Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 162 (2019) 1–8

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynlme

Sleep accelerates re-stabilization of human declarative memories T


a b c a,⁎
Malen D. Moyano , Susanne Diekelmann , María E. Pedreira , Cecilia Forcato
a
Unidad Ejecutora de Estudios de Neurociencias y Sistemas Complejos, CONICET, Universidad Nacional Arturo Jauretche, Hospital de Alta Complejidad en Red El Cruce
“Néstor Kirchner”, Florencio Varela, Argentina
b
Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany
c
Laboratorio de Neurobiología de la Memoria, Departamento de Fisiología y Biología Molecular y Celular, IFIBYNE-CONICET, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales,
Universidad de Buenos Aires, Pab. II, 2do piso, Buenos Aires, Argentina

ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT

Keywords: Consolidated memories can return to a labile state upon presentation of a reminder, followed by a period of re-
Reconsolidation stabilization known as reconsolidation. This period can take several hours, and if an amnesic agent (e.g. new
Declarative memory learning) is administered inside the time window of reconsolidation (when the memory is still labile) the
Interference memory is impaired, whereas the memory remains unaffected if the amnesic agent is administered outside this
Sleep
time window. Sleep plays a fundamental role in the consolidation and integration of new memories, and recently
Slow wave activity
sleep has also been implicated in memory reconsolidation. Here, we studied the role of sleep in accelerating the
reconsolidation time window. On day 1, participants learned a list of syllable-pairs (List 1). On day 2, they
received a reminder, followed by interference learning (List 2) administered either after 90 min of wakefulness,
after 90 min of sleep, or after 10 h of wakefulness. On day 3, participants had to recall List 1 first, followed by
List 2, and we assessed the Retrieval-Induced-Forgetting Effect (RIF) on List 2 as a measure of List 1 memory
stability. We found that the 90 min sleep group showed an intact RIF effect similar to the 10 h wake group,
reflecting stable List 1 memory after 90 min of sleep and after 10 h of wakefulness. However, the RIF effect was
absent after 90 min of wakefulness, suggesting that the List 1 memory was still labile at that time. Moreover, the
RIF effect in the 90 min sleep group was associated with power density in the slow oscillation frequency band
(0.5–1 Hz) during SWS and S2. These findings suggest that 90 min of sleep accelerate memory re-stabilization
after reminder presentation, shortening the reconsolidation time window and protecting the memory against
subsequent interference. This rapid memory re-stabilization may depend on slow oscillation activity during
NREM sleep.

1. Introduction inhibitor, new learning, beta-blocker) is presented inside the re-


consolidation time window, the re-stabilization process and subsequent
After acquisition, new memories are in a labile state and they need memory performance are impaired. However, if the amnesic agent is
to stabilize in order to persist, a process known as consolidation. This presented outside the reconsolidation time window (after more than
process involves modifications of the synapses concerning the engram 6 h, Nader et al., 2000; Walker et al., 2003; Forcato et al., 2007;
(synaptic consolidation) as well as a redistribution of information to Herszage & Censor, 2017), the memory trace remains intact. This
long-term storage areas (system consolidation) (Dudai, Karni, & Born, process of labilization and reconsolidation has been demonstrated in
2015). However, once consolidated, memories are not fixed. After the different species and types of memories including declarative memories
presentation of a reminder, stored memories can return to a labile state, in humans (Forcato et al., 2007; Hupbach, Gomez, Hardt, & Nadel,
and become susceptible to enhancement or impairment again (Nader, 2007; Nader et al., 2000) and represents a unique opportunity to
Schafe, & Le Doux, 2000). These memories then require reconsolidation modify stored information (Forcato, Fernández, & Pedreira, 2014).
in order to persist, which involves the re-stabilization of the memory Forcato et al. (2007) showed that a consolidated declarative
trace taking time to be accomplished (around 6 h, Nader et al., 2000; memory (list of syllable pairs, List 1) can be reactivated by the pre-
Walker, Brakefield, Hobson, & Stickgold, 2003; Forcato et al., 2007; sentation of a cue-reminder and a second learning session (of another
Herszage & Censor, 2017). If an amnesic agent (e.g. protein synthesis list of syllable pairs, List 2) interferes with its re-stabilization when it is


Corresponding author at: Unidad Ejecutora de Estudios de Neurociencias y Sistemas Complejos, CONICET, Universidad Nacional Arturo Jauretche, Hospital de
Alta Complejidad en Red El Cruce “Néstor Kirchner”, Av. Calchaquí 5400, 1888 Florencio Varela, Buenos Aires, Argentina.
E-mail address: cecilia.forcato@gmail.com (C. Forcato).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2019.04.012
Received 4 December 2018; Received in revised form 25 March 2019; Accepted 24 April 2019
Available online 25 April 2019
1074-7427/ © 2019 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
M.D. Moyano, et al. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 162 (2019) 1–8

