Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

The Attractiveness of the Average Face

Margaret Collins

This article explores the connection between perceived beauty and average-
ness. Although throughout the history of mankind attractiveness was linked
to averageness, this concept is now being questioned in light of more recent
developments in Evolutionary Psychology. What follows is in an attempt to
open up the discussion on one aspect of beauty, averageness, including
literature from the Humanities and Evolutionary Psychology. (Semin Orthod
2012;18:217-228.) © 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

have approached this article following the After Roentgen’s discovery of x-rays in 1895,4
I recommendation of Edward Angle (1865-
1930): All who hope to attain real success in the
and Pacini’s (1922)5 and Hofrath’s (1931)6 stan-
dardization of skull radiographs,7 craniometry was
correction of malocclusion should cultivate a love for transferred to the radiographic image.8 Cephalo-
art and the beautiful . . . An appreciation and intelli- metric analyses based on norms began in 1952
gent application of the principle of art must ever go when Downs averaged a sample of 12-17 year olds
hand in hand with the successful practice of ortho- who had normal occlusion.9 Others followed in
dontia.1 pursuit of this ideal average. Ricketts10 averaged
Averages are important for orthodontists findings of 1000 clinical cases. McNamara11 com-
when planning treatment. Their use in anthro- bined the average values of the Bolton, Burling-
pometry date back to the second half of the 19th ton, and Ann Arbour growth studies. It is notewor-
century, when the basic laws of statistics were thy that the cephalometric analysis undertaken by
well established. In 1842, the Swedish anatomist Reidel on 30 “Seattle Seafair Princesses” showed
Anders Retzius (1796-1860) introduced the cra- that each beauty queen was close to the average,
nial index categorizing skulls as dolicho-, meso-, and the degree of similarity between these girls was
and brachycephalic. The Facial Index subse- striking, mirroring findings recorded in previous
quently categorized faces into lepto-, meso-, and studies of normal occlusion. The girl selected as
euryprosopic2 and the Nasal index into lepto-, queen among this group of princesses recorded
meso-, and platyrhine.3 In each case, the breadth measurements, which fell within 1 degree or 1 mm
was related to the length and the midpoint or of the mean in almost every instance. Reidel12
average was identified and prefixed by the term concluded that the public’s concept of acceptable
“meso.” These indexes continued into the 20th facial aesthetics was close to the average and in
century when Farkas developed normal propor- close agreement with standards established by or-
tional indexes based on measurements from thodontists on the basis of normal occlusion. Oth-
1312 Caucasians’ heads.2 ers have also found a preference for a normal
(Skeletal Class I) profile.13
However, cephalometric norms/averages are
not always available to define beauty. An exam-
Consultant Orthodontist, Department of Orthodontics, The Den- ple of this is seen in the Sculpture of Queen Nefertiti
tal Institute, King’s College Hospital, Denmark Hill, London, Eng-
land; Consultant Orthodontist, Department of Orthodontics, Queen
(1370-1330 BC), which dates back to the dawn of
Mary’s Hospital, Sidcup, Kent, England. Egyptian civilization, a time when the Nile valley
Address correspondence to Margaret Collins, BDS, FDSRCPS, swamps were settled by people from Africa and
DOrth, MSc, MOrthRCS, MA, Orthodontic Department, The Den- Asia. Nefertiti has been extolled as a standard of
tal Institute, King’s College Hospital, London SE5 9RS, England. beauty throughout the history of man,14 not sur-
E-mail: margaretcollins4@hotmail.co.uk
© 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
prising, as her features represented the average
1073-8746/12/1803-0$30.00/0 of the population in which she lived, of Asian
http://dx.doi.org/10.1053/j.sodo.2012.04.007 and African origin. However, we would struggle

Seminars in Orthodontics, Vol 18, No 3 (September), 2012: pp 217-228 217


218 Collins

beautiful of virgins.” The citizens of Croton gath-


ered the virgins into 1 place from which Zeuxis
chose 5:
For he did not believe that it was possible to find in one body
all the things he looked for in beauty, since nature has not
refined to perfection any single object in all its parts. Conse-
quently, Zeuxis selected the best features of each of the virgins
whom he had chosen to serve as models for his paintings.16

The Greek physician, Galen (c.129-199 AC),


subsequently reinforced this idea when he ob-
served that “whatever form is most beautiful in
man” or animal is found by seeking “the mean
within each genus.”17
The Renaissance artists, in turn, were strongly
influenced by the Greeks, mentally merging the
images of many, to produce the ideal. When the
fresco of the beautiful and Greek mythological
nymph Galatea was finished (1483-1520), Raphael
was asked where he had found a model of such
beauty; he replied that:

Figure 1. Bust Sculpture of Queen Nefertiti. c. 1348-


1336/5 BC. Painted Limestone. Height 48.3 cm.
Ägyptisches Museum, Berlin. Nefertiti’s appealing fea-
tures represent the average of the Asian and African
population in which she lived. (Reprinted with per-
mission from bpk, Berlin/Aegyptisches Museum, Sta-
atliche Museen, Berlin, Germany/Margarete Büsing/
Art Resource, NY.) (Color version of figure is available
online.)

