Springer Science+Business Media

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Sex Roles

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-018-0917-5

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Does Yoga Help College-Aged Women with Body-Image Dissatisfaction


Feel Better About Their Bodies?
Aviva H. Ariel-Donges 1 & Eliza L. Gordon 1 & Viviana Bauman 1 & Michael G. Perri 1

# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
A majority of U.S. college-aged women experience body-image dissatisfaction, which puts them at heightened risk for the
development of an eating disorder. However, evidence-based psychological interventions for body-image dissatisfaction in this
population are not broadly available due to the limited number of trained counselors. We evaluated the efficacy of yoga as a novel
treatment for body-image dissatisfaction in otherwise healthy U.S. college-aged women. Female participants between the ages of
18–30 were randomly assigned to twice weekly yoga classes for 12 weeks or to a wait-listed control condition. Compared to
participants in the control condition, participants in the yoga condition reported significantly greater improvements in appearance
evaluation and satisfaction with specific body areas at post-test. Participants in the yoga condition also reported larger reductions
in the amount of time and energy spent preoccupied with their appearance. The results of the current study suggest that yoga,
which is widely available across the country, could help college-aged women develop healthier relationships with their bodies.
Health professionals and college administrators may want to encourage young women with body-image dissatisfaction to
practice yoga in order to improve their self-image.

Keywords Body image . Yoga . College students . Human females

Body-image dissatisfaction, defined as a negative self-attitude a sexual object (Fredrickson and Roberts 1997). Specifically,
about one’s physical appearance, is rampant among young the normative performance of femininity in Western cultures
women (Johnson et al. 2015; Neighbors and Sobal 2007). involves displaying certain beliefs and behaviors related to the
Fully 87% of U.S. college-aged women report body-image body, dieting for weight loss is very common among women
dissatisfaction (Runfola et al. 2013), and 40% report they regardless of their actual body weight, and women are more
would consider undergoing cosmetic surgery in the near future likely to discuss their own and others’ weight, body size, and
(Sarwer et al. 2005). Although gender-based body ideals neg- shape (Chrisler and Johnston-Robledo 2017).
atively affect both men and women, research has shown the Given that negative body image is a robust predictor of
prevalence and severity of body image disturbance is higher in eating disorders in women (Dakanalis et al. 2016), effective
women than in men (Corson and Anderson 2002; Stice et al. treatments for body-image dissatisfaction have the potential to
2013). Objectification theory posits that women are accultur- prevent both the psychological distress caused by eating pa-
ated from childhood to accept others’ views on their ideal thology and the significant financial burden associated with
physical presentation such that they internalize their body as treatment (Levine and Piran 2004; Stice and Shaw 2002).
However, the positive impact of current gold-standard treat-
ments for body-image dissatisfaction in college-aged women
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article
(i.e., cognitive dissonance and media literacy interventions) is
(https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-018-0917-5) contains supplementary
material, which is available to authorized users. limited by the finite number of trained interventionists
(Becker et al. 2017; Kilpela et al. 2014). As such, it is critical
* Aviva H. Ariel-Donges that more broadly available interventions for body-image dis-
ahariel@phhp.ufl.edu satisfaction in young women are developed (Douglass 2009;
Halliwell 2015; Smith-Jackson et al. 2014).
1
Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University of Florida, Yoga is easily accessible in the United States via gyms,
P.O. Box 100165, Gainesville, FL 32610-0165, USA community centers, and private studios, and it has the
Sex Roles

potential to reduce body objectification, body-image dissatis- Puymbroeck et al. 2011). Only one control trial study that
faction, and negative affect by introducing young women to used random assignment, to our knowledge, has evaluated
mindfulness skills (Boudette 2006; Douglass 2009; Dijkstra the effect of a yoga intervention on body-image dissatisfaction
and Barelds 2011; Germer 2004; Stewart 2004). Moreover, in college-aged women. Mitchell et al. (2007) randomly
yoga is a popular form of exercise that is not associated with assigned female undergraduates who reported dissatisfaction
the stigma of seeking mental health treatment (Douglass with their bodies into one of three conditions: a cognitive
2009). Yoga is defined as an integrative physical and spiritual dissonance intervention, a once-weekly yoga class interven-
practice that aims to connect a person’s mind, body, and spirit tion, or a wait-listed control condition for 6 weeks. The cog-
(Salmon et al. 2009). Although the many approaches to and nitive dissonance intervention elicited significant decreases in
types of yoga practice (e.g., Hatha, Ashtanga, Iyengar, symptoms of disordered eating, drive for thinness, and body-
Bikram) make it difficult to establish a broadly accepted de- image dissatisfaction, but the yoga intervention was not
scription of yoga, there are three core elements shared by the associated with any significant changes in the measured
various forms: controlled breathing, meditative techniques, variables. Despite their null findings regarding yoga, the
and physical postures (Ospina et al. 2007; Pascoe et al. authors concluded that additional research on yoga in
2017). Mindfulness is a key meditative component in tradi- this population is needed because it is likely that the dose
tional yoga classes such that yoga is often considered of the yoga intervention was insufficient (i.e., 270 min of
Bmindfulness in movement^ (Germer 2004, p. 28). yoga; Mitchell et al. 2007).
Originally derived from ancient Buddhist practices, With the current experiment, we aimed to determine the
mindfulness is the skill of being non-judgmentally aware of effect of a 12-week yoga intervention on body-image dissat-
internal and external experiences in the current moment isfaction in U.S. college-aged women compared to a wait-
(Germer 2004). listed control condition. Based upon consultation with the
Studies using yoga as an intervention for body-image dis- experienced yoga instructors involved in the study, the current
satisfaction, however, are preliminary at best (Neumark- intervention increased the dosage of yoga from Mitchell et al.
Sztainer 2014). Although it has been reported that women (2007) by more than 400% (i.e. from 270 total minutes of
who regularly participate in yoga are less likely to report body yoga offered to 1440 min total) in an attempt to reach a thresh-
dissatisfaction than those who do not engage in yoga (Dittmann old that would result in changes in body-image dissatisfaction
and Freedman 2009; Hafner-Holter et al. 2008; Neumark- while being compatible with an undergraduate academic
Sztainer et al. 2011), extant studies of the body-image benefits schedule. We hypothesized that participants in the yoga inter-
of yoga are largely cross-sectional or non-experimental. For vention would report significantly greater improvements in
example, Daubenmier (2005, p. 207) found that women who body-image dissatisfaction than those in the wait-listed con-
self-identified as yoga practitioners reported higher Bawareness trol condition. Secondary aims included examining changes in
of and responsiveness to bodily sensations, lower self-objecti- depressive symptomatology, disordered eating, mindfulness,
fication, greater body satisfaction, and fewer disordered eating and multidimensional measures of body-image dissatisfac-
attitudes^ than did female aerobic exercisers and sedentary tion. The present research is significant because it fills a gap
women. In another cross-sectional study, Delaney and Anthis in the literature regarding the efficacy of yoga as an interven-
(2010) surveyed female yoga practitioners at five yoga studios tion for body-image dissatisfaction in otherwise healthy
in Connecticut and found that greater self-reported internaliza- young women.
tion of yogic principles is associated with greater body satisfac-
tion and higher levels of sense of body control. For their non-
experimental longitudinal study, Impett et al. (2006) followed Method
women enrolled in a 2-month yoga immersion program in
California and found that they experienced significantly re- Participants
duced self-objectification, improved self-acceptance, and in-
creased life satisfaction. Additionally, a qualitative study of A total of 75 women were randomly assigned to either a yoga
women with obesity and binge eating disorder who partic- condition (n = 37) or a waitlist control (n = 38). There were no
ipated in 12 weeks of yoga classes reported increased significant between-group differences at baseline in age (over-
connections to their body, more energy, and improvements all M = 21.3, SD = 2.5 years), p = .92; BMI (M = 24.3, SD =
in their ability to relax (McIver et al. 2009). 5.1 kg/m2), p = .06; race (73.3% White), p = .65; ethnicity
In an experimental study of yoga for female breast cancer (24.0% Latina), p = .95; student status (96.0% current stu-
survivors’ body-image dissatisfaction, participants qualita- dents), p = .57; depressive symptomatology (M = 7.6, SD =
tively reported positive changes in body image after 75-min 3.7, on BDI-2), p = .72; mindfulness (M = 35.5, SD = 7.0 on
weekly yoga sessions for 2 months, but the effects were not FMI), p = .85; or disordered eating (M = 8.4, SD = 6.2, on
statistically different from a light exercise control group (van EAT-26), p = .93.
Sex Roles