presented 5 min or 6 h after the reminder (inside the time window of (50 women, 36 men). None of the participants reported ongoing med-
reconsolidation) but not 10 h later when the memory is already re- ication, health problems, medical interventions, or history of psychia-
stabilized (outside the time window). Impaired re-stabilization of the tric, neurological, or sleep disorders. Subjects that reached at least 60%
target memory inside the time window of reconsolidation is typically of correct responses during the last four training trials were included in
indicated by the absence of a Retrieval-Induced Forgetting effect the analysis. 14 subjects were excluded from the analysis because they
(Forcato et al., 2007; Forcato, Argibay, Pedreira, & Maldonado, 2009; did not reach the learning criterion (9), did not sleep (2), did not un-
Forcato, Fernández, & Pedreira, 2013). Retrieval-Induced Forgetting derstand the instruction (1), did not complete all experimental sessions
(RIF, Anderson, Bjork, & Bjork, 1994; MacLeod & Macrae, 2001) allows (1), or rehearsed the syllables during the retention interval (1). The
for an assessment of the integrity of the memory trace, i.e. whether a final sample included 72 participants, with ages ranging from 18 to 36
stored memory is intact or impaired, beyond simple retrieval perfor- (23.87 ± 0.50 years).
mance. According to the RIF effect, retrieval of one memory (e.g. List 1) All the experiments were carried out in accordance with the Code of
can temporarily block the retrieval of a related memory (e.g. List 2, Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki).
Forcato et al., 2007). Importantly, this RIF effect is only observed when Before their participation in the experiment, subjects signed a written
the memory trace that is recalled first is intact, whereas the RIF effect is informed consent that had been approved by the “Comité de Ética del
not observed when the first recalled memory trace in impaired. That is, Hospital en Alta Complejidad en Red El Cruce, Dr. Néstor Kirchner”.
if the stored memory of List 1 is intact, its retrieval blocks the sub-
sequent retrieval of the memory of List 2 (evidenced as intact RIF ef- 2.2. Experimental groups
fect). On the other hand, if the stored memory of List 1 is impaired, its
retrieval does not block the subsequent retrieval of the List 2 memory Participants were randomly assigned to one of five groups. All
(evidenced by no RIF effect; Forcato et al., 2007, 2009, 2013). Apart groups performed the List 1 training on day 1 and were tested on day 3
from the RIF effect, memory performance at retrieval may also be in- (Fig. 1A). The groups differed in the treatment they received on day 2.
fluenced by simultaneous retrieval interference. If memories of both The “Reactivation-90minwake-Interference” group (n = 15) received
List 1 and List 2 are recalled during the same session at retrieval, they the reminder and 90 min later the interference task, within the re-
may interfere with each other leading to acute performance decre- consolidation time window (Forcato et al., 2007). The “Reactivation-
ments, which are however, independent of the integrity of the under- 10hwake-Interference” group (n = 14) received a reminder and 10 h
lying memory trace (Forcato et al., 2007, 2009). later the interference task, outside the reconsolidation time window
Sleep plays a key role in the consolidation of new information (Forcato et al., 2007). The “Reactivation-90minsleep-Interference”
(Rasch & Born, 2013). For declarative memories, sleep strengthens and group (n = 13) received a reminder, and learned the interference task
stabilizes newly encoded representations and integrates them into pre- after sleeping for 90 min. Two more control groups were assessed. The
existing long-term stores (Diekelmann & Born, 2010; Diekelmann, control List 1 group (“CTL-L1”, n = 19) received the reminder on day 2
Büchel, Born, & Rasch, 2011; Dudai et al., 2015). This positive effect of but did not learn the interference task. Participants in the control List 2
sleep is not only observed after a full night of sleep but also after short group (“CTL-L2”, n = 11) skipped List 1 training on day 1, learned the
naps of 6–90 min (Alger, Lau, & Fishbein, 2012; Diekelmann, Biggel, interference task on day 2 without any reminders and were tested on
Rasch, & Born, 2012; Lahl, Wispel, Willigens, & Pietrowsky, 2008). day 3.
Recent evidence suggests that sleep also benefits memory reconsolida-
tion (Walker et al., 2003; Klinzing, Rasch, Born, & Diekelmann, 2016; 2.3. Procedure
Kindt & Soeter, 2018; but see Hardwicke, Taqi, & Shanks, 2016). Slow
wave sleep (SWS) and slow wave activity (0.5–4 Hz) in particular have For the “Reactivation-90minsleep-Interference” group, all subjects
been linked to both consolidation and reconsolidation of declarative spent an adaptation day in the sleep lab including placement of elec-
memories (Alger et al., 2012; Diekelmann et al., 2012; Klinzing et al., trodes before the experimental day. On the adaptation day, participants
2016; Macdonald & Cote, 2016; Wilhelm et al., 2011). arrived at the lab between 12:00 and 14:00 h. First, they signed the
Here, we asked whether a diurnal 90 min-nap accelerates memory re- informed consent form and filled out the personal data questionnaire.
consolidation by shortening the time window during which labile mem- Thirty minutes later they were prepared for polysomnographical re-
ories are susceptible to interference. To test this question, participants cordings and were then allowed to sleep in a quiet, darkened room.
learned a list of syllable pairs on day 1 (List 1) and received a reminder on They were awakened after 90 min of sleep and electrodes were re-
day 2. Reminder presentation was followed by an interference task moved. The adaptation day and the experimental day 1 were separated
(learning of a second list of syllable pairs, List 2) either after 90 min of by at least 48 h. For the experiment proper, participants returned to the
wakefulness, after a 90 min nap, or after 10 h of wakefulness. Participants lab on three consecutive days. On day 1, they arrived at the lab between
were then tested on both lists on day 3, specifically assessing the RIF effect 12:00 h and 15:00 h and had to complete the Stanford Sleepiness Scale
on List 2. We expected that after 90 min of wakefulness, the memory is still first (SSS, 7-point scale, ranging from 1 “feeling active, vital, alert, or
labile (inside the reconsolidation window) and thus impaired by inter- wide awake” to 7 “no longer fighting sleep, sleep onset soon, having
ference learning, as evidenced by the absence of a RIF effect on List 2. After dream-like thoughts”). After that, they learned the first list of syllable
10 h of wakefulness, we expected the memory to be already re-stabilized pairs (List 1) on the computer and then left the lab. On day 2, between
(outside the reconsolidation window) and thus not to be affected by in- 12:00 and 14:00 h, the participants returned to the lab and the elec-
terference learning, evidenced by an intact RIF effect on List 2. Importantly, trodes were placed. The procedure took twenty minutes. After that,
we hypothesized that 90 min of diurnal sleep following reminder pre- they completed the SSS, received the reminder and were then allowed
sentation accelerates memory reconsolidation (i.e. shortens the re- to sleep for 90 min. The experimenter counted 90 min from the first
consolidation window) such that the memory would be protected against spindle or K-Complex detected online. After being awakened, partici-
interference learning already after 90 min and this effect would be revealed pants waited 30 min to avoid sleep inertia. During this time, electrodes
by an intact RIF effect on List 2. were removed and participants were allowed to freshen up. Then they
performed the SSS again, and after that learned the interference task
2. Materials and methods (List 2). They were then allowed to leave the lab and returned on the
next day. On day 3, they completed the SSS again and were tested for
2.1. Participants retrieval on both lists.
The participants in the “Reactivation-90minwake-Interference”
86 undergraduate and graduate students volunteered for the study group arrived at the lab on day 1 between 12:00 h and 15:00 h. First,