to find a set of cephalometric norms to accu-


rately represent her features, although radio-
graphic examination of the mummified remains
of ancient Egyptians confirm a high incidence of
bimaxillary prognathism (Fig. 1).15

The Attractiveness of the Average Face


From a Historical Perspective
Early Greek sculpture, such as the late 2nd cen-
tury BC Venus de Milo illustrate the Greek por- Figure 2. Detail from Venus de Milo. c. 2nd-1st century
trayal of beauty (Fig. 2). The link between it and BC. Greek marble. Height 204 cm. Musée du Louvre,
the average/mean was documented by Cicero Paris. Ancient Greek sculpture, such as Venus de Milo,
(106-44 BC) who reported that when the people was influenced by the existing philosophy that beauty
should be sought from the mean of the population.
of Croton hired the esteemed artist, Zeuxis of (Reprinted with permission. © RMN-Grand Palais/Art
Herakleia, to paint a beautiful Helen, he re- Resource, NY.) (Color version of figure is available
quested that they provide him “with the most online.)
Attractiveness of the Average Face 219

man21 sought to determine whether stabilizing


selection, favoring average traits, was the key to
attractiveness. They superimposed one photo-
graphic facial image on top of another, to obtain
an average composite face. Composites look fa-
miliar and attractive because they are average or
representative faces, resembling many faces.20
This idea dates back to the philosopher Im-
manuel Kant22 (1724-1804) who, in “The Cri-
tique of Judgment,” referred to the mind’s abil-
ity to “superimpose as it were one image upon
another” and from this obtain a “mean contour”
or “average size,” which “underlies the normal
idea of a beautiful man.”
The fundamental relevance of the mean/av-
erage was developed further by the Belgian as-
tronomer and statistician Adolphe Quetelet
(1796-1874) who defined the “average man”
(l’h’omme moyen) in his treatise, Sur L’homme.
Quetelet compared his study of human bodily
proportions with those of the artist Albrecht

Figure 3. Raphael The nymph Galatea. c. 1512-1514.


Fresco.3 ⫻ 2.2 m. Sala di Galatea, Villa Farnesina,
Rome. The Renaissance artist Raphael created The
nymph Galatea by merging the features of “several
beauties.” (Reprinted with permission from Scala/Art
Resource, NY.) (Color version of figure is available
online.)

In order to paint a beauty I would have to see several


beauties, but since there is a scarcity of beautiful women, I
use a certain Idea that comes to mind (Fig. 3).

Similarly, Leonardo da Vinci (1511-1574) ap-


plied these principles when painting The head of
Leda and advised that to obtain beauty, the artist
should “consider what he saw and consult him-
self, choosing the most excellent part of every-
thing” (Fig. 4).18
This idea was taken further by Albrecht Dürer
(1471-1528) in Four Books on Human Proportion,
when he took detailed measurements from sev-
eral hundred individuals in an attempt to come
up with the average perfect canon of beauty, Figure 4. Detail from Leonardo da Vinci The head of
realizing that no single canon of beauty could be Leda. c. 1505-1506. Pen and ink over black chalk.
achieved.19 The Queens Gallery, Buckingham Palace, London.
The preference for facial averageness has Leonardo da Vinci, who struggled to find beauty in
been explained in terms of stabilizing selection, any single individual, sought instead the “most ex-
cellent part of everything” to create beauty seen in
where sexual selection favors the average (as The head of Leda. (Reprinted with permission from
opposed to directional selection which favors Royal Collection Trust/© HM Queen Elizabeth II
extremes traits).20 In 1990, Langlois and Rogg- 2012.) (Color version of figure is available online.)
220 Collins

