Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

ARTICLE IN PRESS

Environmental Hazards 7 (2007) 302–311


www.elsevier.com/locate/hazards

Efficient and effective? The 100-year flood in the communication and


perception of flood risk
Heather M. Bella,, Graham A. Tobinb
a
Department of Geography and Regional Studies, University of Miami, 1000 Memorial Dr. (Ferre 205) Coral Gables, FL 33124, USA
b
Department of Geography, University of South Florida, 4202 E. Fowler Ave (NES 107), Tampa, FL 33620-5250, USA

Abstract

This paper presents a synopsis of several terms used to describe US policy’s benchmark flood and a preliminary study of how such
terms are interpreted. Questionnaire surveys were conducted in a flood prone community with residents living in and out of official flood
plains. Comparable questions regarding uncertainty, perceived need for protection, and concern were asked in connection with four
descriptive methods: a 100-year flood; a flood with a 1 percent chance of occurring in any year; a flood with a 26 percent chance of
occurring in 30 years; and a flood risk map. Statistical analysis and qualitative observation showed a disjuncture between understanding
and persuasion, potential problems with the 26 percent chance method, and a preference for concrete references in describing risk.
r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Flood hazards; Risk communication; Risk perception; 100-year flood

1. Introduction passed the National Flood Insurance Act (NFIA), and a


campaign to map flood prone communities across the
From 1980 through 2005, 67 weather-related disasters in country began shortly thereafter. In order to map the
the United States exceeded one billion dollars each in direct nation as quickly as possible, using multiple organizations,
damage (Ross and Lott, 2006). In over half of them, efficiently coded policy requirements were imperative. The
flooding was either the primary cause or a significant depicted Base Flood Elevations and Special Flood Hazard
component of a compound disaster like a hurricane. Areas (SFHA), as well as the associated building and
According to an updated version of Pielke et al’s. (2002) insurance guidelines outlined in the NFIA and expanded
reanalysis of National Weather Service data (Flooddama- upon in the Flood Disaster Protection Act of 1973, were all
gedata.org), direct physical flood losses over 72 years prior based on the parameters of a flood with a return period
to 2003 exceeded 171 billion dollars. This statistic does not of 100 years, i.e. a 1 percent chance of occurring in any
include the catastrophic losses related to Hurricane given year.
Katrina. The Federal Emergency Management Agency The 1 percent chance flood was chosen because it
(FEMA) (2006a) reports that almost $16 billion in claims ‘‘constitutes a reasonable compromise between the need for
were paid out by the National Flood Insurance Program building restrictions to minimize potential loss of life and
(NFIP) in 2005. property and the economic benefits to be derived from
Effective loss prevention and distribution must begin floodplain development’’ (FEMA, 2006b, p. 2). The initial
well before a flood event, but the benchmarks and goal of adopting the 100-year flood criterion was not
terminology we use to manage and communicate flood effective communication of risk or risk policy, but efficient
risk may be working against this goal. In 1968, Congress administration and implementation. The efficiency with
which the Flood Insurance Administration set about
Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 (305) 284 4781. blanketing the country (Reuss, 1993), combined with the
E-mail addresses: hmbell@miami.edu (H.M. Bell), gtobin@cas.usf.edu efficient verbal and visual coding of the 100-year flood,
(G.A. Tobin). may well have led to the term’s institutionalization before

1747-7891/$ - see front matter r 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.envhaz.2007.08.004
ARTICLE IN PRESS
H.M. Bell, G.A. Tobin / Environmental Hazards 7 (2007) 302–311 303

