Selective Attention and Anxiety A Perspe

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

c 2001)

Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, Vol. 23, No. 3, September 2001 (°

Selective Attention and Anxiety: A Perspective on


Developmental Issues and the Causal Status

Merel Kindt1,2 and Marcel Van Den Hout1

A plethora of studies on selective information processing in anxiety have been carried out over the
past two decades. One of the most robust findings is that anxiety is associated with selective attention
(SA) for threatening information. The rationale of research into SA is that it is assumed to play a
vital role in the maintenance, and even in the etiology of anxiety disorders. It is the aim of this paper
to explicate on the validity of this assumption. There is ample evidence that anxiety enhances SA.
Although there is a lack of studies on the effect of SA on anxiety, there is now some evidence that
SA increases the level of anxiety. This leads us to conclude that SA is not a by-product of anxiety
only. Hence, the suggestion that SA plays a role in the maintenance of anxiety disorders seems to
be justified. Studies on SA in children suggest that if SA plays any role at all in the development of
anxiety disorders, it is not the SA in itself that is a vulnerability factor, but the continuation of this
SA. Individuals who develop an anxiety disorder could have difficulty learning to inhibit this SA.
Considering the finding that SA increases the level of anxiety, the authors suggest that anxiety in
childhood causes failure to inhibit SA, which in turn enhances the vulnerability to anxiety disorder
in adulthood.

KEY WORDS: selective attention; anxiety.

During the past 20 years, insights from cognitive psy- processes. The nature of research on anxiety disorders
chology have been applied to research on emotional disor- has changed dramatically as a result of this paradigmatic
ders. This has caused major changes in clinical research. shift.
Firstly, cognitive theories have been formulated that can be According to information processing models, anxi-
used to derive highly specific hypotheses on the nature and ety is presented in a cognitive anxiety network or schema
origin of emotional disorders. At the same time, and pos- (Beck, 1976; Beck, Emery, & Greenberg, 1986; Bower,
sibly not less important, experimental procedures became 1981, 1987; Bower & Forgas, 2000). These anxiety net-
available as a welcome addition to introspective meth- works are assumed to lead to selective processing of threat-
ods. In particular, the information-processing approach ening information. Over the past two decades, there has
to emotional disorders provided experimental paradigms been a great deal of research on the selective attention (SA)
broadening the range of methodologies. It allowed to study that anxious individuals are thought to have for threaten-
cognitive processes such as selective attention, encoding, ing information. The idea is that SA could play a role in
and retrieval, instead of only the content of cognitions. the maintenance, and even in the etiology of anxiety disor-
Moreover, the information-processing approach provided ders. From the point of view of an anxious individual, SA
the possibility to study conscious as well as unconscious is functional, because the sooner a threat is perceived, the
sooner an adequate response can be given. If and insofar as
SA reinforces anxiety, and anxiety is irrational, SA is dys-
1 Department of Medical, Clinical and Experimental Psychology, functional. In this way, SA may force anxious individuals
Maastricht University, The Netherlands.
2 To whom correspondence should be addressed at Department of Med- into a vicious circle of more attention to threat → more
ical, Clinical and Experimental Psychology, Faculties of Medicine anxiety → more attention to threat (see e.g., MacLeod,
and Health Sciences, Maastricht University, P.O. Box 616, 6200 MD 1991). The result of SA in anxious persons would be
Maastricht, The Netherlands; e-mail: merel.kindt@dep.unimaas.nl. that threat stimuli easily activate the anxiety network and

193
0882-2689/01/0900-0193$19.50/0 °
C 2001 Plenum Publishing Corporation
194 Kindt and Van Den Hout

hence increase its impact on information processing. Al- ential effects on different processes of information pro-
though the operation of this alleged “vicious circle” has cessing (Oatley & Johnson-Laird, 1987). These models
been widely suggested in studies on SA and anxiety, the indicate that anxiety primarily determines which infor-
question may be raised how much support there actually mation enters the cognitive system (MacLeod & Mathews,
is for this assumption. If true, this would imply that SA 1991a). There is indeed a great deal of evidence available
plays indeed a role in the maintenance of anxiety disor- to support the idea that anxious persons have an attentional
ders. In pursuing the role of SA in the etiology of anxiety preference for threatening information. There is, however,
disorders, it is assumed that individuals with an “anxious little evidence that suggests that anxious persons have a
personality” who also tend to selectively attend to threat, memory preference for this information.
would then be vulnerable to develop anxiety disorders. In Studies of SA generally make use of the emotional
contrast, “nonanxious personalities” would tend to turn Stroop paradigm, a modification of the traditional color–
attention away from threat in similar situations. Again the word Stroop task (Stroop, 1935). In Stroop tasks, words
question may be raised how much support there is for this are presented in different colors. The subject is asked to
assumption. name the color of the word as quickly as possible (target
To clarify the role of SA in the maintenance of anxiety stimulus) and to ignore the content of the word (distractor
disorders, the authors will try to answer two main ques- stimulus). Numerous Stroop studies have been published
tions. The first question is whether anxiety promotes SA. (for an overview, see MacLeod, 1991; Williams, Mathews
The second question is whether, reversibly, SA stimulates & MacLeod, 1996). An important finding was that sub-
anxiety. There are only a few researchers who addressed jects are unable to ignore the content of the word entirely.
this second—complex—question. Concerning the ques- The processing of the meaning of the word interferes with
tion whether SA plays a role in the etiology of anxiety the process of naming the color. It takes more time to name
disorders, no prospective studies are available that show the color of a word printed in red, than to name the color of
evidence for this theoretical account. Nevertheless, several red meaningless symbols, such as “XXXXX.” The inter-
studies on SA in children are available that may shed some ference effects are greater when the meaning of the word is
light on the etiology issue. These studies and their impli- more difficult to ignore. Warren (1972, 1974) found more
cations will be discussed. Before these causality issues are interference on words that had been presented auditively
addressed, a brief outline of the underlying theories, the immediately prior to the Stroop task. Geller and Shaver
most common research methods in the field and a number (1976) found more interference on words relating to the
of salient and frequently reported findings will be given. subjects themselves when the Stroop task was performed
in front of a mirror or a camera. Interference occurs be-
cause attention is given to the word, that is, processing
SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND ANXIETY: of the word cannot be completely inhibited. The attention
THEORIES, METHODS, AND that is paid to the processing of the word affects the at-
GENERAL FINDINGS tention that can be given to naming the color. The result
is a delayed response time. If anxious individuals have
Cognitive anxiety networks (Lang, 1977, 1979; an SA for information that is relevant for their anxiety,
McNally & Steketee, 1985) are held to consist of (a) infor- they should be slower in naming the color of threatening
mation about the feared stimulus situation (e.g. a spider); stimuli, compared to neutral stimuli.3
(b) information about verbal responses (e.g. screaming), Another method of identifying SA is the “Visual Dot-
physiological responses (e.g. palpitations), and behavioral Probe Task” (VDT). In this response time task, two words
responses (e.g. running away); and (c) information about are simultaneously presented on a computer screen: one
the meaning of the stimulus (dirty spider) and the meaning word immediately above the centre of the screen, the other
of response elements of the anxiety network (e.g. palpi- immediately below the centre of the screen. Shortly after
tations mean panic). Anxiety networks would lead to a
prioritizing in the processing of threatening information. 3 There is currently a discussion on the question whether the emotional
According to Beck and Bower, all phases of information Stroop task does in fact measure SA. This discussion derives from the
processing are disturbed in anxious individuals: attention different definitions of attention. The Stroop task measures relatively
is selectively focused on the origins of the threat, ambigu- late selection processes, which are based on a semantic analysis of
ous stimuli are more easily interpreted as threatening, and the word (see e.g., Driver & Tipper, 1989). Some believe that only
relatively early selection processes should be regarded as part of the
threatening information is memorized better (Beck et al., attention processes. We regard relatively late selection processes also
1986; Bower, 1981, 1987; Bower & Forgas, 2000). Other as part of SA. This does not mean, however, that these only concern
models, however, suggest that emotions may have differ- conscious attention processes.
Selective Attention and Anxiety 195

