Liu Et Al (2020) - Factors Affecting Infant Toy Preferences. Age Gender Experience Motor Development and Parental Attitude.

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 25

DOI: 10.1111/infa.

12352

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Factors affecting infant toy preferences: Age,


gender, experience, motor development, and
parental attitude

Liquan Liu1,2,3,4 | Paola Escudero2,3 | Christina Quattropani2 |


Rachel A. Robbins1,5

1
School of Psychology, Western Sydney
University, Sydney, NSW, Australia
Abstract
2
The MARCS Institute for Brain, In contrast to the anecdotal claim that “male infants like
Behaviour and Development, Western cars and female infants like dolls,” previous studies have
Sydney University, Sydney, NSW, Australia
reported mixed findings for gender-related toy prefer-
3
Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of
ences in infancy. In Experiment 1, we explored the emer-
Language, Australian Research Council,
Canberra, ACT, Australia gence of gender-related preferences using face–car pairs
4
Center of Multilingualism across the (Experiment 1a, n = 51, 6–20 months) or face–stove pairs
Lifespan, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway (Experiment 1b, n = 54, 6–20 months). In Experiment 2
5
Research School of Psychology,
(n = 42, 14–16 months), we explore the effect of toy proper-
Australian National University, Canberra,
ACT, Australia ties, infants' past toy exposure, activity levels, and parental
attitudes on such preferences using a wider range of toys.
Correspondence
For both studies, infants demonstrated a general prefer-
Liquan Liu, School of Psychology, Western
Sydney University, 56 Second Avenue, ence for faced stimuli over other objects, except for male
Kingswood 2747, Sydney, NSW, Australia. infants who showed no preference between dolls and cars at
Email: l.liu@westernsydney.edu.aus
around 15 months. Infants' prior experience participating in
Funding information motor-intensive activities, with wheeled toys and parental
Australian Research Council, Grant/Award attitudes appeared to relate to female infants' preferences
Number: FT160100514; Western Sydney
University , Grant/Award Number: Career for dynamic toys. These results indicate a range of factors
Interruption Grant (20311 87617) and influence gendered toy preferences and suggest that nurture
Researcher of the Year 2014; European
plays an important role.
Union's Horizon 2020 research and
innovation program, Grant/Award Number:
798658

This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs License, which permits use and
distribution in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non-commercial and no modifications or adaptations are
made.
© 2020 The Authors. Infancy published by Wiley Periodicals LLC on behalf of International Congress of Infant Studies

Infancy. 2020;25:593–617.  wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/infa | 593


|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
594    LIU et al.

I NT RO D U C TIO N

The debate about whether there are “boy toys” and “girl toys” or just toys has swung back and forth
across recent decades. Some studies suggest that male children prefer “male-typical” toys (i.e., ve-
hicles, construction tools, guns) and female children prefer “female-typical” toys (i.e., dishes, dolls
accessories, cosmetics) (Connor & Serbin, 1977; Fulcher & Hayes, 2018; Golden & Jacoby, 2018;
Liss, 1981; Pasterski et al., 2005; Reich, Black, & Foliaki, 2018; Zosuls et al., 2009), whereas others
present mixed (e.g., Connellan, Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Batki, & Ahluwalia, 2000) or counter-
evidence (e.g., Servin, Bohlin, & Berlin, 1999). Further, it remains unclear when any gendered toy
preferences emerge and what factors may drive such preferences. This paper examines gender differ-
ences in the emergence of visual preferences for faces and toys among infants aged between 6 and
20 months (Experiment 1), and the influence of a range of previously underexplored factors including
stimulus properties, toy exposure, levels of motor-intensive activities, and parental attitudes on such
preferences (Experiment 2).
Results of infants' preference for gender-congruent toys are mixed. Connellan et al. (2000) showed
that more male newborns look longer to a physical-mechanical mobile with a picture made from
a scrambled face than to a whole face picture (43% vs. 25% of the sample), whereas more female
newborns look longer to a whole face picture (36% vs. 17%). However, the same study finds 32% of
male and 47% of female neonates do not show any preference between the pictures. Similarly, male
3- to 8-month-old infants make more fixations to a toy truck than to a doll but do not show longer
looking time, whereas their female peers look longer to a doll compared to a toy truck, although
the number of fixations does not differ between the toy types (Alexander, Wilcox, & Woods, 2009).
Moving to slightly older infants, Lutchmaya and Baron-Cohen (2002) found that at 12 months, male
infants paid more attention to moving cars, whereas female infants paid more attention to moving
faces. A recent study by Boe and Woods (2018) also found male infants aged 12 months showed a
preference for trucks over dolls in a grasping task, whereas 12-month-old female infants showed no
such preference. Similarly, an observational study during a laboratory play session reported that fe-
male but not male infants aged 10–18 months interact longer with dolls than other toys such as trucks
(Roopnarine, 1986). However, Servin et al. (1999) found no consistent gender-specific toy preferences
for 1-, 3- and 5-year-olds when comparing “male-typical” (e.g., cars, construction toy), “female-typi-
cal” (e.g., doll, tea set), and neutral (e.g., view-master, playing cards) toys. In addition, a considerable
amount of literature illustrates that face-like stimuli seem to be preferred over other stimuli for both
male and female infants from the prenatal stage (Reid et al., 2017) to infancy (Farroni et al., 2005;
Johnson, Dziurawiec, Ellis, & Morton, 1991; Leo & Simion, 2009; Mondloch et al., 1999; Pascalis, de
Haan, & Nelson, 2002; Quinn, Kelly, Lee, Pascalis, & Slater, 2008). Using a preferential looking para-
digm with paired stimuli presented simultaneously, a general looking preference for dolls or doll faces
over cars or trucks for both male and female infants was observed at 5 months (Boe & Woods, 2018;
Escudero, Robbins, & Johnson, 2013) and at 12 months (Jadva, Hines, & Golombok, 2010). Crucially,
these observations challenge claims on gender-specific toy preferences. Last but not least, a recent
meta-analysis by Todd et al. (2018) measured 1,600 children aged between 12 and 93 months and
found a positive correlation between male children's age, but a negative quadratic correlation between
female children's age, and percentage of time playing with gender-congruent toys. This indicates that
male children play more with male-typical toys as they grow older, and female children's preference
for female-typical toys decreases at around 48 months and increases thereafter. Studies with infants
younger than 12 months were not included in the meta-analysis.
Despite the discrepancies in previous literature, if infants do present gender-specific toy prefer-
ences (at least in some studies), the driving forces become an inevitable question. To date, several
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
LIU et al.    595

explanations have been proposed, yet none has been deemed decisive either due to mixed findings or
lack of direct evidence. The first sets of evidence across ages and species suggest some role for bio-
logical/hormonal differences between males and females that contribute to early gender-specific toy
preferences. Prenatal sex hormones have been shown to modify sex-dimorphic behavior and tempera-
mental sex differences (Ehrhardt & Meyer-Bahlburg, 1981), and human androgen levels appear to be
related to gender-related toy preferences, such that high androgen level increases male toy preference
among females (Berenbaum, 1999; Berenbaum & Hines, 1992; Hines & Kaufman, 1994). Similarly,
infants' testosterone levels before 6 months were associated with their gender-typed behavior at
14 months (Lamminmäki et al., 2012). Further, young monkeys reach for “gender-typical” toys (male:
car, ball, wheeled toys; female: doll, pot, plush toys) more than toys that are typical for the other gen-
der in humans, arguing for some biological rather than social basis for toy preferences (Alexander &
Hines, 2002; Hassett, Siebert, & Wallen, 2008). Nineteen-month-olds with more “male-typical” digit
ratios, which are linked to higher exposure to masculinizing hormones in the womb, are likely to be
rated as having higher activity during play and show less “female-typical” toy preference (Alexander
& Saenz, 2012). Once again, it has to be noted that not all studies on hormonal effects reveal congru-
ent findings. While a positive correlation was observed between male infants' (n = 63) gender-spe-
cific toy preferences (e.g., trucks over dolls) during structured play and their prenatal progesterone
levels, no such correlation was found for female infants (n = 63) and no correlation was observed
between gender and prenatal estradiol and testosterone levels (van de Beek, van Goozen, Buitelaar, &
Cohen-Kettenis, 2009).
Alternatively, many have suggested that environmental factors may play a role in gendered toy
preference (Bandura, 1977; Ehrhardt & Meyer-Bahlburg, 1981; Fagot & Hagan, 1991; Langlois &
Downs, 1990; Martin, 1999; Martin, Wood, & Little, 1990). Roopnarine (1986) hypothesized that
caretakers may contribute to the gender-specific socialization of their infants. Since we ran our own
studies, there have been several, which are particularly relevant to this question. Boe and Woods
(2018) showed that across age and gender, infants' preference for trucks was predicted by the duration
of home play with trucks, which was further predicted by the number of trucks in the home. This
evidence suggests that parental selection of types of toys (e.g., dolls vs. trucks), or early gender social-
ization, can alter infants' development of social identities in the first year after birth. An alternative
interpretation would be that parents may be selecting toys according to their perceptions of the infants'
pre-existing preferences. Parents rate gender-congruent and gender-neutral toys as more desirable than
gender-incongruent toys for their children aged 3–6 years (Kollmayer, Schultes, Schober, Hodosi, &
Spiel, 2018). A recent online survey was conducted by Weisgram and Bruun (2018) in which 238 pro-
spective and 96 current parents' preferences on gender-specific toy purchases were measured. Results
showed that parents planned to purchase gender-congruent toys for their prospective or own children.
Additionally, mothers with non-traditional gender-typed toys as a child would also purchase non-tra-
ditional gender-typed toys for their own children. This indicates that parental attitudes toward gen-
der may determine what toys appear in the home environment, influencing children's toy preference.
Though being a crucial factor, direct evidence on the environmental impact on infant gender-specific
toy preferences is lacking.
With respect to the emergence of infant gender-specific toy preference, previous studies found no
gender-specific or general toy preferences when comparing toys with faced stimuli among 5-month-
olds (Boe & Woods, 2018; Escudero et al., 2013) and even 12-month-olds (Jadva et al., 2010; Servin
et al., 1999). This indicates experience may be required for infants to develop robust gender-specific
toy preference in the second year after birth. Meanwhile, a group of studies suggest that transitions
in infant gender-related preferences occur at and after 14 months (Lamminmäki et al., 2012), though
perhaps in a gradual fashion (Lauer & Yhang, 2016). Zosuls et al. (2009) reported infants' mild gender
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
596    LIU et al.