2
M.D. Moyano, et al. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 162 (2019) 1–8

they signed the informed consent form, filled out the personal data disappeared and another cue-syllable was shown one line below and the
questionnaire and completed the SSS. After that, they learned the first process was repeated until the list was complete. Each cue-syllable was
list of syllable-pairs (List 1) and then left the lab. On day 2, between taken at random and successively from the list of five until the run was
12:00 and 14:00 h, the participants returned to the lab, completed the complete. Thus, in the first run the participants observed how all five
SSS and received the reminder. Following the reminder presentation, pairs were completed once. In the following nine runs, participants had
they were allowed to leave the lab and returned 90 min later. During to complete the syllable-pairs themselves. Each of those nine runs began
the 90 min period, they were allowed to continue with their normal with the presentation of one cue-syllable at the left top side of the
activities but they were instructed not to fall asleep. Upon their return monitor’s screen and an empty response box on the right top. Subjects
to the lab, they completed the SSS again and learned the interference were required to write down the corresponding response-syllable
task (List 2). They were then allowed to leave the lab and returned on within 5 s. Depending on the subject’s response, there were three pos-
the next day, day 3, between 12:00 and 14:00 h. They completed the sible outcomes: first, if no syllable was written down, the correct one
SSS again and were tested for retrieval on both lists. was shown for 4 s; second, if an incorrect syllable was written, it was
The “Reactivation-10hwake-Interference” group arrived at the lab replaced by the correct one and it was shown for 4 s; and third, if the
on day 1 between 7:00 h and 9:00 h, signed the informed consent form, correct response was given, it stayed for a further 4 s. Immediately
filled out the personal data questionnaire, completed the SSS, and thereafter, the syllable pair disappeared and another cue-syllable was
learned the first list of syllable-pairs (List 1). They returned to the lab on shown one line below and the process was repeated until the list was
day 2 between 7:00 h and 9:00 h and followed the same procedure as complete. Each cue-syllable was taken at random and successively from
the “Reactivation-90minwake-Interference” group, except that they had the list of five until the run was complete. After the whole list was
a 10 h period between the reminder and the interference task. During presented, a black background was shown for 3 s and the procedure was
this period they were allowed to leave the lab and continue with their repeated until the entire list of syllable-pairs was completed nine times.
normal activities. They were instructed not to sleep during this period. This training procedure (with syllable pairs being presented in-
They were tested for retrieval on day 3, at the same time they were dividually and consecutively for 4 s) was based on previous studies
trained on the first day. showing that this procedure resulted in an optimal level of memory
The procedures for the control groups were similar to the experi- strength at encoding (Forcato et al., 2007; Stickgold, 2009). The List 1
mental wake groups but they only learned one list of syllables. The training session took about 10 min.
“CTL-L1” group arrived at the lab on day 1 between 12:00 h and
15:00 h. First, they signed the informed consent form, filled out the 2.4.2. Reminder
personal data questionnaire and completed the SSS. After that, they The reminder included the context of List 1 (i.e. blue background
learned the List 1 and then left the lab. On day 2, between 12:00 and color, an image of an Italian coast and a tarantella melody) followed by
14:00 h, the participants returned to the lab and completed the SSS. one cue-syllable taken at random, at the left top side of the monitor’s
After that, they received the reminder and then left the lab. They re- screen and an empty response box on the top right. After 2 s, a notice
turned on day 3, between 12:00 and 14:00 h, completed the SSS again was displayed on the monitor announcing that the session was inter-
and were then tested for retrieval on List 1. The “CTL-L2” group started rupted, thus not allowing the subject to write down the response-syl-
the experiment directly on day 2. They arrived at the lab between lable (Fig. 1D). It has been shown that this procedure triggers memory
12:00 h and 15:00 h. First, they signed the informed consent form, filled reconsolidation (Forcato et al., 2007).
out the personal data questionnaire and completed the SSS. After that,
they learned the List 2 and then left the lab. On day 3, between 12:00 2.4.3. Interference task
and 14:00 h, the participants returned to the lab, completed the SSS and Learning of the interference task was similar to the List 1 training
were tested for retrieval on List 2. session, but with a different context (red background colour, the image
of a forest and classical music). The interference task consisted of the
2.4. The task learning of another list of syllable pairs, List 2, which was formed by
five different pairs of nonsense cue-response syllables: OEN-SRO, DRI-
The task consisted of memorizing five pairs of nonsense syllables, CRE, AIC-POA, TIU-PLA, KEC-CLO (Fig. 1B). Like the List 1 training
associated with a context formed by a background color on the com- session, it was formed by 10 runs. In the first run, participants observed
puter screen, an image and music, presented through headphones. The how the five syllable pairs were completed once and in the successive
syllables were formed by three letters (Fig. 1B). We have previously nine runs they had to write down the corresponding response-syllables.
shown that including a context associated to the list of syllables im- Feedback procedures were as for List 1 training. The interference task
proves memory retention (Forcato et al., 2007). took about 10 min.

