Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 3

Personality and Individual Differences 82 (2015) 58–60

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Short Communication

Relationships between general self-efficacy, planning for the future,


and life satisfaction
Nicole Azizli a, Breanna E. Atkinson b, Holly M. Baughman b, Erica A. Giammarco b,⇑
a
Department of Psychology, Adler Graduate Professional School, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
b
Department of Psychology, The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: In this study we investigated relationships between general self-efficacy, the propensity to plan for the
Received 12 February 2015 future, the consideration of future consequences, and overall life satisfaction. The sample consisted of
Received in revised form 2 March 2015 242 university students, with ages ranging from 16 to 31 (M = 18.5, SD = 1.7). Participants completed
Accepted 4 March 2015
the New General Self-Efficacy Scale (NGSES), the Continuous Planning Scale (CPS), the Consideration of
Future Consequences Scale (CFCS), and the Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS). Analyses demonstrated
that all variables were significantly intercorrelated, with general self-efficacy being most strongly related
Keywords:
to the SWLS, followed by the CFCS and the CPS. Implications are discussed.
General self-efficacy
Planning for the future
Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Future consequences
Life satisfaction

1. Introduction Four primary factors influence self-efficacy (Bandura, 1977).


The most significant factor is previous experiences with goal suc-
A major function of intelligent thought is to enable people to cess: specifically, self-efficacy is in part established as a result of
predict the probability of future events, and subsequently to exer- whether past events have been met with failure or success.
cise control. Perceived self-efficacy refers to individuals’ beliefs Vicarious experience is the second factor, whereby an individual
concerning their ability to meet desired outcomes in life (i.e., to models the behavior of another who is successful at accomplishing
affect events which influence their lives or to achieve goals; their goals. Vicarious experience not only contributes to self-
Bandura, 2010). According to Bandura’s (2001) Social Cognitive efficacy, but also allows for improvement of necessary skills.
Theory, humans are able to exercise self-motivation and control Third, individuals who are more easily socially persuaded are more
in order to monitor their behavior. Self-efficacy is also considered likely to exert greater effort to achieve goals. Lastly, physiological
to influence how individuals reason, experience emotions, and factors also contribute to individual differences in self-efficacy:
incentivize themselves (Bandura, 1977). individuals with high self-efficacy may perceive physiological
Investigations have identified numerous personality traits arousal as enhancing performance rather than hindering it.
which correlate with self-efficacy; including self-esteem, opti-
mism, well-being, as well as academic and career performance
(e.g., Luszczynska, Gutierrez-Dona, & Schwarzer, 2005). Given that 1.1. Self-efficacy and planning for the future
self-efficacy is associated with goal-oriented behavior, individuals
with a strong sense of self-efficacy are more likely to approach Since performance on a future task is in part predicted via social
goals and to exert more effort to attain them (Bandura, 1994). comparison or personal experiences, it seems likely that general
Further, these individuals are more likely to perceive failure as self-efficacy influences the extent to which individuals engage in
internally caused and to be motivated to review their capabilities future planning. Planning is a life management strategy which
in order to improve their perceived weaknesses following failure allows individuals to structure and to manifest control in their
(Bandura, 2001). lives (Prenda & Lachman, 2001). Those who adopt a future-
oriented approach are more likely to be prepared for future tasks
⇑ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, Social Science Centre, and to exert more control while completing a task (Gollwitzer,
University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6A 4C2, Canada. Tel.: +1 519 661 1996; Friedman & Lackey, 1991). These goal-oriented skills allow
3682. individuals to focus on new opportunities to achieve goals
E-mail address: egiammar@uwo.ca (E.A. Giammarco). (Prenda & Lachman, 2001). Studies have identified domain-specific

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.03.006
0191-8869/Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
N. Azizli et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 82 (2015) 58–60 59

self-efficacy as having a moderating effect on types of planning and research pool. The participants ranged in age from 16 to 31 years
behavior (e.g., Luszczynska, Schwarzer, Lippke, & Mazurkiewicz, old (M = 18.5, SD = 1.7). Participants completed the study to satisfy
2011). the requirements of their course.