Dürer. He argued that “the greater the number enable me to hang them up one in front of the other, like a
of individuals observed, the more do individual pack of cards, upon the same pair of pins, in such a way that
peculiarities, whether physical or moral, become the eyes of all the portraits shall be nearly as possible
superimposed.
effaced.” He noticed that the anthropometric
data he collected fell into a bell-shaped curve, He also recognized the attractiveness of the
the central portion of which, was called “the average image created:
mean.” This point represented the “average
man” who was the “type of all which is beauti- All composites are better looking than their components be-
cause the averaged portrait of many persons is free from the
ful— of all which is good.”23 During the same
irregularities that variously blemish the looks of each of them.
time era, others were trying to come up with a
visual version of the average. The English phi- Fueled by this success, Galton attempted,
losopher Herbert Spencer (1820-1903), famous through the creation of average composites, to
for coining the term “survival of the fittest,” define the “criminal type” using photographic
devised an instrument for tracing the longitudi- portraits of criminals convicted of murder, man-
nal, transverse, and horizontal sections of heads slaughter, or robbery accompanied with vio-
onto transparent paper, to superimpose them. lence. He failed to find the villainous face he
At the same time but on the opposite side of the sought. On the contrary, the average criminal
world, a New Zealander, A.L. Austin, came up face was, to his surprise, attractive:
with a similar idea and wrote a letter to Charles
..The features of the composite are much better looking than
Darwin observing that:
those of the components. The special villainous irregularities
I find by taking two ordinary carte-de-visite photos of two in the latter have disappeared, and the common humanity
different persons’ faces, the portraits being about the same that underlies them has prevailed . . . They represent, not the
sizes, and looking about the same direction, and placing criminal, but the man who is liable to fall into crime.14
them in a stereoscope, the faces blend into one in a most
remarkable manner, producing in the case of some ladies’ In 1885, Galton26 was commissioned by the
portraits, in every instance, a decided improvement in Jewish historian, Joseph Jacobs (1854-1916), to
beauty.24 produce a composite image of Jewish boys, “The
Jewish Type.” The aim was to “bring together all
Galton believed that the composite portrait the data, scientific or historical, which bear
was the pictorial equivalent of Quetelet’s statis- upon the purity of the Jewish race.” Galton took
tical tables photographs of 13 Jewish boys, chosen at ran-
The process is one of pictorial statistics, suitable to give us dom from the “Jews’ Free School, Bell Lane” and
generic pictures of man, such as Quetelet obtained in outline by superimposing one on top of the other, cre-
by ordinary numerical methods of statistics, as described in ated a series of composite photographs, which
his work on Anthropométrie . . . By the process of composites he called “The Jewish Type.”27 The composite of
we obtain a picture and not merely an outline.25 the Jewish face turned out to be “wonderfully
beautiful” (Fig. 5).26 He also attempted to use
Galton reinforced Quetelet’s view, that the
facial composite photography as a means of dif-
“average” was attractive. He developed a way of
ferentiating phthisical patients (suffering from
creating a composite photograph, which was the
tuberculosis) from non-phthisical patients. But
physical equivalent of Kant’s imaginary superim-
he failed to find a connection. The composite
posed images when he submitted a composite
face did not reveal the illness.28 The French
facial image to the Anthropological Institute in
photographer Arthur Batut (1846-1919) further
1879, which he achieved by throwing “faint im-
developed Galton’s composite methodology. His
ages of the several portraits, in succession, upon
composites were designed to reveal the local
the same sensitized photographic plate.” The
characteristics of the beauty of the Arlésienne
result of the composite was “an imaginary figure
(thought to be a mixture of Roman and Saracen
possessing the average feature of any given
blood) with their allegedly less attractive sisters
group of men.” He described how he created the
in the port of Agde in the Languedoc (thought
image:
to be of Greek origin).29
I begin by collecting photographs of the persons with whom I By the end of the 1840s, photography was
propose to deal . . . I make two pinholes in each of them, to heavily involved in documentation and quantita-
Attractiveness of the Average Face 221

Figure 5. The average image obtained from early composite photography revealed that The Jewish Type was
“wonderfully beautiful.” (Reprinted with permission from Jacobs.27) (Color version of figure is available online.)

tive measurement. Darwin’s theory of evolution The Attractiveness of the Average Face
led to a new way of developing social structures, From a Contemporary Perspective
which required stratification of man. Photogra-
phy was used to measure and classify the human In 1990, Langlois and Roggman21 used the same
body in an attempt to define the characteristics principles as those described by Galton to digi-
of a particular race, class, or social group.30 In tize and mathematically average male and fe-
1875, an Anthropometric and Racial Committee male students’ faces and produce computer-gen-
was set up in by the British Association for the erated composite images, which were then
Advancement of Science. Photography was the ranked for attractiveness. The averaged compos-
medium used for its documentation.29 Galton’s ite of the 32 faces were judged as significantly
composite photographs were a part of this pre- more attractive than the individual faces that
occupation with categorization of man. Follow- yielded them. In addition, the composite face
ing Charles Darwin’s (Galton’s first cousin) pub- became more attractive as more faces were en-
lication of On the Origin of Species (1859) and The tered. They concluded that attractive faces were
Descent of Man (1871), Galton also became pre- only average, and that this was “consistent with
occupied with the idea of improving one’s race. evolutionary pressures that favor characteristics
In 1904, Galton coined the term “eugenics,” close to the mean of the population” (Fig. 6).21
which comes from the Greek, “eu” meaning This view is supported by another study that
“well,” and gen, meaning “produce.”31 showed that full-face and profile views were per-
Eugenics is the science which deals with all influences that ceived as less attractive as they were morphed
improve and develop inborn qualities of a race. The aim of away from the average.34 The profile view added
eugenics is to represent each class or sect by its best specimens, an additional advantage in that it allowed a face
causing them to contribute more than their proportion to the to be morphed toward an average shape without
next generation.32
impacting on symmetry.34 Furthermore, aver-
Galton, however, never made the connection aged composite profile photographs have been
between his 2 great interests, the composite fa- shown to be more attractive than the original
cial average and “good genes.”33 The link be- photographs from which the composite was de-
tween the 2 would take almost another century rived.35
to establish by the evolutionary psychologists There are a number of possible explanations
Langlois and Roggman. for the attractiveness of the average face.
222 Collins