its usefulness to risk policy and risk communication was conditions, data constraints, and human error. At least
examined. Its relation to behavior and attitude was not some of these uncertainties were internalized by those
considered. The influences of informal communication and conducting the studies. An efficiently coded message,
contextual differences in meaning on the interpretation of however, will be filtered differently by the general public
flood risk were not fully anticipated either. than by those who originally produced its content.
The usefulness of the 100-year flood as a policy criterion Uncertainty is likely to be eliminated as an incoming
has been upheld (NRC, 2000), though it is still debated message is recoded into existing mental categories (Boisot,
(GFWNFPF, 2004; Association of State Floodplain 1995). The coding of the 100-year flood is both recogniz-
Managers (ASFPM), 2007). Practitioners and researchers able and potentially relevant, and is thus particularly
alike have questioned the effectiveness of 100-year flood malleable.
terminology in public communication (NRC, 1995, 2000; As the 100-year flood designation moved out of the
Smith, 2000; Gruntfest et al., 2002; GFWNFPF, 2004). Its arena of implementation and into the political and public
use may emphasize risk dichotomies and mask the spheres, it ‘‘rapidly transformed in the community mind to
irregularity and uncertainty associated with both the a definition of flood free and flood prone, with areas
timing and consequences of flooding. In a 2006 publica- above the designated flood perceived to be flood free—
tion, the National Research Council (NRC) linked the a misconception often reinforced by flood maps that shade
misunderstanding of uncertainty to poor decisions with only those portions that are subject to the designated
potentially disastrous results in the face of hazardous flood’’ (Smith, 2000, p. 255). While more research is needed
events. There is worry that misunderstandings associated to confirm this, Smith’s assertion is reasonable and general
with 100-year flood terminology might attenuate concern risk communication research supports such a conclusion
and reduce the likelihood of mitigative behavior. The (Slovic, 1986; Hance et al., 1989; Tobin, 1995). Kates and
effectiveness of the 100-year flood as a means to change White (1961) also indicate that map lines, like levees, may
attitude or motivate behavior has not been assessed, nor produce a false sense of security. These potential differ-
has its utility in increasing public understanding of flood ences in audience interpretation were not addressed prior
risk and uncertainty. New terms have been introduced, but to the implementation of the NFIP and by the time risk
their effectiveness also remains unproven. This research communicators were concerned with context, new mean-
provides a preliminary evaluation of four methods used to ings were already entrenched.
communicate flood risk to the public. All refer to US The 100-year flood came to prominence during what
policy’s benchmark flood. Powell and Leiss (1997) describe as the first phase of risk
communication. The focus was on comparable risk
2. Risk communication and the 100-year flood assessment and the identification of physical risk cate-
gories; communication was authoritarian, privileging
Covello et al. (1986) identified four components of scientists and officials and downplaying a public who
risk communication: the message source; message design; thought and talked about risk differently. In the second
delivery channel; and target audience. This model phase, communication remained instrumental and one-
depicts risk communication as linear, and is described sided, but those communicating risk began to tailor their
by Kasperson and Stallen (1991) as the ‘‘engineering messages and an effort was made to recognize public needs.
approach’’. Risk communication is viewed as a means of Experts were privileged, but, like marketers, had to
transmitting technical or scientific information regarding acknowledge audience characteristics in order to take
risk from a known, intentional source (the experts) along advantage of them.
designated channels, to specified recipients (the public). Communication was considered effective if it success-
Discussions between friends, overheard conversations, fully influenced a receiver’s attitude and behavior in a
the influence of movies, and other types of informal manner desired by the sender (Boisot, 1995). This
and sometimes unintentional communication are not conceptualization is based on persuasion and is referred
accounted for. to as the dominance model (McQuail and Windhal, 1981).
Most official flood risk communication operates under There are many who contest such a conceptualization and
the narrow model; flood risk information is assumed to be instead stress understanding as the key to effective
transferred from experts to a general audience via maps, communication (e.g. Belsten, 1996; Parker, 2000; Trumbo,
pamphlets, and policy. Because of the efficiency of the 2000; Kasemir et al., 2003), but persuasion continues to be
message and the shared language of experts and public, a the practical, if not the theoretical, model of much risk
shared code was initially assumed. However, in using the communication. Perhaps because the NFIP was developed
100-year flood as the basis of verbal and visual commu- prior to the emphasis on social context, stakeholder dialog,
nication, risk communicators unintentionally substituted a and trust building that characterizes the third phase
contextual sub-group (the experts) for the public as the (Powell and Leiss, 1997), flood risk communication
target audience. tends to follow the persuasive model. More inclusive
Embedded in the flood parameters is uncertainty models of policy making and flood risk communication
resulting from natural variability, changing environmental have begun to be explored and implemented, however
ARTICLE IN PRESS
304 H.M. Bell, G.A. Tobin / Environmental Hazards 7 (2007) 302–311

(e.g. Larson and Plasencia, 2001; Environmental Agency, the 100-year event, but would be associated with lower
2005; Frech, 2005). levels of concern and less of a need for protection. This
Government agencies recognize the inadequacy of the description would thus be less persuasive.
100-year flood in communicating flood risk and the danger 2. Events described as having a 26 percent chance of
of reinforcing risk dichotomies (NRC, 1995, 2000); over occurring in a 30-year period would be perceived as
half of flood losses (Smith, 2000; Frech, 2005) and more variable than the 100-year flood, but comparably
approximately 25 percent of paid insurance claims persuasive.
(FEMA, 2006a) occur outside SFHA. As a result, there 3. Current floodplain maps would be perceived as illus-
has been a move to use more effective terminology without trating absolute boundaries, and thus fail to convey
forsaking the efficiency necessary to carry out a nationwide uncertainty as well as even 100-year flood terminology.
program. In 2000, the NRC recommended several other Persuasion would vary with location, being higher than
messages thought to better convey uncertainty. Suggestions the 100-year flood inside the depicted floodplain and
included the 1 percent chance flood, percent chance of lower outside of it.
flooding during a 30-year mortgage (essentially a 1 in 4, A secondary purpose of this study was to explore possible
and described as a 26 percent chance), and an analogy improvements to flood risk communication by looking at
linking the chance of flooding in 50 years to the chance of the ways in which people describe flooding and what
tossing a coin and coming up with heads (NRC, 2000). concerns them about flooding, as well as by comparing
Other organizations, like the Lincoln Institute of Land abstract and concrete descriptions.
Policy, have suggested a similar shift in terminology
(Faber, 1996). 4. Methods
The NRC (2006) has positioned the understanding of
uncertainty concepts as a stepping stone to better decision 4.1. Study site
making and, ultimately, behavior. Persuasion is believed
to be contingent upon understanding of the uncertainty Research was carried out in August of 2004 in the village of
concepts originally associated with the 100-year flood Wimberley, located approximately 50 km southwest of Austin
(NRC, 2000), but it is not clear that a better perception of in Texas Hill Country. Wimberley lies at the confluence of
uncertainty and probability will bring the risk perception Cypress Creek and the Blanco River and numerous washes
of expert and general public closer together or induce a run through residential neighborhoods. The Guadalupe River
desired response. Anecdotal evidence from areas using the Basin, of which the Blanco River watershed is a part, has
1 percent chance description points to a potential problem been called ‘‘Flash Flood Alley’’ and is one of the three most
with persuasion. After all, there’s ‘‘a 99 percent chance of it dangerous regions in the US for flash floods (GBRA, 1999).
not happening!’’ (www.coxsmeadow.homestead.com). It is particularly important in areas such as these to
communicate flood risk effectively and encourage appropriate
3. Scope of the study behavior by individuals and communities.
While it serves a portion of the greater Wimberley
Arkin (1989) has emphasized the importance of carefully Valley, the village itself is fairly small, consisting of 2710
testing messages prior to official communication. Practi- people according to the 2003 Census Bureau estimate.
tioners and other researchers have also indicated a need to Wimberley has several neighborhoods both in and out of
research the effectiveness of different messages (Connelly the officially designated floodplain and has experienced a
and Knuth, 1998; NRC, 2006). However, the suggested variety of large flood events over the last 10 years. The
terms, like the 100-year flood before them, have not yet most damaging in terms of life and property occurred in
been tested as to their efficacy in influencing public October 1998. While the majority of the population does
understanding of uncertainty concepts, attitude, or beha- live outside the SFHA, flooding cuts off road access to
vior. The primary purpose of this research is a preliminary multiple neighborhoods, flooding does occur outside the
comparison of four descriptions currently used in official SFHA, and the tourist and business center of the village
flood risk communication: (i) the 100-year flood; (ii) a flood lies within the 100-year floodplain. Flooding is a real threat
with a 1 percent chance of occurring in any year; to both personal and public property. Additionally, the
(iii) a flood with a 26 percent chance of occurring in 30 population of Wimberley is growing, and many immigrants
years; and (iv) a flood risk map depicting the 100-year may not be familiar with the flood hazard. In general,
floodplain. The map was, at the time of fieldwork, available Wimberley’s population is more white, older, somewhat
online through a partnership between ESRI and Project better educated, and slightly wealthier than the rest of Hays
Impact. Three hypotheses were tested regarding relative County or Texas (US Census Bureau, 2004).
effectiveness:
4.2. Data collection and questionnaire
1. An event described as having a 1 percent chance of
occurring in any given year would be perceived more Data were collected using a structured face-to-face
accurately in terms of its random nature compared to questionnaire administered to a stratified random sample
ARTICLE IN PRESS
H.M. Bell, G.A. Tobin / Environmental Hazards 7 (2007) 302–311 305