Procedure and Measures in a private, dimly lit classroom space on the campus of a large
U.S. southeastern state university. Yoga mats, blocks, and
Recruitment straps were provided to participants for in-class use. Soft
acoustic music was played throughout each class, and the
College-aged women were recruited from a large southeastern yoga instructors read pre-selected poems related to mindful-
university in the U.S. via electronic, paper, and in-person an- ness themes (e.g., acceptance, present-moment focus, non-
nouncements. The study was advertised as free yoga classes judgment) at the end of each class.
for young women Binterested in feeling better about their Over the course of the intervention period, participants
bodies.^ Of the 196 participants screened for the study, 78 attended classes taught by at least two of the three instructors
were eligible for inclusion; 3 eligible participants withdrew to avoid bias due to instructor effects, and participant atten-
before the intervention began. (More detailed recruitment dance varied at each yoga class to avoid bias introduced by
and retention information is available in the online within-class interactions. All sessions were audio recorded to
supplement.) allow for replication by other researchers. A trained re-
Participants were eligible if they were female, ages 18–30, search coordinator evaluated six yoga classes (12.5% of
had little or no prior experience practicing yoga (i.e., ≤ 3 yoga classes offered) using a treatment fidelity checklist
classes over the past 6 months), could safely participate in based upon Sherman’s (2012) guidelines to assess con-
physical activity, reported normal eating behaviors over the sistency across instructors and adherence to the proto-
past 6 months, had a non-depressed mood (i.e., Beck col; no issues were identified, and feedback was provided
Depression Inventory-Second Edition score ≤ 13), and reported to the instructors.
body-image dissatisfaction (i.e., Multidimensional Body-Self
Relations Questionnaire-Appearance Evaluation score ≤ 3.0). Wait-Listed Condition
Participants were excluded if they reported a history of inpa-
tient hospitalization for an eating disorder or other psychiatric Wait list participants abstained from practicing yoga during
reasons, had a current clinical diagnosis of Major Depressive the 12-week intervention period. Participants randomly
Disorder, or had a current clinical diagnosis of another psychi- assigned to the wait-listed condition were given the opportu-
atric disorder that could interfere with participation. nity to participate in free yoga classes after completing their
post-test assessment.
Random Assignment
Schedule of Assessments
After signing an IRB-approved informed consent, participants
completed screening measures online using the secure elec- Baseline measures (i.e., demographics, health behaviors,
tronic data capture tool, Research Electronic Data Capture MBSRQ-AS, BDI-2, EAT-26, FMI, and anthropometry) were
(REDCap; Harris et al. 2009). Eligible participants were ran- collected during an in-person meeting via REDCap up to
domly assigned to receive either a 12-week group yoga inter- 10 weeks before the intervention period began. Post-test mea-
vention or to be in a 12-week wait-listed control condition. (A sures (i.e., demographics, health behaviors, MBSRQ-AS,
flowchart of participant recruitment, random assignment, and BDI-2, EAT-26, FMI, and anthropometry) were collected dur-
retention is available in the online supplement.) ing an in-person meeting via REDCap within 2 weeks of the
intervention period’s completion. All measures were adminis-
Yoga Condition tered at both time points in the order listed in the following.

Three certified female yoga instructors (ages 69, 28, and 21) Attendance, Demographics, and Health Behaviors
led four yoga classes each week, and participants in the yoga
condition were asked to attend two of the classes weekly for Of the 37 participants in the yoga condition, 18 (48.7%)
12 weeks (i.e., 24 yoga classes total). The 60-min yoga classes attended 20 or more yoga classes (M = 20.7, SD = 1.1 classes);
focused on breathing, meditation, and active poses for begin- 13 participants (35.1%) attended 10–16 classes (M = 12.2,
ner students in the Vinyasa and Ashtanga yoga styles. The SD = 2.1 classes); and 6 participants (16.2%) attended five
yoga instructors based the intervention’s format and tone on or fewer classes (2.5 ± 1.6 classes). Overall, mean attendance
classes they typically taught at local community studios (i.e., in the yoga condition was 14.73 classes (SD = 6.9,
5–15 min warm up, 30–50 min flow of poses, and 5–15 min range = 0–24, mode = 20, median = 16). All participants
cool down and final relaxation). The yoga instructors avoided reported age, race, ethnicity, current student status, prior
making comments about weight, body shape, or physical at- experience with yoga, medical conditions that would
tributes; commentary was limited to general affirmations and limit ability to do yoga safely, and dieting behaviors
statements about body acceptance. The yoga classes were held over the prior 3 months.
Sex Roles