prototype is the central representation of any


category, the average or mean value; it is per-
ceived as “typical.” The average of a set of faces
is perceived as attractive because it appears more
facelike and more representative of the popula-
tion. Any face will be judged as attractive if it is
close to the average of the central tendency of
the population.21 Prototypes might be recog-
nized faster and more easily, and thus may cre-
ate higher nervous excitation. Our preference
for average stimuli may be because our brain
accepts such better-fitting stimuli more willingly
(neuroaesthetics).36 Typical faces close to the
population average are consistently rated as
more attractive than distinctive faces. Further-
more, the attractiveness of individual faces can
be increased or reduced, by moving their con-
figurations toward or away from an average con-
figuration for that sex, which supports the view
that average faces are attractive.37 Prototypes
help explain why the appreciation of beauty ap-
pears to be innate and present from birth.
Young infants, for example, looked longer at
attractive faces that are prototypical, easier to
classify, and possibly elicit a strong response
from the perceptual system. Through prototyp-
ing, our beauty standards adjust to the popula-
tion in which we live, with the most attractive
being close to the mean of that population. Pro-
totyping expands our possibilities of mate
choice. If we had an inflexible biological tem-
plate for the judgment of attractiveness, we
could risk never meeting somebody fitting the
template.21 However, some argue against proto-
typing because average faces are recognized
poorly, as they do not deviate from the templates
we use to store faces. Therefore, attractive peo-
ple with average faces would be disadvantaged as
individual recognition is the basis for social in-
teraction. Thus, one should expect deviations
Figure 6. The averaged composite of faces becomes from averageness to be found in the most beau-
more attractive as more faces are added which is seen tiful faces. A beautiful face has to be recogniz-
as you move down the column. (Reprinted with per- able and distinct to be memorable. Adding an
mission from Langlois and Roggman.21) (Color ver- individual touch to averageness could thus make
sion of figure is available online.) an attractive face beautiful.36
Evolutionary psychology also linked attrac-
Langlois and Roggman21 put forward the idea tiveness of individuals to their value as mates.
that the attractiveness of the average face is in Thus, individuals who were attentive to cues of
“keeping with the cognitive processes that favor high mate value, left behind healthier and more
prototypical category members.” The model of a fecund children than those who failed to attend
prototype helps explain how we evaluate visual to those cues.38 Mating with an attractive partner
stimuli and classifying these into categories.20 A would increase the probability of successful repro-
Attractiveness of the Average Face 223

duction and genetic transmission.39 We choose a gene). Thus, individuals possessing rare alleles
mates for genetic disease resistance by scrutiny of or uncommon proteins to which parasites are
features whose expression is dependent on health. poorly adapted, generally have higher fitness
Attractive traits signal mate quality, so that prefer- with regard to parasitic infection. The more
ences increase offspring viability.40 Average pro- heterozygous (the greater the genetic diversity)
portions often signal good health, good design, an individual, the more uncommon alleles it
and genetic fitness; for example, human babies may possess, and the better the defense against
that are born larger or smaller than average are parasites.44
less likely to survive. As evolutionary pressures fa- In summary, Symons concluded that the av-
vor the mean rather than the extremes of the erage of a population is attractive, as it repre-
population, one could maximize the fitness of sents a functionally optimal design and the fit-
one’s offspring by being attracted to and mating test in many species. Evolutionary pressures
with those who displayed average features.41 Facial operate against the extremes of the population.
averageness reflects developmental stability, the Facial averageness is associated with above-average
ability to maintain normal development despite performance in tasks, such as chewing and breath-
the environmental and genetic stresses. Develop- ing, and hence during evolutionary history, Natu-
mental instability, by contrast, results in deviations ral Selection favored it.45 Furthermore, homozy-
from the average and craniofacial abnormalities.42 gous individuals tend to be over-represented at the
There is modest evidence that averageness indi- extreme tails of the distributions. By contrast,
cated health, specifically, facial distinctiveness, the heterozygous individuals tend to be over-repre-
opposite of averageness at 17, a prime age for mate sented at the middle of such distributions.36
choice, was associated with poor childhood health
in males and poor current and adolescent health
The Average Is Not Necessarily the
in females. Marked deviations from facial average-
Most Attractive Face
ness are diagnostic of some chromosomal disor-
der, and more subtle deviations may prove cues to Alley and Cunningham counterargued in favor
health of potential mates. A preference for average of attractiveness related to directional selection
faces could therefore have evolved because it en- for extreme characteristics, such as dominant
hanced reproductive success, either because features (large eyes) or juvenile features (blonde
healthy mates provide better parental care or be- hair) in females. In their opinion, evolutionary
cause they confer genetic benefits to their off- selection dictated that the ideal mate for a male
spring.43 was a female with a high reproductive capacity,
Finally, preference for average facial features that is, a young woman. Hence, youthful traits
could have evolved because it denotes genetic may take preference over average ones. Further-
heterozygosity, “good genes.” Heterozygosity re- more, some individual faces were more attractive
flects an outbred mate who provides genetic diver- than the corresponding composite. They con-
sity in defense against parasites. Facial averageness cluded that although average faces were attrac-
reflects parasite-resistant genetic makeup, as it cor- tive, exceptionally attractive faces were atypical
relates with high individual protein heterozygosity, in some way. They argued that the composite
and thus with the presence of an array of alleles to produced an attractive face in part by the pro-
which the parasite must adapt.40 Attractive faces duction of an unusually symmetric face and also
therefore signal parasite resistance. Parasites are by the elimination of skin blemishes or surface
ubiquitous and so humans, via their immune sys- irregularities, creating a “soft focus” as well as
tem, evolve defensive adaptations against parasites. smooth uniform skin tone.46
In response, parasites evolve adaptations for cir- Perrett et al47 supported directional selection,
cumventing these defensive mechanisms. There is rather than stabilizing selection by showing that
a continuing process of host-parasite coevolu- the mean shape of a set of attractive faces is
tion, during which both hosts and parasites are preferred to the mean shape of the sample
subjected to constant shifting selection pres- from which the faces were selected. The aver-
sures. Parasites are best adapted to most com- age face from 60 females was less attractive
mon proteins and least adapted to the protein than a face representing the average of the 15
products of rare alleles (sequences that code for most attractive female faces from the same
224 Collins

