theorizing to provide legitimization, we must acknowl-
inexperienced donors that it may not differentiate those edge that the pattern of results may be due to some
who will or will not donate again; however, as donors idiosyncratic feature of the research design.
gain experience many, but not all, master their anxieties. Our presentation and discussion of the results also
Consistent with this reasoning, function 4 of Table 3 contains an assumption that, as one gains more expe-
502 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

rience with donation, one's motives for the behavior important improvements needed in the measurement
may change and become more internalized (cf. Piliavin area (cf. Havlena and Holbrook 1986).
et al. 1982, 1984). We thus treat the four experience Finally, data in this project indicate that attitude does
groups as representing stages in a developmental pro- not mediate the effect of emotive experience on behav-
cess; alternatively, the four groups may simply capture ior. Hypothesis 3 was formulated and tested to provide
individual differences in motives that yield different direct assessment of this mediational question. It should
donation rates. For example, those who associate shame be emphasized, however, that this test was conducted
with donation (see Table 4) may be driven to donate in a single behavioral domain. Blood donation can be
more frequently than those associating sadness with accompanied by diverse and intense emotions; fur-
donation (see Table 3). The correlational nature of the thermore, the nature and intensity of this emotive ex-
data cannot distinguish between these competing in- perience may change over time. Caution is advisable
terpretations concerning the meaning of the experience in generalizing our findings to consumption contexts
categories; however, our conclusion that emotion sup- that may have very different emotional dynamics.
plements attitude for predicting behavior seems war- Whether attitude judgments will prove uniformly inef-
ranted in either case. fectual in mediating emotion's influence across behav-
Another alternative interpretation for the results ioral domains is a question for future research.
stems from the fact that attitude and emotion are mea-
sured via different scale formats. That is, the emotion CONCLUSION
measure encouraged retrospection and prompted the
respondent to report how often various feelings were This research demonstrates that emotive reports can
experienced in the donation context. The attitude mea- supplement attitudinal judgments for behavioral pre-
sure, however, asked for the subject's evaluation ofthe diction and may also prove valuable for diagnosing
act of donating with the traditional, semantic-differ- motives. The findings encourage integration of emotive
ential format. Differences in predictive ness for the constructs into research about consumption phenom-
measures could be due to format per se. ena that share one or both of the focal features of blood
In defense of these measures, we would argue that donation: when behavior is likely to be manded or when
they are conventional and logically consistent with the it is accompanied by complex or conflicting emotive
phenomena of interest. It is common in measuring experience, explicit consideration of emotion may en-
emotion to ask individuals to retrospect about how they rich prediction and understanding.
felt in a specified context (e.g., Madden et al. 1988; There are many consumption domains that meet one
Westbrook 1987; Westbrook and Oliver 1991). More- or both of the above conditions. For instance, blood
over, while traditional attitude measures reflect an im- donation is just one potential exemplar of a much larger
plicit assumption that evaluations are stored in and ac- class of gift-giving behaviors (cf. Sherry 1983). The rit-
cessible from semantic memory (Fazio 1986), emotional ualistic nature of gift giving combined with the donor's
reports commonly encourage retrieval from episodic desire to please the recipient can yield behavior that is
memory. Hence, although our measures are conven- heavily manded as well as emotionally charged (Sherry
tional, they are also consistent with Tulving's (1983) 1983; Sherry, McGrath, and Levy 1991). Sherry et al.
proposal that it is logical and efficient to use different (1991) emphasize that explicit consideration of diverse
scale formats when assessing attitude judgments versus negative emotions will be particularly valuable in re-
emotive experience because of differences in how each search on gift giving. Another broad consumption cat-
is stored in memory. egory that will benefit from considering emotions en-
While defending our selection of measures, we do compasses all products or ads that provoke nostalgic
not mean to suggest that "conventional" measures of experience (e.g., Havlena and Holak 1991; Miller 1990).
emotion cannot be improved on for consumer research. Nostalgic consumption is of interest not because of its
As Westbrook (1987) notes, measures such as Izard's mandedness but because it is thought to be accompa-
are essentially aided-recall tasks in which individuals nied by a complex and conflicting, or "bittersweet,"
are asked to recollect past episodes and report frequency emotional condition (Havlena and Holak 1991). This
of occurrence. Little is known about how best to direct is just the sort of emotional experience that may be
individuals to recollect emotive experience. For ex- undervalued or misrepresented by the traditional atti-
ample, would detailed descriptions of a particular con- tude construct.
sumption context yield more predictive and diagnostic
emotional reports than just a general reference to the [Received August 1990. Revised July 1991.]
context would? Might the actual presence of artifacts
from the context enrich the nature ofthe reports? Could REFERENCES
mood-induction techniques be adapted to encourage Abelson, Robert P., Donald R. Kinder, Mark D. Peters, and
respondents to re-create their feelings prior to mea- Susan T. Fiske (1982), "Affective and Semantic Com-
surement? If we are to do a better job of integrating ponents in Political Person Perception," Journal of Per-
emotive experience into consumer research, there are sonality and Social Psychology, 42 (April), 619-630.