differences in play at 17 months, which increased at 21 months, and by 19 months, they observed
spontaneous production of gender labels. At 18 and 19 months, male infants spend more time playing
with cars and female infants with dolls when both toys are presented in the experiment, although they
interact with these toys with equal frequency (Alexander & Saenz, 2012). Infants at this age seem to
be aware of gender-specific toy preferences themselves, in that they link girl photographs with dolls
and boy photographs with cars (Serbin, Poulin-Dubois, Colburne, Sen, & Eichstedt, 2001).
In brief, discrepancies have been observed across previous literature on infants' gender-specific
toy preferences. In addition, although the influence of some potential factors has been discussed in
previous literature, very few studies have directly measured their effect on infants' toy preferences (but
see Boe & Woods, 2018). These factors may include, but are not limited to, the age of infants tested,
individual toy experience and parental attitudes, and stimulus properties of the toys themselves. In
the current study, we examined the abovementioned factors across the two experiments. Experiment
1 examined the age at which gender preferences emerge using a large participant age range from
6 to 20 months and the same stimuli used in Escudero et al. (2013; faces–cars, faces–stoves). In
Experiment 2, we studied infant response at 14–16 months, with new toys (cars, tea sets, dolls, stroll-
ers), which tested whether stimuli's visual and motor properties affect infant toy preferences. We also
included parental questionnaires relating to previous toy experience, general activity levels, and pa-
rental attitudes to gender. We predicted that infants' gender-congruent toy preference would emerge as
a function of age: Young infants (close to 5 months after birth) would prefer faced stimuli irrespective
of their gender, whereas older male infants would be more prone to mechanical toys such as cars and
female infants would keep their sensitivity to social toys such as faced stimuli. We also predicted that
toy properties (mechanical locomotive aspects for male, social aspects for female infants), infant prior
toy experience, infant overall motor activity level, and parental attitudes on children's gender-specific
toy play may all positively contribute to infants' gender-specific toy preferences. The examination of
age would not only test replicability of previous findings (i.e., Escudero et al., 2013) but also advance
knowledge on the emergence of gender-specific toy preference. The investigation of the relationship
among infant looking preference, toy characteristics and parental-report measures provides an inno-
vative approach to the field.

1 | E X P ER IME N T 1: E X P LOR ING THE ONSET OF


G EN D E R- S P EC IF IC TOY P R E F E RENCES ( 6–20 M ONTHS)

In Escudero et al. (2013), four- to five-month-old infants' toy preferences with mechanical objects
(cars, stoves) and faced stimuli (humans, dolls) were tested using an eye-tracking preferential look-
ing paradigm. The study used multiple exemplars per stimulus category and presented social versus
non-social stimuli side by side, providing infants with the opportunity to express preferences, al-
lowing for more direct comparisons than presenting the items one at a time (i.e., each pair shows an
actual preference rather than just an overall looking time) and subsequently a better understanding
of the relative preferences. The object pictures were assigned to four paired categories, which in-
cluded comparisons of toy versus real objects (two conditions) and mechanical objects versus faces
(two conditions). For the toy versus real conditions, comparisons included (a) toy mechanical objects
(i.e., toy cars, toy stoves) versus real objects in the corresponding category, and (b) doll faces versus
human faces. For comparison of mechanical objects versus faces, pairs included (c) toy mechani-
cal objects versus doll faces, and (d) real mechanical objects versus human faces (Figures 1 and 4).
The previous results demonstrated that at 5 months, both male infants and female infants prefer face
stimuli over mechanical objects (Escudero et al., 2013), consistent with previous studies reporting
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
LIU et al.    597

F I G U R E 1 Stimuli for Experiment 1a. The four stimuli categories from top to bottom rows: Real female faces,
female doll faces, toy cars, and real cars

infants' preference for face-like stimuli over other objects (Johnson et al., 1991; Leo & Simion, 2009).
Crucially, no gender-specific toy preference was observed. However, it has been argued that infants
show gendered toy preferences in independent play at 9 months and their critical gender knowledge
advances at 18 months (Todd, Barry, & Thommessen, 2017). Meanwhile, a transition of preference
for male infants from dolls to cars has been suggested to occur between 12 and 18 months (Jadva
et al., 2010). These findings lead to the hypothesis that such infants' gender-specific toy preferences
may emerge later than 5 months.
To examine this hypothesis and to extend upon our previous research, we targeted a large partic-
ipant age range from 6 months (just above the age tested in Escudero et al., 2013) to 20 months (just
above that in Jadva et al., 2010) in Experiment 1. Partially functioning as a replication verification of
the previous study, the same stimuli and testing paradigm as Escudero et al. (2013) were used, with
cars and female faces tested in Experiment 1a and stoves and male faces tested in Experiment 1b.
These stimuli would reveal not only infants' potential preference between mechanic and social stimuli
but also whether their preference is linked to the level of “reality” in the objects.

1.1 | EXPERIMENT 1A: FEMALE FACES VERSUS CARS

1.1.1 | Methods

Participants
Infants were recruited by telephone call and emails through a private database, which consisted of
parents and infant details. The final sample of participants comprised 51 Australian infants aged from
6 to 20 months: 27 female and 24 male infants (see Figure 2 for age distributions). The number of
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
598    LIU et al.

F I G U R E 2 Scatterplot of male infants' (blue diamonds) and female infants' (red squares) age tested and looking
time preferences in Experiment 1a for: (a) female doll faces versus toy cars; (b) toy cars versus real cars; (c) female
doll faces versus real female faces; (d) real female faces versus real cars. In each case, 50% is no preference

participants per gender satisfied G power analysis with a medium effect size (f = 0.5). An additional
23 infants were observed but not included in the final sample due to crying (n = 2), being inattentive
to the experiment (n = 7), parental interference (n = 1), second-time participation (n = 2), or fewer
than 4 usable trials in which infants look at the screen in one or more condition (n = 11). The last
criterion was set to enhance the data reliability at the individual level in order to examine the spread
of results for individuals and the correlation between age and preference. As discussed in Escudero
et al. (2013), many previous studies have only used two trials per condition, but this gives less reliable
results (although the analysis showed the general pattern of results was the same with only two tri-
als). All infants were full-term and with no known developmental difficulties. All caregivers provided
informed consent.

Stimuli and design


The stimuli were the same as in Escudero et al. (2013, Experiment 1a). They were static pictures
from one of four categories: real female faces, female doll faces, toy cars, or real cars. Each category
consisted of 6 different pictures, adding up to 24 pictures in total (Figure 1). Pictures were assigned to
four paired categories for comparison: toy car versus female doll face, toy car versus real car, female
doll face versus real female face, and real car versus real female face. To ensure the generalization of
findings, multiple exemplars of each object type were included. Each paired category consisted of 6
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
LIU et al.    599

pairs, and each pair included one of the 6 different images from the corresponding categories. Side
presentation was counterbalanced, resulting in a total of 48 trials that included all relevant compari-
sons (4 pair types × 6 different pairs × 2 side orders = 48). The number of trials is larger than most
previous studies (e.g., 2 trials in Alexander et al., 2009). The presentation order was randomized
across trials. Picture size was approximately 12.0 × 14.0 cm in height and width respectively, with an
11.4 × 13.3° visual angle at infants' 60-cm viewing distance. The distance between the pictures of a
pair was set as 3.0 cm (2.9°).