group. In addition, the composites were made attractive no matter how smooth, youthful, or
more attractive by exaggerating the shape dif- symmetric. Averageness is fundamental.39
ferences from the sample mean. They con-
cluded that highly attractive facial configura-
tions are not average and moderately extreme The Impact of Globalization on the
shapes enhanced attractiveness. Preference Average Face
for nonaverage face shapes would therefore If facial beauty is even partially determined by
exert a directional selection pressure away average measurements, then it cannot be a pre-
from the population mean. determined standard planted in our mind. The
In addition, it has been demonstrated that mechanism that stores and averages the face is
the public prefer nonaveraged traits, specifically innate and universal, but the composite form is
a fuller more protrusive dentofacial pattern. dependent on the faces it sees.41 In 1872, Charles
This was illustrated when the dentofacial ceph- Darwin recognized this concept, when he argued
alometric measurements of professional models, that if a tribe spread over a continent they would
beauty contest winners, and performing stars gradually subdivide into smaller isolated tribes,
were found to be more protrusive than the av- each of which would subsequently be exposed to
erage.48 a different environment, and by natural selec-
Langlois et al countered their critics by ex- tion, their features would also change to some
plaining that averaged faces can be both average degree:
in the sense of representing the mean and at the
same time extreme in characteristics, such as As soon as this occurred, each isolated tribe would form for
itself a slightly different standard of beauty; and then un-
youthfulness. They disagreed with the argument
conscious selection would come into action through the more
that symmetry explains attractiveness, as symme-
powerful and leading savages preferring certain women to
try by itself is not enough to produce attractive- others . . .
ness. They demonstrated that although a math-
ematically averaged face will be symmetric, a According to Darwin,50 we have survived not
symmetric face is not necessarily highly attractive only through our ability to adapt to our environ-
or close to the mathematical average of a popu- ment through natural selection but also by being
lation of faces. Furthermore, a highly attractive the offspring of those chosen through sexual
face is not necessarily highly symmetric. Hence, selection. The latter, from an aesthetic perspec-
movie stars often want to be photographed from tive, is close to the average of all the faces we
their “good side.” In addition, they showed that have seen.
asymmetric faces were rated as more attractive Because of globalization, we are now exposed
than the perfectly symmetric mirror-image right to a much broader range of faces. This is altering
and left faces (chimeras). our internal averages to reflect a preference for
Langlois et al49 accepted that composite faces the universal face, a composite of all races. This
have a “soft focus” and lack blemishes that in- is evident in the cosmetic surgery people seek.
crease the appearance of youthfulness and sym- For example, in 2003, 1000 plastic surgeons in
metry, but neither alone can guarantee attractive- Korea performed about half a million proce-
ness; a face can be both young and symmetric, yet dures to create a more Western look.51 Mean-
still be unattractive. They agreed however that fa- while, the West is also moving away from its
miliarity is related to averageness and attractive- traditional position. The preferred skin color is
ness. no longer the palest shades, and plumper lips
In conclusion, averageness is essential for fa- are increasingly preferred. This reflects an inter-
cial attractiveness; however, it is not exclusive. nal average where Asian, African, and Hispanic
Youth (in females), symmetry, and sexually di- features are helping to recalibrate norms of
morphic traits are also important, although even beauty.41
they are not enough alone. Averageness is the However, although globalization may lead to
only characteristic discovered to date that is both a more universal standard of beauty across the
necessary and sufficient to ensure facial attrac- globe, fewer can hope to approximate this mean
tiveness—without a facial configuration close to than when the average of our own ethnic group
the average of the population, a face will not be was the most attractive. Moreover, the average is
Attractiveness of the Average Face 225