of residents. Participants were English-speaking adults and the final Protection scale ranged from two to eight
living in single-family homes. Subgroups were created (a ¼ .69 for the 100-year description).
geographically, delineated among those in the 100-year
floodplain, the 500-year floodplain, and those outside both (a) I should protect myself against the 100-year flood.
official floodplains. Of the 45 residents who participated, (b) My community should protect itself against the 100-year
26 lived in one of the two designated floodplains and one- flood.
third lived in the SFHA. Within the identified neighbor-
hoods, sampling was random. The response rate of those The Concern factor was also made up of two questions.
contacted was 90 percent; one person did not complete the Instead of rating their agreement with statements, partici-
survey as a result of a language barrier. pants were asked to indicate the level to which they were
The questionnaire consisted of three major sections concerned about personal loss due to the 100-year flood
relating to individual flood history and experience, (or 1 percent chance flood, etc.). Choices ranged from 1,
methods of description, and demographic information. very unconcerned to 6, very concerned and don’t know was
Questions in the second section were designed to evaluate not given as an option. They were then asked about their
the effectiveness of the flood risk descriptions outlined concern regarding community loss. The combined Concern
above and provided the basis for this paper. For each of scale ranged from two to 12.
the four descriptive methods, effectiveness as persuasion Cronbach’s alpha for the 100-year description was .46.
was measured through expressed levels of concern pertain- Both the 26 percent chance description and the map had
ing to personal and community loss, as well as the higher reliability (a ¼ .62 and a ¼ .60), but alpha for the 1
perceived need for protection of both personal and percent description was similarly low. These results could
community interests. Questions dealing with the perceived reflect higher variability in interpreted concern, especially
variability of flooding over time and space were used to personal concern, for the 100 year and 1 percent descrip-
evaluate understanding. In this section, most questions tions. When Concern and Protection factors were com-
were closed and made use of Likert-type scales. bined to form scales of overall Persuasion, the 100-year
description had the lowest reliability (a ¼ .46). All other
4.3. Analysis descriptions were associated with alpha scores of .71 or
higher. The two components of Persuasion (Protection and
A total of eight questions were asked regarding each of Concern) were adjusted to weight equally and added; the
the four descriptions. Factor analysis was used to group final scale ranged from five to 24.
responses into broader scales of (i) Understanding of One question related to the methods of description was
Uncertainty, (ii) Protection and (iii) Concern. Protection not as clearly grouped by factor analysis and was left out of
and Concern factors were then combined to form a scale of both scales. Participants were asked to rate their agreement
overall Persuasion. For all factors, the higher the score, the with a statement like ‘‘Scientists can accurately assess the
more effective the description was judged to be. size of a 100 year flood’’ on a scale of one to four. This
In order to construct each description’s Uncertainty question had more to do with human error and the
scale, participants were asked to indicate their level of uncertainties of science than environmental uncertainty,
agreement with three statements. Choices ranged from 1, bringing in issues of trust and credibility. It was useful in
strongly disagree to 4, strongly agree. Don’t know was also exploring perception, but less useful in assessing an
an option and was assigned a value of 2.5 in comparative understanding of uncertainty related to flooding.
analyses. Examples using the 100-year flood description are Comparisons of effectiveness for Uncertainty, Protec-
listed below (the full questionnaire is available from the tion, Concern and overall Persuasion factors were made
authors). using paired t-tests or Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, depend-
ing on scale distribution, range, and the number of cases.
(a) A 100-year flood could happen one or more times in any Because the hypotheses were directional, tests were one-
year. tailed. Comparative tests not directly related to the
(b) If our community experiences a 100-year flood this year, hypotheses were two-tailed. Relationships between factors
we will be safe from one next year. were evaluated using Spearman’s or Pearson’s correlation
(c) The size of the 100-year flood will change over time. coefficients. All statistical tests were conducted at a .05
level of significance using SPSS. The majority of analysis
Responses were weighted equally and added, resulting in was quantitative, but the face-to-face format allowed for
a final Uncertainty scale that ranged from three to 12 the collection of some qualitative data as well.
(a ¼ .65). The scale was treated as continuous in analysis.
Participants were given the same instructions for 5. Results and discussion
questions related to Protection. Each description’s Protec-
tion scale consisted of the added agreement ratings of two Godber (2005) has used policy guidelines to represent the
statements similar to those given below. Response possi- level of risk deemed acceptable by government. This study
bilities for individual questions ranged from one to four used policy standards to represent the behavior and level of
ARTICLE IN PRESS
306 H.M. Bell, G.A. Tobin / Environmental Hazards 7 (2007) 302–311