2.4.1. List 1 training session 2.4.4. Testing session


List 1 was formed by five pairs of nonsense cue-response syllables: On day 3, List 1 and List 2 memory was tested. List 1 memory was
ITE-OBN, ASP-UOD, FLI-AIO, NEB-FOT, COS-GLE (bold type: cue- always tested first before List 2 memory. For both lists, testing was
syllable; regular type: response-syllable, Fig. 1B). The training session formed by two runs each, similar to the training session, i.e. including
of List 1 consisted of 10 runs, associated to a context consisting of a blue recall of each of the syllable pairs twice. Cue-syllables were taken at
background color, an image of an Italian coast and a tarantella melody random and successively from the list of five. Subjects were required to
(Fig. 1C). Each run was formed by five trials, including the presentation write down the corresponding response-syllable within 5 s. The testing
of the entire list of five pairs of syllables. In the first run, the entire list session took about 4 min (2 min per List).
of all five syllable pairs was presented once. The first run started with The written responses were registered and an error was considered
the presentation of the context: first, the image of the Italian coast with when a participant wrote an incorrect response-syllable or if they did
blue background color appeared on the screen for 4 s. Then, a tarantella not answer at all. We took the total errors made in the two runs and
melody played along with the image and background color for another calculated the percentage of errors for each subject for List 1 and List 2
4 s. After that, the context continued while the syllables were presented. testing. Furthermore, we classified the errors into four categories:
First, one cue-syllable appeared at the left top side of the monitor’s “void”, when no response was written down; “intralist”, when a wrong
screen and an empty response box was displayed on the right top. Next, response-syllable from the same list was written down; “intrusion”,
the corresponding response-syllable appeared in the response box and when a syllable from the other list was written down; and “confusion”,
stayed there for 4 s. Immediately thereafter, the syllable pair when the written response-syllable was not included in any of the lists.

3
M.D. Moyano, et al. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 162 (2019) 1–8

Fig. 1. Experimental design and memory task. A. Protocol. The experiment was run on three consecutive days. On day 1, all groups received the List 1 training
(except for the “CTL-L2” group) and were tested on day 3. The groups differed with regard to the treatment on day 2. The “CTL-L1” group received only the reminder.
The “Reactivation-90minwake-Interference” group received the reminder and 90 min later the interference task (List 2). The “Reactivation-10hwake-Interference”
group received the reminder and 10 h later the interference task. The “Reactivation-90minsleep-Interference” group received the reminder, slept for 90 min and
received the interference task after being awakened. The “CTL-L2” group only learned the interference task on day 2 and was tested on day 3. B. Lists. List 1 (L1) and
List 2 (L2, interference task) differed with regard to the cue-response syllable pairs. Each list contained five pairs of nonsense cue syllables (indicated in gray boxes)
and response syllables (indicated in white boxes). C. Training session. Each list was associated with a specific context, i.e. a background color, an image and specific
music (for List 1: blue background, image of an Italian coast, tarantella music). The training session consisted of 10 runs. Each run started with the presentation of the
context, i.e. the background color and the image for 4 s, followed by the same stimuli accompanied by the music for another 4 s. After that, the first cue-syllable
appeared on the left side of the screen, with an empty response box on the right. During the first training run, the response-syllable was then presented automatically
in the response box on the right. After that, the next cue-syllable appeared one line below and the procedure continued until all five syllable pairs were presented
once. After the first run, a black screen was displayed for 3 s before the next run started. The following nine runs were identical to the first run, except that the
response-syllables were no longer presented automatically but participants had to write down the response-syllables in the response box on the right. D. Reminder.
The reminder session was similar to one training trial of List 1, with presentation of the context (background color, image, music) and one cue-syllable. However, the
trial was interrupted 2 s after the presentation of the cue-syllable without allowing subjects to complete the trial with the response-syllable. (For interpretation of the
references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