the words have disappeared from the screen, a clearly visi- valence of the words and specific relatedness to the dis-
ble dot (visual dot-probe) sometimes appears in one of the order were manipulated independently, showed that SA is
word positions. The task of the subject is twofold: the up- determined more by “negative valence” than by “related-
per word must be read aloud and a button must be pressed ness” (Lavy, van Oppen, & Van Den Hout, 1994).
as quickly as possible after the dot has appeared. This task Initially, the so-called “card version” of the Stroop
focuses on the speed of dot recognition. The only reason task was used to measure SA. In this version, words from
why the upper word must be read aloud is to keep the one category are printed on a card: one card for threaten-
focus of attention constant. SA would lead to more rapid ing words of type A, another card for threatening words
responses if the dot appeared in the same position as the of type B, a third card for neutral words, etc. In 1990,
threatening word. SA would lead to slower responses if McNally et al. introduced the first computerized version
the dot did not appear in the position of the threatening of the emotional Stroop task (McNally et al., 1990). This
word. The VDT can be used to determine the direction of increased the reliability of the task. In the card version,
attention, whereas the Stroop task enables us to measure the experimenter registered the total response time. But
the degree of distraction. Both phenomena are reasonably in a clinical setting the experimenter often knows whether
regarded as indications of SA. subjects are patients or not. If the experimenter also knows
The first study on the relationship between anxiety what the theory predicts with respect to the response time
and SA was done more than 20 years ago among students of patients for the different cards, there is a danger that
preparing for an important examination (Ray, 1979). The the stopwatch is pressed slightly sooner/later for a certain
subjects responded more slowly to examination-related combination of subject and card. Also, the response time
colored words than to neutral words, and this effect was was not corrected for errors made during color naming.
greater in subjects who reported more state anxiety, as as- The computerized version had another important advan-
sessed by the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory tage: the threatening and neutral stimuli could be presented
(Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970). Mathews and as a mix. This eliminated the possibility of the delay being
MacLeod (1985) found a similar result in patients with the result of a mood effect resulting from the cumulation
a generalized anxiety disorder. Patients responded more of threatening words: A Vietnam veteran who burst out
slowly to words that referred to social threat (e.g. “fail- into tears after reading dozens of words like “Vietnam,”
ure” or “lonely”) or physical threat (e.g. “coffin” or “dis- “Charlie,” “Body bag,” “dead,” “torture,” undoubtedly ex-
ease”). In addition, the degree of delay to the socially or hibits a delay, but this is an emotion effect rather than an
physically threatening words was related to the nature of attention effect. Studies conducted with the computerized
the anxiety in the subjects (social or physical). These two version yielded the same convincing results as the card
findings were replicated by Mogg, Mathews and Weinman version (Kindt, Bierman, & Brosschot, 1996; Williams
(1989). This allowed for the conclusion that anxious in- et al., 1996).
dividuals exhibit SA for threatening information and that It is actually surprising that for years, SA has been
there is specificity: the selective attention concerns the in- studied almost exclusively by means of threatening words.
formation that is particularly frightening for this particular Anxious individuals are, after all, not afraid of words, but
person and not threatening information in general. of stimuli that can be referred to with these words. This
During the 80s and 90s, a similar Stroop effect was issue prompted studies in which threatening images and
replicated in various anxiety disorders, such as spider words were compared (Kindt & Brosschot, 1997; Lavy &
phobia (e.g. Lavy, Van Den Hout, & Arntz, 1993), so- Van Den Hout, 1993). The studies were conducted among
cial phobia (Hope, Rapee, Heimberg, & Dombeck, 1990), spider phobics with the advantage that the stimuli were
generalized anxiety disorder (Mathews & MacLeod, unequivocally threatening. Phobic subjects rated the im-
1985), panic disorder (McNally, Riemann, & Kim, 1990), ages of spiders as more aversive than words like “spider”
posttraumatic stress disorder (e.g. Thrasher, Dalgleish & or “web.” This is not surprising; a picture of a spider looks
Yule, 1994), and obsessive–compulsive disorder (e.g. Foa, more like a real spider than the word “spider” (Kindt &
Ilai, McCarthy, Shoyer, & Murdock, 1993). SA has not Brosschot, 1997). Contrary to the expectations, the SA for
been found in anxious individuals only. Normal subjects words equalled that for images. That is, although phobics
also appear to exhibit SA for stimuli that are related to their rated the pictures as more threatening, slowing down on
individual concerns (e.g. Riemann & McNally, 1995). color-naming spider pictures, relative to neutral pictures,
However, the Stroop effect in anxiety disorders cannot was as large as slowing down on spider words relative
be explained exclusively by the degree to which threat- to neutral words. Allocation of SA to threatening infor-
ening stimuli are relevant for the patient’s concerns. A mation is possibly done on the basis of a fast and super-
study of obsessive–compulsive patients, in which negative ficial decision-making process, in which the individual
196 Kindt and Van Den Hout