concern policy makers wish the public to display. In US Table 2


flood policy, the 100-year flood is considered a high risk to Comparing perceived accuracy
both individuals and communities, but does the public view Meana Std. dev. Median jZ statisticj for
it as such? Are alternative descriptions more or less comparison to
persuasive? Results of comparative tests of effectiveness 26% accuracyb
are discussed below, as are some potential improvements
100-year 1.96 .86 2.0 3.85**
to flood risk communication operating within a narrow
accuracy
model. 1% accuracy 1.87 .79 2.0 3.53**
26% accuracy 1.51 .71 1.0 NA
Map accuracy 2.16 .97 2.0 3.04**
5.1. Comparisons of effectiveness: understanding of
uncertainty *pp.05; **pp.01.
a
Total points possible equals 4.
b
It was predicted that, when compared to the 100-year Two-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank test (N ¼ 45).
flood description, both the 1 percent and 26 percent chance
descriptions would be associated with better understanding
of uncertainty. The map was expected to have a lower manner, several participants exclaimed, ‘‘Who do they
Uncertainty score. Comparative results (Table 1) show that think they are, God?’’ Reactions were immediate and often
the 1 percent chance description had the highest mean forceful and appeared to be directly related to the terms
score (10.14), while the 100-year description had the lowest used. None of the other methods of description elicited
(9.64). The difference between the 100 year and 1 percent such a strong response.
chance Uncertainty scores was significant at the .05 level, Quantitative results presented in Table 2 also reflect this
but a t-test could not confirm variation between the 100 pattern. The map was viewed as the most accurate overall
year and 26 percent descriptions. The small difference in and the 26 percent description as least accurate. In addition
means between the 100-year description and the map had to having the lowest median score, the 26 percent chance
the opposite sign of what was hypothesized. In exploratory description had the lowest mean and the smallest standard
comparisons of non-100-year descriptions using two-tailed deviation. Over 62 percent of the participants chose
paired t-tests, Uncertainty scores associated with the 1 ‘‘Strongly Disagree’’. Wilcoxon tests showed the 26 percent
percent chance flood were significantly higher than those of chance description scored consistently lower than each of
both the map and the 26 percent chance description. No the other methods. There were no significant differences
other significant differences were found. For this sample, between the other three descriptions.
the 1 percent description appeared to be more effective While skepticism is useful and may indicate a better
than any of the other verbal or visual descriptions when understanding of uncertainty, the total dismissal of
evaluated based on understanding. probabilities, parameters, and those assessing them is
The question regarding assessment of size addressed a detrimental to both persuasion and understanding. While
somewhat different type of uncertainty and was not all four of the descriptions’ median scores fell in the bottom
included in the Uncertainty scale. It did, however, render half of the scale, the strength of disagreement associated
useful qualitative data that shed light on more general with 26 percent terminology and accuracy makes other
perceptions of the descriptions as well as their potential data gathered using the term suspect. This judgment is
drawbacks. Participants were uncomfortable with the reinforced by the fact that 64 percent of respondents asked
apparent exactness of both the percentage and the for clarification. The 26 percent chance description may be
timeframe in the 26 percent chance in 30 years description. more difficult to conceptualize than the others. There was
When asked to what extent they agreed that scientists could no significant correlation, however, between requests for
accurately assess the size of a flood described in this clarification and low ratings. Additionally, if participants
asked for clarification, they answered questions about the
26 percent chance description only after they were told that
Table 1 the described flood had an approximately one in four
Comparing effectiveness as understanding of uncertainty
chance of occurring during a 30-year mortgage. Taken
Meana Std. dev. Difference from 100-year together, these points indicate that the low ratings and
uncertainty meanb vehement responses cannot be attributed to confusion
alone.
100-year 9.64 1.77 NA
uncertainty
1% uncertainty 10.14 1.38 .50* 5.2. Comparisons of effectiveness: Persuasion
26% uncertainty 9.71 1.41 .06
Map uncertainty 9.66 1.67 .01 Factors of Protection and Concern were used to evaluate
*pp.05; **pp.01.
persuasion. These scales were both created by adding the
a
Points possible range from three to 12. scores of two questions related to personal and commu-
b
One-tailed paired t-test (N ¼ 45). nity interests. Protection and Concern scales were then
ARTICLE IN PRESS
H.M. Bell, G.A. Tobin / Environmental Hazards 7 (2007) 302–311 307