Body-Image Dissatisfaction Disordered Eating

The Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire- The Eating Attitudes Test-26 (EAT-26; Garner et al. 1982) is a
Appearance Scales (MBSRQ-AS; Cash 2000) is a 34-item 26-item self-report measure of symptoms and concerns related
self-report measure for women over 14-years-old that evalu- to eating disorders (e.g., BI am preoccupied with a desire to be
ates attitudinal, cognitive, and behavioral components of thinner^ or BI feel that food controls my life^). Individual
body-image dissatisfaction. The MBSRQ-AS contains five items are rated by the respondent on a 6-point Likert scale
subscales: Appearance Evaluation (7 items), Body Areas from 1 (never) to 6 (always), with select items reversed for
Satisfaction (9 items), Appearance Orientation (12 items), scoring, and total EAT-26 scores reflect a sum of individual
Overweight Preoccupation (4 items), and Self-Classified item scores. Higher EAT-26 scores indicate greater risk for a
Weight. (Self-Classified Weight was not included in the pres- clinical eating disorder. The EAT-26 has been shown to have
ent study given that changes in weight status were not expect- satisfactory reliability and construct validity (Garner et al.
ed to occur during the 3-month intervention period.) 1982; Koslowsky et al. 1992). In the present study, the coef-
Individual items are rated by the respondent on a 5-point ficient alpha was .84.
Likert scale from 1 (definitely disagree) to 5 (definitely agree).
Overall subscale scores on the MBSRQ-AS subscales are Mindfulness
means of item scores, with select items reversed for scoring.
For Appearance Evaluation (e.g., BI like my looks just the way The Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI; Walach et al.
they are^) and Body Areas Satisfaction (e.g., BHow dissatis- 2006) is a 14-item self-report measure that assesses mindful
fied or satisfied are you with each of the following areas or presence, non-judgmental acceptance, openness to experi-
aspects of your body: …Face^), lower scores indicate greater ence, and insight (e.g., BI am open to the experience of the
dissatisfaction with one’s appearance and specific body parts. present moment^). Individual items are rated by the respon-
For Appearance Orientation (e.g., BI check my appearance in a dent on a 4-point Likert scale from 1 (rarely) to 4 (almost
mirror whenever I can^) and Overweight Preoccupation (e.g., always), with select items reversed for scoring. The total
BI constantly worry about being or becoming fat^), higher FMI score is a sum of all items, and higher scores on the
scores indicate greater preoccupation with one’s appearance FMI indicating greater use of mindfulness principles. In the
and weight. Internal consistency across studies is appropriate present study, the coefficient alpha was .90.
with Cronbach’s alphas for the MBSRQ-AS subscales ranging
from .84 to .89 (Brown et al. 1990; Cash 2000; Cash and Anthropometry
Hrabosky 2003; Engeln-Maddox 2006), and 1-month test-re-
test reliability of .91 has been established (Cash 2000). In the A trained research coordinator measured participants’ body
present study, the coefficient alphas for the MBSRQ-AS sub- height (to the nearest .1 cm) and weight (to the nearest
scales were .75 for Appearance Evaluation, .85 for Body .25 kg) using a calibrated Health-o-Meter Professional
Areas Satisfaction, .80 for Appearance Orientation, and .66 stadiometer and balance beam scale. Height and weight were
for Overweight Preoccupation. Given that the alpha for used to calculate body mass index (BMI; kg/m2), which is
Overweight Preoccupation was below .70 and that the inter- commonly included in explanatory models of body dissatis-
action effect for this subscale did not yield significant results faction (e.g., Stice and Bearman 2001). Baseline BMIs ranged
(p = .25), analyses for this subscale are not presented in the from 16.5–45.1 kg/m2 (median = 23.5 kg/m2, mode = 22.5 kg/
present paper. m2). As noted previously, there was no statistical difference
between the yoga and wait-listed participants’ BMIs at base-
Depressive Symptomatology line, t(73) = −1.90, p = .062.

The Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition (BDI-2; Statistical Analyses


Beck et al. 1996) consists of 21 self-report items to assess
the presence and intensity of depressive symptoms over the Data were analyzed with SPSS® 24.0 for Windows (SPSS
past 2 weeks. Each item on the BDI-2 lists statements ar- Inc., IL). Measure x Time x Treatment Condition ANOVAs
ranged in increasing severity about a particular symptom of were used to examine main effects and interactions for
depression (e.g., hopelessness, fatigue, or suicidal ideation). Appearance Evaluation, Body Areas Satisfaction,
Total scores on the BDI-2 are a sum across all items. In Appearance Orientation, mindfulness, depressive symptom-
college-aged students, the BDI-2 has shown high internal con- atology, and disordered eating. Intent-to-treat analyses includ-
sistency and concurrent validity with other self-report mea- ed all 75 participants regardless of how many yoga sessions
sures of depression and anxiety (Storch et al. 2004). In the those in the yoga condition attended (e.g., even if they only
present study, the coefficient alpha was .82. attended three of the 24 sessions). Per-protocol analyses were
Sex Roles

further conducted to illustrate outcomes and effect sizes under Appearance Evaluation
ideal circumstances; per-protocol analyses only utilized par-
ticipants in the wait-listed condition who completed both as- There was a significant main effect of Time on Appearance
sessments (n = 37) and only participants in the yoga condition Evaluation, F(1,73) = 74.54, p < .001, ηp2 = .505, such that
who attended at least 20 of the 24 yoga sessions (≥ 80% participants across treatment conditions experienced signifi-
attendance as instructed in the informed consent; n = 18). cant improvements in Appearance Evaluation, ps < .001 (see
Chi-square tests for independence were utilized to determine Table 2a). There was also a significant Time x Treatment
whether the proportion of participants reporting post-test im- Group interaction effect for Appearance Evaluation,
provements in body-image dissatisfaction and the proportion F(1,73) = 8.96, p = .004, ηp2 = .109 (see Fig. 1). Although
of participants reporting body image satisfaction at post-test there were no significant between-group differences in
differed significantly across treatment conditions. Pearson Appearance Evaluation at baseline, p = .77, participants in
product moment correlations were utilized to characterize the yoga condition reported significantly higher post-
the association between yoga attendance, change in test Appearance Evaluation than did participants in the
body-image dissatisfaction, and mindfulness. All post wait-listed condition, p = .003. In the per protocol sen-
hoc analyses were Bonferroni-corrected to adjust for sitivity analyses, both the main effect of Time,
multiple comparisons. F(1,53) = 58.67, p < .001, ηp2 = .525, and the Time x
Treatment Condition interaction effect for Appearance
Missing Data Evaluation remained significant, F(1,53) = 9.93, p = .003,
ηp2 = .158.
There were no missing data at baseline. At post-test, 6.7% of Treatment condition also had a significant effect on the
participants (n = 5) did not complete their assessment: 3 par- proportion of participants reporting positive post-test
ticipants in the yoga condition and 1 participant in the wait- Appearance Evaluation (i.e., > 3.0 on the subscale), χ2(1) =
listed condition were lost to follow-up, and 1 participant in the 11.23, p = .001, Cramer’s V = .39; a significantly greater pro-
wait-listed condition only completed the MBSRQ-AS portion of participants in the yoga condition reported positive
Appearance Evaluation subscale at post-test. Data are be- post-test Appearance Evaluation compared to participants in
lieved to be missing at random with a non-significant Little’s the wait-listed condition (70.3% vs. 31.6%; see Table 3).
MCAR test, p = .94. Baseline values were carried forward for Among those in the yoga condition, higher attendance was
missing post-test weight data. Given that mean imputation, associated with greater improvement in Appearance
list-wise deletion, and 10 multiple imputations in SPSS® Evaluation at post-test, r = .398, p = .015. Number of yoga
yielded comparable results for all analyses, mean imputation classes attended predicted change in Appearance Evaluation
statistics are presented in the following. Original scores, as at post-test, F(1,35) = 3.98, p = .015, R2 = .158, such that sub-
well as the imputed scores, were normally distributed with scale scores increased by .032 points for each yoga class
non-significant skewness and kurtosis. attended.