4
M.D. Moyano, et al. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 162 (2019) 1–8

2.4.5. Demo memory at testing. All correlations are reported uncorrected, but note
Before the List 1 training session, participants were presented with a that none of the correlations remained significant after Bonferroni
demo program to receive all the instructions and to make sure that all correction for multiple comparisons (with a corrected alpha of 0.05/
participants understood the task. The demo program consisted of two 16 = 0.003). The percentage of “void”, “intralist”, “confusion” and
trials, similar in structure as the training session but with another “intrusion” type of errors was analyzed with independent one-way
context and two different pairs of nonsense syllables. ANOVAs. For all analyses, we applied a significance threshold of
p = 0.05.
2.5. Sleep recordings
3. Results
2.5.1. Polysomnography
Standard polysomnographic recordings were obtained using 3.1. Sleep accelerates memory re-stabilization
SOMNOscreen™ (SOMNOmedics), including electroencephalography
(EEG), electromyography (EMG), and electrooculography (EOG). Six Initial learning performance of List 1 and List 2 was similar between
electrodes were used for EEG, placed at F3, F4, C3, C4, P3 and P4, groups (Fig. 2, insets; List 1: Fgroup(3,56) = 2.13, p = 0.11; List 2:
according to the International 10–20 system, referenced to electrodes Fgroup(3,49) = 0.74, p = 0.53). All groups showed successful learning
attached to the mastoids. Data were recorded at a sampling frequency across the learning trials of both List 1 (Frun(8,448) = 78.70,
of 256 Hz and bandpass filtered between 0.2 and 30 Hz. Recordings p < 0.001; Fgroupxrun(24,448) = 0.95, p = 0.54) and List 2
were scored according to standard criteria (Rechtschaffen & Kales, (Frun(8,392) = 90.35, p < 0.001; Fgroupxrun(24,392) = 0.77, p = 0.76).
1968). Analysis was based on epochs free of visually identified EEG However, striking differences between groups developed at testing on
artifacts. day 3 when examining the RIF effect on List 2 (Fig. 2A, ANOVA, F
(3,49) = 5.20, p = 0.003). As expected, the “Reactivation-90minwake-
2.5.2. Power spectral analysis Interference” group showed a similar performance than the
Power density was calculated separately for all sleep epochs of sleep “CTL-L2” group that was only trained and tested on List 2 (List 2
stage 2, SWS (sleep stages 3 and 4) and Rapid Eye Movement sleep errors: 26.0 ± 5.1% vs. 12.7 ± 0.5%, respectively, p = 0.105), evi-
(REM). Only artifact-free intervals were analyzed by Fast Fourier dencing no RIF effect, whereas the “Reactivation-10hwake-
Transformations (FFT) with an adapted Hanning window applied to Interference” group showed a significantly higher percentage of List 2
subsequent blocks of 2048 data points (∼8 s), with 205 data points errors than the “CTL-L2” group, evidencing an intact RIF effect (List 2
overlap (∼0.8 s). Individual mean power density was averaged across errors: 37.9 ± 6.4% vs. 12.7 ± 0.5%, p = 0.003). Thus, the inter-
all electrodes in the following frequency bands: slow oscillations ference task presented 90 min after the reminder interfered with the re-
(0.5–1 Hz), delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), slow spindle (9–12 Hz), fast stabilization of the List 1 memory (evidenced by no RIF effect on List 2).
spindle (12–15 Hz), beta (15–30 Hz), and alpha (8–13 Hz). One parti- However, when the interference task was presented 10 h later, when the
cipant was excluded from the power spectral analysis because of too reconsolidation time window was already closed, it did not impair List
much noise in the EEG signal. 1 memory re-stabilization (evidenced by an intact RIF effect).
Regarding the sleep group, we observed that the “Reactivation-
2.6. Data analysis and statistics 90minsleep-Interference” group showed similar performance than the
“Reactivation-10hwake-Interference” group. Participants in the
We analyzed two different measures to assess retrieval and the “Reactivation-90minsleep-Interference” group had a significantly
stability of the target memory (List 1). First, we analyzed the Retrieval- higher percentage of List 2 errors at testing than the “CTL-L2” group
Induced Forgetting (RIF) effect (Anderson et al., 1994; MacLeod & (List 2 errors: 42.3 ± 5.4% vs. 12.7 ± 0.5%, p = 0.001), showing an
Macrae, 2001) by calculating the percentage of List 2 errors, indicating intact RIF effect (Fig. 2A). The RIF effect on List 2 was comparable
to what extent the prior retrieval of the target memory (List 1) blocks between the “Reactivation-90minsleep-Interference” group and the
the retrieval of related memories (List 2). The presence of the RIF effect, “Reactivation-10hwake-Interference” group (p = 0.57), whereas the
i.e. reduced List 2 retrieval, speaks for a stable List 1 target memory, “Reactivation-90minsleep-Interference” group differed significantly
whereas the absence of the RIF effect, i.e. unaffected List 2 retrieval, is from the “Reactivation-90minwake-Interference” group (p = 0.04).
an indicator of an impaired List 1 target memory (Forcato et al., 2007, However, the difference between the “Reactivation-10hwake-Inter-
2009). Second, we analyzed the retrieval performance of the List 1 ference” group and the “Reactivation-90minwake-Interference” group
target memory by calculating the percentage of List 1 errors. This did not reach significance (p = 0.12).
measure indicates to what extent the retrieval of List 1 is subject to When examining the percentage of errors at testing of the memory
simultaneous retrieval interference at the time of recall (due to si- of List 1, all three experimental groups showed higher List 1 errors than
multaneous recall of List 1 and List 2 in the same session), an effect that the “CTL-L1” group that was only trained and tested on List 1 and re-
is independent of the integrity of the stored List 1 memory trace ceived the reminder on day 2 (Fig. 2B, ANOVA F(3,57) = 5.44,
(Forcato et al., 2007, 2009). p = 0.002, LSD comparisons all p < 0.032), with no differences be-
The percentage of errors at training of List 1 and List 2 were ana- tween the single experimental groups (all p > 0.19). These findings
lyzed with repeated measures ANOVA with “group” as a between suggest that in addition to the intact or absent RIF effect on List 2,
subjects factor and “run” as a within subjects factor. The percentage of retrieval of List 1 was subject to acute simultaneous retrieval inter-
total errors at List 1 and List 2 testing was analyzed with one-way ferences (Forcato et al., 2007).
ANOVA with “group” as a between subjects factor followed by LSD Furthermore, when analyzing the percentage of type of errors, the
Post-hoc comparisons. Results of the LSD Post-hoc tests are reported main differences we observed stem from “void” errors (and fewer “in-
uncorrected; but note that the main results remain essentially un- tralist” and “confusion” errors), but almost no “intrusions”. There was a
changed when Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons is ap- significant difference in the percentage of List 2 “void” type errors
plied (with a corrected alpha of 0.05/12 = 0.0042). Pearson correlation between groups (ANOVA F(3,49) = 4.57, p = 0.005). There were no
analysis was performed to determine the correlation between power significant differences between groups for “intralist”, “confusion”, or
density during sleep and the percentage of errors in List 1 and List 2 “intrusion” type errors for List 2 (all Fs(3,49) > 0.53, p > 0.14). For

5
M.D. Moyano, et al. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 162 (2019) 1–8

Fig. 2. Sleep accelerates memory re-stabilization. A. Retrieval-Induced Forgetting (RIF) effect. The mean percentage of errors ( ± SEM) at List 2 testing on day 3 is
presented. The “Reactivation-10hwake-Interference” and “Reactivation-90minsleep-Interference” groups show intact RIF effects at List 2 recall, indicating stable List
1 memory. The “Reactivation-90minwake-Interference” group does not show a RIF effect, indicating labile List 1 memory. B. Retrieval of List 1 Memory. The mean
percentage of errors ( ± SEM) at List 1 testing on day 3 is presented. All groups show simultaneous retrieval interference effects, i.e. reduced recall of List 1 memory.
Significances indicate planned post-hoc comparisons between experimental groups and the respective control groups (CTL-L2 in A, CTL-L1 in B). Insets show learning
performance during the training session for List 2 (in A) and List 1 (in B), respectively.*, p < 0.05; **, p < 0.01; ***, p < 0.001; NS, p > 0.10.