determines whether the stimulus is relevant or not. The fact that the patients were, afterwards, unable to report
degree of relevance may not be important yet. To gain pro- which threatening words had been presented, made the
cessing priority, the stimulus must have a certain minimal authors conclude that the object of SA was not consciously
amount of relevance (threshold value; Cohen, Dunbar, & perceived.
McClelland, 1990). Above the threshold value, it does not According to Holender (1986), however, the dichotic
seem to matter how threatening the stimulus is. The only listening paradigm is not suitable to measure unconscious
condition for SA is that the stimulus is regarded as rele- processes. It cannot be excluded that during shadow-
vant. LeDoux (1990, p. 39) illustrates this phenomenon in ing of the neutral words, the attention is repeatedly and
the following passage: very briefly focussed on the threatening stimulus in the
other ear. The subject need not remember these rapid
An animal in the wild does not always have time to con- shifts of attention. In reply to this criticism, the sublimi-
template the full semantic ramifications of, for example,
nal emotional Stroop task was introduced. In contrast to
the sound of rustling leaves nearby. Rapid organisation
of defensive responses on the basis of incomplete stim- a resource-limited paradigm, the Stroop task is a data-
ulus identification may make the difference between life limited paradigm, which require processing of physically
and death. The cost of a false positive response in such a impoverished stimuli that cannot be consciously identi-
situation is far less than the cost of a false negative. The fied (Norman & Bobrow, 1975). In the subliminal Stroop
defensive flight or fight response can always be aborted if
task, emotional words are presented so briefly (e.g. 20 ms)
more detailed processing indicates that the stimulus is not
really threatening or at least not predicting real danger. that they cannot be perceived consciously. Each stimu-
lus is followed by a masking stimulus of the same color
Hence, the finding that spider phobics showed a simi- as the emotional word. This is done to prevent afterim-
lar SA to spider pictures as to spider words, can be in- ages, and hence awareness of the emotional words. The
terpreted in terms of an incomplete but rapid decision- subject has to name the color of the masking stimulus as
making process—both stimulus categories are threatening quickly as possible. Various studies have revealed that both
enough. The degree to which the stimuli are considered to nonclinically and clinically anxious individuals have SA
be threatening, plays no role in this adaptive function of for threatening information presented subliminally (below
SA. The latter requires refined semantic analysis, which the perception threshold). As expected, clinically anxious
can be postponed till after SA did its job of alerting the individuals suffering from a generalized anxiety disorder
individual. The suggestion is therefore that, under the in- also showed an attention preference for supraliminally
fluence of anxiety, the allocation of processing capacity to presented stimuli (Mogg, Bradley, Williams, & Mathews,
threatening stimuli is an automatic process. And automatic 1993; Bradley, Mogg, Millar, & White, 1995). Nonclini-
processing does not generally require consciousness. Sev- cally anxious individuals, on the other hand, showed only
eral studies have been conducted on SA toward stimuli that SA for the subliminal stimuli, and not for the supraliminal
are supposed to be inaccessible to phenomenal awareness. stimuli (Van Den Hout, Tenney, Huygens, Merckelbach,
The first study on unconscious SA was done us- & Kindt, 1995; MacLeod & Hagan, 1992; MacLeod &
ing a so-called dichotic listening task. This task involves Rutherford, 1992). Hence, it appears that all anxious in-
the simultaneous presentation of threatening and neutral dividuals give priority to threatening information at the
auditive information. The subject is asked to “shadow” unconscious level. SA may result from a rapid and auto-
the neutral words presented to one ear (i.e. to repeat the matic analysis of the threat stimulus. As long as the feared
words loudly) and to ignore the threatening words in the cues remain unconscious, all anxious people, regardless of
other ear. This task is based upon the principle of lim- whether they suffer from a clinical disorder, seem to pref-
ited information-processing capacity, and is referred to as erentially process threat information. When the stimulus
a resource-limited paradigm (Norman & Bobrow, 1975). information becomes conscious, however, nonclinically
Although both the threatening and the nonthreatening anxious people may be able to inhibit selective process-
words might be consciously perceived, the capacity over- ing, whilst clinically anxious people may not be able to
load on the shadowed message forces the nonshadowed do so. Thus, with regard to attention, it appears that both
information below the perimeter of awareness (McNally, clinically anxious people and anxious people who do not
1995). Thus, in the dichotic listening task, the stimuli (i.e. suffer from straightforward clinical anxiety do not differ in
threatening words) on which the attention is not focussed responding to subliminal threat information—regardless
are supposed to be registered unconsciously. of clinical status, threat information is preferentially pro-
Mathews and MacLeod (1986) used such a dichotic cessed. Meanwhile, the two groups differ in the reaction to
listening task to show that patients with a generalized anx- supraliminally perceived threat cues. Although nonclini-
iety disorder exhibit SA for threatening information. The cally anxious people tend to strategically inhibit selective
Selective Attention and Anxiety 197