Apparatus and procedure


A Tobii corneal reflection eye tracker (Model 1750, Tobii Technology) was used to record and meas-
ure infant gaze patterns. A standard 9-point calibration was used prior to the experiment. Stimuli were
presented on a 27 cm height × 34 cm width Tobii screen via software E-Prime 2.0.
Each infant was tested individually. During the session, infants sat on caregivers' lap on a chair,
while the eye tracker recorded infants' eye movements. Before each trial, an “attention-getter” (a short
graphic clip with sound) appeared at the center of the screen. The experimenter sat in an adjacent
room and initiated each trial when participants' attention was fixated on the attention-getter. The par-
ticipant was presented with 48 trials of 5 s (4 min total). Parents were asked to close their eyes during
calibration and were naive to the hypotheses until the study was completed. Each family received a toy
for participation after the experiment.

Ethics
The present study was conducted according to guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki,
with written informed consent obtained from a parent or guardian for each child before any assess-
ment or data collection. All procedures involving human subjects in this study were approved by the
Human Ethics Committee at Western Sydney University.

1.1.2 | Results and discussion

Analyses were conducted on each of the four pairs: toy car versus female doll face, toy car versus
real car, female doll face versus real female face, and real female face versus real car. Preference
scores were calculated based on accumulated fixations (i.e., dwell times) and were normalized for
trial duration and number of trials. Infants did not provide usable data for all trials, but as noted in
the participants' section, the number of trials in each condition was always above 4. A criterion of 4
or more trials per condition provided more reliable results than the criterion was 2 or more, although
the overall pattern of results remained the same, replicating the previous study in terms of the number
of trials shown (Escudero et al., 2013). Dwell time was calculated as the total fixation/number of 5 s
trials for each item in a pair. The preference score was calculated as dwell time for an item in the pair
(e.g., dolls in doll–car pairs) across repeats divided by the total dwell times × 100, such that no prefer-
ence is 50% (cf., Escudero et al., 2013).
As age was a variable of primary interest, relations between age and infant preference scores were
examined through the Pearson correlation coefficients for each gender (see Figure 2 and Table 1). For
male infants, medium-sized significant correlations were found between age and preference for cars
over faces (age and real female face-real car: r = −.521, p = .009, age and female doll face-toy car:
r = −.474, p = .019); however, these were smaller and not significant for female infants (age and real
female face–real car: r = −.084, p = .676, age and female doll face–toy car: r = −.308, p = .118).
Even collapsed over gender to give more power, there were no significant correlations for the other
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
600    LIU et al.

T A B L E 1 r and p values for correlations between age (in months) and infants' paired stimuli preferences in
Experiment 1a

Female doll face– Female doll face–real Real female


toy car Toy car–real car female face face–real car
Male infants r = −.474 r = .048 r = −.121 r = −.521
(n = 24) p = .019 p = .823 p = .574 p = .009
Female infants r = −.308 r = −.137 r = −.112 r = −.084
(n = 27) p = .118 p = .496 p = .579 p = .676
All infants r = −.411 r = −.016 r = −.112 r = −.387
(n = 51) p = .003 p = .913 p = .434 p = .005
Note: A positive correlation corresponds to an increased preference for the first listed item in a pair with increasing age, whereas a
negative correlation corresponds to an increased preference for the second item in a pair with increasing age.

T A B L E 2 t and p values of infant normalized fixation preference for each of the four pair types in Experiment
1a, compared with no preference (50%)

Female doll face– Female doll face–real Real female


toy car Toy car–real car female face face–real car
Male infants t(23) = 0.975 t(23) = 5.613 t(23) = 4.268 t(23) = 3.244
p = .340 p < .001 p < .001 p = .004
Female infants t(26) = 6.853 t(26) = 8.693 t(26) = 3.665 t(26) = 8.927
p < .001 p < .001 p = .001 p < .001
All infants t(50) = 4.510 t(50) = 9.724 t(50) = 5.531 t(50) = 7.368
p < .001 p < .001 p < .001 p < .001
Note: The Bonferroni-corrected alpha p = .004 (see also Figure 3).

pairs (ps > .433; Table 2). Results suggested an increased preference toward cars with increasing age
primarily for male infants.
To assess whether the current results replicated Escudero et al. (2013), preference scores were
compared between males and female infants for each pair, and to 50% (no preference) for male and
female infants via one-sample t tests (see Table 2). A significant difference was found between male
and female preferences for toy cars versus female doll faces (t (49) = 3.011, p = .004; see Figure 3).
However, preference did not differ between male infants and female infants for the other three pairs
(ps ≥ .139). Importantly, when compared to 50% (no preference) results showed significant prefer-
ences in almost all pairs (after Bonferroni correction; see Table 3). These results replicate Escudero
et al. (2013), in the current older infants, and with a wider age spread, but also show a significant
preference for doll over real faces (the trend was in the same direction in Escudero et al., 2013).
Interestingly, male infants' preference did not differ between toy cars and female doll faces when
collapsed across ages, but female infants' preferences do, leading to a gender difference. Figure 3
illustrates these normalized total fixation preference scores.
To summarize, experimental results revealed three main patterns: (a) Collapsed across ages, in-
fants exhibited general preference for faced stimuli over objects; (b) all infants preferred toy objects to
real objects across age and gender; and (c) preferences for gendered toys changed with age, such that
older male infants no longer had a preference for faces over cars, whereas female infants still did. In
Experiment 1b, we tested whether findings in Experiment 1a would generalize to another set of faces
and mechanical non-face objects. As in Escudero et al. (2013), we used stoves, which are mechanical
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
LIU et al.    601

F I G U R E 3 Experiment 1a: Infant normalized fixation preference for each of the four pair types. Preference is
shown as closer to one or other item, with 50% = no preference. Error bars = ±1 SEM. Where male symbols (blue
diamonds) are not visible, it is because males have an almost identical value to females (red squares)

TABLE 3 r and p values for correlations between age and infants' paired stimuli preferences in Experiment 1b

Male doll face–toy Toy stove–real Male doll face–real Real male
stove stove male face face–real stove
Male infants r = −.053 r = −.213 r = .092 r = .002
(n = 25) p = .801 p = .307 p = .661 p = .991
Female infants r = −.073 r = .199 r = −.002 r = .017
(n = 29) p = .708 p = .300 p = .991 p = .931
All infants r = −.068 r = .016 r = .055 r = .026
(n = 54) p = .623 p = .909 p = .691 p = .849
Note: A positive correlation corresponds to an increased preference for the first listed item in a pair with increasing age, whereas a
negative correlation corresponds to an increased preference for the second item in a pair with increasing age.

but might be considered “female”-gendered in terms of stereotypes, and male faces, which are less
preferred by younger infants (Quinn, Yahr, Kuhn, Slater, & Pascalis, 2002).

1.2 | EXPERIMENT 1B: MALE FACES VERSUS STOVES

1.2.1 | Methods

Participants
The same recruitment and inclusion criteria as Experiment 1a were adopted. The final sample of
participants comprised 54 infants from 6 to 20 months: 29 female and 25 male infants (see ages in
Figure 5 scatterplot). The number of participants per gender satisfied G power analysis with a medium
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
602    LIU et al.

F I G U R E 4 Stimuli for Experiment 1a. The four stimuli categories from top to bottom rows: Real male faces,
male doll faces, toy stoves, and real stoves

effect size (f = 0.5). An additional 11 infants were observed but not included in the final sample due
to calibration failure (n = 3), crying (n = 4), experimental error (n = 1), or less than 4 valid trials for
infants in each condition (i.e., the criterion applied in Experiment 1a; n = 3).

Stimuli and design


The design was the same as Experiment 1a, with stimuli the same as in Escudero et al. (2013,
Experiment 1b). Static pictures were from one of four categories: real male faces, male doll faces, toy
stoves, or real stoves. As in Experiment 1a, each category consisted of 6 different pictures, adding up
to 24 pictures in total (Figure 4). These pictures were further assigned to four paired categories for
preference comparison: male doll face versus toy stove, toy stove versus real stove, male doll face
versus real male face, and real male face versus real stove. Side presentation was counterbalanced,
resulting in a total of 48 trials (4 pair types × 6 different pairs × 2 side orders = 48). The presentation
order was randomized across trials. Picture size, visual angle, and viewing distance were the same as
in Experiment 1a.

Apparatus and procedure


The same apparatus and procedure as in Experiment 1a were used.
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
LIU et al.    603

TABLE 4 t and p values of infant normalized fixation preference for each of the four pair types in Experiment 1b

Male doll face–toy Toy stove–real Male doll face–real Real male
stove stove male face face–real stove
Male infants t(24) = 6.529 t(24) = 8.466 t(24) = 5.376 t(24) = 7.636
p < .001 p < .001 p < .001 p < .001
Female infants t(28) = 7.275 t(28) = 12.126 t(28) = 6.547 t(28) = 11.709
p < .001 p < .001 p < .001 p < .001
All infants t(53) = 9.831 t(53) = 14.356 t(53) = 8.507 t(53) = 13.469
p < .001 p < .001 p < .001 p < .001
Note: The Bonferroni-corrected alpha p = .004 (see also Figure 6).

