Tan 2015

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

International Psychogeriatrics (2015), 27:10, 1649–1660 

C International Psychogeriatric Association 2015


doi:10.1017/S1041610215000939

REVIEW
Can clock drawing differentiate Alzheimer’s disease from other
dementias?
...........................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................

Lynnette Pei Lin Tan,1 Nathan Herrmann,2 Brian J. Mainland3 and Kenneth Shulman4
1
Department of Psychiatry, Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, Canada; Department of Psychological Medicine, Tan Tock Seng Hospital, Singapore
2
Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, Canada; Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto,
Canada
3
Department of Psychology, Ryerson University, Toronto, Canada
4
Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, Canada; Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Canada

ABSTRACT

Background: Studies have shown the clock-drawing test (CDT) to be a useful screening test that differentiates
between normal, elderly populations, and those diagnosed with dementia. However, the results of studies
which have looked at the utility of the CDT to help differentiate Alzheimer’s disease (AD) from other
dementias have been conflicting. The purpose of this study was to explore the utility of the CDT in
discriminating between patients with AD and other types of dementia.
Methods: A review was conducted using MEDLINE, PsycINFO, and Embase. Search terms included clock
drawing or CLOX and dementia or Parkinson’s Disease or AD or dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) or
vascular dementia (VaD).
Results: Twenty studies were included. In most of the studies, no significant differences were found in
quantitative CDT scores between AD and VaD, DLB, and Parkinson’s disease dementia (PDD) patients.
However, frontotemporal dementia (FTD) patients consistently scored higher on the CDT than AD patients.
Qualitative analyses of errors differentiated AD from other types of dementia.
Conclusions: Overall, the CDT score may be useful in distinguishing between AD and FTD patients, but
shows limited value in differentiating between AD and VaD, DLB, and PDD. Qualitative analysis of the type
of CDT errors may be a useful adjunct in the differential diagnosis of the types of dementias.

Key words: clock-drawing test, Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia, Parkinson’s dementia, dementia with Lewy bodies, frontotemporal
dementia

Introduction execution (praxis), and executive function (Royall


et al., 1998; Shulman, 2000). While the literature
The CDT is a simple and valid screening on the use of the CDT in screening for the
tool for dementia (Shulman, 2000; Pinto and syndrome of dementia is extensive (Sunderland
Peters, 2009), correlating highly with global et al., 1989; Wolf-Klein et al., 1989; Mendez
cognitive function (Wolf-Klein et al., 1989; et al., 1992; Rouleau et al., 1992; Shulman,
Brodaty and Moore, 1997). A wide range of 2000), it is not clear whether the CDT is
cognitive functions is assessed by the CDT, valid for discriminating between the subtypes of
including orientation, selective and sustained dementias.
attention, auditory comprehension, verbal working Due to the variety of skills required for successful
memory, numerical knowledge, visual memory and completion of the CDT, specific errors have been
reconstruction, visuospatial organization, motor linked to different types of dementias (Eknoyan
et al., 2012). Hence, it would be reasonable to
assume that scores and the pattern of errors on
Correspondence should be addressed to: Lynnette Pei Lin Tan, Psychological
the CDT would differ between different dementia
Medicine Department, Tan Tock Seng Hospital, Annex Building Level 3, 11 and hence the CDT may aid in the differential
Jalan Tan Tock Seng, Singapore 308433, Singapore. Phone: +65 63577841; diagnosis of dementia. The type of clock-drawing
Fax: +65 63573088. Email: lynnette_tan@ttsh.com.sg. Received 14 Feb 2015;
revision requested 8 Apr 2015; revised version received 12 May 2015; accepted
errors can contribute to the clinical evaluation of
22 May 2015. First published online 3 July 2015. patients with suspected neuropsychiatric disorders
1650 L. P. L. Tan et al.