Body Areas Satisfaction


Results
There was a significant main effect of Time on Body Areas
Zero order correlations varied by treatment group and assess- Satisfaction, F(1,73) = 51.47, p < .001, ηp2 = .413, such that
ment time point. For example, better Appearance Evaluation participants across treatment conditions reported significant
at pre-test was associated with better Body Areas Satisfaction increases in Body Areas Satisfaction (see Table 2b). There
in both the yoga and wait-listed control groups, and these was also a significant Time x Treatment Condition interaction
associations remained significant at post-test (ps < .001). In effect for Body Areas Satisfaction, F(1,73) = 7.40, p = .008,
the yoga condition, better Appearance Evaluation was also ηp2 = .092. Although there were no significant differences at
associated with less disordered eating symptomatology and baseline, p = .26, participants in the yoga condition reported
lower BMI at pre-test (ps < .05), yet none of these pre-test significantly higher post-test Body Areas Satisfaction than did
associations were significant in the wait-listed condition. In participants in the wait-listed condition, p = .002. In the per
the yoga condition, better Appearance Evaluation was associ- protocol sensitivity analyses, both the main effect of Time,
ated with lower BMI at post-test (p < .05); in the wait-listed F(1,53) = 35.70, p < .001, ηp 2 = .402, and the Time x
condition, better Appearance Evaluation was associated with Treatment Condition interaction effect for Body Areas
higher mindfulness and less depressive symptomatology at Satisfaction remained significant, F(1,53) = 6.56, p = .013,
post-test (ps < .001). Pre- and post-test correlations among ηp2 = .110. Treatment condition also had a significant effect
all study variables within the yoga and wait-listed control on the proportion of participants reporting positive post-test
conditions are presented in Table 1. Body Areas Satisfaction (i.e., > 3.0 on the subscale), χ2(1) =
Sex Roles

Table 1 Correlations among


study variables by yoga and wait- Correlations
list conditions
Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

(a) Pre-test
1. Appearance evaluation – .51*** .00 .26 −.11 −.46** −.52***
2. Body areas satisfaction .60*** – −.33* −.05 −.10 −.43** −.49**
3. Appearance orientation .20 .03 – .01 −.04 .22 −.05
4. Mindfulness .30 .03 .22 – .15 −.17 −.16
5. Depressive symptomatology −.21 −.08 −.11 −.30 – .14 −.23
6. Disordered eating .03 .15 .04 .34* −.10 – .48**
7. Body mass index (BMI) −.01 .13 −.11 −.03 −.25 −.08 –
(b) Post-test
1. Appearance evaluation – .70*** .02 .25 −.02 −.32 −.62***
2. Body areas satisfaction .84*** – −.29 .28 −.27 −.59*** −.48**
3. Appearance orientation .00 .02 – −.13 .39* .38* −.08
4. Mindfulness .55*** .46** .07 – −.49* −.09 .02
5. Depressive symptomatology −.62*** −.5*** −.07 −.49** – .51*** −.03
6. Disordered eating −.27 −.18 .34* −.03 .33* – .24
7. Body mass index (BMI) −.21 −.20 .01 −.21 .03 .07 –

Correlations for participants in the yoga condition are reported above the diagonal of the correlation matrix; for
those in the wait-listed control condition, below the diagonal
*p < .05. **p ≤ .01. ***p ≤ .001

13.52, p < .001, Cramer’s V = .43; a significantly greater pro- ηp2 = .081. Treatment condition also had a significant effect
portion of participants in the yoga condition reported positive on the proportion of participants reporting healthy post-test
post-test Body Areas Satisfaction compared to participants in Appearance Orientation (i.e., < 3.0 on the subscale), χ2(1) =
the wait-listed condition (81.1% vs. 39.5%; see Table 3). 5.36, p = .021, Cramer’s V = .27; a significantly greater pro-
Among those in the yoga condition, higher attendance was portion of participants in the yoga condition reported healthy
associated with greater improvement in Body Areas post-test Appearance Orientation compared to participants in
Satisfaction from baseline to post-test, r = .353, p = .032. the wait-listed condition (32.4% vs. 10.5%; see Table 3).
Number of yoga sessions attended predicted change in Body However, attendance among those in the yoga condition was
Areas Satisfaction at post-test, F(1,35) = 5.00, p = .032, not associated with change in Appearance Orientation at post-
R2 = .125, such that subscale scores increased by .02 points test, p = .88.
for each yoga class attended.
Mindfulness
Appearance Orientation
There was a significant main effect of Time on mindfulness,
There was a significant main effect of Time on Appearance F(1,73) = 10.60, p = .002, ηp2 = .127, such that participants
Orientation, F(1,73) = 2.84, p < .001, ηp2 = .215, such that across treatment conditions reported significant increases in
participants across treatment conditions reported significant mindfulness. The Time x Treatment Condition interaction ef-
decreases in Appearance Orientation (see Table 2c). There fect for mindfulness and the association between treatment
was also a significant Time x Treatment Condition interaction condition and mindfulness at post-test did not approach sig-
effect for Appearance Orientation, F(1,73) = 10.14, p = .002, nificance, ps ≥ .21. However, change in mindfulness at post-
ηp2 = .122. Although there were no significant differences at test was associated with improvements in body-image dissat-
baseline, p = .26, participants in the yoga condition reported isfaction at post-test, r = .454, p < .001.
significantly lower post-test Appearance Orientation than did
participants in the wait-listed condition, p = .028. In the per Depressive Symptomatology and Disordered Eating
protocol sensitivity analyses, both the main effect of Time,
F(1,53) = 12.05, p = .001, ηp 2 = .185, and the Time x There was no significant main effect of Time on depressive
Treatment Condition interaction effect for Appearance symptomatology, p = .51. There was no significant main effect
Orientation remained significant, F(1,53) = 4.70, p = .035, of Time on disordered eating, p = .64.
Sex Roles