ATTITUDES AND EMOTIONS AS PREDICTORS 503

Ahtola, Olli T. (1985), "Hedonic and Utilitarian Aspects of Consumer Behavior," in Advances in Consumer Re-
Consumer Behavior: An Attitudinal Perspective," in Ad- search, Vol. 18, ed. Rebecca H. Holman and Michael R.
vances in Consumer Research, Vol. 12, ed. Elizabeth C. Solomon, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Re-
Hirschman and Morris B. Holbrook, Provo, UT: Asso- search, 323-329.
ciation for Consumer Research, 7-10. - - - and Morris B. Holbrook (1986), "The Varieties of
Allen, Chris T. and William R. Dillon (1983), "Self-Perception Consumption Experience: Comparing Two Typologies
Development and Consumer Choice Criteria: Is There a of Emotion in Consumer Behavior," Journal of Con-
Linkage?" in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 10, sumer Research, 13 (December), 394-404.
ed. Richard P. Bagozzi and Alice M. Tybout, Ann Arbor, Hirschman, Elizabeth C. and Morris B. Holbrook (1982),
MI: Association for Consumer Research, 45-50. "Hedonic Consumption: Emerging Concepts, Methods
- - - , Karen A. Machleit, and Susan S. Marine (1988), "On and Propositions," Journal of Marketing, 46 (Summer),
Assessing the Emotionality of Advertising via Izard's Dif- 92-101.
ferential Emotions Scale," in Advances in Consumer Re- Holbrook, Morris B. (1986), "Emotion in the Consumption
search, Vol. 15, ed. Michael J. Houston, Provo, UT: As- Experience: Toward a New Model of the Human Con-
sociation for Consumer Research, 226-231. sumer," in The Role ofAffect in Consumer Behavior, ed.
Bagozzi, Richard P. (1981), "Attitudes, Intentions, and Be- Robert A. Peterson et aI., Lexington, MA: Heath, 17-
havior: A Test of Some Key Hypotheses," Journal of 52.
Personality and Social Psychology, 41 (October), 607- - - - and Rajeev Batra (1987), "Assessing the Role of
627. Emotions as Mediators of Consumer Responses to Ad-
Baron, Reuben M. and David A. Kenny (1986), "The Mod- vertising," Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (Decem-
erator-Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psycho- ber), 404-420.
logical Research: Conceptual, Strategic, and Statistical - - - and Elizabeth C. Hirschman (1982), "The Experiential
Considerations," Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Aspects of Consumption: Consumer Fantasies, Feelings,
chology, 51 (December), 1173-1182. and Fun," Journal of Consumer Research, 9 (September),
Batra, Rajeev (1986), "Affective Advertising: Role, Processes, 132-140.
and Measurement," in The Role of Affect in Consumer Izard, Carroll E. (1977), Human Emotions, New York:
Behavior, ed. Robert A. Peterson et aI., Lexington, MA: Plenum.
Heath, 53-85. Kotsch, William E., David W. Gerbing, and Lynne E.
Bern, DarylJ. (1972), "Self-Perception Theory," in Advances Schwartz (1982), "The Construct Validity of the Differ-
in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 6, ed. Leonard ential Emotions Scale as Adapted for Children and Ad-
Berkowitz, New York: Academic Press, 1-62. olescents," in Measuring Emotions in Infants and Chil-
Blumenthal, Karen (1989), "Marketing with Emotion: Wal- dren, ed. Carroll E. Izard, New York: Cambridge
Mart Shows the Way," Wall Street Journal (November University Press, 251-278.
13),BI. Madden, Thomas J., Chris T. Allen, and Jacquelyn L. Twible
Breckler, Steven J. and Elizabeth C. Wiggins (1989), "On De- (1988), "Attitude toward the Ad: An Assessment of Di-
fining Attitude and Attitude Theory: Once More with verse Measurement Indices under Different Processing
Feeling," in Attitude Structure and Function, ed. Anthony Sets," Journal ofMarketing Research, 25 (August), 242-
R. Pratkanis et aI., Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 407-427. 252.
Charng, Hong-Wen, Jane A. Piliavin, and Peter L. Callero Miller, Cyndee (1990), "Nostalgia Makes Boomers Buy,"
(1988), "Role Identity and Reasoned Action in the Pre- Marketing News (November 26), 1-2.