1.2. Self-efficacy and consideration of future consequences 2.2. Measures

Individual differences exist in the extent to which people adopt 2.2.1. New General Self-Efficacy Scale (NGSES)
an immediate versus a distant approach when conceptualizing the Self-efficacy was measured using the NGSES (Chen, Gully, &
future consequences of their behavior. Individuals who focus on Eden, 2001). The NGSES comprises eight items that require
long term consequences are more likely to sacrifice immediate individuals to rate the extent to which they agree with statements
gains in exchange for long term benefits. Alternatively, individuals on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree).
who are less concerned with future consequences tend to adopt a Example items from this measure are, ‘‘I can solve most problems
present-oriented approach to life and to behave in ways that maxi- if I invest the necessary effort,’’ and, ‘‘I can remain calm when fac-
mize immediate self-gratification (Strathman, Gleicher, Boninger, ing difficulties because I can rely on my coping abilities.’’ Chen
& Edwards, 1994). et al. (2001) reported Cronbach’s reliability coefficients for the
In a cross-cultural comparison of self-efficacy and other vari- NGSES ranging from .86 to .90.
ables, Luszczynska, Gutierrez-Dona, and Schwarzer (2005) found
that individuals who scored higher on the General Self-Efficacy 2.2.2. Continuous Planning Scale (CPS)
Scale were more future-oriented than those who scored lower. The extent to which individuals plan for the future was mea-
However, it should be noted that this study used a condensed ver- sured using the CPS (Prenda & Lachman, 2001). The CPS consists
sion of the scale and a replication study is needed to confirm its of five items that require individuals to rate the extent to which
findings. statements are representative of themselves, on a 4-point Likert
scale (4 = a lot, 1 = not at all). An example item from this scale is,
1.3. Self-efficacy and life satisfaction ‘‘I find it helpful to set goals for the near future.’’ Cronbach’s alphas
for this scale ranging from .63 to .67 have been reported (Prenda &
Life satisfaction is defined as a subjective sense of well-being Lachman, 2001), reflecting its brevity.
that alludes to one’s overall happiness with life (Lent et al.,
2005). Life satisfaction is evaluated based on a cognitive judgmen- 2.2.3. Consideration of Future Consequences Scale (CFCS)
tal process in which individuals compare their current state with The CFCS is a self-report questionnaire in which individuals’
their desired level of satisfaction (Emmons & Diener, 1985). Life propensity to consider the future implications of their behaviors
satisfaction and general well-being are influenced by various per- is assessed through 12 items (Strathman et al., 1994). Individuals
sonality traits, cognitive processes, behavioral characteristics, as are asked to indicate whether or not statements are characteristic
well as positive and negative affect (See Emmons & Diener, 1985, of them on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = extremely uncharacteristic,
or Bandura, 2001 for review). 5 = extremely characteristic). An example statement is, ‘‘I consider
Results from Luszczynska et al.’s (2005) cross-cultural study how things might be in the future, and try to influence those things
demonstrated a positive relationship between general self-efficacy with my day to day behavior.’’ This scale has demonstrated good
and academic, vocational, and social satisfaction. A model of internal consistency, with alphas ranging from .80 to .86
subjective well-being constructed by Lent et al. (2005) showed that (Strathman et al., 1994).
goal self-efficacy was correlated with life satisfaction. Specifically,
goal self-efficacy was directly related to domain satisfaction, and 2.2.4. Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS)
indirectly related to overall life satisfaction. Thus, individuals Life satisfaction was measured using the SWLS (Diener,
experience positive emotions and satisfaction with success in a Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985), which consists of 12 items.
specific domain, and this in turn contributes to their sense of Individuals are asked to indicate their agreement with statements
overall life satisfaction. on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = strongly agree, 7 = strongly disagree).
Example statements are ‘‘In most ways my life is close to my ideal,’’
1.4. Present study and ‘‘If I could live my life over, I would change almost nothing.’’
Cronbach’s alpha of .87 has been reported for this measure
The purpose of the present study is to further investigate (Diener et al., 1985).
relationships that these planning and satisfaction variables have
with general self-efficacy, as compared to domain-specific efficacy. 2.3. Procedure
Specifically, we examine the propensity to engage in future plan-
ning, to consider future consequences, and overall life satisfaction Participants were first provided with a letter of information and
in relation to general self-efficacy. It is predicted that: (1) those were requested to give informed consent. Participants were then
higher in general self-efficacy will be more motivated to plan for directed to the website Survey-Monkey, where they completed
the future, and will be more likely to consider the long term con- the study electronically. On this website, participants were asked
sequences of their actions; (2) those higher in general self-efficacy to complete a demographics form, as well as the CFCS, CPS,
will experience greater life satisfaction; and (3) those higher in SWLS, and NGSES. Upon completion of the study, participants read
planning for the future will experience greater life satisfaction. a debriefing form and were thanked for their participation.

2. Method 3. Results

2.1. Participants Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations for scores on the


NGSES, CPS, CFCS, and SWLS are presented in Table 1. As reported
Participants were 242 undergraduate students (64 males, 171 along the main diagonal, Cronbach’s reliability coefficients indicate
females, 7 unspecified) who were recruited from a first year moderate to acceptable internal consistency for each of the four
60 N. Azizli et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 82 (2015) 58–60

Table 1 & Lackey, 1991). This can further be explained by the positive
Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations for NGSES, CPS, CFCS and SWLS. correlation between continuous planning and life satisfaction, indi-
Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 cating that those who are more satisfied with their lives benefit
1. General self-efficacy 3.72 0.87 .84 .41 ***
.64 ***
.67*** from their future-oriented approach to life and therefore continue
2. Continuous planning 1.97 0.65 .63 .15* .22** to engage in future planning.
3. Consideration of future 3.37 0.84 .77 .49***
consequences 4.1. Limitations and future research
4. Satisfaction with life 4.64 1.47 .84