Table 2 Change in body-image


dissatisfaction by yoga and wait- Baseline Post-test Within-group change Within-group
list conditions over time M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) t-test

(a) Appearance evaluation


Intent to treat:
Yoga 2.40 (.46) 3.28 (.56) .88 (.55) p < .001
Wait list 2.37 (.52) 2.79 (.78) .43 (.75) p < .001
Between-group t-test p = .77 p = .003
Per protocol:
Yoga 2.48 (.38) 3.54 (.45) 1.1 (.49) p < .001
Wait list 2.35 (.53) 2.79 (.79) .44 (.75) p < .001
Between-group t-test p = .34 p < .001
(b) Body areas satisfaction
Intent to treat:
Yoga 2.70 (.45) 3.25 (.51) .55 (.39) p < .001
Wait list 2.58 (.43) 2.83 (.61) .25 (.55) p = .002
Between-group t-test p = .26 p = .002
Per protocol:
Yoga 2.79 (.40) 3.41 (.42) .62 (.38) p < .001
Wait list 2.59 (.44) 2.83 (.62) .25 (.56) p = .004
Between-group t-test p = .09 p = .001
(c) Appearance orientation
Intent to treat:
Yoga 3.71 (.58) 3.24 (.46) −.47 (.64) p < .001
Wait list 3.57 (.47) 3.49 (.52) −.07 (.41) p = .36
Between-group t-test p = .26 p = .03
Per protocol:
Yoga 3.71 (.70) 3.26 (.55) −.46 (.84) p = .001
Wait list 3.60 (.48) 3.49 (.52) −.11 (.38) p = .26
Between-group t-test p = .47 p = .12

Intent-to-treat analyses include all randomly assigned participants (n = 75); per-protocol analyses (n = 55) only
include participants who completed the post-test assessment and yoga participants with ≥80% attendance
(≥ 20 yoga classes)

Discussion However, the current study did not find significant between-
group differences in change in mindfulness at post-test, nor
The current study aimed to determine whether a 12-week yoga did the study find significant post-test changes in depressive
intervention would significantly reduce college-aged symptomatology or disordered eating.
women’s body dissatisfaction compared to a wait-listed con- Participants in the yoga condition did not endorse signifi-
trol condition. The results of the current study suggest that cantly greater increases in mindfulness as expected. There are
yoga is an efficacious treatment for improving body-image several possible explanations for these null results. It is possi-
dissatisfaction in this population. Participants randomly ble that participants in both conditions reported increases in
assigned to the yoga condition reported significantly greater mindfulness due to demand characteristics of a study about
post-test improvements in several facets of body-image dis- yoga, that mindfulness takes longer than 12 weeks to cultivate,
satisfaction than those in the wait-listed condition did; they or that the FMI is not sensitive to short-term intervention
evaluated their appearance more positively, became more sat- effects in this population. In a systematic review of mindful-
isfied with specific body areas, and decreased the amount of ness measures, Park et al. (2013) reported that the FMI was
time and energy spent focused on their appearance. Effect developed for audiences with some knowledge of insight
sizes for these facets of body-image dissatisfaction were mod- meditation and, therefore, may have limited validity among
erate (ηp2 ranging from .09–.12), suggesting that yoga likely populations such as undergraduate college students.
addresses some of the psychological and behavioral processes Mindfulness scales with stronger psychometric properties,
underlying women’s unhealthy focus on their physical selves. such as the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale or the Five
Sex Roles

3.5
Overall, the current findings largely support Mitchell
et al.’s (2007) conclusion that the dose of yoga administered
in their study (i.e., 6 weeks of 45-min weekly sessions) was
MBSRQ-AS Appearance Evaluation

*
insufficient for eliciting meaningful changes in body-image
3.0
dissatisfaction. Results from the present study suggest that
consistent yoga attendance over an extended period is neces-
sary to experience changes in body-image dissatisfaction.
2.5 Approximately half of the participants randomly assigned to
the yoga condition attended at least 20 yoga classes (≥ 80%
Yoga
attendance), yet a substantial minority (16.2%) attended five
Wait List
classes or fewer over the 3-month intervention period.
2.0 Therefore, about half of the participants struggled to consis-
Baseline Post-Test
tently attend twice-weekly yoga classes, likely due to college-
Assessment Timepoint
aged women’s personal and academic responsibilities.
Fig. 1 Time x Treatment Condition interaction effect for Appearance Notably, there appears to be a dose-response for the effects
Evaluation, p = .004. Gray dotted horizontal line indicates the threshold
between positive and negative Appearance Evaluation (Appearance of yoga on body-image dissatisfaction. Greater percentages of
Evaluation = 3.0), with scores above the gray line representing positive participants reported body image satisfaction depending on
body image; error bars represent ±SEM. *p < .05. MBSRQ-AS = whether they attended five or fewer classes, 10–16 classes,
Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire-Appearance or at least 20 classes over the 12-week period (see Table 3).
Scales.
Although attendance stratification is confounded by self-se-
lection, there appears to be an incremental benefit for attend-
Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (Park et al. 2013), may have ing more yoga classes, with the strongest effect seen for those
been more sensitive to possible intervention effects in the who attended at least 20 yoga classes.
current study. Finally, participants in the current study did not report sig-
Given that mindfulness as measured in the current study nificant improvements in depressive symptomatology or disor-
was not statistically associated with improvements in body- dered eating. Prior research has established that higher depres-
image dissatisfaction, the question remains: How does yoga sive symptomatology predicts disordered eating in college-
exert its positive effects on body-image dissatisfaction? It is aged women (Bergstrom and Neighbors 2006; Bradford and
apparent that the current study was not powered to detect Petrie 2008; Gitimu et al. 2016; Liechty and Lee 2013; Stice
between-group differences on the FMI (post-hoc observed and Bearman 2001) and that body-image dissatisfaction pre-
power = .193), so it is plausible that mindfulness is the under- dicts the onset of disordered eating (Farrell et al. 2006; Levine
lying mediator despite the lack of support in the present study. and Piran 2004). However, the current study excluded partici-
Based on sociocultural models of body-image dissatisfaction pants with elevated depressive symptomatology at baseline in
(e.g., Puccio et al. 2016, it is also possible that other factors not an effort to isolate the efficacy of yoga for body-image dissat-
measured in the current study (e.g., increased embodiment, isfaction given that the positive effects of yoga on depression
stronger distress tolerance skills, decreased internalization of have already been established (Pilkington et al. 2005). As such,
the thin ideal, or improved social support) are the true medi- all participants started the intervention with minimal or no de-
ators of the relationship between practicing yoga and de- pressive symptomatology (BDI-2 ≤ 13), likely causing a floor
creased body-image dissatisfaction. effect that limited statistical significance. When the present