and syndromes as these errors likely represent Method


different neuropsychological functions (Eknoyan
et al., 2012). There are two general scoring Study design
approaches – qualitative (Rouleau et al., 1992; A literature search of electronic databases, MED-
Kozora and Cullum, 1994; Libon et al., 1996) and LINE, PSYCINFO, and EMBASE was conducted.
quantitative (Sunderland et al., 1989; Mendez et al., Searches included the following keywords – clock
1992; Shulman et al., 1993; Watson et al., 1993). drawing or CLOX and dementia or Parkinson’s
There have been at least 19 CDT scoring systems Disease or AD or DLB or VaD. The search was
described since its use in the 1960s (Mainland et al., carried out in November 2013 and no limitation
2014). Rouleau et al. and Libon et al. were among on publication date was used. Citations from the
the first to devise qualitative scoring systems for the reference lists of previously gathered articles were
CDT (Rouleau et al., 1992; Libon et al., 1996). also manually searched to ensure that significant
They found that patients with AD demonstrated work would not be missed. To determine the
different qualitative errors compared to patients usefulness of the CDT in differentiating AD from
with subcortical deficits, such as Huntington’s the other subtypes of dementia and since AD is
disease and ischemic VaD. The types of qualitative the most common form of dementia, studies which
errors that can be found also depend on the specifically compared performance on the CDT
method used for clock drawing, e.g. whether or between patients with AD and other dementias
not a pre-drawn circle is used (Rouleau et al., were selected. Studies were assessed for inclusion
1992; Libon et al., 1996). In one of the most based on the following criteria: (1) studies that
commonly used systems of evaluating qualitative used the CDT, independent of the scoring system
clock-drawing errors (Rouleau et al., 1992), errors applied; (2) studies that compared AD patients with
include: (i) graphical difficulties, in which lines are patients of other types of dementias; (3) diagnosis
not precise resulting in distortions of the clock face of dementia in patients were made based on
or numbers that are difficult to read; (ii) stimulus- clearly defined criteria and/or autopsy; (4) studies
bound responses, where there is the tendency of published in English.
the drawing to be dominated or guided by a single The combined searches yielded 866 potentially
stimulus e.g. the hands are set at 10–11 instead of relevant references. Where a title or an abstract
10 after 11; (iii) conceptual deficits, comprised of appeared to describe a study that included the
deficits in accessing the knowledge of the attributes, comparisons of cognitive testing in people with
features, and meaning of a clock, resulting in a AD and other types of dementias, the full article
drawing that does not look like a clock or drawing was retrieved and examined for relevance. Fifty-
with hands that do not communicate a time; (iv) three articles were retrieved and reviewed for final
spatial/planning deficits, which are errors in the inclusion; 33 of these had to be excluded because
layout of the numbers of the clock; and finally (v) they did not fulfill the selection criteria. Twenty
perseveration errors, which are the continuation or publications met the inclusion criteria. Given
recurrence of activity without appropriate stimulus the small and heterogeneous studies identified, a
(e.g. presence of more than two hands or writing quantitative review was not performed.
of numbers beyond 12 or inappropriate recurrence
of numbers). Rouleau et al. (1992) found that
patients with AD were more likely to draw larger Results
clocks than patients with Huntington’s disease,
which may be a result of poor visuospatial planning Papers were published between 1992 and 2013 and
due to impairment of executive and visuospatial were mostly conducted in the USA (Barr et al.,
functioning. Another finding was that graphical 1992; Libon et al., 1993; 1996; Rascovsky et al.,
errors were more common in Huntington’s disease 2002; Cahn-Weiner et al., 2003; Hamilton et al.,
patients than AD patients, probably as a result 2008; O’Brien et al., 2009; Wiechmann et al., 2010)
of secondary disruption of frontostriatal circuits (n = 8). Two studies were from Italy (Moretti
essential for the functions of fine motor control and et al., 2002; Cagnin et al., 2013) and two studies
planning (Rouleau et al., 1992). were from Korea (Lee et al., 2009; 2011). The
Currently, the literature reveals conflicting data remainder came from Israel (Heinik et al., 2002),
regarding the ability of the CDT to distinguish Japan (Kitabayashi et al., 2001), UK (McGuinness
patients with various types of dementia. The present et al., 2010), Brazil (Matioli and Caramelli, 2010),
qualitative review aims to determine the usefulness Saudi Arabia (Sallam and Amr, 2013), Turkey
of the CDT in differentiating AD from the other (Saka and Elibol, 2009), Sweden (Palmqvist et al.,
subtypes of dementia, including VaD, DLB, PDD, 2009), and Canada (Blair et al., 2006). Eleven
and FTD. studies (Barr et al., 1992; Libon et al., 1993; 1996;
Clock-drawing test in AD and other dementias 1651

Kitabayashi et al., 2001; Heinik et al., 2002; Moretti diagnosis of the movement disorder at least 1 year
et al., 2002; Lee et al., 2009; Matioli and Caramelli, prior to the onset of the dementia or using the
2010; McGuinness et al., 2010; Wiechmann et al., clinical diagnostic criteria for dementia associated
2010; Lee et al., 2011; Sallam and Amr, 2013) with Parkinson’s disease (Emre et al., 2007). FTD
compared CDT scores between AD and VaD was diagnosed using the Neary et al. (1998) and
patients (Table 1). Six studies (Cahn-Weiner et al., McKhann et al. (1984) criteria in one study (Blair
2003; Lee et al., 2009; O’Brien et al., 2009; Saka et al., 2006), the Lund and Manchester (Clinical
and Elibol, 2009; Lee et al., 2011; Sallam and and neuropathological criteria for frontotemporal
Amr, 2013) were identified which compared CDT dementia. The Lund and Manchester Groups,
scores between AD and PDD patients (Table 2). 1994) guidelines in another study (Moretti et al.,
Four studies (Cahn-Weiner et al., 2003; Hamilton 2002), and was autopsy confirmed in the third study
et al., 2008; Palmqvist et al., 2009; Cagnin et al., (Rascovsky et al., 2002).
2013) compared CDT scores between AD and The versions of CDT used in the studies also
DLB patients (Table 3); and three studies (Moretti differed in the methods of administration and
et al., 2002; Rascovsky et al., 2002; Blair et al., 2006) scoring. The studies included used either a blank
compared CDT scores between AD and FTD sheet of paper with instructions to draw a clock
patients (Table 4). Five of the studies (Moretti face or a pre-drawn circle for the CDT. Most
et al., 2002; Cahn-Weiner et al., 2003; Lee et al., patients were instructed to set the clock hands
2009; 2011; Sallam and Amr, 2013) compared at 10 past 11 (11:10), although other times such
CDT scores between AD patients and more than as 2:45 and 1:45 were also used less frequently
one type of dementia. Patients were recruited in the studies that utilized the Sunderland et al.
from memory clinics, geriatric assessment clinics, (1989) CDT, and the CLOX (Royall et al., 1998)
neurology clinics, and research centers. methods. Some studies used a clock-copying task
such as simple instructions to copy a clock shown
or the CLOX (Royall et al., 1998) test. Scoring
Methodological characteristics of the studies systems varied from a simple three-point system
There are considerable methodological variations (Hamilton et al., 2008) to a 15-point scoring system
in the studies included. For example, sample (Freedman, 1994; Royall et al., 1998). Qualitative
characteristics differ widely across studies. The error analyses, when included, followed the Rouleau
sample sizes varied from 50 to 235 patients, et al. system (Rouleau et al., 1992).
with samples consisting of patients with differing
severity of dementia. Diagnosis of AD was mostly
based on the NINCDS-ADRDA (McKhann et al., Differentiating AD from VaD
1984). DSM-IV (Diagnostic and statistical manual In general, quantitative scores evaluating per-
of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.), 2000) was formance on the CDT were not shown to be
used in one study (Heinik et al., 2002) and useful in differentiating AD from VaD (Table 1).
two studies (Rascovsky et al., 2002; Hamilton The findings were inconsistent across studies and
et al., 2008) had autopsy-confirmed diagnoses. although AD patients were found to perform better
The diagnosis of probable VaD was made using than VaD patients in clock copying scores in three
the NINDS-AIREN (Roman et al., 1993) criteria studies (Libon et al., 1993; 1996; Matioli and
in most studies, except one (Barr et al., 1992) Caramelli, 2010), the majority (Barr et al., 1992;
that used the DSM-III-R (Diagnostic and statistical Libon et al., 1993; 1996; Kitabayashi et al., 2001;
manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., revised.), 1987) McGuinness et al., 2010; Wiechmann et al., 2010)
criteria, two (Heinik et al., 2002; Matioli and found no significant differences in clock drawing to
Caramelli, 2010) that used the DSM-IV(Diagnostic command scores between AD and VaD patients.
and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., One study (Moretti et al., 2002) found that AD
text rev.), 2000) criteria and one study (Libon patients scored lower than VaD patients and two
et al., 1996) used the California Criteria of Chui studies (Heinik et al., 2002; Sallam and Amr, 2013)
et al. (1992). The diagnosis of DLB was made found that VaD patients scored lower than AD
according to the consensus criteria recommended patients in the CDT. However, for the four studies
by the Consortium of DLB (McKeith et al., 1996) (Libon et al., 1996; Kitabayashi et al., 2001; Lee
in all the studies except one study had autopsy- et al., 2009; 2011) that included qualitative error
confirmed diagnosis of DLB (Hamilton et al., analyses, significant differences could be identified
2008). PDD was diagnosed when the Parkinson’s between AD and VaD patients. VaD patients
disease patients met the DSM-IV (Diagnostic and appeared to demonstrate more spatial/planning
statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text deficits (Kitabayashi et al., 2001; Lee et al.,
rev.), 2000) criteria for dementia with the clinical 2009), graphical difficulties (Libon et al., 1996;
Table 1. Comparison of clock-drawing test between Alzheimer’s dementia and patients with vascular dementia