diction of Repeated Behavior," Social Psychology Quar- Mowen, John C. (1988), "Beyond Consumer Decision Mak-
terly, 51 (December), 303-317. ing," Journal of Consumer Marketing, 5 (Winter), 15-
Cohen, Joel B. (1990), "Attitude, Affect, and Consumer Be- 25.
havior," in Affect and Social Behavior, ed. Bert S. Moore O'Guinn, Thomas C. and Ronald J. Faber (1989), "Com-
and Alice M. Isen, New York: Cambridge University pulsive Buying: A Phenomenological Exploration,"
Press, 152-206. Journal of Consumer Research, 16 (September), 147-157.
Condie, Spencer J., W. Keith Warner, and David C. Gillman Peterson, Robert A., Gerald Albaum, and Richard F. Beltra-
(1976), "Getting Blood from Collective Turnips: Vol- mini (1985), "A Meta-analysis of Effect Sizes in Con-
unteer Donation in Mass Blood Drives," Journal ofAp- sumer Behavior Experiments," Journal ofConsumer Re-
plied Psychology, 61 (June), 290-294. search, 12 (June), 97-103.
Engel, James F., Roger D. Blackwell, and Paul W. Miniard Piliavin, Jane A., Peter Callero, and Dorcas E. Evans (1982),
(1990), Consumer Behavior, Orlando, FL: Dryden. "Addiction to Altruism: Opponent-Process Theory and
Fazio, Russell H. (1986), "How Do Attitudes Guide Behav- Habitual Blood Donation," Journal of Personality and
ior?" in Handbook of Motivation and Cognition, ed. Social Psychology, 43 (December), 1200-1213.
Richard M. Sorrentino and E. Tory Higgins, New York: - - - , Dorcas E. Evans, and Peter Callero (1984), "Learning
Guilford, 204-243. to Give to Unnamed Strangers: The Process of Com-
Friestad, Marian and Esther Thorson (1986), "Emotion-elic- mitment to Regular Blood Donation," in Development
iting Advertising: Effects on Long-term Memory and and Maintenance of Prosocial Behavior, ed. Ervin Staub
Judgment," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 13, et aI., New York: Plenum, 471-492.
ed. Richard J. Lutz, Provo, UT: Association for Con- Plutchik, Robert (1980), Emotion: A Psychoevolutionary
sumer Research, 111-116. Synthesis, New York: Harper & Row.
Havlena, William J. and Susan L. Holak (1991), "The Good Ronis, David L., J. Frank Yates, and John P. Kirscht (1989),
Old Days: Observations on Nostalgia and Its Role in "Attitudes, Decisions, and Habits as Determinants of
504 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Repeated Behavior," in Attitude Structure and Function, Tybout, Alice M. and Richard F. Yalch (1980), "The Effects
ed. Anthony R. Pratkanis et aI., Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, of Experience: A Matter of Salience?" Journal of Con-
213-239. sumer Research, 6 (March), 406-413.
Rosenhan, David L., Peter Salovey, Jerzy Karylowski, and Westbrook, Robert A. (1987), "Product/Consumption-
Kenneth Hargis (1981), "Emotion and Altruism," in Al- based Affective Responses and Postpurchase Processes,"
truism and Helping Behavior, ed. J. Philippe Rushton and Journal of Marketing Research, 24 (August), 258-
Richard M. Sorrentino, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 233-248. 270.
Sherry, John F. (1983), "Gift Giving in Anthropological Per- - - - and Richard L. Oliver (1991), "The Dimensionality
spective," Journal of Consumer Research, 10 (Septem- of Consumption Emotion Patterns and Consumer Sat-
ber), 157-168.
isfaction," Journal of Consumer Research, 18 (June), 84-
- - - , Mary Ann McGrath, and Sidney J. Levy (1991), "The
Dark Side of the Gift," working paper, Department of 91.
Marketing, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL Woodruff, David, Karen L. Miller, Larry Armstrong, and
60208. Thane Peterson (1990), "A New Era for Auto Quality,"
Tomkins, Silvan S. (1970), "Affect as the Primary Motiva- Business Week (October 22), 84-96.
tional System," in Feelings and Emotions, ed. Magda B. Zanna, Mark P. and John K. Rempel (1988), "Attitudes: A
Arnold, New York: Academic Press, 101-110. New Look at an Old Concept," in The Social Psychology
Tulving, Endel (1983), Elements of Episodic Memory, New of Knowledge, ed. Daniel Bar-Tal and Arie Kruglanski,
York: Oxford University Press. New York: Cambridge University Press, 315-334.

View publication stats

You might also like