combined to form a scale of overall Persuasion. While the 1 Table 4


percent chance description was expected to perform better Comparing effectiveness as persuasion: concern
than the 100-year description in comparisons of under- Meana Std. dev. Difference from 100-year
standing, it was predicted to be less persuasive. The 26 concern meanb
percent chance description was not expected to differ
significantly from the 100-year descriptions with regards to 100-year concern 7.87 2.68 NA
1% concern 7.27 2.57 .60*
measures of persuasion. The relative effectiveness of the
26% concern 7.69 2.84 .18
map was expected to vary with location. Comparative Map concern 8.13 2.79 .27
results for Protection, Concern and Persuasion factors are
presented below. *pp.05; **pp.01.
a
Points possible range from two to 12.
b
One-tailed paired t-test (N ¼ 45).
5.2.1. Comparing protection
Protection scores for all descriptions were left skewed,
Table 5
but Wilcoxon tests and t-tests yielded similar results. The
Comparing effectiveness as persuasion: overall
results of the t-tests are included in Table 3. As with
Uncertainty, the largest difference in Protection scores Meana Std. dev. Difference from 100-year
occurred between the 1 percent chance and 100-year persuasion meanb
descriptions, though their relative positions were reversed. 100-year 17.90 3.76 NA
The difference was significant (p ¼ .021). There was not persuasion
enough evidence to conclude that the scores of the 100 year 1% persuasion 16.68 4.39 1.22**
and the 26 percent chance descriptions differed. Both 26% persuasion 17.57 4.25 .33
results supported the stated hypotheses. Two-tailed tests Map persuasion 17.85 4.69 .05
showed potential variation between the 1 percent and 26 *pp.05; **pp.01.
percent chance descriptions as well (t-test, p ¼ .051; a
Points possible range from five to 24.
b
Wilcoxon, p ¼ .047). One-tailed paired t-test (N ¼ 45).
Since most participants lived outside the SFHA, the
direction of the difference between the map and 100 year
scores also supported the hypotheses. Separate tests were
then conducted for those in the 100-year floodplain variation was found between the 100-year description and
(N ¼ 15) and those who lived in either the 500-year the map in either the whole sample or the subsets. This
floodplain or outside both floodplains (N ¼ 30). pattern may in part be attributable to public familiarity
A Wilcoxon test showed no significant variation between with 100-year flood terminology and a strong showing
the 100 year score and the map score in the SFHA subset, from the map in areas perceived to border the SFHA.
but in the larger group, the map score was indeed lower In exploratory two-tailed t-tests, the map had a higher
(.48, p ¼ .040). mean score than both the 1 percent chance (.87, p ¼ .007)
and 26 percent chance (.44, p ¼ .044) descriptions. Maps
may serve as an effective jolt to those on the fringes of the
5.2.2. Comparing Concern SFHA who have not sought out a lot of information and
Results of Concern comparisons are included in Table 4. have not been required to get insurance. Several respon-
The map generated the highest mean Concern score (8.13) dents remarked that they did not realize they were ‘‘so close
and the 1 percent chance description the lowest (7.27). The to danger’’. Combined with the map’s higher accuracy
mean score of the 100-year description ranked second and, rating, this comment may indicate that maps are seen as
again, only the score of the 1 percent description was more concrete and thus more clearly relevant, though it
shown to differ significantly from it. No significant also points to a potentially detrimental risk dichotomy.

Table 3 5.3. Comparing Persuasion


Comparing effectiveness as persuasion: protection

Meana Std. dev. Difference from 100-year The overall Persuasion scale was made up of both the
protection Meanb Protection and Concern scales. Comparative results are
similar to those of the constituent factors and are included
100-year 6.69 1.31 NA
protection
in Table 5. The 100-year description had the highest mean
1% protection 6.28 1.59 .41* and the 1 percent chance description the lowest; a t-test
26% protection 6.59 1.31 .10 found significant variation (p ¼ .009). In fact, two-tailed
Map protection 6.48 1.67 .21 t-tests showed the 1 percent description to perform worse
*pp.05; **pp.01.
than both the 26 percent description (.89, p ¼ .042)
a
Points possible range from two to eight. and the map (1.17, p ¼ .037) as well. The null hypothesis
b
One-tailed paired t-test (N ¼ 45) of equality could not be rejected in the remaining
ARTICLE IN PRESS
308 H.M. Bell, G.A. Tobin / Environmental Hazards 7 (2007) 302–311

comparisons. When the sample was separated into SFHA their concern levels regarding flooding up to specific areas
and non-SFHA residents, the directions of the differences of their properties and houses.
between the map and the 100-year descriptions were
consistent with those hypothesized, but no significant 5.4.1. Respondent descriptions
variation was confirmed. Sixty-four percent of respondents identified themselves
While hypotheses regarding relative map scores and as living in Wimberley during the 1998 flood. These
persuasive factors were not clearly supported in the spatial individuals were asked to describe the size of the flood in
subsets, other patterns found in the general sample were their own words. Not surprisingly, return period was one
more pronounced. In the SFHA grouping, Wilcoxon tests of the two most frequently used methods to describe the
showed that the scores for the 1 percent chance description flood. It is the method most commonly used by both the
were consistently lower than those of all other descriptions media and officials in communication. The same number of
for both Concern and overall Persuasion. The 1 percent people described the flood in terms of its size relative to
chance description’s Protection score ranked lower than other floods. This method could be useful, but depends on
the scores of both the map and the 26 percent chance a single collective memory to which newcomers may not
description. The magnitudes of the differences in means have access. However, it might be possible to create such a
were also larger. This indicates that the 1 percent chance memory by erecting visual reminders of events in public
description might be least persuasive in the very areas that places, as Siegrist and Gutscher (2006) have suggested. In
have the highest risk of flooding. A more systematic Wimberley, the square and the multiple low water crossings
exploration of the effectiveness of specific descriptions over along the Blanco River would be appropriate places for
spatial and experiential categories is warranted. these reminders. Several participants showed me their own
Within the context of Wimberley, the 1 percent chance recordings of past flood levels painted or notched onto
description appeared to be less persuasive than the garages or trees. Flood stage was the third most frequently
100-year description, and perhaps less persuasive than the used method, and might not be currently utilized to its full
map and the 26 percent chance descriptions as well. There potential.
was not much evidence indicating significant variation
between the map, 26 percent chance and 100-year 5.4.2. Primary concern of flooding
descriptions. The 26 percent chance results should be eyed Participants were asked to identify the aspect of flooding
warily, though, in light of the other issues mentioned of most concern to them. Four choices were given: (i) level
above. of flood water; (ii) frequency of flooding of any level;
When a scale of total effectiveness including both (iii) a combination of level and frequency; and (iv) other.
Uncertainty and Persuasion factors was evaluated for Approximately half (49 percent) of the people interviewed
reliability, Cronbach’s alpha was negative for three of the identified flood level as the most concerning aspect of
four descriptions. Normally, this would signify a direc- flooding. Another 42 percent cited a combination of level
tional coding problem. Here, it represented contradictory and frequency. None chose frequency alone. This may
results and potential difficulty linking understanding constitute a problem, since the method most often used to
and persuasion. Only the map scores were somewhat describe floods, the return period, is focused on frequency,
directionally consistent. The 100-year description had the as are methods suggested as substitutes.
lowest mean Uncertainty score and the highest Persuasion Flood risk communication cannot hope to be effective if
score. The reverse was true of the 1 percent description. it does not clearly address the concerns of those it is
The disparity was most pronounced in the SFHA subset intended to reach. The same can be said of flood policy.
and may be one of the most difficult hurdles for policy Frequency methods might be effective if linked to specific
makers and risk communicators to overcome. In this heights or damages, as the NRC (2000) and Smith (2000)
subset, the magnitude of significant correlations between have suggested. Used alone, their effectiveness is question-
Uncertainty, Protection, Concern, and Persuasion able. A flood stage linked to specific physical markers
ranged from .56 to .63. Managers and policy makers (ASFPM, 2007) may be more effective than current
may have to decide whether understanding or persuasion is methods in communicating flood risk and inducing
more important in the current framing of flood risk appropriate pre-event attitude and behavior. Collectively
communication. agreed upon flood levels might also be more useful in
creating and enforcing flood policy.
5.4. Towards more effective methods
5.4.3. Comparing specific and abstract descriptions
In addition to comparing existing methods of flood risk The proposals mentioned above are intended to make
communication, this research sought to identify possible abstractions more concrete. In this study, participants were
components of more effective descriptions. To this end, asked to rate their degree of concern regarding specific
respondents were asked to describe the size of specific flood water levels associated with theoretical floods. The range of
events in their own words and the aspect of flooding that possible answers was identical to that of the questions used
most concerned them. Participants were also asked about in the Concern factor. Flood levels were given relative to
ARTICLE IN PRESS
H.M. Bell, G.A. Tobin / Environmental Hazards 7 (2007) 302–311 309