processing of threat, clinical patients seem unable to do so. experimentally induced or not—leads to the occurrence or
In conclusion, it is the response to consciously perceived intensification of SA. This type of research was described
rather than unconsciously perceived threat that marks the earlier. But reversely, a decrease of anxiety should also
transition from nonclinical anxiety to clinical anxiety. lead to the disappearance or decrease of SA. Behavioral
therapists have become quite successful in treating anxiety
disorders. Some anxiety disorders require between 10 and
SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND ANXIETY: 20 sessions. Single phobias like spider phobia may be sat-
THE CAUSAL DIRECTION isfactorily reduced after a single treatment session. Does
SA disappear after a successful treatment of anxiety?
The relationship between selective attention for In 1986, English researchers first established the ef-
threat and anxiety has become one of the best documented fects of therapy on SA (Watts, McKenna, Sharrock, &
phenomena in experimental psychopathology. But what is Trezise, 1986). Spider phobics took part in a 6-week
the causal status of SA? Is it a result of anxiety? Or does group therapy treatment. The treatment consisted of the
it cause anxiety. Or are both alternatives valid? method of “systematic desensitization” that was still ex-
tensively used at that time. This method consists of a
combination of relaxation and exposure to threatening
Effects of Anxiety on Selective Attention stimuli. Contrary to expectations, the treatment resulted
in a significant decrease of SA.4 Following Watts et al.,
Research on the effects of anxiety on SA initially the therapeutic effect of SA has been repeated for var-
yielded contradictory results. The relative contribution of ious anxiety disorders and different forms of treatment.
state anxiety to SA and anxiety as a personality charac- A reduction of SA was found, for example, in spider
teristic are unclear. Mathews and MacLeod (1985), for phobics after a 2.5-hr exposure treatment (Lavy et al.,
example, found that SA was best predicted by state anx- 1993), in patients with a generalized anxiety disorder af-
iety, whereas Mogg et al. (1989) found that trait anxiety ter seven sessions in which relaxation, cognitive coping
was the best indicator. A follow-up study by the same strategies, and gradual exposure were used (Mathews,
authors (MacLeod & Mathews, 1988) concentrated sys- Mogg, Kentish, & Eysenck, 1995), and in social phobics
tematically on the relative contribution of both factors. who had taken 12 weeks of cognitive Behavioral ther-
In individuals with low and with high trait anxiety, SA apy (Mattia, Heimberg, & Hope, 1993). In the last study,
was determined twice by means of the VDT: once a few it was also shown that only patients who had benefited
weeks before an important examination, so at a time of from the treatment exhibited a decrease of SA. Therapy
low state anxiety, and once during the week of the exam- also appears to have an effect on unconscious SA (Van
ination, at a time of high state anxiety. During the first Den Hout et al., 1997; Mogg, Bradley, Millar, & White,
session, neither of the groups exhibited SA. During the 1995).
second session, however, the high anxiety group exhib- The above-mentioned results and the studies de-
ited clear SA for examination-relevant threatening words, scribed earlier support the option that anxiety leads to SA:
whereas in the low anxiety group, attention was averted when anxiety increases, SA occurs (at least in individuals
from these stimuli. At the same time, state anxiety had who are trait anxious). In anxious persons who manage to
increased to a similar extent in both groups between the overcome their anxieties by means of behavioral therapy,
first and the second session. This study therefore showed SA decreases; this applies to SA for threatening informa-
that SA was best predicted by the two factors together, and tion that can be perceived consciously, but also for threat-
that neither state anxiety by itself, nor anxiety as a person- ening stimuli that are so “degraded” that they cannot be
ality characteristic by itself was sufficient to induce SA. perceived. These findings are interesting and behavioral
This result has been repeated by other researchers (e.g.
Broadbent & Broadbent, 1988). With respect to the ques- 4 This finding was not only of clinical importance, but also has theoretical
tion of the causal relationship between anxiety and SA, consequences. There has always beeen an alternative explanation for
these findings suggest a shaded pattern. Neither state anx- SA: the “familiarity hypothesis.” Anxious individuals would simply be
iety nor trait anxiety appears to be sufficient to be able to more familiar with the threatening stimuli and hence processing such
observe SA. SA may only occur if the two conditions are stimuli would be more automatic. The degree of automatized process-
combined. ing, after all, also has an effect on the regular Stroop effect. The more
familiar a word is, the slower the response (Klein, 1964). The study
The question whether anxiety leads to SA can, in by Watts et al. undermines this familiarity hypothesis. It shows that
principle, be studied in two ways. Firstly, the assumed re- the greater familiarity with spider stimuli (training in therapy) in fact
lationship suggests that an increase of anxiety—whether decreases SA.
198 Kindt and Van Den Hout