not best adapted to all environments. Face


shape, for example, is adapted to climate. The
most advantageous head shape in a cold cli-
mate would be rounded as it dissipates less
heat, as seen in brachycephalic Arctic Inuits.
Hot equatorial climates, by contrast, favor
long-headedness, which dissipates more heat as
seen in the dolichocephalic equatorial Africans.52
Nose shape is similarly affected. Platyrrhine is
best suited and more commonly found in hot
moist climates in comparison with leptorrhine in
cold dry ones. Eye shape is another adaptation.
The epicanthic fold present in the Asian face
protects against the glare of the sun, which
would have been essential for survival in the last
glaciation, and similarly the flatter facial form
protected against frostbite. Dark helicotrichous
hair in Africans has a protective value in areas of
strong sunlight. The converse is true for those Figure 7. An attractive composite image has been
created for artistic purposes by merging photographic
with blond cymotrichous hair in Northern cli-
images of 1000 females from the town of Haverhill.
mates.3 There is also a strong correlation be- (Reprinted with permission. © Christian Dorley-
tween latitude, ultraviolet radiation, and skin Brown and Haverhill Town Council.) (Color version
color. In equatorial sunshine, only the darkest of figure is available online.)
skin offers protection against skin cancer or fo-
late depletion. The fairest skin by contrast is best
adapted to northern latitudes offering greater a computer software to create a female image of
resistance to Rickets.53 a multiethnic face of America. She had the facial
In summary, the average may not be best features of a woman who was 15% Anglo-Saxon,
adapted to all climates, and it might be more 17.5% Middle Eastern, 17.5% African, 7.5%
appropriate to temper our selection to the cli- Asian, 35% Southern European, and 7.5% His-
mate in which we live, rather than seek the panic. This was hailed as the face of melting pot
universal mean. America. This magazine argued that with global-
ization, Eastern and Western perceptions of
beauty are merging to produce a global stan-
The Impact of Digital Technology on
dard of beauty.55 Newsweek matched this,
the Average Face
coming up with their own version of the new
The importance of the average face is evident global face of beauty. Saira Mohan, who fea-
in the use of composite images to portray at- tured on their cover in 2003 was of mixed
tractiveness in the public domain. The photog- ancestry, French-Irish-Canadian on her moth-
rapher Christian Dorley-Brown achieved pleas- er’s side and North Indian, Punjab on her
ing results in his project “Haverhill 2000,” father’s side; her instant appeal was said to be
which set out to establish a permanent and because she represented a type of global
accessible image of a town’s population. Using mean51 (Fig. 8).
computer software, 2000 individual photo- To complicate matters further, we are increas-
graphs were merged. This hyper-real and ingly exposed to standards of beauty set by cos-
somewhat ghostly image is created from pho- metically enhanced and digitally altered facial
tographs of 1000 females and 1000 males aged images. Digital technology is also creating virtual
6 months to 80 years, covering 50 ethnic models, such as the virtual model Maya56 who
groups (Fig. 7).54 looks similar to the artistically created sculpture
Time magazine succeeded in producing the of Nefertiti described earlier (Fig. 9). This preoc-
first alluring global composite image of a female cupation with artificial beauty has been ex-
face. In a special issue cover, Fall 1993, they used tended to Miss Digital World, which was set up
226 Collins

Figure 8. Left: Computer software has created an averaged face from the American melting pot, which is 35%
Southern European, 17.5% African, 17.5% Middle Eastern, 15% Anglo-Saxon, 7.5% Asian, and 7.5% Hispanic.
(Reprinted with permission from Time, special issue, Fall:cover, 1993.) Right: Saira Mohan was considered by
Newsweek to be symbolic of a global beauty because of her mixed ancestry, which was French-Irish-Canadian on her
mother’s side and North Indian, Punjab on her father’s side. (Reprinted with permission from Newsweek, November:
cover, 2003, www.newsweek.com. ©2003 The Newsweek/Daily Beast Company LLC. All rights reserved. Used by permission
and protected by the Copyright Laws of the United States. The printing, copying, redistribution, or retransmission of the
Material without express written permission is prohibited.) (Color version of figure is available online.)

by Franz Cerami in 2001.57 Boundaries are being pansion,60 or one bracket system in favor of an-
blurred between the real and virtual face. The other. Treatment philosophies are advocated by
airbrushed and digitally enhanced media images individual orthodontists whose internal average
of celebrities add to the problem. When the
media raise beauty standards too high, unreal
expectations emerge. If the mean is more beau-
tiful than reality, no mate selection can occur,
and this leads to a greater percentage of sin-
gles.20
With increasing globalization and digital im-
agery, the ever-changing average is difficult to
determine.

Conclusions
The appeal of averageness or as it has been coined,
Koinophilia, the preference for mates with pre-
dominantly common traits, is now an accepted
Figure 9. Left: Virtual model. Maya. (Reprinted with per-
finding.58 Thus, should we strictly adhere to or mission from Rene Morel. © Rene Morel.) Right: Bust
move away from the average, defined by cephalo- Sculpture of Queen Nefertiti. c. 1348-1336/5 BC. Painted Lime-
metric norms, in planning orthodontic and or- stone. Height 48.3 cm. Ägyptisches Museum, Berlin. (Re-
thognathic movements? Some advocate moving printed with permission from bpk, Berlin/Aegyptisches
away from average, toward a broader more protru- Museum, Staatliche Museen, Berlin, Germany/Marga-
rete Büsing/Art Resource, NY.) Digital technology is
sive smile, although defining the latter is not clear. used to create virtual models, such as Maya who has
Moreover, there is no agreement on the merits similar features to those seen in the sculpture of Nefertiti.
of extraction/nonextraction,59 expansion/nonex- (Color version of figure is available online.)
Attractiveness of the Average Face 227