List 1 testing, there was a significant difference between groups for the with List 1 errors). Participants in the “Reactivation-90minsleep-Inter-
“confusion” type errors (F(3,57) = 2.76, p = 0.05). There were no ference” group slept on average for 81.6 ± 5.7 min, with
significant differences between groups for any of the other types of 4.3 ± 1.2 min in wakefulness, 17.1 ± 3.5 min in Stage 1,
errors (all Fs(3,57) > 1.55, p > 0.08). 42.0 ± 4.5 min in Stage 2, 7.1 ± 2.5 min in Stage 3, 0.2 ± 0.1 min in
Stage 4 and 11.0 ± 2.4 min in REM sleep. Mean power density in S2,
SWS and REM sleep are shown in Table 1.
3.2. Slow wave activity is involved in memory re-stabilization

3.3. Control measures


When examining the correlation between power density during
sleep in the “Reactivation-90minsleep-Interference” group and the RIF
There were no significant differences for the Stanford Sleepiness
effect, i.e. List 2 errors at testing, we observed a significant positive
Scale between groups during any of the sessions (Table 2, all p ≥ 0.12).
correlation with power density of slow oscillations (0.5–1 Hz) during S2
as well as during SWS (Fig. 3, r = 0.65, p = 0.02; r = 0.66, p = 0.02,
respectively). We also found a positive correlation between the RIF 4. Discussion
effect and power density of delta waves (1–4 Hz) during S2 (r = 0.67,
p = 0.02). Thus, the higher the power density in slow oscillations and Here we show that sleep plays a fundamental role in the re-stabi-
delta during the 90 min nap on day 2, the stronger the RIF effect on day lization of consolidated memories, shortening the reconsolidation time
3, an indirect measure of List 1 memory stabilization. No other corre- window triggered by the presentation of a reminder. First of all, we
lations reached significance (all p > 0.06, including any correlations replicated previous results in the wake state, showing that a reminder

Fig. 3. Association between slow oscillations and the Retrieval-Induced Forgetting (RIF) effect. Graphs display correlations between the RIF effect (i.e. percentage of
List 2 errors at testing) and slow oscillation power density (0.5–1 Hz) in A. stage 2 sleep (S2) and B. slow wave sleep (SWS).