individuals’ residences and ranged from ‘‘the yard/garden’’ explain both policy and general consequences on an
to ‘‘over 1 foot’’ in the house. Only concern related to individual and community level. These and other concrete
personal loss was evaluated. The number of people descriptions may be more effective than abstract descrip-
answering ‘‘very concerned’’ jumped 24 percentage points tions of flood risk in motivating desired attitude and
when the ‘‘flood’’ contacted the outside walls of the behavior prior to and in the warning stages of an event.
residence. Another large increase (to 84 percent) was
observed when the theoretical flood waters moved inside 6. Summary and conclusions
the residence. These points and associated consequences
could perhaps be used in flood risk communication to The comparative effectiveness of four methods used to
make the abstract threat of flooding more concrete and to describe US policy’s benchmark flood was evaluated
personalize potential damages. through both Understanding of Uncertainty and Persua-
Wilcoxon tests were used to compare the distribution of sion. Persuasion was broken down into factors of Protec-
personal concern scores of the four descriptions and those tion and Concern. Three hypotheses were made regarding
of the two lowest specific levels given (the yard/garden and the performance of the 1 percent chance description, the 26
the outside walls). Because the abstract methods refer percent chance description, and a flood risk map relative to
to a flood inundating the SFHA, only those living in the that of 100-year flood terminology. The 1 percent chance
SFHA were used in the comparison. The yard/garden is a description was expected to be associated with better
conservative comparison. The comparison using the out- understanding and lower persuasion. The 26 percent
side walls is appropriate because participants were categor- description was also predicted to have higher Uncertainty
ized as being in the SFHA if a building was at least partly scores, but have similar Protection, Concern and Persua-
within the mapped 100-year floodplain. Comparative sion scores. The map was expected to perform poorly in the
results are included in Table 6. Uncertainty comparison, while its persuasiveness was
The two specific descriptions elicited the highest ratings predicted to vary by SFHA status.
of personal concern. The 1 percent chance had the lowest Only the 1 percent description consistently performed as
mean score and a median two points lower than any other hypothesized, being more effective in conveying uncer-
description. In fact, almost 40 percent of those living in the tainty than the 100-year description and less effective
SFHA indicated they were ‘‘very unconcerned’’ about in motivating attitudes of concern or protection. The
personal losses due to a flood with a 1 percent chance of Uncertainty and Persuasion scores of the 26 percent
occurring in any year. These results reinforce the concerns chance description were not found to differ significantly
raised in the previous section regarding the relative from those of the 100-year description, a result which
persuasiveness of the 1 percent chance description. supports only one part of the related hypothesis. In
In the conservative comparison using the yard/garden, all but the non-SFHA comparison of Protection
the 1 percent chance description was the only one whose scores, the map, too, could not be differentiated from the
distribution differed significantly (p ¼ .018). When the 100-year description, performing somewhat better than
outside walls were used as the reference point, all abstract expected in factors of Uncertainty, Concern, and overall
descriptions scored significantly lower. Though the sample Persuasion.
size was small, these results support the potential use of Further relationships were not hypothesized, but ex-
concrete reference points in communication. During the ploratory analysis indicated that, for the total sample, the
interviews, participants appeared more involved when mean Concern score of the map was higher than the scores
discussing such specifics. While policy itself cannot be of both the 1 percent chance and 26 percent chance
made specific to individual gardens, walls, or streets, descriptions. As a visual representation, the map may have
physical reference points can be used to illustrate and greater relevance, though the extent to which it contributes

Table 6
Comparing personal concern: specific and abstract descriptions

Meana Std. dev. Median jZ statisticj for comparison to jZ statisticj for comparison to
yard/garden personal concernb outside walls personal concernb