1652
AUTHORS COUNTRY PATIENTS N SCORING METHOD CLOCK-DRAWING TEST S U M M A R Y O F FI N D I N G S
............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................

L. P. L. Tan et al.
Barr et al. (1992) US AD 37 (Albert, 1980; Shulman To command, score on a No significant differences in
VaD 31 et al., 1993) 0–5 scale. (Albert, 1980) command/copy CDT scores between
To copy, score on a 0–5 VaD and AD
scale. (Albert, 1980)
Heinik et al. Israel AD 52 (Freedman, 1994) Score on a 0–15 scale Freedman’s (Freedman, 1994) scoring
(2002) VaD 36 (Freedman, 1994) method total and hands subscore
control 26 significantly more impaired in VaD than
AD
Kitabayashi et al. Japan AD 67 (Rouleau et al., 1992) Score on a 0–10 scale No differences found between very mild
(2001) VaD 44 (Rouleau et al., 1992), AD and VaD
controls 8 Qualitative error analysis In mild dementia, spatial/planning deficits
(Rouleau et al., 1992) are more frequent in VaD than AD
In moderate dementia, graphic difficulties
are more frequent in VaD than AD
Lee et al. (2009) Korea AD 71 (Wolf-Klein et al., 1989), Four scoring protocols AD made more conceptual deficit errors
VaD 39 (Shulman et al., 1993), (Wolf-Klein et al., 1989; than VaD
PDD 33 (Watson et al., 1993), Shulman et al., 1993; VaD had more type A stimulus bound
(Manos and Wu, 1994) Watson et al., 1993; response than AD (10–11 instead of 10
(Rouleau et al., 1992) Manos and Wu, 1994), after 11)
Qualitative error analysis VaD had more spatial/planning deficits and
(Rouleau et al., 1992) perseveration errors (perseveration of
numbers) than AD
Lee et al. (2011) Korea AD 94 (Manos and Wu, 1994) Score on a 0–10 scale No clear differences in longitudinal
VaD 22 (Rouleau et al., 1992) (Manos and Wu, 1994), changes on CDT scores amongst the
PDD 119 Qualitative error analysis three dementia subtypes
(Rouleau et al., 1992) AD had more conceptualization errors than
spatial/planning deficits errors after 18
months from baseline compared to VaD
Libon et al. US AD 34 Adapted from Sunderland Score on a 0–10 scale No differences in clock drawing to
(1993) VaD 30 et al. (1989) (Sunderland et al., 1989) command score between AD and VaD
controls 34 On clock copying score, AD performed
better than VaD
Libon et al. US AD 31 Adapted from Sunderland Score on a 0–10 scale No differences in clock drawing to
(1996) VaD 27 et al. (1989) (Sunderland et al., 1989) command score between AD and VaD
Adapted from (Rouleau Qualitative Scale-2 of 10 On clock copying score, AD performed
et al., 1992) and (Kozora clock-drawing error types better than VaD
and Cullum, 1994) (Rouleau et al., 1992; On clock drawing to command, VaD made
Kozora and Cullum, more graphomotor errors∗
1994) On clock copying, VaD made more
executive control errors∗∗
Table 1. Continued.
AUTHORS COUNTRY PATIENTS N SCORING METHOD CLOCK-DRAWING TEST S U M M A R Y O F FI N D I N G S
............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................