6
M.D. Moyano, et al. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 162 (2019) 1–8

Table 1 conditions. On the contrary, the RIF effect was absent after 90 min of
Power density (µV2/Hz). wakefulness, suggesting that List 1 memory was not yet re-stabilized at
S2 this time and sensitive to disruption by interference learning. Thus, the
specific pattern of intact and absent RIF effects in the different ex-
Slow Oscillations (0.5–1 Hz) 94.72 ± 14.61 perimental groups allows drawing conclusions about the integrity of the
Delta (1–4 Hz) 27.07 ± 2.86
underlying target memory traces. Specifically, we conclude that after
Slow Spindle (9–12 Hz) 1.64 ± 0.19
Fast Spindle (12–15 Hz) 1.89 ± 0.34
reactivation, the target memory returns to a labile state and requires
Alpha (8–13 Hz) 1.79 ± 0.18 time to re-stabilize, which takes up to 10 h during wakefulness. How-
SWS
ever, intervening sleep speeds up the re-stabilization process, with the
Slow Oscillations (0.5–1 Hz) 253.40 ± 43.98 target memory already being re-stabilized after 90 min. It can be
Delta (1–4 Hz) 57.37 ± 9.33 speculated that the reconsolidation of declarative memories during
Slow Spindle (9–12 Hz) 1.26 ± 0.22 sleep relies on spontaneous ‘replay’ of the recently reactivated memory
Fast Spindle (12–15 Hz) 1.39 ± 0.27
traces, similar to the replay of newly acquired memories during initial
Alpha (8–13 Hz) 1.65 ± 0.31
sleep-dependent consolidation, promoting memory re-stabilization and
REM
re-distribution (Diekelmann & Born, 2010; Eschenko, Ramadan, Mölle,
Theta (4–8 Hz) 3.07 ± 0.70
Beta (15–30 Hz) 0.70 ± 0.36
Born, & Sara, 2008; Rasch & Born, 2013).
In addition to the RIF effect, we also evaluated memory perfor-
Mean power density ± SEM during the 90 min nap in the mance of List 1. This is important to differentiate actual storage im-
“Reactivation-90minsleep-Interference” group. S2 (stage 2 sleep), SWS pairments of the target memory from simple retrieval failures. When
(slow wave sleep, combined stage 3 and stage 4 sleep), REM (rapid eye testing two related memories at the same time in a retrieval test,
movement sleep). memory performance can be reduced due to simultaneous retrieval
interferences. Considering that memories are stored in complex asso-
Table 2 ciative networks, one memory cue can activate different traces si-
Stanford sleepiness scale. multaneously (e.g. List 1 and List 2). In this case, memories can com-
Day 1 Day 2 Day 2 (List 2 Day 3 (Testing) pete at retrieval leading to impaired recall performance despite the
(Training (Reminder) Interference stored memory being intact (Forcato et al., 2007). For example, when
List 1) task) retrieving List 1 memory, items of List 2 can compete for retrieval re-
CTL-L1 2.22 ± 0.43 2.87 ± 0.51 – 2.78 ± 0.46 sulting in reduced List 1 recall at testing. This is indeed what we ob-
R-90minwake-Int 1.93 ± 0.21 2.40 ± 0.35 2.20 ± 0.22 2.13 ± 0.31 served in all experimental groups in the present study. Specifically, we
R-10hwake-Int 1.64 ± 0.20 2.07 ± 0.22 1.86 ± 0.21 1.79 ± 0.19 found reduced recall (i.e. more errors) in retrieval of List 1 in all re-
R-90minsleep-Int 2.62 ± 0.38 2.92 ± 0.33 2.46 ± 0.24 2.15 ± 0.42 activation groups, i.e. in the groups that were required to recall two sets
CTL-L2 – – 2.64 ± 0.43 2.73 ± 0.38
of memories simultaneously at testing (List 1 and List 2), when com-
p-value p = 0.12 p = 0.25 p = 0.23 p = 0.25
pared to the control group that recalled only one set of memories (CTL-
Mean ratings in the Stanford Sleepiness Scale ± SEM and p-values for one- L1). These findings show that simultaneous retrieval interferences were
way ANOVAs separately for each session (i.e. Training List 1, Reminder, List 2 present at testing of List 1, yet these acute effects were independent of
Interference task, Testing). the long-term integrity of the underlying memory trace, as evidenced
by differential outcomes for the experimental groups in the RIF effect.
presented 24 h after learning, labilized the consolidated List 1 memory, Regarding the type of errors, we observed for both lists that the
and a second learning task (List 2, interference task) impaired List 1 re- majority of errors stem from “void” errors, i.e. no answer given, with
stabilization when presented inside the time window of reconsolidation only a few “intralist” and “confusion” errors and almost no “intrusions”.
(i.e. 90 min after reactivation in the “Reactivation-90minwake- From previous research, it might be expected that reconsolidation ef-
Interference” group). No impairment was observed when the inter- fects mainly result from intrusion types of errors. Hupbach et al. (2007)
ference task was presented outside the reconsolidation time window used a list of objects task and found that reconsolidation of List 1 was
10 h after reactivation (“Reactivation-10hwake-Interference” group) revealed by an increment of intrusions from List 2. These divergent
(Forcato et al., 2007). This result indicates that 90 min after reactiva- results with regard to intrusions could be due to differences in the
tion, the memory was still in a labile state, while 10 h later it was al- learning paradigm and design. While we applied learning lists of pairs
ready re-stabilized and insensitive to the action of amnesic agents. of nonsense syllables, Hupbach and colleagues used concrete objects in
However, we further show that if we allowed participants to sleep after a basket. Moreover, we assessed retrieval in a cued recall test, whereas
reactivation, the interference learning of List 2 did not impair List 1 re- Hupbach and colleagues used free recall testing. Future studies will
stabilization 90 min after reactivation (in the “Reactivation-90min- have to test these factors and their role in memory reconsolidation more
sleep-Interference” group). This finding suggests that the intervening systematically.
sleep period accelerated the re-stabilization process of the labilized List When examining potential sleep features that may be involved in
1 memory such that the reconsolidation window was already closed the observed acceleration of memory re-stabilization processes during
after 90 min, which is normally only observed after 10 h of wakefulness. sleep, we observed an association with slow wave activity (slow oscil-
We used the Retrieval-Induced Forgetting effect (RIF) as a tool to lations and delta activity) during SWS and S2. We found significant
demonstrate memory reconsolidation impairment of List 1. According correlations between the RIF effect and power density of slow oscilla-
to this effect, the retrieval of a target memory can temporarily block tions (0.5–1 Hz) in S2 as well as in SWS in the 90 min sleep group. The
subsequent retrieval of a related memory (Anderson et al., 1994; correlation between power density of delta waves (1–4 Hz) and the RIF
Forcato et al., 2007). Thus, if List 1 memory storage is intact, its re- effect was significant for S2 but not for SWS. Thus, higher power den-
trieval can interfere with subsequent List 2 memory retrieval (intact sity in slow wave activity (slow oscillations and delta waves) during
RIF). On the other hand, if List 1 memory storage is impaired, its re- NREM sleep may be associated with a stronger RIF effect as an indicator
trieval does not interfere with List 2 retrieval (absent RIF, Forcato et al., of greater re-stabilization of the List 1 memory. These findings are in
2007). Hence, we evaluated impairment of List 1 memory by analyzing keeping with previous reports of an involvement of NREM sleep and
the presence of the RIF effect on List 2. As expected, we observed an slow wave activity in declarative memory consolidation and re-
intact RIF effect after 10 h of wakefulness as well as after 90 min of consolidation (Alger et al., 2012; Diekelmann et al., 2012; Klinzing
sleep, indicating successful re-stabilization of List 1 memory in both et al., 2016; Macdonald & Cote, 2016; Wilhelm et al., 2011). However,