therapists were pleased to observe that antianxiety ther- responses to migration to Australia for further education
apy at the same time decreases SA. Theoretically, how- (MacLeod, 1999, p. 469).
ever, this suggests a disappointing conclusion: SA seems Although all three studies seem to provide some sup-
to be an epiphenomenon of anxiety. port for the hypothesis that SA is a vulnerability factor,
strictly speaking, the data do not allow for conclusions on
causation. All three studies were correlational and do not
Effects of Selective Attention on Anxiety exclude the operation of third variables: prediction does
not imply causation. Highly interesting, however, are re-
The first major study to investigate whether SA is cent studies, in which for the first time the causal relation-
in fact a vulnerability factor, was conducted by MacLeod ship between SA and anxiety responses were experimen-
and Hagan (1992). They investigated whether SA would tally investigated. In the first study, SA was manipulated
predict stronger negative responses to a stressful medical by training normal subjects through the use of a modi-
event. Thirty-one women awaiting colposcopy investiga- fied dot-probe task (MacLeod, Ebsworthy, & Rutherford,
tion, were screened on both self-report measures of emo- submitted). Each trial briefly presented a word pair, con-
tional vulnerability, and on an emotional Stroop. In 15 of taining a threat word and a nonthreat word. Following
these patients, cervical pathology was diagnosed subse- this word pair presentation, a linear probe stimulus al-
quently. Eight weeks after SA was assessed, the affective ways appeared in one of the two screen locations, and this
responses of these 15 patients to the life-threatening di- line could slant either left or right. Subjects were required
agnosis were determined. SA for subliminal threat stim- to rapidly discriminate the slope of each probe. For one
uli appeared to be the best predictor for the dysphoric group, virtually all the probes appeared in the same screen
responses elicited by the subsequent diagnosis of cervi- location as the preceding threat word. These subjects were
cal pathology. Contrary to the self-reporting methods that trained to focus attention to the threatening stimuli. For
were administered in the first assessment session (BDI; the other group, in contrast, virtually all these probes ap-
Beck, 1967; STAI; Spielberger et al., 1970), SA predicted peared in the opposing screen location to that previously
not only anxiety symptoms, but also depression symptoms occupied by the threat word, and it was intended that this
as reactions to the subsequent diagnosis. These emotional would induce attentional avoidance of the threat stimuli.
reactions were assessed by a Postdiagnostic Mood Assess- Following this attentional training procedure, all subjects
ment Questionnaire developed by the authors. Although were exposed to a stress manipulation that involved their
this concerns a correlational study that does not bypass the being videotaped while completing a difficult anagram
third variable problem, the study was at least prospective: task, under timed conditions and while receiving failure
it is impossible that the subsequent anxiety and depression feedback. This manipulation served to elevate dysphoric
caused the SA observed earlier. It may imply that the pres- mood state across both training groups. The crucial finding
ence of SA is a vulnerability factor in stressful situations. was, however, that the magnitude of this dysphoric mood
On the basis of the finding that SA was a better predic- response was attenuated for those subjects who had ex-
tor than the self-reporting measures, it may be advisable perienced the training procedure that induced attentional
to determine cognitive styles also by means of objective avoidance of threat. Recently, this finding has been repli-
Behavioral measurements instead of only using subjective cated (MacLeod, 1999). Note that the emotional effect
measurements. of attention training was not visible during and immedi-
The fact that the response to unconsciously perceived ately after the attention training, but was only observed
words is a good predictor of the response to stress, was in a subsequent stressful situation. These studies convinc-
not only observed in Australian gynaecology patients. A ingly demonstrate that SA is causally related to emotional
highly similar effect was observed in the Netherlands (Van vulnerability.
Den Hout et al., 1995). In the latter case, emotional re-
sponses were not measured in a stressful situation, but
subjects were asked to imagine themselves in 15 differ- DEVELOPMENT OF ANXIETY AND SELECTIVE
ent stressful situations and to indicate how they would ATTENTION DURING CHILDHOOD
respond. This study also showed that unconscious SA was
the best predictor of the degree of negative emotional re- The question whether SA plays a role in the etiology
sponses to stressful situations. of anxiety disorders can also be approached from a devel-
A similar correlational link was recently found in opmental perspective. Studying the development of SA
students from Singapore (MacLeod & Ng, submitted). SA and anxiety during childhood can provide useful informa-
proved to be a successful predictor of negative emotional tion. If SA for threatening information is already present in
Selective Attention and Anxiety 199

anxious children, it shouldn’t of course prove the hypoth- version, in which the stimuli were presented one by one.
esis that SA plays a pathogenic role. It would, however, at In the card version, the stimuli were presented within a
least justify a prospective study that investigates the role context of the same emotional valence, which could rein-
of SA in the development of anxiety disorders in adult- force the emotional impact of the stimuli (McNally, Amir,
hood. And if SA is not yet present in anxious children, & Lipke, 1996). The differences between the samples and
this would be a reason for modifying the theory. Stroop versions only explain that, contrary to Martin et al.,
Research among nonclinically low and high anxi- no differential effect between high and low anxiety chil-
ety children (8–9 years) in a stressful situation (prior to dren was observed in the Kindt et al. study. However, they
vaccination) showed that the anxious children exhibited do not provide a satisfactory explanation for the finding
SA for words relating to the medical situation (e.g. in- that even low anxiety children show SA for threat.
jection, fainting; Kindt, Brosschot, & Everaerd, 1997). A Other research on SA in children also illustrated dif-
surprising finding, however, was that low anxiety children ferences between high and low anxious children. In chil-
also exhibited this SA. This result was replicated in a sec- dren and adolescents (aged 9–17) suffering from PTSD,
ond experiment, in which similar groups were selected. an anxiety-related bias was observed (Moradi, Taghavi,
This time, the SA in a neutral situation was measured. Neshat-Doost, Yule, & Dalgleish, 1999). In this study,
The observation that low anxiety children also gave prior- children and adolescents showed increased Stroop inter-
ity to threatening information, contradicts earlier findings ference on trauma-related words. In contrast to the previ-
among adults and suggests a cognitive development hy- ous Stroop studies in children, these subjects suffered from
pothesis (Kindt, Brosschot, & Everaerd, 1997). During an anxiety disorder. Also in the laboratory of the authors, a
the development, the information-processing capacity in- moderate association between spider phobia and selective
creases and cognitive skills are acquired in order to regu- attention in children (aged 8–12) was observed, using a
late emotional responses. A modified Stroop task may tap Stroop task in which words were presented on colored cir-
the functioning of this regulating mechanism. The sub- cles (Kindt & Brosschot, 1999). Moreover, several other
jects are explicitly asked to ignore the content of words. studies also showed SA in anxious children (Bijttebier,
Children, whether anxious or not, may lack the skills to 1998; Taghavi, Neshat-Doost, Moradi, Yule, & Dalgleish,
inhibit the SA for emotionally relevant information. This 1999; Vasey, Daleiden, Williams, & Brown, 1995; Vasey,
lack may be due to their restricted processing capacity, or Elhag, & Daleiden, 1996). These studies did not make use
the lack of experience with threatening stimuli. of the emotional Stroop task, but used the VDT described
Although the observations by Kindt et al. seem to earlier. Two studies found that SA was only present in the
indicate that there is no relation between anxiety and SA clinically anxious children (Taghavi et al., 1999; Vasey
in children of 8–9 years old, Martin, Horder and Jones et al., 1995). In other studies, nonclinically test-anxious
(1992) observed SA in spider phobic children of 6–13 children also exhibited SA (Vasey et al., 1996), as well
years old. Martin and Jones (1995) replicated this effect as children with a high “trait anxiety” (Bijttebier, 1998).
in children aged 4–9, using a variation of the card format of It should be noted that the children in these studies were
the modified Stroop task (i.e. pictorial stimuli). There are, generally older than in the Kindt et al. studies, varying
however, two important differences between our studies from 9 to 18 years old. It must also be said that Bijttebier
and the ones by Martin et al. that could explain the diverg- only found indications for SA when the anxious children
ing findings. First, the type of anxiety that was studied were in stressful situations, when they were in hospital
differed between the two studies. SA appears to be a ro- prior to medical examination. In a neutral situation, the
bust finding particularly in specific fears, such as phobias low anxiety children also exhibited SA. This finding thus
(for an overview, see Williams et al., 1996). On the other partly matches the Kindt et al. findings. Hence, research on
hand, SA is not always found in high anxiety persons who anxiety-related bias in children do not produce unequivo-
do not fear specific situations (Fox, 1994; MacLeod & cal observations. One explanation for the conflicting pat-
Rutherford, 1992; Martin, Williams, & Clark, 1991). In the terns of results is that the children in the studies in which
case of specific fears, the fear-inducing stimuli are unam- an anxiety-related SA was revealed were older than in the
biguous and hence it is easier for the researcher to present studies in which this relation was absent.
stimuli that selectively activate the cognitive fear network. In order to obtain greater insight in the development
It is therefore possible that matching the Stroop stimuli to of SA in children, a follow-up study was carried out among
the fear of the subjects was more successful in the study by spider phobic children (8–12 years; Kindt, Bierman, &
Martin et al. than in the Kindt et al. study. The other differ- Brosschot, 1997). In this study, the number of subjects was
ence between the two studies was that Martin et al. used large enough to allow for examination of age-effects. SA
a card format, whereas Kindt et al. used a computerised was established by means of both versions of the Stroop
200 Kindt and Van Den Hout