F I G U R E 5 Scatterplot of male infants' (blue diamonds) and female infants' (red squares) age tested and looking
time preferences in Experiment 1b for: (a) Male doll faces versus toy stoves; (b) toy stoves versus real stoves; (c) male
doll faces versus real male faces; (d) Real male faces versus real stoves. In each case, 50% is no preference

1.2.2 | Results and discussion

Because the same questions applied as in Experiment 1a, the same statistics, figures, and tables as
Experiment 1a were included. As age was a variable of primary interest, the effect of age on infant
preference scores was also examined through the Pearson correlation coefficients for each gender (see
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
604    LIU et al.

F I G U R E 6 Infant normalized fixation preference for each of the four pair types in Experiment 2. Preference is
shown as closer to one or other item, with 50% = no preference. Error bars = ±1 SEM. Where male symbols (blue
diamonds) are not visible, it is because males have an almost identical value to female (red squares)

Figure 5 and Table 3). Unlike Experiment 1a, no significant correlations were observed (rs < .199,
ps > .300). Preferences scores were compared with 50% (no preference) for male and female in-
fants. Results showed significant preferences in all pairs after Bonferroni correction (see Figure 6 and
Table 4).
In summary, experimental results revealed general preferences for faced stimuli over objects and
for toys over real objects, similar to Experiment 1a. Unlike Experiment 1a, no gender-specific toy
preference was observed, showing that male infants' emergent lack of preference between cars and
faces does not apply to all mechanical toys, again replicating Escudero et al. (2013). Experiment 1
suggests that there may be differences in gendered toy preference depending on exactly what stimuli
are used, but does not allow us to discern whether it is the characteristics inherent to the object, or
something to do with the gendered nature of toys based on prior exposure. We explore these ideas in
Experiment 2.

2 | E X P ER IME N T 2. ST IMU LU S CHARACTERISTICS,


PRE V IO U S EX P O SU R E , OV E R A LL ACTIVIT Y LEVEL S, AND
PAR EN TA L AT T IT U D E S

In Experiment 2, we explore the potential factors that may explain male infants' developmental
decrease in preference for faces when compared to cars but not when compared to stoves. These
factors include toy properties, infants' previous toy experience, infants' overall activity types, and
parental attitudes toward gender-specific toys. First and foremost, although cars and stoves are
mechanical objects that can be manipulated, cars are smaller and therefore more graspable. Further,
cars have a locomotive aspect that may be appealing to male infants (Zosuls et al., 2009), and this
may also relate to overall activity levels. In order to test whether other graspable and locomotive
toys would yield similar effects as cars, we included tea sets and strollers in the stimuli, given that
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
LIU et al.    605

strollers are locomotive but not as graspable to infants, whereas this is the opposite for tea sets.
Experiment 2 thus tested toy preference in male and female infants for dolls, toy cars, strollers, and
tea sets, including all possible pair comparisons. The choice of strollers and tea sets was also driven
by stereotypical gender roles for these toys, as both might be considered more female-gendered by
traditional parents. Whole dolls instead of doll heads were used to match many previous gender-
specific toy preference studies and to resemble infants' actual experience. If stereotypical gender
roles play a part in infant preference, we would expect female infants to prefer dolls, strollers, and
tea sets over cars, while we would expect male infants to prefer cars over all other objects. If, how-
ever, male infants' preference for cars relates to their locomotive aspect, we would expect them to
prefer cars or strollers over the other toys. If it is the graspability of toys that is appealing, strollers
would be the least preferred objects. As Experiment 1a illustrated male infants' change in prefer-
ence for cars versus dolls with age, we focused on testing a narrower age range (14–16 months) in
Experiment 2. This age was also chosen to be consistent with previous work suggesting differences
in gender preference at or after 12 months (Boe & Woods, 2018; Jadva et al., 2010; Lutchmaya &
Baron-Cohen, 2002; Roopnarine, 1986).
A parental questionnaire was also included assessing infants' exposure to the various toys, activi-
ties they participated in, and parental attitudes to various gendered statements. Importantly, although
observations and speculations have been provided (e.g., Zosuls et al., 2009) for the link between
parental attitudes and toy preference among young infants, no previous study had directly measured
this factor at the time the current study was conducted. However, many have found that parents illus-
trate stereotypical behaviors correspondent to their infants' gender during pregnancy and through-
out childhood: Caretakers typically show enhanced physical relationship with male infants and more
emotional relationship with female infants (Frisch, 1977; Kitamura & Burnham, 1998, 2003; Leaper,
Anderson, & Sanders, 1998; Leaper, Leve, Strasser, & Schwartz, 1995; Pomerleau, Malcuit, Turgeon,
& Cossette, 1997; Smith & Lloyd, 1978).

2.1 | Methods

2.1.1 | Participants

The same recruitment and inclusion criteria as Experiment 1 were adopted. The final sample of par-
ticipants comprised 42 infants from 14 to 16 months: 22 female infants (mean age in months = 15.40,
SD = 0.54) and 20 male infants (mean = 15.40, SD = 0.48). The number of participants per gender
satisfied G power analysis with medium effect size (f = 0.5). An additional 17 infants were observed
but not included in the final sample due to crying (n = 2), experimental error and calibration failure
(n = 2), or less than 4 valid trials for infants in each condition (i.e., the criterion applied in Experiment
1; n = 13).

2.1.2 | Materials and design

For the preference task, the same design as in Experiment 1 was used with a different set of stimuli.
Static pictures were from one of four categories: cars, dolls, strollers, or tea sets. As in Experiment
1, each category consisted of 6 different pictures, making a total of 24 images across categories
(Figure 7). The car images were the same images as in Escudero et al. (2013), and the other pic-
tures were chosen from the Internet to show a similar range of colors and shapes in each category.
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
606    LIU et al.

F I G U R E 7 Experiment 2. Four stimuli categories from top to bottom rows: Dolls, cars, strollers, and tea sets.
Numbers show which items were paired together (e.g., doll 1 with car 1 or stroller 1, etc.)

These pictures were further assigned to six paired categories for preference comparison: doll versus
car, car versus stroller, car versus tea set, doll versus stroller, doll versus tea set, and tea set versus
stroller, with each item from one category paired with one item from another to give 6 pairs for each
comparison (e.g., doll 1–car 1; doll 2–car 2 … car 1–stroller 1; doll 1–stroller 1 … tea set 6–stroller
6). Side presentation was counterbalanced, resulting in a total of 72 trials (6 pair types × 6 different
pairs × 2 side orders = 72). The presenting order was pseudo-randomized within each comparison
(1–6) to ensure that the same pair of images did not appear one after another. Picture size, visual
angle, and viewing distance were similar to Experiment 1. A parental questionnaire included some
demographic information, followed by a list of activities that children might participate in and a
time/week, as well as toys which parents were asked to say whether their child played with and
approximately how long for per week. Finally, parents were asked to agree or disagree with 10
statements such as “Female infants are more social than male infants,” “Male infants like physical
activities more than female infants,” and “Female infants prefer trucks to dolls” (see the full list
in Table 7). The parental questionnaire was designed for this study (see Appendix S1 for the full
questionnaire).

2.1.3 | Apparatus and procedure

Similar apparatus and procedure as in Experiment 1 were used with two differences. Participants
were presented with 72 trials of 5 s in length (6 min total, compared with 48 trials and 4 min
for each of Experiments 1a and 1b). Caregivers completed the parental questionnaire after the
experiment.
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
LIU et al.    607