100-year personal concern 3.87 1.77 4 1.28 2.43**


1% personal concern 2.67 1.76 2 2.37* 2.89**
26% personal concern 3.60 1.99 4 1.85 2.97**
Map personal concern 3.80 1.97 4 1.65 2.69**
Yard/garden personal concern 4.53 1.60 5 NA NA
Outside walls personal concern 5.20 1.61 6 NA NA

*pp.05; **pp.01.
a
Total points possible equals 6.
b
Two-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank test (N ¼ 15).
ARTICLE IN PRESS
310 H.M. Bell, G.A. Tobin / Environmental Hazards 7 (2007) 302–311

to risk dichotomies is unclear in this study. A more using a small sample from one community; future research
systematic trend was identified for the 1 percent chance in communities large and small, experienced and non-
description. In comparisons of Understanding of Uncer- experienced, from multiple regions may help identify
tainty, each of the three other descriptive methods had components relevant to effective communication and
lower scores. However, when comparing persuasion contextually effective codes. The use of multivariate
factors, the 1 percent description consistently ranked lower analysis adjusted for contextual relationships might assist
than the other methods, a pattern which was more in this search. Message effectiveness will likely vary with
pronounced in the SFHA subset. These results point to language and translation, factors not addressed in this
potential variation in the effectiveness of descriptions over study. More extensive use of qualitative methods would
space and are just one indication of a disjuncture between also help in the practical interpretation of statistical
understanding and persuasion. relationships. Further research assessing the effectiveness
of flood risk communication could result in new, demo-
6.1. Implications for flood risk communication cratic methods of description that may help restructure the
relationship between understanding and persuasion. If
In addition to the comparative results described above, effectiveness is valued over efficiency, loss and suffering
quantitative analysis and qualitative observation identified due to flooding might be reduced.
three trends related to the effective communication of flood The role of flood policy itself cannot be ignored,
risk and flood policy. however. In this project, and in general assessments of
effectiveness, all individuals and communities were judged
 First, effectiveness conceptualized as understanding and against a single perceptual and behavioral standard. The
effectiveness conceptualized as persuasion appear to be questions Murphy (1958) and White (Reuss, 1993) raised,
at odds using current methods of description. The and that others (Smith, 2000; Larson and Plasencia, 2001;
results of this research challenge the claim of the NRC ASFPM, 2007) continue to put forth regarding the
(1995, 2000, 2006) that persuasion is dependent upon an effectiveness of a uniform flood policy, are still relevant.
understanding of uncertainty. More contextual study In order to truly assess the effectiveness of flood risk
using a greater number of variables is necessary to communication, researchers must continue to look at,
clarify the relationship of understanding and persuasion evaluate, and improve the effectiveness of ‘efficient’ flood
if flood plain management and flood risk communica- policy in multiple social and spatial contexts.
tion is to be more effective.
 Second, the description of a flood with a 26 percent References
chance of occurring in 30 years induced confusion,
vehemence, and dismissal in the sample group. Further Arkin, E., 1989. Translation of risk communication for the public:
message development. In: Covello, V., McCallum, D., Pavlova, M.
use of this term should be reconsidered in light of this (Eds.), Effective Risk Communication: The Role and Responsibility of
reaction.1 Government and Nongovernment Organizations. Plenum Press, New
 Third, survey participants were more concerned about York, pp. 127–136.
flood levels than flood frequency and were more ASFPM (Association of State Floodplain Managers), 2007. National
Flood Programs and Policies in Review—2007, Draft Version.
effectively persuaded when specific physical references
Available at: /http://www.floods.orgS.
were used. These results support recommendations by Belsten, L.A., 1996. Environmental risk communication and community
the NRC (1995, 2000, 2006), Smith (2000), Siegrist and collaboration. In: Muir, S., Veenendall, T. (Eds.), Earthtalk: Commu-
Gutscher (2006) and the ASFPM (2007) to include nication Empowerment for Environmental Action. Praeger, Westport,
damage estimates and concrete references and may point CT, pp. 27–41.
the way towards more effective communication. Boisot, M., 1995. Information Space: A Framework for Learning in
Organizations, Institutions and Culture. Routledge, London.
Connelly, N.A., Knuth, B.A., 1998. Evaluating risk communication:
6.2. Generalizations and future directions examining target audience perceptions about four presentation formats
for fish consumption health advisory information. Risk Analysis 18
(5), 649–659.
In addition to reinforcing calls to attach concrete Covello, V.T., von Winterfeldt, D., Slovic, P., 1986. Communicating risk
references to abstract methods of communication, results information to the public. Risk Abstracts 3, 1–14.
supported researchers who have questioned the linear Environmental Agency, 2005. Improving Community and Citizen
relationship between understanding, attitude and behavior Engagement in FRM Decision Making, Delivery and Flood Response.
outlined in hierarchical models of behavior change Environment Agency, Bristol. Available at: /http://publications.
environment-agency.gov.uk/pdf/SCHO1005BJTC-e-e.pdfS.
(e.g. Tierney, 1993; Valente et al., 1998; Mileti and Peek, Faber, S., 1996. On Borrowed Land: Public Policies for Floodplains.
2002). However, this preliminary study was conducted Lincoln Institute of Land Policy, Cambridge, MA.
Federal Emergency Management Agency (FEMA), 2006a. Flood Statistics.
1
The terms used in this research were also discussed in an upper level Available at: /http://www.floodsmart.gov/floodsmart/pages/statistics.
undergraduate hazards class; several students indicated a preference for jsp;jsessionid=CFBC9E0E291618DD5993AD29BCF29E1DS.
the 26 percent description. The context in which this term is introduced Federal Emergency Management Agency (FEMA), 2006b. National
may be an important factor in its relative effectiveness. Flood Insurance Program: Answers to Questions about the NFIP.
ARTICLE IN PRESS
H.M. Bell, G.A. Tobin / Environmental Hazards 7 (2007) 302–311 311