Table 3 Proportion of
participants reporting body-image Yoga condition Wait list
satisfaction at post-test
MBSRQ-AS subscales ≥ 20 10–19 ≤5 0
yoga classes yoga classes yoga classes yoga classes
n = 18% (n) n = 13% (n) n = 6% (n) n = 38% (n)

Appearance evaluation 83.33% (15) 69.23% (9) 33.33% (2) 31.57% (12)
Body areas satisfaction 83.33% (15) 84.62% (11) 66.67% (4) 39.47% (15)
Appearance orientation 38.89% (7) 23.08% (3) 33.33% (2) 10.53% (4)

MBSRQ-AS = Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire-Appearance Scales. Body-image satisfac-


tion is indicated by scoring beyond the mid-point for each of the MBSRQ-AS subscales (i.e., > 3.0 for Appearance
Evaluation and Body Areas Satisfaction, < 3.0 for Appearance Orientation)
Sex Roles

study excluded participants with clinically relevant depressive also be powered to assess the minimum dose of treatment
symptomatology at baseline, it appears that participants with needed to cause clinically-relevant improvements and to eval-
elevated risk for disordered eating were inadvertently excluded uate potential mediators (e.g., mindfulness, embodiment, in-
(M EAT-26 score at baseline = 8.4), which limited our ability to ternalization of the thin ideal) of the relationship between yoga
determine the effects of yoga on disordered eating behaviors and change in body-image dissatisfaction.
and attitudes. Most importantly, further research is needed to determine
which components of yoga represent the Bactive ingredients^
Limitations necessary for producing change in body-image dissatisfaction
(i.e., specific poses, breathing, mindfulness teachings, group
There are several limitations to the current study. First, the contact) and how to adjust these factors according to the needs
study did not compare yoga to the gold standard of cognitive of the setting (i.e., colleges, high schools, gyms, community
dissonance treatment (Stice et al. 2013) or a self-help program centers). Potential components to examine include type of
(Jarry and Ip 2005) to determine the relative efficacy of each yoga, session length, session frequency, session location,
treatment. Second, the study did not measure thin-ideal inter- and instructor style (Klein and Cook-Cottone 2013).
nalization or social pressures for thinness, which have been Although the benefits of yoga for women with body-image
identified as key factors in the development and maintenance dissatisfaction appear promising, more rigorous methodolog-
of body-image dissatisfaction (Durkin et al. 2007; Grabe et al. ical evaluation of these key factors will not only inform the
2008; Johnson et al. 2015). Third, although excluding partic- design of effective interventions, but also increase the likeli-
ipants with clinically-relevant depressive symptomatology hood of obtaining stakeholders’ support in establishing pro-
controlled for its potentially confounding effect, it limits gen- grams (Serwacki and Cook-Cottone 2012). Additionally, giv-
eralizability to non-depressed college-aged women seeking en the substandard alpha for Overweight Preoccupation found
treatment for body-image dissatisfaction. Generalizability in the current study, analyses were not presented and further
should also be view in light of the limited racial and ethnic work is needed utilizing this subscale.
diversity in the present sample. Fourth, prior experience with
yoga was assessed at baseline, but the study did not assess Practice Implications
whether participants had prior experience with meditation or
mindfulness, which may confound the results of the study. Current gold standard treatments for body-image dissatisfaction
Finally, the current study did not utilize a follow-up period are not available on most college campuses because they re-
in order to determine if improvements in body-image dissat- quire well-trained group leaders, as well as administrative and
isfaction were sustained after the intervention ended. financial resources to organize session materials. Although re-
The current study also had numerous strengths, including searchers are working on developing internet-based dissonance
the use of random assignment with a wait-listed control and interventions and expanding treatment availability using train-
high retention of the intent to treat sample (only 6.7% missing the-trainer models, most college-aged women do not have ac-
data at post-test). Recruitment from a large state university cess to these effective interventions at present (Kilpela et al.
allowed for results to be applicable to a large segment of the 2014). In contrast, yoga is a widely available way for young
female college population in the United States. Additionally, women to get physical activity and improve body-image dis-
multiple certified yoga instructors were utilized to avoid bias satisfaction without the barriers and stigma associated with tra-
due to instructor effects, and the yoga classes were structured ditional mental health treatment (Halliwell 2015). Importantly,
and audio-recorded to allow for reproducibility in future yoga is broadly accessible throughout the country at campus
studies. gyms, community centers, and private yoga studios as well as
from community practitioners. College counselors and admin-
Future Directions istrators may want to consider incorporating yoga into new
student orientations, counseling center offerings, sorority pro-
Given that research on yoga and body-image dissatisfaction is gramming, and other student-wide initiatives so that young
nascent, there are numerous directions for future studies. women will be more readily exposed to its benefits at the start
Researchers should examine whether combining yoga with of their college experience.
cognitive dissonance or media literacy treatment enhances
the effectiveness of either treatment in college-aged women; Conclusions
whether improvements in body-image dissatisfaction are
sustained over a longer follow-up period; and whether indi- The results of the current study largely support prior re-
viduals with clinical depression or other comorbid psychiatric searchers’ assertions that yoga would be an efficacious
conditions have differential outcomes. Future studies on the treatment for body-image dissatisfaction in college-aged
effectiveness of yoga for body-image dissatisfaction should women (e.g., Boudette 2006; Stewart 2004). In line with
Sex Roles