2.2 | Results and discussion

Analyses were conducted on each of the six pairs: doll versus car, car versus stroller, car versus tea set,
doll versus stroller, doll versus tea set, and tea set versus stroller. The same preference score (dwell
times) calculation method and infant trial selection criteria as in Experiment 1 were adopted. We first
compared male infants and female infants' preferences within each pair using an independent-samples
t-test with infants' preferential looking score as the test variable and gender (male vs. female) as
the grouping variable. No significant gender difference was observed across pairs (ts (40) < 0.860,
ps > .394) except for a marginal effect in the doll versus car comparison (t (40) = 1.981, p = .054):
Female infants prefer dolls more than their male peers.
As in Experiment 1, all pair types were compared with 50% (no preference) via one-sample t tests.
Results are presented both for males and female infants separately and for all infants combined (see
Table 5). Results showed significant preferences for infants in almost all pairs. An evident trend was
that both male and female infants appeared to prefer dolls over other toys, except for the doll–car pair
for males, where they show no preference, replicating Experiment 1a. Interestingly, no preference
between cars and tea sets was observed for either male or female infants. Figure 8 illustrates these
normalized total fixation preference scores.
We further linked infants' preferential looking patterns with their motor activity, and toy experience
collected through parental reports. Six motor activities were included in the questionnaire (dancing,
running, swinging, sliding, swimming, and riding), but most participants only did some of these activ-
ities. To gain sufficient power for analysis, we coded the motor variable as having two levels, less than
or more than three activities. This showed a significant interaction between gender and motor activity
on infants' preference for cars over tea sets, F(1, 38) = 4.86, p = .034 𝜂 2p = 0.113. Specifically, females
participating in more types of motor-intensive activities showed increased attention to cars over tea
sets and male infants revealed the same pattern if they engaged in fewer motor-intensive activities.
The other interactions were not significant (ps > .195). We also looked at the correlation between the
time (average minutes per week) infants spend on activities and their preferential patterns, altogether
(N = 42) and splitting by gender (Nmale = 20, Nfemale = 22). Infants' time spent on motor-intensive
activities appeared to be positively correlated with their preference for cars (car over doll: r = .323,
p = .037; car over tea set: r = .341, p = .027). Although the sample size after splitting by gender is
relatively small, significance was observed: Female infants who spent more time on motor activities
showed increased attention to cars over tea sets (r = .587, p = .004), and male infants who spent more
time on motor activities were more attentive to strollers than tea sets (r = .472, p = .035). The general
trend seemed to suggest that infants' preferences for dynamic over static toys are positively correlated
with their time spent on motor activities.
Regarding the factor of child toy exposure, there were many toys that very few infants had been
exposed to (e.g., Naction figure = 2). To increase power, toys with similar properties were grouped
(Table 6). That is, to examine the effect of locomotion we grouped walkers together with strollers,
and cars with trucks. Similarly, for static toys, dolls and action figures were grouped, as were tea
sets and stacking cups. Significant differences can be observed between male and female infants in
their experience of cars + trucks, t = 3.27, p = .002, 𝜂 2p = 0.211, as well as tea sets + cups, t = 3.21,
p = .003, 𝜂 2p = 0.205, but not of walkers + strollers, t = 1.19, p = .241, 𝜂 2p = 0.034, or dolls + action
figures, t = 1.32, p = .196, 𝜂 2p = 0.041. When the presence or absence of each toy was then compared
with the preference data, a significant interaction was observed between gender and the presence of
walker/stroller on infants' preference for strollers over tea sets, F(1, 38) = 9.07, p = .005, 𝜂 2p = 0.193.
Splitting gender, female infants with walker/stroller experience paid more attention to strollers than
those without the experience, F(1, 20) = 7.39, p = .013, 𝜂 2p = 0.270, whereas this difference was not
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
608    LIU et al.

F I G U R E 8 Experiment 2: Infant normalized fixation preference for each of the four pair types. Preference is
shown as closer to one or other item, with 50% = no preference. Error bars = ±1 SEM. Where male symbols (blue
diamonds) are not visible, it is because males have an almost identical value to female (red squares)

T A B L E 5 t and p values of infant normalized fixation preference for each of the six pair types in Experiment 2,
compared with no preference (50%)

Doll–car Car–stroller Car–tea set Doll–stroller Doll–tea set Tea set–stroller


Male infants t = 0.966 t = 1.805 t = 0.555 t = 6.486 t = 6.079 t = 3.095
(n = 20) p = .346 p = .087 p = .585 p < .001 p < .001 p = .006
Female infants t = 4.308 t = 2.812 t = −0.669 t = 11.582 t = 7.452 t = 3.523
(n = 22) p < .001 p = .010 p = .511 p < .001 p < .001 p = .002
All infants t = 3.469 t = 3.067 t = −0.033 t = 11.919 t = 9.382 t = 4.575
(n = 42) p = .001 p = .004 p = .974 p < .001 p < .001 p < .001
Note: The Bonferroni-corrected alpha p = .003 (see also Figure 8).

significant for male infants' preference, F(1, 18) = 3.36, p = .085, 𝜂 2p = 0.157. No other interactions
reached statistical significance (ps > .187). Motor-intensive activities and wheeled toy experiences
appeared to have more effects on female than on male infants' toy preferences, though these effects
do not seem to be large overall. It was not possible to look at the time spent playing with various toys
compared to preferences, as there were generally too many infants with zero time for many of the toys.
Finally, we examined parental attitudes toward gender stereotypes. Group results showed that
Australian parents seemed to provide generally non-stereotypical answers (Table 7). However, paren-
tal answers seemed to be more biased toward their own children's gender; that is, they were more likely
to agree with a claim if it favors the gender of their own child as compared to when the claim favors
the opposite gender (e.g., agree on girls are more social if they have a baby girl).
We also scored parental general attitudes across the claims to give a combined score where strongly
agree = 1, agree = 2, disagree = 3, and strongly disagree = 4 for a typical claim, with 5 and 6 re-
verse-coded and 10 left out because it was a repeat. The lower the score, the stronger bias parents
would have toward gender stereotypes. While no significant correlation was observed between the pa-
rental attitude score and overall infant looking preferences (rs < ±.265, ps > .089) or those with male
infants (rs < ±.144, ps > .544), female infants' preferences for strollers over tea sets were negatively
correlated with their parents' attitudes (r = −.641, p = .001). That is, preference for tea sets was even
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
LIU et al.    609

TABLE 6 Percentage of infants (14–16 months) reported as playing with each kind of toy

Tea sets
Walkers + strollers Cars + trucks Dolls + action figures + cups
Female 73% 55% 50% 91%
Male 55% 95% 30% 50%

T A B L E 7 Parental responses toward gender-related questions along a 4-point scale from strongly disagree to
strongly agree

Parents of male infants Parents of female infants All parents


Claim (n = 20) (n = 22) (n = 42)
01 Girls are more social 25.00% 45.45% 35.70%
02 Boys are more active 45.00% 31.82% 38.10%
03 Girls engage more with 30.00% 63.64% 47.62%
language
04 Boys like physical play 45.00% 27.27% 35.71%
more
05 Girls prefer trucks to dolls 05.00% 31.82% 19.05%
06 Boys prefer dolls to 10.00% 09.09% 09.52%
trucks
07 Girls prefer pink to blue 35.00% 27.27% 30.95%
08 Boys prefer blue to pink 20.00% 36.36% 28.57%
09 Girls have better 00.00% 09.09% 04.76%
imagination
10 Boys have better 20.00% 00.00% 09.52%
imagination
Note: The higher the percentage, the more parents agree or strongly agree with the claim.

stronger among female infants whose parents reported a less stereotypical view of gender specificity,
possibly due to a novelty effect.
A reviewer questioned whether our results could be influenced by other-race effects, given that
the faces in Experiment 1 were all Caucasian. Infants in our experiments were mostly, but not all,
Caucasian growing up in a majority Caucasian country. More importantly, our task was to compare
faces to other objects, not to compare faces from different groups. Results showed that faces or dolls
were always preferred except in the case of faces/dolls for male infants with their age growth. As we
replicate that lack of difference across two experiments with different sets of stimuli (including dolls
with a range of skin tones), it seems unlikely that other-race effects could explain the results.
To summarize, dolls remained the primary toy of interest for all infants, followed by cars and tea
sets, and strollers appear to be the least attractive across toys. The trend seemed to favor that infants
prefer toys with strong graspability (e.g., toy cars over strollers). In terms of gender-specific differ-
ences, female infants appear to prefer dolls to cars, whereas male infants do not show this preference.
Regarding the impact of nurture on toy preference, factors such as motor-intensive activities, wheeled
toy experiences and parental attitudes all predicted infants' preferences for dynamic/wheeled toys over
static toys, and such influence seemed to be overall stronger among female than male infants.
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
610    LIU et al.

3 | D IS C U S SION

The current study explored various factors that may play a role in infants' gender-specific toy prefer-
ences. Experiment 1 examined the age at which infants' gender-specific toy preference may emerge
testing infants aged 6–20 months on dolls versus cars or stoves (cf. Escudero et al., 2013), and showed
a change in preference such that male infants lose preference for dolls over cars as their age increases.
Experiment 2 then tested infants at 14–16 months, examining stimuli characteristics of locomotion,
graspability, children's participation in motor activities, their toy experience and parental attitudes to
gendered statements. In general, infants again preferred dolls and then graspable toys (tea sets or cars)
over strollers. The lack of preference between cars and dolls for males was replicated in this study.
Experiment 2 also showed female infants' preferences for dynamic over static toys are positively associ-
ated with motor activities, wheeled toy experiences, and less stereotypical gender attitudes in parents.

3.1 | Preference for faces

Infants demonstrated striking preferential similarities across experiments and studies: Faces are typi-
cally preferred over other stimuli (Leo & Simion, 2009; Pascalis et al., 2002; Quinn et al., 2008).
Recent evidence suggests that such tracking ability appears in prenatal period and therefore may be an
innate mechanism: A recent experiment projecting patterned stimuli through maternal tissue to fetuses
and detecting their reaction using 4D ultrasound shows that the third-trimester human fetuses actively
look toward three dots configured like a face but not toward three inverted configuration dots (Reid,
Dunn, Donovan, & Young, 2018; Reid et al., 2017). A similar trend continues upon birth (Farroni
et al., 2005; Johnson et al., 1991; Mondloch et al., 1999) and at 5 months (Escudero et al., 2013). The
current experiments extend such preference to 20 months after birth. Arguably, infants' attention to
faces may indicate their preference for surrounding cues that are socially relevant. Parents use facial
expressions to communicate with their children in addition to languages and gestures (Chong, Werker,
Russell, & Carroll, 2003), and infants' attention to faces is crucial for their survival as caretakers'
negative facial expression may indicate danger (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002; Montague & Walker-
Andrews, 2002). Infants' preference for face-like stimuli could fairly be described as innate given that
it appears before birth.1 Many previous studies on gendered toy preferences have chosen dolls as
“female-typical” toys. In the current studies, we used both doll faces (Experiment 1) and whole dolls
(Experiment 2), and found a general preference in all infants for dolls, except for males with age. An
interesting extension to this might be that infants prefer faces on toys in general. We leave this open
for future research.