Matioli et al. Brazil AD 15 (Royall et al., 1998) CLOX-1 and CLOX-2 VaD scored worse than AD in CLOX-2
2010 (Matioli VaD 15 (Royall et al., 1998) (copy clock).
and Caramelli, controls 30 No significant difference in CLOX-1
2010) between AD and VaD
ROC curve displayed low specificity
(60.0%) for differential diagnosis
between the VaD and AD.
McGuinness UK AD 76 (Royall et al., 1998) CLOX-1 and CLOX-2 No significant difference in CLOX-1 and
et al. (2010) VaD 46 (Royall et al., 1998), CLOX-2 between AD and VaD
control 28 Drawing and copying
task, 15 point scoring
scale
Moretti et al. Italy AD 30 (Manos and Wu, 1994) Score on a 0–10 scale AD scored worst in CDT compared to
(2002) VaD 30 (Manos and Wu, 1994) VaD and FTD
FTD 30
Sallam et al. 2013 Saudi AD 51 CERAD; (Morris et al., Score on a 0–4 scale AD had significantly higher scores than
(Sallam and Arabia VaD 31 1989) (Morris et al., 1989) VaD
Amr, 2013) PDD 23 No significant differences in CDT scores
Other 86 amongst other groups

Clock-drawing test in AD and other dementias


dementias 30
Control
Wiechmann US AD 125 (Sunderland et al., 1989) Score on a 0–4 scale Four-point CDT (Sunderland et al., 1989)
(Wiechmann VaD 75 has good sensitivity (100%) and
et al., 2010) controls 24 specificity (70%) to distinguish between
normal and dementia
However, mean CDT scores were not
significantly different between AD and
VaD
OVERALL Qualitative analyses could
demonstrate differences between AD
and VaD
Inconsistent findings in total scores
between AD and VaD
∗ Includes clock size and shape of clock errors.
∗∗ Includes turning of paper when writing numbers, numbers written in counterclockwise order regardless of deficits in spatial layout or location within the clock and number perseveration errors.

1653
1654
Table 2. Comparison of clock-drawing test results between Alzheimer’s dementia and Parkinson’s disease dementia patients
AUTHORS COUNTRY PATIENTS N SCORING METHOD CLOCK-DRAWING TEST S U M M A R Y O F FI N D I N G S

L. P. L. Tan et al.
............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................

Cahn-Weiner USA AD 22 (Rouleau et al., 1992) Score on a 0–10 scale No significant group differences in global
et al. (2003) PD 17 (Rouleau et al., 1992), quantitative measure
DLB 20 Qualitative error analysis PD had more planning errors than AD
(Rouleau et al., 1992) CDT confers limited discrimination between
DLB/AD/PD
Lee et al. (2009) Korea AD 71 (Wolf-Klein et al., 1989), Four scoring protocols No significant group differences in global
VaD 39 (Shulman et al., 1993), (Wolf-Klein et al., 1989; quantitative measure in all four scoring protocols
PDD 33 (Watson et al., 1993), Shulman et al., 1993; AD made more conceptual deficit errors than PDD
(Manos and Wu, 1994) Watson et al., 1993; PDD had more type A stimulus bound response
(Rouleau et al., 1992) Manos and Wu, 1994), than AD (i.e. hands are set for 10–11 instead of
Qualitative error analysis 10 after 11.)
(Rouleau et al., 1992) PDD had more spatial/planning deficits (layout of
clock) and perseveration errors (perseveration of
numbers) than AD
Lee et al. (2011) Korea AD 94 (Manos and Wu, 1994) Score on a 0–10 scale No clear differences in longitudinal changes on
VaD 22 (Rouleau et al., 1992) (Manos and Wu, 1994), CDT scores amongst the three dementia
PDD 119 Qualitative error analysis subtypes
(Rouleau et al., 1992) AD had more conceptualization errors than
spatial/planning deficits errors after 18 months
from baseline compared to PDD
O’Brien et al. US AD 25 (Royall et al., 1998) CLOX-1 (Royall et al., No significant differences between CLOX-1 in
(2009) PDD 25 1998), Drawing task, 15 PDD and AD although PDD scored lower than
point scoring scale AD
Saka et al. Turkey AD 32 (Rouleau et al., 1992) Score on a 0–10 scale PDD score lower than AD
2009(Saka and PDD 26 (Rouleau et al., 1992; CDT also helpful in differentiating aMCI and
Elibol, 2009) aMCI 34 Strauss, 2006) PDMCI
PDMCI 19
Sallam et al. 2013 Saudi AD 51 CERAD; (Morris et al., Score on a 0–4 scale PDD score lower than AD
(Sallam and Arabia VaD 31 1989) (Morris et al., 1989) No significant differences in CDT scores amongst
Amr, 2013) PDD 23 other groups
Other 86
dementias 30
Control
OVERALL Qualitative analyses could demonstrate
differences between AD and PDD
PDD score lower than AD in CDT in two of
the six studies

Notes: aMCI, amnestic mild cognitive impairment; PDMCI, mild cognitive impairment associated with Parkinson’s disease.
Table 3. Comparison of clock-drawing test results between Alzheimer’s dementia and patients with Lewy bodies dementia
AUTHORS COUNTRY PATIENTS N SCORING METHOD CLOCK-DRAWING TEST S U M M A R Y O F FI N D I N G S
............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................