task. In line with our earlier results, SA was found both of this SA. Individuals who develop an anxiety disorder
in spider phobic and in control children, regardless of the could have difficulty learning to inhibit this SA.
Stroop version. More interesting is the observation that
SA appeared to decrease in normal children as they were
older, whereas in the anxious group SA maintained with CONCLUSION
age. This result need not be inconsistent with the obser-
vations by, for instance, Vasey et al. (1996) in nonclin- In the past two decades, dozens of studies have been
ically anxious individuals in the age group between 11 carried out on the relationship between SA and anxiety.
and 14. Kindt et al. found that SA in the spider phobic The presence of such an association is a robust finding.
group maintained with age (8–12 years), whereas it de- The rationale of these studies was that SA plays a role in
creased in the control group. This result has recently been the maintenance and etiology of anxiety disorders. The
replicated in children of 8–11 years old (Kindt, Van Den question addressed in this paper was to what extent this
Hout, de Jong, & Hoekstra, 2000). In the light of these supposition is justified. Research shows that when individ-
results, it must be doubted whether SA is properly re- uals are anxious, they show more attention toward threat.
lated to the development of anxiety disorders. SA in itself However, this does not apply to everyone. Only people
may not be a risk factor, but that inadequate inhibiting of with a general tendency toward anxiety exhibit this so-
SA puts one at risk. In other words inhibiting SA must called attention priority. SA in turn a reinforcing effect on
be learnt. Anxious adults could be characterized by in- the anxiety level. There is some evidence that suggests a
adequate cognitive control when confronted with threat- reciprocal relationship between anxiety and SA: anxiety
ening information. They may be unable to suppress the reinforces SA and SA reinforces anxiety. Hence the as-
tendency to give priority to threatening information, even sertion that SA plays a role in the maintenance of anxiety
though they are required to do so. A study by MacLeod disorder seems to be justified.
and Mathews (1991b) did indeed find that anxiety only Studies among children, however, casts doubt upon
leads to SA when the subject is confronted with two si- the assumption that SA in itself (i.e. as separate from in-
multaneous stimuli and priority had to be given to one hibition) is responsible for the development of anxiety
stimulus while the other stimulus had to be ignored. When disorders. The fact that all young children give priority
the subjects received single stimuli, no effect of anxiety to threatening information illustrates that SA is first and
was observed. According to these authors, the cognitive foremost a normal phenomenon. As age increases, SA de-
effect of anxiety may best be described as a priority for creases among nonanxious children, whereas it maintains
the processing of certain stimuli. Because of the limited among anxious ones. The authors suggest that normal chil-
processing capacity, this has a negative effect on the pro- dren learn to inhibit SA. Anxious children, on the other
cessing of other information. According to development hand, would not learn to inhibit SA. The observation that
psychologists, (Flavell, 1985) the processing capacity in SA decreases with age in normal children and increases
children is smaller than in adults. Hence our findings that in anxious children is compatible with two explanations:
SA is equal in low anxiety children and in high anxiety (1) that SA is an epiphenomenon of anxiety and (2) that
children, could be the result of an age-related lack of cog- not learning to inhibit SA is a vulnerability factor. Con-
nitive capacity. This explanation of the results is also in line sidering the finding that SA has a reinforcing effect on
with the general emotion theories, which state that emo- the anxiety level, the authors suggest that anxiety in child-
tions primarily affect the control over cognitive processes hood → not learning to inhibit SA → anxiety disorder in
(Oatley & Johnson-Laird, 1987; MacLeod & Mathews, adulthood. This may imply that SA indirectly plays a role
1991b). in the etiology of anxiety disorders.
It is not possible yet to draw an unequivocal picture Cognitive research on anxiety has provided useful
of the findings in the field of SA in children. However, the knowledge on a variety of dysfunctional aspects of SA.
fact that the results are not unequivocal at least justifies The insights would not have been gained if researchers
the conclusion that, if there is any relationship between had solely relied on introspective measuring methods. In
anxiety and SA in children, this relationship is a weak several aspects, there appears to be a clear differentia-
one, in particular among nonclinically anxious children of tion between results obtained with information-processing
8–10 years old. It is therefore contrary to the hypothesis procedures and the results obtained with subjective anxi-
that SA plays a causal role in the etiology of anxiety dis- ety measurements. Self-report measures of anxiety do not
orders. Our results suggest that if SA plays any role at all always concur with observations of SA. That is, absence
in the development of anxiety disorders, it is not the SA of self-reported fear may concur with observed SA in chil-
in itself that is a vulnerability factor, but the continuation dren. And the presence of self-reported fear may concur
Selective Attention and Anxiety 201