3.2 | Females prefer dolls, but what do males prefer?

A number of studies have argued that transitions in infant gender-related preferences occur between
14 and 21 months after birth during which self-awareness toward gender-specific toys also increases
(Alexander & Saenz, 2012; Lauer & Yhang, 2016; Serbin et al., 2001; Zosuls et al., 2009). The previ-
ously reported developmental time window is in agreement with the outcomes of the current study,

1
Note we are not interested in a debate of whether the preference is for a face per se; if humans have evolved to prefer shapes
which, in a normal human environment, would direct their attention to faces more than other things, we consider that
sufficient.
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
LIU et al.    611

in which one of the main difference between male and female infants is that male infants around
15 months showed no preference for cars and dolls, unlike females who overall preferred dolls to cars.
Experiment 1 showed a developmental increase in male infants' preference for cars compared
to dolls, consistent with the finding from the recent meta-analysis (Todd et al., 2018). In addition,
Experiment 2 showed that male infants tended to prefer cars more than female infants when compared
to dolls. This finding is in line with previous literature showing female infants' consistent preference
for dolls (Bem, 1989; Blakemore, LaRue, & Olejnik, 1979; Connellan et al., 2000; Lutchmaya &
Baron-Cohen, 2002; Serbin et al., 2001; Signorella, Bigler, & Liben, 1993; Thompson, 1975), as
well as the meta-analysis (Lauer & Yhang, 2016) from which trends of stable gender-specific toy
preferences for female infants and age-related development for male infants were shown. Possible
explanations for this developmental difference in gender-specific preferences are discussed in the
following sections.

3.3 | Locomotion and motor-intensive activities

Though under debate (Lutchmaya & Baron-Cohen, 2002), one explanation to account for male in-
fants' shift toward a preference for cars may come from the locomotive status of the objects: Cars
are generally associated with the movement or mechanical motion as compared to dolls, which has
been argued to represent more social properties (Campbell, Shirley, Haywood, & Crook, 2000;
Zosuls et al., 2009). Though lacking direct evidence, it has been hypothesized that male infants may
have innate preferences for mechanical motion and female infants for social stimuli (Alexander &
Hines, 2002; Campbell et al., 2000; Zosuls et al., 2009). Benenson, Tennyson and Wrangham (2011)
observed that male infants are more likely to emulate propulsive movement than female infants at
6–9 months, and Todd et al. (2017) hypothesized that this may promote male infants' play styles that
are associated with their attraction to toy features that allow them to move in space. Alexander and
Hines (2002) argue that sexually dimorphic preferences for features like movement may evolve from
differential selection pressures based on the different behavioral roles of males and females, and that
evolved object feature preferences may contribute to gender-related toy preferences in children, and
Alexander and Saenz (2012) further suggest that prenatal hormones play a role in such preference.
In Experiment 2, data were gathered from participating families regarding infants' typically at-
tended activities, toy experiences and parental attitudes toward gender stereotypes. We found that
infants' preferences for some dynamic over static toys appear to be positively correlated with their time
spent on motor activities, leaning toward the hypothesis that children that spend more time in loco-
motive activities may have a stronger preference for the locomotive aspect of toys. In addition, female
infants' preferences for wheeled toys appeared to be positively related to the type of motor-intensive
activities and their previous experience with wheeled toys, but negatively correlated with parental
gender stereotypes. Male infants, however, do not show an evident similar trend. Thus, if anything, our
study supports the relationship of movement and locomotor aspects of female more than of male toy
preferences. However, the overall correlation between infants' engagement with movement activities
and their toy preferences suggests that individual experience rather than gender per se may play a role.

3.4 | Graspability

Male infants' emergent preference for cars over faces does not apply to all mechanical toys; that is,
they prefer cars but not stoves. However, male infants, as well as female infants, also prefer cars and
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
612    LIU et al.

tea sets over strollers but show no preference between cars and tea sets. Linking the two experiments
in the current study, graspability seems to play a larger role than gender. Although it is difficult to
interpret null results, if something linked to gender was the primary reason for a preference then we
would expect male infants to prefer cars to tea sets, and female infants, the reverse. Toy cars, dolls,
and tea sets may be more graspable for 15-month-olds than stoves and strollers due to their sizes and
shapes. It must be noted that examining and interpreting preferences based on specific toy characteris-
tics is challenging because it is difficult to adequately control the numerous lower-level variables and
differences that may be driving category-level preferences. Our speculation with graspability needs to
be treated with caution and further examined in future research.

3.5 | Parental attitude and familiarity

The establishment of gender-specific toy preferences has also been argued to be largely depend-
ent on postnatal influences from the ambient environment (Bandura, 1977; Ehrhardt & Meyer-
Bahlburg, 1981; Fagot & Hagan, 1991; Langlois & Downs, 1990; Martin, 1999; Martin et al., 1990).
Experiment 2 reported a small but not robust correlation between parental attitudes and infants' gen-
der-specific toy preferences. As Table 7 illustrates, parents appear to be generally aware of gender
stereo-typicality. For instance, the majority of parents disagree with the claim that male infants are
more physically active than female infants or that male infants like blue and female infants like pink.
However, these responses do not necessarily guide parental actions and choices for their infants in
real-life settings, as shown by infants' actual toy experience in Table 7: Male and female 14- to
16-month-old infants have a clear difference in their experience with cars and trucks (95% vs. 55%).
One possibility is that parents were aware of the gendered issue and provided responses that are so-
cially desirable, irrespective of their actual toy choices for their children (which appears to be fairly
gender-specific), and thus conforming to the claim that parents contribute to their infants' gender-
specific socialization (Boe & Woods, 2018; Roopnarine, 1986; Weinraub et al., 1984; Weisgram &
Bruun, 2018), even though they may be unaware of their choices. Note that the current study is unable
to tell which of these may be true.
Infant preferences for toys, or at least those of female infants as observed in the current study,
appear to be influenced by parents' selection, which is often “gendered” from the beginning of life.
Children's physical and social environments are typically gender-congruent as they engage in corre-
sponding conducts and activities (Blakemore et al., 1979). Playing with socially defined gender-con-
gruent toys, for instance, may be rewarding to infants through shared attention with significant others
(Alexander et al., 2009). It should also be noted that the majority of parental responses were from
mothers. As they are the primary caregivers of participants, it is likely that they were also responsible
for selecting the toys for their children. There may be differences between male and female caretakers
on their attitudes to toys and gender. We leave these potential factors for future research, but encour-
age greater diversity in children's toy play to promote a reduction in gender stereotypes (Dinella &
Weisgram, 2018; Joel et al., 2015; Kollmayer et al., 2018; Spinner, Cameron, & Calogero, 2018).

3.6 | Methodological considerations

Our study had several methodological strengths: Besides examining the effect of age in a large cohort
of infants between 6 and 20 months, the range of individual differences in infants' preference was
also examined at a smaller time window. In addition, the current design directly measured infants'
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
LIU et al.    613

preferences between two items, addressing the limitations of previous studies looking at one item at a
time (cf. Alexander et al., 2009). In addition, a substantial number of trials per pair were included to
improve validity. Furthermore, we used a range of items with a range of colors as gender-typed colors
have been shown to affect preschool children's toy preferences (Weisgram, Fulcher, & Dinella, 2014;
Yeung & Wong, 2018), thus improving the generalizability of results.
It is, however, important to consider the inclusion of observational and behavioral measures in fu-
ture research as infant looking preferences may not accurately represent or translate to actual behavior.
It would be interesting to examine whether looking preferences at around 15 months predict actual
play/behavior using a grasping task or a longitudinal design. Controlling for gender, a recent study
by Lauer, Ilksoy, and Lourenco (2018) showed a positive correlation between infants' gender-specific
toy preference at 6–13 months and their gender-typed play preferences at age 4. Findings indicate
children's gender-specific toy preferences may have a long-lasting effect along their developmental
trajectory.