Cagnin et al. Italy AD 45 (Mondini, 2003) Score on a 0–10 scale No difference between AD and DLB total score
(2013) DLB 81 (Mondini, 2003) In DLB group, DLB with visual hallucinations
scored significantly lower in CDT than DLB
without visual hallucinations
Cahn-Weiner USA AD 22 (Rouleau et al., 1992) Score on a 0–10 scale No significant group differences in global
et al. (2003) PD 17 (Rouleau et al., 1992), quantitative measure
DLB 20 Qualitative error analysis DLB had more conceptual errors than AD
(Rouleau et al., 1992) DLB had more planning errors than AD
CDT confers limited discrimination between
DLB/AD/PD. (The overall classification
accuracy of the type of dementia was 69% in the
discriminant function analyses.)
Hamilton et al. US AD 44 Not specified Clock drawing test- copy, No differences in baseline CDT scores between
(2008) DLB 22 scored 0–3 points AD and DLB

Clock-drawing test in AD and other dementias


(autopsy DLB with impaired clock-copy scores (<3)
con- declined more rapidly than those with intact
firmed) scores over 1–2 yr period.
CDT predicts rate of subsequent global decline in
DLB but not AD
Palmqvist et al. Sweden AD 66 (Shulman, 2000) Score on a 0–5 scale DLB had lower scores than AD
(2009) DLB 33 (Shulman, 2000)
OVERALL Qualitative analyses could demonstrate
differences between AD and DLB
Three out of Four studies showed no
difference in CDT scores between DLB and
AD

1655
1656 L. P. L. Tan et al.

............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................
Kitabayashi et al., 2001), and perseveration errors

AD scored worst in CDT compared to VaD and


(Lee et al., 2009) than AD patients. When evaluated

conceptual deficits and spatial/planning errors


over a period of 18 months, AD patients tend to

Qualitative analyses could demonstrate


FTD had fewer stimulus bound responses,

discriminate FTD from control and AD.


Both global and qualitative analysis helped
show more conceptual errors than VaD patients

FTD performed better than AD in CDT

differences between AD and FTD


FTD score higher than AD in CDT
compared to baseline (Lee et al., 2011).

FTD scored higher than AD


S U M M A R Y O F FI N D I N G S
Differentiating AD from PDD
Patients with PDD scored lower than AD patients
in three of the six studies which compared CDT in
PDD and AD patients (O’Brien et al., 2009; Saka
and Elibol, 2009; Sallam and Amr, 2013), although
in one study, this was not statistically significant

Table 4. Comparison of clock-drawing test results between Alzheimer’s dementia and frontotemporal dementia patients

than AD
(O’Brien et al., 2009) (Table 2). In the other

FTD
three studies, no significant differences were found
in CDT scores between PDD and AD patients
(Cahn-Weiner et al., 2003; Lee et al., 2009; 2011).
Qualitative analyses of the type of errors seem to

CLOCK-DRAWING TEST

(Manos and Wu, 1994)


suggest a difference between AD and PDD patients.

Qualitative error analysis


(Rouleau et al., 1992),

specified (Salmon and


(Rouleau et al., 1992)

Command, copy clock;


In PDD patients, more spatial/planning errors were

Score on a 0–10 scale

Score on a 0–10 scale

scoring system not


observed compared to AD patients (Cahn-Weiner

Butters, 1992)
et al., 2003; Lee et al., 2009). AD patients made
more conceptual deficit errors than PDD patients
(Lee et al., 2009; 2011).

Differentiating AD from DLB


SCORING METHOD

(Rouleau et al., 1992)

In patients with DLB, no significant differences

(Salmon and Butters,


were found between total CDT scores in three of the (Manos and Wu,
four studies included (Table 3) (Cahn-Weiner et al.,
2003; Hamilton et al., 2008; Cagnin et al., 2013).
The fourth study however, found that DLB patients
1994)

1992)
had lower scores than AD patients (Palmqvist et al.,
2009). In the only study examining qualitative
differences between these groups, DLB patients
were found to have more conceptual and planning
25
36
25

30
30
30
28
14

errors than AD patients (Cahn-Weiner et al., 2003).


N

Interestingly, it was also found that DLB patients


PATIENTS

with visual hallucinations scored lower than DB


firmed)
(autopsy

patients without visual hallucinations in one study


Control

con-

(Cagnin et al., 2013), and another study showed


FTD

FTD

FTD
VaD
AD

AD

AD

that the clock-drawing score predicted the rate of


subsequent global decline in DLB but not AD
patients (Hamilton et al., 2008).
LOCATION

Canada

Differentiating AD from FTD


Italy

US

In FTD patients, all three studies (Moretti et al.,


2002; Rascovsky et al., 2002; Blair et al., 2006)
Blair et al. (2006)

found that FTD patients scored higher than


Rascovsky et al.

AD patients (Table 4). In the only study that


Moretti et al.

OVERALL
AUTHORS

examined the clock-drawing errors qualitatively,


(2002)

(2002)

FTD patients had fewer stimulus-bound responses,


conceptual deficits, and spatial/planning errors than
AD patients (Blair et al., 2006).
Clock-drawing test in AD and other dementias 1657