face may/may not comply with others and may/ 6. Hofrath H: Die bedeutung der röntgenfern und ab-
may not be influenced by other aesthetic goals, standsaufnahme für die diagnostik der kieferanomalien.
Fortschr Orthodont 1:232-258, 1931
such as youth and sexual dimorphism. Further- 7. Broadbent BH: Craniofacial growth, in Broadbent BH,
more, our patients, influenced by images por- Bolton GW (eds): Standards of Dentofacial Develop-
trayed in the media, may request treatment, which mental Growth. St Louis, MO, C.V. Mosby, 1975, pp
does not comply with the average. 34-37
The use of cephalometric norms is tempting, as 8. Jacobson A: The role of radiographic cephalometry in
diagnosis and treatment planning, in Jacobson A, Cau-
it is clear that the average face is attractive. How- field PW (eds): Introduction to Radiographic Cepha-
ever, this does not mean that all attractive faces are lometry. Philadelphia, Lea & Febiger, 1985, pp 1-13
average or that average faces are optimally attrac- 9. Downs WB. The role of cephalometrics in orthodontic
tive. Nevertheless, average facial configurations are case analysis and diagnosis. Am J Orthod 52:162-182,
more attractive than most faces.37 But, as cephalo- 1952
10. Ricketts RM. A foundation for cephalometric communi-
metric norms are merely the average of the faces cation. Am J Orthod, 46:330-357, 1960
from which they were sourced, they may not accu- 11. McNamara JA: Method of cephalometric evaluation.
rately represent the patient we are treating, even if Am J Orthod 86:449-468, 1984
matched for ethnicity because “No one knows 12. Reidel RA: An analysis of dentofacial relationships. Am J
what a pure race is.”61 Substantial overlap can occur Orthod 43:103-119, 1957
13. De Smit A, Dermaut L: Soft-tissue profile preferences.
between populations, invalidating the concept of Am J Orthod 86:67-73, 1984
races as discrete groupings. Populations have only 14. Gore R: Pharaohs of the Sun. London, National Geo-
been partially isolated and seldom demarcated by graphic, 2001
precise genetic boundaries; race is socially, not 15. Peck H, Peck S: A concept of facial esthetics. Angle
scientifically, constructed.62 The paradigms of hu- Orthod 40:284-317, 1970
16. Cicero: De inventione 2.1.1, in Pollitt JJ (ed): The Art of
man identity based on “races,” as biological con- Ancient Greece: Sources and Documents. Cambridge,
structs are being questioned in light of the find- Cambridge University Press, 2001, pp 150-151
ings from the Human Genome Project; “race” is 17. Galen: De temperamentis 1.9, in: Pollitt JJ (ed): The Art
poorly correlated with genes.63 of Ancient Greece: Sources and Documents. Cambridge,
In conclusion, the average face is attractive Cambridge University Press, 2001, p 76
18. Bellori G: Lives of the modern painters, sculptors and
and reflects good genetic heterozygosity. But, it architects, 1672, in Harrison C, Wood P, Gaiger J (eds):
is not optimally attractive, being neither distinc- Art in Theory. 1648-1815. An Anthology of Changing
tive nor memorable. With globalization and dig- Ideas. London, Blackwell Publishing, London, 2000, pp
ital imagery, the ever-changing average is diffi- 77-78
cult to determine. 19. Bartrum G: Albrecht Dürer and his Legacy. London,
The British Museum Press, 2002
I hold that the perfection of form and beauty is contained in 20. Grammer K, Fink B, Juette A, et al: Female faces and
the sum of all men. bodies: N-Dimensional feature space and attractiveness,
-Albrecht Dürer19 in Rhodes G, Zebrowitz L (eds): Facial Attractiveness.
Evolutionary, Cognitive and Social Perspectives. West-
port, CT, Ablex, 2002, pp 91-127
21. Langlois JH, Roggman LA: Attractive faces are only av-
erage. Psychol Sci 1:115-121, 1990
References 22. Kant I: Critique of judgement, in Preziosi D (ed): The
1. Angle EH: Treatment of Malocclusion of the Teeth. Art of Art History: A Critical Anthology. Oxford, Oxford
Philadelphia, PA: SS White Dental Manufacturing Co, University Press, 1998, pp 76-106
1907 23. Sekula A: The body and the archive, in Bolton R (ed):
2. Hajnis K: Categories in classical anthropometric propor- The Contest of Meaning. London, MIT Press, 1992, pp
tion systems, in Farkas L, Munro I (eds): Anthropomet- 343-379
ric Facial Proportions in Medicine. Springfield, IL, 24. Galton F: Composite portraits, made by combining those
Charles C. Thomas Publisher, 1987, pp 9-18 of many different persons in a single resultant figure. J
3. Coon CS: What is Race? Atlantic Monthly 200:103-108, Anthropol Inst 8:132-142, 1879
1957 25. Galton F: Generic images. Nineteenth Century 6:162-
4. Viteporn S: The technique of cephalometric radiogra- 163, 1879
phy, in Athanasiou AE (ed): Orthodontic Cephalometry. 26. Galton F: Photographic composites. The Photographic
London, Mosby-Wolfe, 1995, pp 9-20 News 243-245, April 1885
5. Pacini AJ: Roentgen ray anthropometry of the skull. J 27. Jacobs J: On the racial characteristics of modern Jews. J
Radiol 3:322-331, 1992 Anthropol Inst XV:23-62, 1885
228 Collins