recommendations for fostering positive body image on col- handbook of theory, research, and clinical practice (pp. 192–199).
New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
lege campuses (Smith-Jackson et al. 2014), it is possible that
Dakanalis, A., Timko, A., Serino, S., Riva, G., Clerici, M., & Carrà, G.
recommending yoga for college-aged women with body- (2016). Prospective psychosocial predictors of onset and cessation of
image dissatisfaction would be an effective alternative to tra- eating pathology amongst college women. European Eating
ditional mental health treatment. Whether combined with Disorders Review, 24(3), 251–256. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2433.
Daubenmier, J. J. (2005). The relationship of yoga, body awareness, and
existing evidence-based counseling for body-image dissatis-
body responsiveness to self-objectification and disordered eating.
faction or as a standalone treatment, yoga warrants further Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29(2), 207–219. https://doi.org/
exploration as a way to help college-aged women improve 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2005.00183.x.
their body-image dissatisfaction and prevent the development Delaney, K., & Anthis, K. (2010). Is women's participation in different
types of yoga classes associated with different levels of body aware-
of eating disorders.
ness satisfaction? International Journal of Yoga Therapy, 1(1), 62–
71. https://doi.org/10.17761/ijyt.20.1.t44l6656h22735g6.
Compliance with Ethical Standards Dijkstra, P., & Barelds, D. P. (2011). Examining a model of dispositional
mindfulness, body comparison, and body satisfaction. Body Image,
The research was approved by the University of Florida IRB, and all 8(4), 419–422. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.05.007.
procedures were conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of Dittmann, K. A., & Freedman, M. R. (2009). Body awareness, eating
the IRB and the 1964 Helsinki Declaration. attitudes, and spiritual beliefs of women practicing yoga. Eating
D i s o rd e r s , 1 7 ( 4 ) , 2 7 3 – 2 9 2 . h t t p s : / / d o i . o rg / 1 0 . 1 0 8 0 /
10640260902991111.
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all participants
Douglass, L. (2009). Yoga as an intervention in the treatment of eating
in the study.
disorders: Does it help? Eating Disorders, 17(2), 126–139. https://
doi.org/10.1080/10640260802714555.
Conflict of Interest There are no conflicts of interest to declare. Durkin, S. J., Paxton, S. J., & Sorbello, M. (2007). An integrative model
of the impact of exposure to idealized female images on adolescent
girls’ body satisfaction. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
37(5), 1092–1117. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2007.
References 00201.x.
Engeln-Maddox, R. (2006). Buying a beauty standard or dreaming of a
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck Depression new life? Expectations associated with media ideals. Psychology of
Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. https:// Women Quarterly, 30(3), 258–266. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-
doi.org/10.1037/t00742-000. 6402.2006.00294.x.
Becker, C. B., Perez, M., Kilpela, L. S., Diedrichs, P. C., Trujillo, E., & Farrell, C., Shafran, R., & Lee, M. (2006). Empirically evaluated treat-
Stice, E. (2017). Engaging stakeholder communities as body image ments for body image disturbance: A review. European Eating
intervention partners: The Body Project as a case example. Eating Disorders Review, 14(5), 289–300. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.693.
Behaviors, 25, 62–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.03.015. Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory:
Bergstrom, R. L., & Neighbors, C. (2006). Body image disturbance and Toward understanding women's lived experiences and mental health
the social norms approach: An integrative review of the literature. risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21(2), 173–206. https://doi.
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 25(9), 975–1000. https:// org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1997.tb00108.x.
doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2006.25.9.975. Garner, D. M., Olmsted, M. P., Bohr, Y., & Garfinkel, P. E. (1982). The
Boudette, R. (2006). Yoga in the treatment of disordered eating and body Eating Attitudes Test: Psychometric features and clinical correlates.
image disturbance: How can the practice of yoga be helpful in re- Psychological Medicine, 12(4), 871–878. https://doi.org/10.1017/
covery from an eating disorder? Eating Disorders, 14, 167–170. s0033291700049163.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10640260500536334. Germer, C. (2004). What is mindfulness? Insight Journal, 22, 24–29.
Retrieved from http://www.drtheresalavoie.com/userfiles/253125/
Bradford, J. W., & Petrie, T. A. (2008). Sociocultural factors and the
file/insight_germermindfulness.pdf.
development of disordered eating: A longitudinal analysis of com-
Gitimu, P. N., Jameson, M. M., Turel, T., Pohle-Krauza, R., Mincher, J.,
peting hypotheses. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 55(2), 246–
Rowlands, Z., & Elias, J. (2016). Appearance issues, depression,
262. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.55.2.246.
and disordered eating among college females. Cogent Psychology,
Brown, T. A., Cash, T. F., & Mikulka, P. J. (1990). Attitudinal body- 3(1), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1080/23311908.2016.1196512.
image assessment: Factor analysis of the body-self relations ques- Grabe, S., Ward, L. M., & Hyde, J. S. (2008). The role of the media in
tionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, 135–144. https:// body image concerns among women: A meta-analysis of experi-
doi.org/10.1080/00223891.1990.9674053. mental and correlational studies. Psychological Bulletin, 134(3),
Cash, T. F. (2000). The MBSRQ users’ manual (3rd ed.). Norfolk, VA: 460–476. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.460.
Old Dominion University. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-287- Hafner-Holter, S., Kopp, M., & Guenther, V. (2008). Klinik, diagnostik,
104-6_3. therapie und rehabilitation: Organ der gesellschaft osterreichischer
Cash, T. F., & Hrabosky, J. I. (2003). The effects of psychoeducation and nervenarzte und psychiater. Neuropsychiatrie [Effects of fitness
self-monitoring in a cognitive-behavioral program for body-image training and yoga on well-being stress, social competence and body
improvement. Eating Disorders, 11(4), 255–270. https://doi.org/10. image], 23(4), 244–248. https://doi.org/10.5414/nepband23244.
1080/10640260390218657. Halliwell, E. (2015). Future directions for positive body image research. Body
Chrisler, J. C., & Johnston-Robledo, I. (2017). Woman's embodied self: Image, 14, 177–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.03.003.
Feminist perspectives on identity and image. Washington, DC: Harris, P. A., Taylor, R., Thielke, R., Payne, J., Gonzalez, N., & Conde, J.
American Psychological Association. G. (2009). Research Electronic Data Capture (REDCap)—a
Corson, P. W., & Anderson, A. E. (2002). Body image issues among boys metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing
and men. In T. F. Cash & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body image: A translational research informatics support. Journal of Biomedical
Sex Roles

Informatics, 42(2), 377–381. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbi.2008.08. meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 86, 152–168. https://doi.