with the absence of SA in nonclinically anxious individu- by a card format and a single-trial format of the Stroop task. Journal
als. Furthermore, SA may under certain circumstances be a of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 163–179.
Kindt, M., & Brosschot, J. F. (1997). Phobia-related cognitive bias for
better predictor of emotional responses than the self-report pictorial and linguistic stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
measurements. Thus, the use of information-processing 106, 644–648.
paradigms seems to be a fruitful addition to the self-report Kindt, M., & Brosschot, J. F. (1999). Cognitive bias in spider-phobic
children: Comparison of a pictorial and a linguistic spider Stroop.
measurements in order to gain insights in the pathologic Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Asessment, 21, 207–
mechanisms of anxiety disorders. Hardly any research has 220.
been done on the practical/clinical usefulness of tests for Kindt, M., Brosschot, J. F. , & Everaerd, W. (1997). Cognitive processing
bias of children in a real life stress situation and a neutral situation.
SA. This is undeserved and such research may be highly Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64, 79–97.
promising. Kindt, M., Van Den Hout, M. A, Jong, de P., & Hoekstra, B. (2000).
Cognitive bias for pictorial and linguistic threat cues in children.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 22, 201–
219.
REFERENCES Klein, G. S. (1964). Semantic power measured through the interference
of words with color naming. American Journal of Psychology, 77,
576–588.
Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatment. Pennsylvania: Lang, P. J. (1977). Imagery in therapy: An information processing anal-
University of Pennsylvania Press. ysis of fear. Behavior Therapy, 8, 862–886.
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New Lang, P. J. (1979). A bio-informational theory of emotional imagery.
York: International University Press. Psychophysiology, 16, 495–512.
Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. L. (1986). Anxiety disorders Lavy, E., & Van Den Hout, M. (1993). Selective attention evidenced by
and phobias: A cognitive perspective. New York: Basic Books. pictorial and linguistic Stroop tasks. Behavior Therapy, 24, 645–
Bijttebier, P. (1998). Monitoring and blunting coping styles in children. 657.
Unpublished thesis, University of Louvain, Belgium. Lavy, E., Van Den Hout, M., & Arntz, A. (1993). Attentional bias and
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, spider phobia: Conceptual and clinical issues. Behaviour Research
129–148. and Therapy, 31, 17–24.
Bower, G. H. (1987). Commentary on mood and memory. Behaviour Lavy, E., van Oppen, P., & Van Den Hout, M. (1994). Selective pro-
Research and Therapy, 25, 443–456. cessing of emotional information in obsessive–compulsive disorder.
Bower, G. H., & Forgas, J. P. (2000). Affect, memory and social cogni- Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 243–246.
tion. In E. Eich, J. F. Kihlstrom, G. H. Bower, J. P. Forgas, & P. M. LeDoux, J. E. (1990). Information flow from sensation to emotion: Plas-
Niedenthal (Eds.), Cognition and emotion (pp. 87–168). London: ticity in the neural computation of stimulus value. In M. Gabriel
Oxford University Press. & J. Moore (Eds.), Learning and computational neuroscience:
Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Millar, N., & White, J. (1995). Selective pro- Foundations of adaptive networks (pp. 3–52). Cambridge, MA:
cessing of negative information: Effects of clinical anxiety, concur- MIT Press.
rent depression, and awareness. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, MacLeod, C. (1991). Clinical anxiety and the selective encoding of
104, 532–536. threatening information. International Review of Psychiatry, 3,
Broadbent, D. E., & Broadbent, M. (1988). Anxiety and attentional bias: 279–292.
State and trait. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 165-183. MacLeod, C. (1995). The information-processing approach to emotional
Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of pathology: Adding a cognitive core to the best of behaviourism
automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of (Keynote). In International Conference on Cognitive and Behav-
the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332–361. ioral Therapy (Abstract, p. 24). Copenhagen, Denmark.
Driver, J., & Tipper, S. P. (1989). On the nonselectivity of selective MacLeod, C. (1998). Attentional Bias and Anxiety Vulnerability. In
seeing: Contrasts between interference and priming in selective at- World Congress of Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies. Acapulco,
tention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception Mexico (Abstract, p. 106).
and Performance, 15, 304–314. MacLeod, C. (1999). Anxiety and anxiety disorders. In T. Dalgleish &
Flavell, J. H. (1985). Cognitive development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: M. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 447–
Prentice-Hall. 477). New York: Wiley.
Foa, E. B., Ilai, D., McCarthy, P. R., Shoyer, B., Murdock, T. (1993). In- MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect:
formation processing in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Cognitive An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203.
Therapy and Research, 17, 173–189. MacLeod, C. M., Ebsworthy, G., & Rutherford, E. M. Selective attention
Fox, E. (1994). Attentional bias in anxiety: A defective inhibition hy- and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their
pothesis. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 165–195. association through the experimental induction of attentional bias.
Geller, V., & Shaver, P. (1976). Cognitive consequences of self- Manuscript submitted for publication.
awareness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12, 99– MacLeod, C., & Hagan, R. (1992). Individual differences in the selective
108. processing of threatening information and emotional responses to a
Holender, D. (1986). Semantic activation without conscious identifica- stressful life event. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30, 151–161.
tion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 9, 1–66. MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1988). Anxiety and the allocation of
Hope, D. A., Rapee, R. M., Heimberg, R. G., & Dombeck, M. J. (1990). attention to threat. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Representations of the self in social phobia: Vulnerability to social A, Human Experimental Psychology, 38, 659–610.
threat. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 177–189. MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. M. (1991a). Cognitive-experimental ap-
Kindt, M., Bierman, D., & Brosschot, J. F. (1996). Stroop versus Stroop: proaches to the emotional disorders. In P. R. Martin (Ed.), Hand-
Comparison of a card format and a single-trial format of the standard book of behavior therapy and psychological science: An integrative
color–word Stroop task and the emotional Stroop task. Personality approach (pp. 116–150). New York: Pergamon.
and Individual Differences, 21, 653–661. MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1991b). Biased cognitive operations in
Kindt, M., Bierman, D., & Brosschot, J. F. (1997). Cognitive bias in spi- anxiety: Accessibility of information or assignment of processing
der fear and control children: Assessment of emotional interference priorities? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29, 599–610.
202 Kindt and Van Den Hout