Discussion Planning errors on the CDT have been shown to be


more common in patients with greater subcortical
This review suggests that qualitative analyses of the involvement (e.g. Huntington’s disease), than those
CDT may be helpful for the differential diagnosis of with predominantly cortical involvement (Rouleau
AD from other dementias such as VaD, PDD, DLB, et al., 1992). Conceptual errors were found to
and FTD. Furthermore, CDT scores may also help be more common in AD than PDD patients in
differentiate AD from FTD. However, quantitative two of the four CDT studies with qualitative
scores in general, regardless of the scoring system analyses (Lee et al., 2009; 2011). This may be
used, were not useful for differentiating AD from explained by the earlier impairment of semantic
VaD, PDD, and DLB. This is likely due to memory in AD patients (Salmon and Bondi, 2009)
the intrinsic nature of the CDT assessing several compared to patients with PDD. In the only
cognitive skills at the same time for its completion. study, that included a qualitative analysis of the
Hence although a single overall score is able to CDT comparing AD and DLB patients, conceptual
demonstrate the presence of cognitive impairment, errors were unexpectedly found to be greater in
it is limited in delineating specific domains of DLB patients than AD patients (Cahn-Weiner
cognitive impairment. An examination of the type et al., 2003). The authors explain that picture
of CDT errors may be more useful in localizing the comprehension is affected in DLB patients due
domain of cognitive dysfunction and assist in the to a combination of semantic and visuoperceptual
diagnosis of the type of dementia. impairments and other studies have also found that
Royall et al. hypothesized that the clock drawing DLB patients demonstrate similar semantic deficits
to command (CLOX1) is more indicative of compared to AD patients (Calderon et al., 2001;
executive impairment than clock copying (CLOX2) Lambon Ralph et al., 2001). Given the difficulty in
(Royall et al., 1998), which focuses more on clinically differentiating PDD and DLB from AD,
visuoconstructional abilities. Whilst there are some patterns of deficits in the CDT may have some
studies that support this theory with lower scores in clinical utility in distinguishing between AD and
CLOX1 than CLOX2 in VaD patients (You et al., PDD/DLB patients, although quantitative scores
2011), there are also studies that conclude that per se are not likely to be useful.
CLOX2 is a more sensitive measure of executive All three studies that administered the CDT
deficits (Libon et al., 1993; Cosentino et al., 2004). to AD and FTD patients found that FTD
This may be explained by AD patients being more patients consistently scored higher than AD patients
impaired in semantic knowledge, resulting in poorer (Moretti et al., 2002; Rascovsky et al., 2002;
CLOX1 scores than VaD patients. This would Blair et al., 2006). This finding is supported by
account for our finding that the total score of the several studies which suggest that the impairment
CDT does not appear to be a sensitive measure in FTD is largely restricted to executive function
to differentiate between AD and VaD. However, rather than other cognitive functions (Forstl
based on the four studies in this review that included et al., 1996; Rascovsky et al., 2002). Another
qualitative analysis of the CDT between AD and study has also found that FTD patients perform
VaD patients, there is a suggestion that the type better on tests of visuospatial abilities sensitive
of CDT errors may help to distinguish between to the dysfunction of medial temporal structures
the two. Most studies agree that frontal executive compared to AD patients (Perri et al., 2005).
dysfunction is most characteristic of VaD (Roh In the only study that included a qualitative
and Lee, 2014), and subcortical VaD differentially analysis of the CDT comparing FTD and AD
affects the frontosubcortical circuits that mediate patients, FTD patients had fewer stimulus-bound
fine motor control and planning. The spatial and responses, conceptual deficits, and spatial/planning
planning deficits of the CDT seen more commonly errors than AD. This is also in keeping with
in patients with VaD than AD are likely to be due to another study showing that behavioral variant
subcortical involvement in VaD (Looi and Sachdev, FTD patients often perform in the normal range
1999). on traditional executive function tests measuring
Patients with PDD or DLB generally have greater working memory, planning, mental flexibility,
visuospatial, attention, and executive function response inhibition, and concept formation, at
impairment than AD patients (Ferman et al., initial evaluation (Gregory and Hodges, 1996; Perri
2006; Salmon and Bondi, 2009). In this review, et al., 2005).
spatial/planning deficits on the CDT were more The comparability of the studies reviewed is
common in PDD or DLB patients than AD patients limited due to the substantial methodological
(Cahn-Weiner et al., 2003; Lee et al., 2009; 2011). heterogeneity including the selection of the sample,
As is the case with VaD, this is likely to be due to the modality of CDT being administered, and the
subcortical involvement present in PDD and DLB. scoring system utilized. Despite these differences,
1658 L. P. L. Tan et al.