28. Galton F, Mahomed FA: An inquiry into the physiog- 43. Rhodes G, Zebrowitz LA, Clark A, et al: Does facial
nomy of phthisis by the method of composite portrai- averageness and symmetry signal health? Evol Hum Be-
ture. Guys Hospital Rep 25:475, 1882 hav 22:31-46, 2001
29. Hamilton P: Policing the face, in Hamilton P, Har- 44. Zuk M: The role of parasites in sexual selection: Current
greaves P (eds): The Beautiful and the Damned. Lon- evidence and future directions. Adv Study Behav 21:39-
don, National Portrait Gallery, 2001, pp 57-107 68, 1992
30. Green D: Veins of resemblance: Photography and eu- 45. Symons D: The Evolution of Human Sexuality. Oxford,
genics. Oxford Art J 7:3-16, 1985 Oxford University Press, 1979
31. Thompson D: The Pocket Oxford Dictionary. Oxford, 46. Alley TR, Cunningham MR: Averaged faces are attrac-
Clarendon Press, 1992 tive, but very attractive faces are not average. Psychol Sci
32. Galton F: Eugenics. Its definition, scope and aims. Na- 2:123-125, 1991
ture 70:82, 1904 47. Perrett DI, May KA, Yoshikawa S: Facial shape and judg-
33. Møller AP, Alatalo RV: Good genes effects in sexual ments of female attractiveness. Nature 368:239-242, 1994
selection: Proc R Soc Lond 266:85-91, 1990 48. Peck H, Peck S: A concept of facial esthetics. Angle
34. Valentine T, Darling S, Donnelly M: Why are average faces Orthod 40:284-317, 1970
attractive? The effect of view and averageness on the attrac- 49. Langlois JH, Roggman LA, Musselman L. What is aver-
tiveness of female faces. Psychon Bull Rev 11:482-487, 2004 age and what is not average about attractive faces? Psy-
35. Spyropoulos MN, Halazonetis DJ: The significance of chol Sci 5:214-222, 1994
50. Darwin C: The descent of man and selection in relation
the soft tissue profile on facial esthetics. Am J Orthod
to sex (1871). In: Wilson EO (ed): From so Simple a
Dentofacial Orthop 119:464-471, 2001
Beginning. The Great Books of Charles Darwin. Lon-
36. Grammer K, Fink B, Møller AP, et al: Darwinian aesthet-
don, Fontana Press, 1999
ics: Sexual selection and the biology of beauty. Biol Rev
51. Guterland F, Hastings M: The global makeover. News-
Camb Philos Soc 78:385-407, 2003
week 54-59, November 2003
37. Rhodes G: The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty.
52. Beals KL: Head form and climatic stress. Am J Phys
Annu Rev Psychol 57:199-226, 2006
Anthropol 37:85-86, 1972
38. Little AC, Penton-Voak IS, Burt DM et al: How cyclic
53. Parra EJ, Kittles RJ, Shriver MD: Implications of corre-
hormonal and self-perceived attractiveness influence fe-
lations between skin colour and genetic ancestry for
male preferences for male faces evolution and individual biomedical research. Nat Genet 36:54-60, 2004
differences in the perception of attractiveness, in Rho- 54. Kemp S: Future Face. London, Profile Books, 2004
des G, Zebrowitz LA (eds): Facial Attractiveness. Evolu- 55. Gaines JR: The new face of America. Time Nov 3, 1993
tionary, Cognitive and Social Perspectives. Westport, CT, 56. Wiedemann J: Digital Beauties. Köln, Taschen, 2002
Ablex, 2002, pp 59-91 57. Kemp S: Facing up to the future. New Sci 184:1-3, 2002
39. Rubenstein AJ, Langlois JH, Roggman LA: What makes a 58. Koeslag JH, Koeslag PD: Koinophilia. J Theor Biol 167:
face attractive and why: The role of averageness in de- 55-65, 1994
fining facial beauty, in Rhodes G, Zebrowitz LA (eds): 59. Işiksal E, Hazar S, Akyalçin S: Smile esthetics: Perception
Facial Attractiveness. Evolutionary, Cognitive and Social and comparison of treated and untreated smiles. Am J
Perspectives. Westport, CT, Ablex, 2002, pp 1-35 Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 129:8-16, 2006
40. Hamilton WD, Zuk M: Heritable true fitness and bright 60. Lee RT: Arch widths and form: A review. Am J Orthod
birds: A role for parasites. Science 218:384-387, 1982 Dentofacial Orthop 115:305-313, 1999
41. Etcoff N: Survival of the Prettiest. The Science of Beauty. 61. Hooton E. Apes, Men, and Morons. New York, NY, Put-
London, Little, Brown and Company, 1999 nam’s Sons, 1937
42. Rhodes G, Harwood K, Yoshikawa S, et al: The attrac- 62. Jorde LB, Wooding SP: Genetic variation, classification
tiveness of average faces: Cross-cultural evidence and and “race”. Nat Genet 36:S28-S33, 2004
possible biological basis, in Rhodes G, Zebrowitz LA 63. Royal CD, Dunston GM: Changing the paradigm from
(eds): Facial Attractiveness. Evolutionary, Cognitive and “race” to human genome variation. Nat Genet 36:S5-S7,
Social Perspectives. Westport, CT, Ablex, 2002, pp 35-59 2004

You might also like