010. org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.08.
Impett, E. A., Daubenmier, J. J., & Hirschman, A. L. (2006). Minding the Pilkington, K., Kirkwood, G., Rampes, H., & Richardson, J. (2005). Yoga
body: Yoga, embodiment, and well-being. Sexuality Research and for depression: The research evidence. Journal of Affective
Social Policy, 3(4), 39–48. https://doi.org/10.1525/srsp.2006.3.4.39. Disorders, 89(1), 13–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2005.08.013.
Jarry, J. L., & Ip, K. (2005). The effectiveness of stand-alone cognitive- Puccio, F., Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M., Ong, D., & Krug, I. (2016). A system-
behavioural therapy for body image: A meta-analysis. Body Image, atic review and meta- analysis on the longitudinal relationship be-
2(4), 317–331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.10.001. tween eating pathology and depression. International Journal of
Johnson, S. M., Edwards, K. M., & Gidycz, C. A. (2015). Interpersonal Eating Disorders, 49(5), 439–454. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22506.
weight-related pressure and disordered eating in college women: A Runfola, C. D., Von Holle, A., Trace, S. E., Brownley, K. A., Hofmeier, S.
test of an expanded tripartite influence model. Sex Roles, 72(1–2), M., Gagne, D. A., … Bulik, C. M. (2013). Body dissatisfaction in
15–24. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-014-0442-0. women across the lifespan: Results of the UNC-SELF and gender
Kilpela, L. S., Hill, K., Kelly, M. C., Elmquist, J., Ottoson, P., Keith, D., and body image (GABI) studies. European Eating Disorders
… Becker, C. B. (2014). Reducing eating disorder risk factors: A Review, 21(1), 52–59. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2201.
controlled investigation of a blended task-shifting/train-the-trainer Salmon, P., Lush, E., Jablonski, M., & Sephton, S. E. (2009). Yoga and
approach to dissemination and implementation. Behaviour mindfulness: Clinical aspects of an ancient mind/body practice.
Research and Therapy, 63, 70–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 16(1), 59–72. https://doi.org/
2014.09.005. 10.1016/j.cbpra.2008.07.002.
Klein, J., & Cook-Cottone, C. (2013). The effects of yoga on eating Sarwer, D. B., Cash, T. F., Magee, L., Williams, E. F., Thompson, J. K.,
disorder symptoms and correlates: A review. International Journal Roehrig, M., … Anderson, D. A. (2005). Female college students
of Yoga Therapy, 23(2), 41–50. Retrieved from http://iaytjournals. and cosmetic surgery: An investigation of experiences, attitudes, and
org/doi/abs/10.17761/ijyt.23.2.2718421234k31854?code=iayt-site. body image. Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, 115(3), 931–938.
Koslowsky, M., Scheinberg, Z., Bleich, A., Mark, M., Apter, A., Danon, Y., https://doi.org/10.1097/01.PRS.0000153204.37065.D3.
… Solomon, Z. (1992). The factor structure and criterion validity of Serwacki, M., & Cook-Cottone, C. (2012). Yoga in the schools: A sys-
the short form of the Eating Attitudes Test. Journal of Personality tematic review of the literature. International Journal of Yoga
Assessment, 58(1), 27–35. https://doi.org/10.1207/ Therapy, 22(1), 101–110. Retrieved from http://www.iaytjournals.
s15327752jpa5801_3. org/doi/abs/10.17761/ijyt.22.1.7716244t75u4l702?code=iayt-site.
Levine, M. P., & Piran, N. (2004). The role of body image in the preven- Sherman, K. J. (2012). Guidelines for developing yoga interventions for
tion of eating disorders. Body Image, 1(1), 57–70. https://doi.org/10. randomized trials. Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative
1016/S1740-1445(03)00006-8. Medicine, 2012, 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/143271.
Liechty, J. M., & Lee, M. J. (2013). Longitudinal predictors of dieting and Smith-Jackson, T., Reel, J. J., & Thackeray, R. (2014). The practical
disordered eating among young adults in the US. International application of promoting positive body image on a college campus:
Journal of Eating Disorders, 46(8), 790–800. https://doi.org/10. Insights from freshmen women. American Journal of Health
1002/eat.22174. Education, 45(2), 105–111. https://doi.org/10.1080/19325037.
McIver, S., McGartland, M., & O'Halloran, P. (2009). BOvereating is not 2013.875965.
about the food^: Women describe their experience of a yoga treat- Stewart, T. M. (2004). Light on body image treatment acceptance through
ment program for binge eating. Qualitative Health Research, 19(9), mindfulness. Behavior Modification, 28(6), 783–811. https://doi.
1234–1245. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049732309343954. org/10.1177/0145445503259862.
Mitchell, K. S., Mazzeo, S. E., Rausch, S. M., & Cooke, K. L. (2007). Stice, E., & Bearman, S. K. (2001). Body-image and eating disturbances
Innovative interventions for disordered eating: Evaluating prospectively predict increases in depressive symptoms in adoles-
dissonance-based and yoga interventions. International Journal of cent girls: A growth curve analysis. Developmental Psychology,
Eating Disorders, 40(2), 120–128. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20282. 37(5), 597–607. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.597.
Neighbors, L. A., & Sobal, J. (2007). Prevalence and magnitude of body Stice, E., & Shaw, H. E. (2002). Role of body dissatisfaction in the onset
weight and shape dissatisfaction among university students. Eating and maintenance of eating pathology: A synthesis of research find-
Behaviors, 8(4), 429–439. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2007.03.003. ings. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53(5), 985–993. https://
Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2014). Yoga and eating disorders: Is there a place doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00488-9.
for yoga in the prevention and treatment of eating disorders and Stice, E., Butryn, M. L., Rohde, P., Shaw, H., & Marti, C. N. (2013). An
disordered eating behaviours? Advances in Eating Disorders: effectiveness trial of a new enhanced dissonance eating disorder
Theory, Research and Practice, 2(2), 136–145. https://doi.org/10. prevention program among female college students. Behaviour
1080/21662630.2013.862369. Research and Therapy, 51(12), 862–871. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Neumark-Sztainer, D., Eisenberg, M. E., Wall, M., & Loth, K. A. (2011). brat.2013.10.003.
Yoga and pilates: Associations with body image and disordered- Storch, E. A., Roberti, J. W., & Roth, D. A. (2004). Factor structure,
eating behaviors in a population-based sample of young adults. concurrent validity, and internal consistency of the Beck
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 44(3), 276–280. https:// Depression Inventory—Second Edition in a sample of college stu-
doi.org/10.1002/eat.20858. dents. Depression and Anxiety, 19(3), 187–189. https://doi.org/10.
Ospina, M. B., Bond, K., Karkhaneh, M., Tjosvold, L., Vandermeer, B., 1002/da.20002.
Liang, Y., … Klassen, T. P. (2007). Meditation practices for health: van Puymbroeck, M., Schmid, A., Shinew, K. J., & Hsieh, P. C. (2011).
State of the research. Evidence Report/Technology Assessment, Influence of Hatha yoga on physical activity constraints, physical
155(1), 1–263. https://doi.org/10.1089/acm.2008.0307. fitness, and body image of breast cancer survivors: A pilot study.
Park, T., Reilly-Spong, M., & Gross, C. R. (2013). Mindfulness: A sys- International Journal of Yoga Therapy, 21(1), 49–60. https://doi.
tematic review of instruments to measure an emergent patient- org/10.17761/ijyt.21.1.n852143rv21x188u.
reported outcome (PRO). Quality of Life Research, 22(10), 2639– Walach, H., Buchheld, N., Buttenmuller, V., Kleinknecht, N., & Schmidt,
2659. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-013-0395-8. S. (2006). Measuring mindfulness – The Freiburg Mindfulness
Pascoe, M. C., Thompson, D. R., & Ski, C. F. (2017). Yoga, mindfulness- Inventory (FMI). Personality and Individual Differences, 40,
based stress reduction and stress-related physiological measures: A 1543–1555. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.025.

You might also like