it still appears that the CDT score itself confers Brodaty, H. and Moore, C. M. (1997). The clock drawing
limited discrimination between AD and VaD, test for dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: a comparison of
PDD and DLB, and should not be used in three scoring methods in a memory disorders clinic.
isolation. Quantitative scores however, do appear International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 12,
to discriminate AD and FTD. Notably, the studies 619–627.
Cagnin, A. et al. (2013). Clinical and cognitive correlates of
that included a qualitative analysis of the CDT
visual hallucinations in dementia with Lewy bodies. Journal
found several significant differences between the of Neurology Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 84, 505–510.
type of errors made by patients with AD compared Cahn-Weiner, D. A., Williams, K., Grace, J., Tremont,
to patients with VaD, PDD, DLB, and FTD. G., Westervelt, H. and Stern, R. A. (2003).
Future studies should continue to evaluate the Discrimination of dementia with lewy bodies from
qualitative aspects rather than the quantitative Alzheimer disease and Parkinson disease using the clock
aspects of the CDT. Qualitative analysis of the drawing test. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 16, 85–92.
CDT may aid as an adjunct tool in the differential Calderon, J., Perry, R. J., Erzinclioglu, S. W., Berrios,
diagnosis of dementia. G. E., Dening, T. R. and Hodges, J. R. (2001).
Perception, attention, and working memory are
disproportionately impaired in dementia with Lewy bodies
compared with Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Neurology
Funding Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 70, 157–164.
None. Chui, H. C., Victoroff, J. I., Margolin, D., Jagust, W.,
Shankle, R. and Katzman, R. (1992). Criteria for the
diagnosis of ischemic vascular dementia proposed by the
State of California Alzheimer’s disease diagnostic and
Conflict of interest disclosure treatment Centers. Neurology, 42, 473–480.
Clinical and neuropathological criteria for
None.
frontotemporal dementia. The Lund and Manchester
Groups (1994). Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery &
Psychiatry, 57, 416–418.
Description of authors’ roles Cosentino, S., Jefferson, A., Chute, D. L., Kaplan, E.
and Libon, D. J. (2004). Clock drawing errors in
K. Shulman and L. Tan formulated the research dementia: neuropsychological and neuroanatomical
question. N. Herrmann and K. Shulman supervised considerations. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 17,
the data collection. L. Tan collected, analysed 74–84.
the data and wrote the paper. N. Herrmann, B. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
Mainland, and K. Shulman assisted with writing (3rd edn, revised.) (1987). Washington, DC: American
the article. Psychiatric Association.
Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(4th edn, text rev.) (2000). Washington, DC: American
Psychiatric Association.
Acknowledgments Eknoyan, D., Hurley, R. A. and Taber, K. H. (2012). The
The authors would like to acknowledge Mr Henry clock drawing task: common errors and functional
Lam for assisting with the library resources at the neuroanatomy. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry & Clinical
Neurosciences, 24, 260–265.
Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre in obtaining
Emre, M. et al. (2007). Clinical diagnostic criteria for
the list of articles for this review. dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Movement
Disorders, 22, 1689–1707.
Ferman, T. J. et al. (2006). Neuropsychological
References differentiation of dementia with Lewy bodies from normal
aging and Alzheimer’s disease. The Clinical
Albert, M. S. and Kaplan, E. (1980). Organic Implications of Neuropsychologist, 20, 623–636.
Neuropsychological Deficits in the Elderly, New Directions in Forstl, H., Besthorn, C., Hentschel, F., Geiger-Kabisch,
Memory and Aging. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum C., Sattel, H. and Schreiter-Gasser, U. (1996). Frontal
Associates, Inc. lobe degeneration and Alzheimer’s disease: a controlled
Barr, A., Benedict, R., Tune, L. and Brandt, J. (1992). study on clinical findings, volumetric brain changes and
Neuropsychological differentiation of Alzheimer’s disease quantitative electroencephalography data. Dementia, 7,
from vascular dementia. International Journal of Geriatric 27–34.
Psychiatry, 7, 621–627. Freedman, M., Leach, L., Kaplan, E., Wnocur, E.,
Blair, M., Kertesz, A., McMonagle, P., Davidson, W. Shulman, K. I. and Delis, D. C. (1994). Clock drawing.
and Bodi, N. (2006). Quantitative and qualitative analyses A Neuropsychological Analysis. Oxford: Oxford University
of clock drawing in frontotemporal dementia and Press.
Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the International Gregory, C. A. and Hodges, J. R. (1996). Clinical features
Neuropsychological Society, 12, 159–165. of frontal lobe dementia in comparison to Alzheimer’s
Clock-drawing test in AD and other dementias 1659