MacLeod, C., & Ng, V. Initial attentional bias to threat predicts later PTSD on the Stroop color-naming task. Psychological Medicine,
emotional response to acculturation stress. Manuscript submitted 29, 415–419.
for publication. Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1975). On data-limited and resource-
MacLeod, C., & Rutherford, E. N. (1992). Anxiety and the selective limited processes. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 44–64.
processing of threatening information: Mediating roles of aware- Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). Toward a cognitive theory of
ness, trait and state variables, and personal relevance of stimulus emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 29–50.
materials. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30, 479–491. Ray, C. (1979). Examination stress and performance on a color word
Martin, M., Horder, P., & Jones, G. V. (1992). Integral bias in naming of interference test. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 49, 400–402.
phobia-related words: Brief report. Cognition and Emotion, 6(6), Riemann, B., & McNally, R. J. (1995). Cognitive processing of person-
479–486. ally relevant information. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 325–340.
Martin, M., & Jones, G. V. (1995). Integral bias in the cognitive process- Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual
ing of emotionally linked pictures. British Journal of Psychology, for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting
86, 419–435. Psychologists Press.
Martin, M., Williams, R. M., & Clark, D. M. (1991). Does anxiety lead Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.
to selective processing of threat-related information? Behaviour Journal of Experimental Psychology, 225, 643–662.
Research and Therapy, 29, 147–160. Thrasher, S. M., Dalgleish, T., Yule, W. (1994). Information processing
Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1985). Selective processing of threat in posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
cues in anxiety states. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 563– 32, 247–254.
569. Taghavi, M. R., Neshat-Doost, H. T., Moradi, A. R., Yule, W., &
Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1986). Discrimination of threat cues with- Dalgleish, T. (1999). Biases in visual attention in children and adole-
out awareness in anxiety states. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, cents with clinical anxiety and mixed anxiety-depression. Journal
95, 131–138. of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 215–223.
Mathews, A., Mogg, K., Kentish, J., & Eysenck, M. (1995). Effect of Van Den Hout, M. A., Tenney, N., Huygens, K., Merckelbach, H., &
psychological treatment on cognitive bias in generalized anxiety Kindt, M. (1995). Responding to subliminal threat cues is related
disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 293–303. to trait anxiety and emotional vulnerability: A successful replication
Mattia, J. I., Heimberg, R. G., & Hope, D. A. (1993). The revised Stroop of MacLeod and Hagan (1992). Behaviour Research and Therapy,
color-naming task in social phobics. Behaviour Research and Ther- 33, 451–454.
apy, 31, 305–313. Van Den Hout, M. A., Tenney, N., Huygens, K., & de Jong, P. J. (1997).
McNally, R. J. (1995). Automaticity and anxiety disorders. Behaviour Preconscious procesing bias in specific phobia. Behaviour Research
Research and Therapy, 33, 747–754. and Therapy, 35, 29–34.
McNally, R. J., Amir, N., & Lipke, H. J. (1996). Subliminal processing Vasey, M. W., Daleiden, E. L., Williams, L. L., & Brown, L. (1995). Bi-
of threat cues in post traumatic stress disorder? Journal of Anxiety ased attention in childhood anxiety disorders: A preliminary study.
Disorders, 10, 115–128. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 267–279.
McNally, R. J., Riemann, B. C., & Kim, E. (1990). Selective processing Vasey, M. W., Elhag, N., Daleiden, E. (1996). Anxiety and the processing
of threat cues in panic disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, of emotionally-threatening stimuli: Distinctive patterns of selective
28, 407–412. attention among high-and low-test-anxious children. Child Devel-
McNally, R. J., & Steketee, G. S. (1985). The etiology and maintenance opment, 67, 1173–1185.
of severe animal phobias. Behavior research and Therapy, 23, 431– Warren, R. E. (1972). Stimulus encoding and memory. Journal of Ex-
435. perimental Psychology, 94, 90–100.
Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Millar, N., & White, J. (1995). A follow-up Warren, R. E. (1974). Association, directionality and stimulus encoding.
study of cognitive bias in generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 151–158.
Research and Therapy, 33, 927–935. Watts, F. N., McKenna, F. P., Sharrock, R., & Trezise, L. (1986). Color
Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Williams, R., & Mathews, A. (1993). Sublimi- naming of phobia-related words. British Journal of Psychology, 77,
nal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression. 97–108.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 304–311. Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional
Mogg, K., Mathews, A., & Weinman, J. (1989). Selective processing of Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3–
threat cues in anxiety states: A replication. Behaviour Research and 24.
Therapy, 27, 317–323. Williams, J. M. G., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1997).
Moradi, A. R., Taghavi, M. R., Neshat Doost, H. T., Yule, W., & Cognitive psychology and the emotional disorders. Chichester, NY:
Dalgleish, T. (1999). Performance of children and adolecents with Wiley.

You might also like