7
M.D. Moyano, et al. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 162 (2019) 1–8

the observed correlations were no longer significant after correction for References
multiple comparisons, and therefore, should be interpreted with cau-
tion. Alger, S. E., Lau, H., & Fishbein, W. (2012). Slow wave sleep during a daytime nap is
Recently, Klinzing et al. (2016) examined whether sleep supports necessary for protection from subsequent interference and long-term retention.
Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 98(2), 188–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
memory reconsolidation of remote memories triggered by a reminder nlm.2012.06.003.
and how this process differs from sleep-dependent consolidation. Par- Anderson, M. C., Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (1994). Remembering can cause forgetting:
ticipants were trained on a visuo-spatial task in the presence of an odor. Retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(5), 1063–1087.
The “reconsolidation” group learned the task on day 1, and on day 2, Diekelmann, S., Biggel, S., Rasch, B., & Born, J. (2012). Offline consolidation of memory
they received a reminder (odor) and a mock recall test before a 40- varies with time in slow wave sleep and can be accelerated by cuing memory re-
minute sleep or wake period. The “remote consolidation” group fol- activations. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 98(2), 103–111. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.nlm.2012.07.002.
lowed the same procedure but did not receive reactivation on day 2. Diekelmann, S., & Born, J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nature Reviews
Participants in the “recent consolidation” group skipped the procedure Neuroscience, 11(2), 114–126. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2762.
on day 1 and learned the task immediately before the sleep or wake Diekelmann, S., Büchel, C., Born, J., & Rasch, B. (2011). Labile or stable: Opposing
consequences for memory when reactivated during waking and sleep. Nature
period. When the memory was tested shortly after the sleep or wake
Neuroscience, 14(3), 381–386. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2744.
interval, the results showed that the 40-min sleep period facilitated the Dudai, Y., Karni, A., & Born, J. (2015). The consolidation and transformation of memory.
reconsolidation of reactivated memories, whereas the consolidation of Neuron, 88, 20–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.004.
non-reactivated remote memories was less affected. Recently encoded Eschenko, O., Ramadan, W., Mölle, M., Born, J., & Sara, S. J. (2008). Sustained increase in
hippocampal sharp-wave ripple activity during slow-wave sleep after learning.
memories did not benefit from sleep at all. Klinzing et al. (2016) also Learning & Memory, 15(4), 222–228. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.726008.
found that the time spent in SWS was associated with subsequent Forcato, C., Argibay, P. F., Pedreira, M. E., & Maldonado, H. (2009). Human re-
memory performance selectively in the “reconsolidation” group. Our consolidation does not always occur when a memory is retrieved: The relevance of
the reminder structure. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 91(1), 50–57. https://
results confirm and extend these findings in suggesting that even after doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2008.09.011.
longer re-stabilization periods of 24 h (compared to only 40 min in Forcato, C., Burgos, V. L., Argibay, P. F., Molina, V. A., Pedreira, M. E., & Maldonado, H.
Klinzing et al., 2016), sleep and particularly slow wave activity may be (2007). Reconsolidation of declarative memory in humans. Learning & Memory, 14(4),
295–303. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.486107.
involved in the re-stabilization of reactivated memories. Forcato, C., Fernández, R. S., & Pedreira, M. E. (2013). The role and dynamic of
In another study on the role of sleep in memory reconsolidation and strengthening in the reconsolidation process in a human declarative memory: What
future relevance, Macdonald and Cote (2016) trained participants on decides the fate of recent and older memories? PLoS ONE, 8(4), e61688. https://doi.
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061688.
two blocks of syllable pairs with the instruction that only one of the Forcato, C., Fernández, R. S., & Pedreira, M. E. (2014). Strengthening a consolidated
blocks would be tested at retrieval. 24 h later, one group of subjects memory: The key role of the reconsolidation process. Journal of Physiology - Paris,
received reactivation with reminders that labilized the memories (“la- 108, 323–333. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.09.001.
Hardwicke, T. E., Taqi, M., & Shanks, D. R. (2016). Postretrieval new learning does not
bile” group), whereas another group of subjects received reactivation
reliably induce human memory updating via reconsolidation. Proceedings of the
with reminders that did not labilize the memories (“stable” group). National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(19), 5206–5211.
Although the authors did not observe any differences between groups in https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1601440113.
memory performance at testing, they found that slow wave power Herszage, J., & Censor, N. (2017). Memory reactivation enables long-term prevention of
interference. Current Biology, 27(10), 1529–1534.e2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.
(0.5–4 Hz) during S2 as well as theta power (4–8 Hz) during REM sleep 2017.04.025.
was associated with the retention of future-relevant memories selec- Hupbach, A., Gomez, R., Hardt, O., & Nadel, L. (2007). Reconsolidation of episodic
tively in the “labile” group. The associations with slow wave power are memories: A subtle reminder triggers integration of new information. Learning &
Memory, 14(1–2), 47–53. https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.365707.
well in line with the correlations we found for power density in the Kindt, M., & Soeter, M. (2018). Pharmacologically induced amnesia for learned fear is
same frequency bands (0.5–1 Hz and 1–4 Hz) in the present study, while time and sleep dependent. Nature Communications, 9, 1316. https://doi.org/10.1038/
we did not observe any correlations with power density in REM sleep. s41467-018-03659-1.
Klinzing, J. G., Rasch, B., Born, J., & Diekelmann, S. (2016). Sleep's role in the re-
These differences could be due to differences in the experimental de- consolidation of declarative memories. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 136,
sign. Most importantly, in the present study we applied a second 166–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.10.004.
learning task to interfere with the re-stabilization process after re- Lahl, O., Wispel, C., Willigens, B., & Pietrowsky, R. (2008). An ultra short episode of sleep
is sufficient to promote declarative memory performance. Journal of Sleep Research,
activation, whereas Macdonald and Cote did not use any amnesic
17(1), 3–10. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00622.x.
agents but simply assessed memory re-stabilization without any inter- Macdonald, K. J., & Cote, K. A. (2016). Sleep physiology predicts memory retention after
ventions. Despite slightly different outcomes, these findings together reactivation. Journal of Sleep Research, 25, 655–663. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.
12423.
lend further support to the view that slow wave activity is involved in
MacLeod, M. D., & Macrae, C. N. (2001). Gone but not forgotten: The transient nature of
the reprocessing and re-stabilization of reactivated memories during retrieval-induced forgetting. Psychological Science, 12(2), 148–152. https://doi.org/
sleep. 10.1111/1467-9280.00325.
Nader, K., Schafe, G. E., & Le Doux, J. E. (2000). Fear memories require protein synthesis
in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature, 406(6797), 722–726.
Financial disclosures https://doi.org/10.1038/35021052.
Rasch, B., & Born, J. (2013). About sleep’s role in memory. Physiological Reviews, 93(2),
681–766. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00032.2012.
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and ana- Rechtschaffen, A., & Kales, A. (1968). A manual of standardized terminology, technique and
lysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The authors scoring system for sleep stages of human sleep. Los Angeles, CA, USA: Brain Information
Service, Brain Information Institute, UCLA.
have declared that no competing interests exist. Stickgold, R. (2009). How do I remember? Let me count the ways. Sleep Medicine Reviews,
13(5), 305–308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2009.05.004.
Walker, M. P., Brakefield, T., Hobson, J. A., & Stickgold, R. (2003). Dissociable stages of
Acknowledgment human memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Nature, 425(6958), 616–620.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nature01930.
We thank Camila Jorge for her help with data collection. This work Wilhelm, I., Diekelmann, S., Molzow, I., Ayoub, A., Mölle, M., & Born, J. (2011). Sleep
selectively enhances memory expected to be of future relevance. Journal of
was supported by AGENCIA (PICT 2014 N° 2143 and PICT 2016 N° Neuroscience, 31(5), 1563–1569. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3575-10.
0229 to CF). 2011.

You might also like