disease. Journal of Neural Transmission Supplementa, 47, bodies (DLB): report of the consortium on DLB
103–123. international workshop. Neurology, 47, 1113–1124.
Hamilton, J. M. et al. (2008). Visuospatial deficits predict McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman,
rate of cognitive decline in autopsy-verified dementia with R., Price, D. and Stadlan, E. M. (1984). Clinical
Lewy bodies. Neuropsychology, 22, 729–737. diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the
Heinik, J., Solomesh, I., Raikher, B. and Lin, R. (2002). NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of
Can clock drawing test help to differentiate between Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on
dementia of the Alzheimer’s type and vascular dementia? A Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology, 34, 939–944.
preliminary study. International Journal of Geriatric Mendez, M. F., Ala, T. and Underwood, K. L. (1992).
Psychiatry, 17, 699–703. Development of scoring criteria for the clock drawing task
Kitabayashi, Y., Ueda, H., Narumoto, J., Nakamura, K., in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the American Geriatrics
Kita, H. and Fukui, K. (2001). Qualitative analyses of Society, 40, 1095–1099.
clock drawings in Alzheimer’s disease and vascular Mondini, S., Mapelli, D., Vestri, A. and Bisiacchi, P. S.
dementia. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 55, (2003). Esame NeuropsicoLogico Breve: Una Batteria Di Test
485–491. Per Lo Screening NeuropsicoLogico. Milano, Italia:
Kozora, E. and Cullum, C. M. (1994). Qualitative features Cortina.
of clock drawings in normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Moretti, R., Torre, P., Antonello, R. M., Cazzato, G.
Assessment, 1, 179–188. and Bava, A. (2002). Ten-point clock test: a correlation
Lambon Ralph, M. A., Powell, J., Howard, D., analysis with other neuropsychological tests in dementia.
Whitworth, A. B., Garrard, P. and Hodges, J. R. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17,
(2001). Semantic memory is impaired in both dementia 347–353.
with Lewy bodies and dementia of Alzheimer’s type: a Morris, J. C. et al. (1989). The consortium to establish a
comparative neuropsychological study and literature registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical
review. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 70, and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease.
149–156. Neurology, 39, 1159–1165.
Lee, A. Y., Kim, J. S., Choi, B. H. and Sohn, E. H. Neary, D. et al. (1998). Frontotemporal lobar degeneration:
(2009). Characteristics of clock drawing test (CDT) errors a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology, 51,
by the dementia type: quantitative and qualitative analyses. 1546–1554.
Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 48, 58–60. O’Brien, T. J., Wadley, V., Nicholas, A. P., Stover,
Lee, J. H., Oh, E. S., Jeong, S. H., Sohn, E. H., Lee, N. P., Watts, R. and Griffith, H. R. (2009). The
T. Y. and Lee, A. Y. (2011). Longitudinal changes in contribution of executive control on verbal-learning
clock drawing test (CDT) performance according to impairment in patients with Parkinson’s disease with
dementia subtypes and severity Archives of Gerontology and dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Clinical
Geriatrics, 53, e179–e182. Neuropsychology, 24, 237–244.
Libon, D. J., Malamut, B. L., Swenson, R., Sands, L. P. Palmqvist, S., Hansson, O., Minthon, L. and Londos, E.
and Cloud, B. S. (1996). Further analyses of clock (2009). Practical suggestions on how to differentiate
drawings among demented and nondemented older dementia with Lewy bodies from Alzheimer’s disease with
subjects. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11, common cognitive tests. International Journal of Geriatric
193–205. Psychiatry, 24, 1405–1412.
Libon, D. J., Swenson, R. A., Barnoski, E. J. and Sands, Perri, R. et al. (2005). Alzheimer’s disease and frontal variant
L. P. (1993). Clock drawing as an assessment tool for of frontotemporal dementia– a very brief battery for
dementia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 8, 405–415. cognitive and behavioural distinction. Journal of Neurology,
Looi, J. C. and Sachdev, P. S. (1999). Differentiation of 252, 1238–1244.
vascular dementia from AD on neuropsychological tests. Pinto, E. and Peters, R. (2009). Literature review of the
Neurology, 53, 670–678. clock drawing test as a tool for cognitive screening.
Mainland, B. J., Amodeo, S. and Shulman, K. I. (2014). Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 27, 201–213.
Multiple clock drawing scoring systems: simpler is better. Rascovsky, K. et al. (2002). Cognitive profiles differ in
International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 29, 127–136. autopsy-confirmed frontotemporal dementia and AD.
Manos, P. J. and Wu, R. (1994). The ten point clock test: a Neurology, 58, 1801–1808.
quick screen and grading method for cognitive impairment Roh, J. H. and Lee, J. H. (2014). Recent updates on
in medical and surgical patients. The International Journal of subcortical ischemic vascular dementia. Journal of Stroke,
Psychiatry in Medicine, 24, 229–244. 16, 18–26.
Matioli, M. N. and Caramelli, P. (2010). Limitations in Roman, G. C. et al. (1993). Vascular dementia: diagnostic
differentiating vascular dementia from Alzheimer’s disease criteria for research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN
with brief cognitive tests. Arquivos de Neuro-psiquiatria, 68, International Workshop. Neurology, 43, 250–260.
185–188. Rouleau, I., Salmon, D. P., Butters, N., Kennedy, C.
McGuinness, B., Barrett, S. L., Craig, D., Lawson, J. and McGuire, K. (1992). Quantitative and qualitative
and Passmore, A. P. (2010). Executive functioning in analyses of clock drawings in Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s
Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. International disease. Brain and Cognition, 18, 70–87.
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25, 562–568. Royall, D. R., Cordes, J. A. and Polk, M. (1998). CLOX:
McKeith, I. G. et al. (1996). Consensus guidelines for the an executive clock drawing task. Journal of Neurology
clinical and pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy Neurosurgery Psychiatry, 64, 588–594.
1660 L. P. L. Tan et al.

Saka, E. and Elibol, B. (2009). Enhanced cued recall and Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S. and Spreen, O. (2006). A
clock drawing test performances differ in Parkinson’s and Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. Oxford: Oxford
Alzheimer’s disease-related cognitive dysfunction. University Press.
Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 15, 688–691. Sunderland, T. et al. (1989). Clock drawing in Alzheimer’s
Sallam, K. and Amr, M. (2013). The use of the mini-mental disease. A novel measure of dementia severity. Journal of the
state examination and the clock-drawing test for dementia American Geriatric Society, 37, 725–729.
in a tertiary hospital. Journal of Clinical Diagnostic Research, Watson, Y. I., Arfken, C. L. and Birge, S. J. (1993). Clock
7, 484–488. completion: an objective screening test for dementia.
Salmon, D. P. and Bondi, M. W. (2009). Journal of the American Geriatric Society, 41, 1235–1240.
Neuropsychological assessment of dementia. Annual Wiechmann, A. R., Hall, J. R. and O’Bryant, S. (2010).
Review of Psychology, 60, 257–282. The four-point scoring system for the clock drawing test
Salmon, D. P. and Butters, N. (1992). Neuropsychological does not differentiate between Alzheimer’s disease and
assessment of dementia in elderly. In R. Katzman, J. Rowe vascular dementia. Psychological Reports, 106, 941–948.
(eds.), Principles of Geriatric Neurology (pp. 144–163). Wolf-Klein, G. P., Silverstone, F. A., Levy, A. P. and
Philadelphia: Davis. Brod, M. S. (1989). Screening for Alzheimer’s disease by
Shulman, K. I. (2000). Clock-drawing: is it the ideal clock drawing. Journal of the American Geriatric Society, 37,
cognitive screening test?. International Journal of Geriatric 730–734.
Psychiatry, 15, 548–561. You, J. S., Chen, R. Z., Zhang, F. M., Zhou, Z. Y., Cai,
Shulman, K. I., Pushkar Gold, D., Cohen, C. A. and Y. F. and Li, G. F. (2011). The chinese (cantonese)
Zucchero, C. A. (1993). Clock-drawing and dementia in montreal cognitive assessment in patients with subcortical
the community: a longitudinal study. International Journal ischemic vascular dementia. Dementia and Geriatric
of Geriatric Psychiatry, 8, 487–496. Cognitive Disorders Extra, 1, 276–282.

You might also like