Booklet MitDiv-2, F-084. Available at: /http://www.fema.gov/business/ National Research Council (NRC), 1995. Flood Risk Management and
nfip/qanda.shtmS. the American River Basin: An Evaluation. National Academy Press,
Flooddamagedata.org. Flood Damage in the United States, 1926–2003: A Washington, DC.
Reanalysis of the National Weather Service Estimates. Available at: National Research Council (NRC), 2000. Risk Analysis and Uncertainty
/http://flooddamagedata.org/S. in Flood Damage Reduction Studies. National Academy Press,
Frech, M., 2005. Flood risk outreach and the public’s need to know. Washington, DC.
Journal of Contemporary Water Research and Education 130, 61–69. National Research Council (NRC), 2006. Completing the Forecast:
GBRA (Guadalupe-Blanco River Authority), 1999. Staying Safe: A Guide Characterizing and Communicating Uncertainty for Better Decisions
to Flooding in the Guadalupe River Basin. Gaudalupe-Blanco River Using Weather and Climate Forecasts. National Academy Press,
Authority, Seguin, TX. Washington, DC. Available at: /http://books.nap.edu/catalog.php?re-
GFWNFPF (Gilbert F. White National Flood Policy Forum), 2004. cord_id=11699S.
Reducing Flood Losses: Is the 1% Chance Flood Standard Sufficient? Parker, D.J., 2000. Managing flood hazards and disasters: international
Report of the 2004 Assembly of the Gilbert F. White National Flood lessons, directions and future challenges. In: Parker, D.J. (Ed.),
Policy Forum, Washington, DC. Floods, vol. II. Routledge, London, pp. 287–306.
Godber, A.M., 2005. Urban floodplain land-use-acceptable risk? The Pielke Jr., R.A., Downton, M.W., Barnard Miller, J.Z., 2002. Flood
Australian Journal of Emergency Management 20 (3), 22–26. damage in the United States, 1926–2000: A Reanalysis of National
Gruntfest, E., Carsell, K., Plush, T., 2002. An Evaluation of the Boulder Weather Service Estimates. UCAR, Boulder, CO.
Creek Local Flood Warning System. University of Colorado at Powell, D., Leiss, W., 1997. Mad Cows and Mother’s Milk: The Perils of
Colorado Springs, Colorado Springs. Poor Risk Communication. McGill-Queen’s University Press, Mon-
Hance, B., Chess, C., Sandman, P., 1989. Improving dialogue with treal.
communities: a risk communication manual for government. In: Reuss, M., 1993. Water Resources People and Issues: Gilbert F. White.
Covello, V., McCallum, D., Pavlova, M. (Eds.), Effective Risk USACE Office of History, Fort Belvoir.
Communication: The Role and Responsibility of Government Ross, T., Lott, N., 2006. Billion Dollar US Weather Disasters, 1980–2005.
and Nongovernment Organizations. Plenum Press, New York, National Climatic Data Center, Ashville, NC.
pp. 191–296. Siegrist, M., Gutscher, H., 2006. Flooding risks: A comparison of lay
Kasemir, B., Jaeger, C., Jaeger, J., 2003. Citizen participation in people’s perceptions and expert’s assessments in Switzerland. Risk
sustainability assessments. In: Kasemir, B., Jaeger, J., Jaeger, C., Analysis 26 (4), 971–979.
Gardner, M. (Eds.), Public Participation in Sustainability Science: A Slovic, P., 1986. Informing and educating the public about risk. Risk
Handbook. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 3–36. Analysis 6 (4), 403–415.
Kasperson, R.E., Stallen, P.M., 1991. Risk communication: the evolution Smith, D.I., 2000. Floodplain management: problems, issues and
of attempts. In: Kasperson, R.E., Stallen, P.M. (Eds.), Communicating opportunities. In: Parker, D.J. (Ed.), Floods, vol. I. Routledge,
Risk to the Public: International Perspectives. Kluwer Academic Press, London, pp. 254–267.
Dordrecht, pp. 1–12. Tierney, K.J., 1993. Socio-economic Aspects of Hazard Mitigation.
Kates, R., White, G., 1961. Flood hazard evaluation. In: White, G. (Ed.), University of Delaware Disaster Research Center, Newark, DE.
Papers on Flood Problems: Department of Geography Research Paper Available at: /http://dspace.udel.edu:8080/dspace/handle/19716/577S.
No. 70. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 135–147. Tobin, G.A., 1995. The levee love affair: a stormy relationship. Water
Larson, L., Plasencia, D., 2001. No adverse impact: a new direction in Resources Bulletin 31 (3), 359–367.
floodplain management. Natural Hazards Review 2 (4), 157–181. Trumbo, C., 2000. Risk, rhetoric, and the reactor next door. In: Coppola,
McQuail, D., Windhal, S., 1981. Communication Models for the Study of N., Karis, B. (Eds.), Technical Communication, Deliberative Rhetoric,
Mass Communications. Longman, Inc., New York. and Environmental Discourse: Connections and Directions. Ablex
Mileti, D., Peek, L., 2002. Understanding individual and social Publishing Corporation, Stamford.
characteristics in the promotion of household disaster preparedness. United States Census Bureau, 2004. State and County QuickFacts.
In: Dietz, T., Stern, P.C. (Eds.), New Tools for Environmental Available at: /http://quickfacts.census.gov/qfd/index.htmlS.
Protection: Education, Information, and Voluntary Measures. Na- Valente, T.W., Paredes, P., Poppe, P.R., 1998. Matching the message to
tional Academy of Sciences, Washington, DC, pp. 125–139. the process: the relative ordering of knowledge, attitudes, and practices
Murphy, F., 1958. Regulating Floodplain Development. University of in behavior change research. Human Communication Research 24 (3),
Chicago Press, Chicago. 366–385.

You might also like