Note: Cronbach’s alpha for each scale reported along the main diagonal. A limitation of the present study was the less than desirable
*
Indicates significance at p < .05. reliability of the CPS. This likely contributed to the relatively low
**
p < .01.
*** correlation between continuous planning and self-efficacy. A sec-
p < .001.
ond limitation was that the sample was undergraduate students
who showed little variability in their age. A study conducted by
Cameron, Desai, Bahador, and Dremel (1977) found a negative
measures, with alphas ranging from .63 to .84. As indicated in relationship between age and the extent to which individuals
Table 1, all correlations between the variables were statistically adopted a future-orientated view towards life.
significant. In particular, the CPS, CFCS and SWLS were all posi- How individuals view their competencies and capabilities is
tively correlated with the NGSES at the p < .001 level. Notably, gen- indicative of their life satisfaction and their consideration of future
eral self-efficacy was most highly correlated with the SWLS consequences. Identifying predictors of general self-efficacy and
(r = .67), followed by the CFCS (r = .64), and the CPS (r = .41). offering services and programs that enhance self-efficacy beliefs
should be of practical value. Future research should focus on
4. Discussion identifying predictors of general and domain-specific self-efficacy.

The present study examined relationships between general References


self-efficacy, planning for the future, consideration of future
consequences, and overall life satisfaction. Continuous planning, Bandura, A. (1994). Self-efficacy. In V. S. Ramachaudran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human
behavior (4, pp. 71–81). New York: Academic Press.
consideration of future consequences, and life satisfaction are all Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change.
significantly positively correlated with general self-efficacy, as well Psychological Review, 84(2), 191–215.
as with each other. Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Annual Review of
Psychology, 52(1), 1–26.
Life satisfaction correlated the most strongly with general
Bandura, A. (2010). Self-efficacy. Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology., 1–3.
self-efficacy. Although the present study’s focus is on general Cameron, P., Desai, K. G., Bahador, D., & Dremel, G. (1977). Temporality across the
self-efficacy and overall life satisfaction, a positive relationship is life-span. The International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 8(3),
consistent with previous studies that examined domain-specific 229–259.
Chen, G., Gully, S. M., & Eden, D. (2001). Validation of a new general self-efficacy
efficacy and life satisfaction (Lent et al., 2005; Luszczynska et al., scale. Organizational Research Methods, 4(1), 62–83.
2005). This is not surprising given that future planning, considera- Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life
tion of future consequences, and facets of self-efficacy are likely to scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(1), 71–75.
Emmons, R. A., & Diener, E. (1985). Personality correlates of subjective well-being.
be predictors of life satisfaction to varying degrees. Future plan- Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11(1), 89–97.
ning produces feelings of control and preparedness, which may Friedman, M. I., & Lackey, G. H. (1991). The psychology of human control: A general
influence life satisfaction. This is supported by evidence that links theory of purposeful behavior. New York, NY, England: Praeger Publishers.
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1996). In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The volitional benefits
control to well-being (Prenda & Lachman, 2001). The present study of planning. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.
also supports the bidirectional relationship evidenced between Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal
affect and well-being (Bandura, 2001). Life satisfaction was posi- achievement: A meta- analysis of effects and processes. Advances in
Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 69–119.
tively correlated with both continuous planning and consideration
Lent, R. W., Singley, D., Sheu, H., Gainor, K. A., Brenner, B. R., Treistman, D., et al.
of future consequences. This is reflected in past research in which (2005). Social cognitive predictors of domain and life satisfaction: Exploring the
planning has been associated with goal achievement (Gollwitzer & theoretical precursors of subjective well-being. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
52(3), 429–442.
Sheeran, 2006). Further, goal achievement is associated with posi-
Luszczynska, A., Gutierrez-Dona, B., & Schwarzer, R. (2005). General self-efficacy in
tive emotions and feelings of satisfaction (Prenda & Lachman, various domains of human functioning: Evidence from five countries.
2001). In a study conducted by Prenda and Lachman (2001), International Journal of Psychology, 40(2), 80–89.
future-oriented planning was also found to be related to overall life Luszczynska, A., Schwarzer, R., Lippke, S., & Mazurkiewicz, M. (2011). Self-efficacy
as a moderator of the planning-behaviour relationship in interventions
satisfaction. It is notable that this relationship was mediated by the designed to promote physical activity. Psychology and Health, 26(2), 151–166.
sense of control that individuals felt over their lives. Prenda, K. M., & Lachman, M. E. (2001). Planning for the future: A life management
Finally, a positive relationship between general self-efficacy and strategy for increasing control and life satisfaction in adulthood. Psychology and
Aging, 16(2), 206–216.
continuous planning for the future was found. Previous research Strathman, A., Gleicher, F., Boninger, D. S., & Edwards, C. S. (1994). The consideration
indicates that individuals who plan for the future also possess a of future consequences: Weighing immediate and distant outcomes of
greater sense of general self-efficacy (Gollwitzer, 1996; Friedman behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(4), 742–752.

You might also like