4 | CO NC LUSION

The gradual trend between 6 and 20 months and the evident display of gender-specific toy preference at
14–16 months in the current study conform to social cognitive theories on infant emerging understanding of
gender (Bussey & Bandura, 1999; Martin, Ruble, & Szkrybalo, 2002). Rather than during the phallic stage
(Freud, 1963), conceptual gender categories and gender identification begin in infancy (Chodorow, 1978;
Courage & Howe, 2002; Poulin-Dubois, Serbin, Eichstedt, Sen, & Beissel, 2002; Poulin-Dubois, Serbin,
Kenyon, & Derbyshire, 1994) and continue to develop in the second year after birth.
The current study provides interesting and relevant contributions to the literature by examining
the relationship between looking preferences to gendered toys, objects, and faces and parents' at-
titudes, as well as children's toy exposure. It is among the first to study toy preferences at a range
of ages in infancy and suggests 15 months to be the time window around which gender-specific
toy differences may emerge. Further, it investigates the relationship between prior exposure and toy
preferences. The study contributes knowledge to theoretical approaches regarding the development
of gender-typed preferences, which may be restricted and related to specific toys under investigation.
Although human evolution provides biological potentialities, the conceptions and roles of gender may
be the product of a broad network of social influences (Bussey & Bandura, 1999). The development
of gender-specific toy preference may be a result of biological, cognitive, and socialization factors
(Alexander & Hines, 2002; Fausto-Sterling, 1992; Hines, 2010; Martin et al., 2002; Ruble, Martin,
& Berenbaum, 1998) working together for children to reach the patterns observed in adulthood
(Alexander & Hines, 2002; Fausto-Sterling, 1992; Hines, 2010). In conclusion, our study illustrated
gender-specific toy preference among infants aged 6 to 20 months, with a gender difference emerging
at around 15 months. Age, stimuli characteristics, infant daily motor activities, experience with toys,
and parental attitudes all appear to play a role in the developmental trajectory of such preference,
shaping the current and future perception and cognition across infants.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors declare no conflicts of interest with regard to the funding source for this study. For data
collection, this study was supported by a Career Interruption Grant from Western Sydney University
awarded to Rachel Robbins, a scholarship from the Western Sydney University Student Research
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
614    LIU et al.

Program awarded to Cristina Quattropani, and a Research Excellence (Researcher of the Year) Award
from Western Sydney University awarded to Paola Escudero. Paola Escudero's contribution to manu-
script writing was supported by an ARC Future Fellowship (FT160100514). Liquan Liu's manuscript
writing was supported by a European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under
the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement No. 798658 who is hosted by Center for Multilingualism
across the Lifespan at the University of Oslo, financed by Research Council of Norway through its
Centers of Excellence funding scheme grant agreement No. 223265. We would like to thank Jessica
Bazouni for her valuable comments, School of Social Sciences and Psychology and the MARCS
Institute for Brain, Behaviour and Development at Western Sydney University for research and tech-
nical support, reviewers of Infancy who helped us improve the manuscript, and, most importantly, all
families that participated in this study.

ORCID
Liquan Liu https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8671-5098
Paola Escudero https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8071-7663
Christina Quattropani https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0111-1215
Rachel A. Robbins https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9363-4528

R E F E R E NC E S
Alexander, G. M., & Hines, M. (2002). Sex differences in responses to children's toys in a non-human primate
(Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus). Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 467–479.
Alexander, G. M., & Saenz, J. (2012). Early androgens, activity levels and toy choices of children in the second year of
life. Hormones and Behavior, 62(4), 500–504. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.08.008
Alexander, G. M., Wilcox, T., & Woods, R. (2009). Sex differences in infants' visual interest in toys. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 38, 427–433. https://doi.org/10.1007/s1050​8-008-9430-1
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bem, S. L. (1989). Genital knowledge and gender consistency in preschool children. Child Development, 60, 649–662.
Benenson, J. F., Tennyson, R., & Wrangham, R. W. (2011). Male more than female infants imitate propulsive motion.
Cognition, 121(2), 262–267.
Berenbaum, S. A. (1999). Effects of early androgens on sex-typed activities and interests in adolescents with congenital
adrenal hyperplasia. Hormones and behavior, 35(1), 102–110.
Berenbaum, S. A., & Hines, M. (1992). Early androgens are related to childhood sex-typed toy preferences. Psychological
Science, 3, 203–206. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb000​28.x
Blakemore, J. E., LaRue, A. A., & Olejnik, A. B. (1979). Sex-appropriate toy preference and the ability to conceptualize
toys as sex-role related. Developmental Psychology, 15(3), 339. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.15.3.339
Boe, J. L., & Woods, R. J. (2018). Parents' influence on infants' gender-typed toy preferences. Sex Roles, 79(5-6),
358–373. https://doi.org/10.1007/s1119​9-017-0858-4
Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological
Review, 106(4), 676. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.676
Campbell, A., Shirley, L., Haywood, C., & Crook, C. (2000). Infants' visual preference for sex-contingent babies, chil-
dren, toys, and activities: A longitudinal study. British Journal of Psychology, 18, 479–498.
Chodorow, N. (1978). Mothering, object-relations, and the female oedipal configuration. Feminist Studies, 4(1), 137–
158. https://doi.org/10.2307/3177630
Chong, S. C. F., Werker, J. F., Russell, J. A., & Carroll, J. M. (2003). Three facial expressions mothers direct to their
infants. Infant and Child Development: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 12(3), 211–232.
Connellan, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Batki, A., & Ahluwalia, J. (2000). Sex differences in human neonatal
social perception. Infant Behavior and Development, 23, 113–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163​-6383(00)00032​-1
Connor, J. M., & Serbin, L. A. (1977). Behaviorally based masculine and feminine activity preferences scales for pre-
schoolers: Correlates with other classroom behaviors and cognitive tests. Child Development, 48, 1411–1416.
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
LIU et al.    615

Courage, M. L., & Howe, M. L. (2002). From infant to child: The dynamics of cognitive change in the second year of
life. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 250. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.250
Dinella, L. M., & Weisgram, E. S. (2018). Gender-typing of children's toys: Causes, consequences, and correlates. Sex
Roles, 79(5–6), 253–259. https://doi.org/10.1007/s1119​9-018-0943-3
Ehrhardt, A. A., & Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. (1981). Effects of prenatal sex hormones on gender-related behavior. Science,
211(4488), 1312–1318.
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). Is there an in-group advantage in emotion recognition?
Escudero, P., Robbins, R., & Johnson, S. (2013). Sex-related preferences for real and doll faces versus real and toy objects
in young infants and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 367–379. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jecp.2013.07.001
Fagot, B. I., & Hagan, R. (1991). Observations of parent reactions to sex-stereotyped behaviors: Age and sex effects.
Child Development, 62, 617–628. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131135
Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., Menon, E., Zulian, L., Faraguna, D., & Csibra, G. (2005). Newborns' preference for face-rel-
evant stimuli: Effects of contrast polarity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America, 102(47), 17245–17250. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.05022​05102
Fausto-Sterling, A. (1992). Myths of gender: Biological theories about women and men. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Freud, A. (1963). The concept of developmental lines. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 18(1), 245–265. https://
doi.org/10.1080/00797​308.1963.11822930
Frisch, H. L. (1977). Sex stereotypes in adult-infant play. Child Development, 48(4), 1671–1675. https://doi.
org/10.2307/1128533
Fulcher, M., & Hayes, A. R. (2018). Building a pink dinosaur: The effects of gendered construction toys on girls' and
boys' play. Sex Roles, 79(5–6), 273–284. https://doi.org/10.1007/s1119​9-017-0806-3
Golden, J. C., & Jacoby, J. W. (2018). Playing princess: Preschool girls' interpretations of gender stereotypes in Disney
princess media. Sex Roles, 79(5–6), 299–313. https://doi.org/10.1007/s1119​9-017-0773-8
Hassett, J. M., Siebert, E. R., & Wallen, K. (2008). Sex differences in rhesus monkey toy preferences parallel those of
children. Hormones and Behavior, 54, 359–364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.03.008
Hines, M. (2010). Sex-related variation in human behavior and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 448–456.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.07.005
Hines, M., & Kaufman, F. R. (1994). Androgen and the development of human sex-typical behavior: Rough-and-tumble
play and sex of preferred playmates in children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH). Child Development, 65,
1042–1053. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131303
Jadva, V., Hines, M., & Golombok, S. (2010). Infants' preferences for toys, colors, and shapes: Sex differences and sim-
ilarities. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39(6), 1261–1273. https://doi.org/10.1007/s1050​8-010-9618-z
Joel, D., Berman, Z., Tavor, I., Wexler, N., Gaber, O., Stein, Y., … Assaf, Y. (2015). Sex beyond the genitalia: The
human brain mosaic. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(50),
15468–15473. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.15096​54112
Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., & Morton, J. (1991). Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and
its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(91)90045​-6
Kitamura, C., & Burnham, D. (1998). Acoustic and affective qualities of IDS in English. In Fifth international confer-
ence on spoken language processing.
Kitamura, C., & Burnham, D. (2003). Pitch and communicative intent in mother's speech: Adjustments for age and sex
in the first year. Infancy, 4(1), 85–110. https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532​7078I​N0401_5
Kollmayer, M., Schultes, M. T., Schober, B., Hodosi, T., & Spiel, C. (2018). Parents' judgments about the desirability of
toys for their children: Associations with gender role attitudes, gender-typing of toys, and demographics. Sex Roles,
79(5–6), 329–341. https://doi.org/10.1007/s1119​9-017-0882-4
Lamminmäki, A., Hines, M., Kuiri-Hänninen, T., Kilpeläinen, L., Dunkel, L., & Sankilampi, U. (2012). Testosterone
measured in infancy predicts subsequent sex-typed behavior in boys and in girls. Hormones and Behavior, 61(4),
611–616. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.02.013
Langlois, J. H., & Downs, A. C. (1990). Mothers, fathers, and peers as socialization agents of sex-typed play behaviors
in young children. Child Development, 51, 1237–1247. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129566
Lauer, J. E., Ilksoy, S. D., & Lourenco, S. F. (2018). Developmental stability in gender-typed preferences between in-
fancy and preschool age. Developmental Psychology, 54(4), 613. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev00​00468
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
616    LIU et al.

Lauer, J. E., & Yhang, E. (2016). The development of gender-typed toy preferences in infancy: A meta-analytic review
and consideration of key variables. (Poster presentation). In ICIS biennial meeting, New Orleans, Louisiana, USA.
Leaper, C., Anderson, K. J., & Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of gender effects on parents' talk to their children: A
meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34(1), 3. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.34.1.3
Leaper, C., Leve, L., Strasser, T., & Schwartz, R. (1995). Mother-child communication sequences: Play activity, child
gender, and marital status effects. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1982, 307–327.
Leo, I., & Simion, F. (2009). Newborns' Mooney face perception. Infancy, 14, 641–653. https://doi.org/10.1080/15250​
00090​3264047
Liss, M. B. (1981). Patterns of toy play: An analysis of sex differences. Sex Roles, 7, 1143–1150. https://doi.org/10.1007/
BF002​87590
Lutchmaya, S., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). Human sex differences in social and non-social looking preferences at 12
months of age. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 319–325. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163​-6383(02)00095​-4
Martin, C. L. (1999). A developmental perspective on gender effects and gender concepts. In W. B. Swann, & J. H.
Langlois (Eds.), Sexism and stereotypes in modern society: The gender science of Janet Taylor Spence (pp. 45–73).
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological
Bulletin, 128(6), 903. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.903
Martin, C. L., Wood, C. H., & Little, J. K. (1990). The development of gender stereotype components. Child Development,
61, 1891–1904. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130845
Mondloch, C. J., Lewis, T. L., Budreau, D. R., Maurer, D., Dannemiller, J. L., Stephens, B. R., & Kleiner-Gathercoal,
K. A. (1999). Face perception during early infancy. Psychological Science, 10(5), 419–422. https://doi.
org/10.1111/1467-9280.00179
Montague, D. P., & Walker-Andrews, A. S. (2002). Mothers, fathers, and infants: The role of person familiarity and
parental involvement in infants’ perception of emotion expressions. Child development, 73(5), 1339–1352.
Pascalis, O., de Haan, M., & Nelson, C. A. (2002). Is face processing species-specific during the first year of life?
Science, 296(5571), 1321–1323.
Pasterski, V. L., Geffner, M. E., Brain, C., Hindmarsh, P., Brook, C., & Hines, M. (2005). Prenatal hormones and post-
natal socialization by parents as determinants of male-typical toy play in girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia.
Child Development, 76, 264–278. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00843.x
Pomerleau, A., Malcuit, G., Turgeon, L., & Cossette, A. L. (1997). Effects of labelled gender on vocal communica-
tion of young women with 4-month-old infants. International Journal of Psychology, 32(2), 65–72. https://doi.
org/10.1080/00207​59974​00872
Poulin-Dubois, D., Serbin, L. A., Eichstedt, J. A., Sen, M. G., & Beissel, C. F. (2002). Men don't put on make-up:
Toddlers' knowledge of the gender stereotyping of household activities. Social Development, 11(2), 166–181.
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00193
Poulin-Dubois, D., Serbin, L. A., Kenyon, B., & Derbyshire, A. (1994). Infants' intermodal knowledge about gender.
Developmental Psychology, 30(3), 436. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.30.3.436
Quinn, P. D., Kelly, D. J., Lee, K., Pascalis, O., & Slater, A. M. (2008). Preference for attractive faces in human infants
extends beyond conspecifics. Developmental Science, 11, 76–83. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00647.x
Quinn, P. C., Yahr, J., Kuhn, A., Slater, A. M., & Pascalis, O. (2002). Representation of the gender of human faces by
infants: A preference for female. Perception, 31(9), 1109–1121. https://doi.org/10.1068/p3331
Reich, S. M., Black, R. W., & Foliaki, T. (2018). Constructing difference: LEGO® set narratives promote stereotypic
gender roles and play. Sex Roles, 79(5–6), 285–298. https://doi.org/10.1007/s1119​9-017-0868-2
Reid, V. M., Dunn, K., Donovan, T., Young, R. (2018). Response to Scheel et al Current Biology, 28(10), R596–R597.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.03.048
Reid, V. M., Dunn, K., Young, R. J., Amu, J., Donovan, T., & Reissland, N. (2017). The human fetus preferentially en-
gages with face-like visual stimuli. Current Biology, 27(12), 1825–1828. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.044
Roopnarine, J. L. (1986). Mothers' and fathers' behaviors toward the toy play of their infant sons and daughters. Sex
Roles, 14(1), 59–68. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF002​87848
Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (1998). Gender development. Handbook of child psychology.
Serbin, L. A., Poulin-Dubois, D., Colburne, K. A., Sen, M. G., & Eichstedt, J. A. (2001). Gender stereotyping in in-
fancy: Visual preferences for and knowledge of gender-stereotyped toys in the second year. International Journal of
Behavioral Development, 25(1), 7–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650​25004​2000078
|

15327078, 2020, 5, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/infa.12352 by Cochrane Chile, Wiley Online Library on [19/02/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
LIU et al.    617

Servin, A., Bohlin, G., & Berlin, L. (1999). Sex differences in 1-, 3-, and 5-year-olds' toy choice in a structured play
session. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 40(1), 43–48. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9450.00096
Signorella, M., Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (1993). Developmental differences in children's gender schemata about
others: A meta-analytic review. Special issue: Early gender-role development. Developmental Review, 13, 147–183.
https://doi.org/10.1006/drev.1993.1007
Smith, C., & Lloyd, B. (1978). Maternal behavior and perceived sex of infant: Revisited. Child Development, 49(4),
1263–1265. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128775
Spinner, L., Cameron, L., & Calogero, R. (2018). Peer toy play as a gateway to children's gender flexibility: The ef-
fect of (counter)stereotypic portrayals of peers in children's magazines. Sex Roles, 79(5–6), 314–328. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s1119​9-017-0883-3.
Thompson, S. R. (1975). Gender labels and early sex role development. Child Development, 46, 339–347. https://doi.
org/10.2307/1128126
Todd, B. K., Barry, J. A., & Thommessen, S. A. (2017). Preferences for ‘gender-typed’ toys in boys and girls aged 9 to
32 months. Infant and Child Development, 26(3), e1986.
Todd, B. K., Fischer, R. A., Di Costa, S., Roestorf, A., Harbour, K., Hardiman, P., & Barry, J. A. (2018). Sex dif-
ferences in children's toy preferences: A systematic review, meta-regression, and meta-analysis. Infant and Child
Development, 27(2), e2064. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2064
van de Beek, C., van Goozen, S. H., Buitelaar, J. K., & Cohen-Kettenis, P. T. (2009). Prenatal sex hormones (maternal
and amniotic fluid) and gender-related play behavior in 13-month-old infants. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38(1),
6–15. https://doi.org/10.1007/s1050​8-007-9291-z
Weinraub, M., Clemens, L. P., Sockloff, A., Ethridge, T., Gracely, E., & Myers, B. (1984). The development of sex role
stereotypes in the third year: Relationships to gender labeling, gender identity, sex-types toy preference, and family
characteristics. Child Development, 55(4), 1493–1503. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130019
Weisgram, E. S., & Bruun, S. T. (2018). Predictors of gender-typed toy purchases by prospective parents and mothers:
The roles of childhood experiences and gender attitudes. Sex Roles, 79(5-6), 342–357. https://doi.org/10.1007/s1119​
9-018-0928-2
Weisgram, E. S., Fulcher, M., & Dinella, L. M. (2014). Pink gives girls permission: Exploring the roles of explicit
gender labels and gender-typed colors on preschool children's toy preferences. Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology, 35(5), 401–409. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2014.06.004
Yeung, S. P., & Wong, W. I. (2018). Gender labels on gender-neutral colors: Do they affect children's color preferences
and play performance? Sex Roles, 79(5–6), 260–272. https://doi.org/10.1007/s1119​9-017-0875-3
Zosuls, K. M., Ruble, D. N., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Shrout, P. E., Bornstein, M. H., & Greulich, F. K. (2009). The
acquisition of gender labels in infancy: Implications for sex-typed play. Developmental Psychology, 45, 688–701.

SUPPORTING INFORMATION
Additional supporting information may be found online in the Supporting Information section.

How to cite this article: Liu L, Escudero P, Quattropani C, Robbins RA. Factors affecting
infant toy preferences: Age, gender, experience, motor development, and parental attitude.
Infancy. 2020;25:593–617. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12352

You might also like