Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Knowledge and Attitudes About Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity


Disorder and Specific Learning Disorder in an Urban
Indian Population
Sayantani Mukherjee, MBBS, MD,* Henal R. Shah, MBBS, MD,*
Seethalakshmi Ramanathan, MD,†‡ and Mantosh Dewan, MD†
ADHD and SLD among children who received a diagnosis of these dis-
Abstract: Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and specific learn- orders. Furthermore, as previous studies have identified the role of ac-
ing disorders (SLDs) are an important cause of scholastic backwardness among ademic performance in the differences in knowledge, we felt that the
children and often go unrecognized. Few studies have examined knowledge Indian society would be a unique society to study, primarily because
and attitudes toward ADHD and SLD among school-aged children. To address of the immense importance the society places on academic perfor-
this deficit, 120 school-aged children, attending a child guidance clinic in Mumbai, mance. Because ADHD and SLD are often considered as part of a spec-
were interviewed using a questionnaire that examined children’s knowledge and trum (Mayes et al., 2000), we included a third frequently occurring
attitudes about ADHD and SLD. The results were compared both qualitatively independent disorder, common cold, as a comparator. We chose com-
and quantitatively with a frequently occurring medical illness, common cold. Ap- mon cold as the comparator primarily because it is a frequently occur-
proximately 80% to 100% of children were aware of their illness; however, a ring physical ailment that would occur in the same frequency in the
large variation was noted in the proportion of children (15%–80%) who could 2 groups. Common cold has also been used as a comparator to ADHD
describe their symptoms, provide accurate attributions for their illness, and in an earlier study (McMenamy and Perrin, 2008). Finally, we aimed to
identify treatment modalities. Children with ADHD reported greater control assess self-esteem and its relation to their understanding of the disorder.
over their illness. The study identified a significant lack of knowledge about We hypothesized that knowledge and understanding of ADHD and
ADHD and SLD among school-aged children in India and discusses implica- SLD would be limited, and that children with more limited knowledge
tions of this finding. about their illness would have lower self-esteem.
Key Words: ADHD, children, knowledge, SLD, stigma
(J Nerv Ment Dis 2016;204: 458–463) METHODS
A cross-sectional study with qualitative and quantitative compo-

A ttention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Greenhill, 2009)


and specific learning disorders (SLDs) (Tannock, 2009) are being
increasingly reported in urban India (due to rising community aware-
nents was carried out. The qualitative part was based on Grounded The-
ory (Martin and Turner, 1986), exploration of the target population’s
awareness and understanding of its own disorder without a prior hy-
ness) and diagnosed, with prevalence rates of approximately 11.33% pothesis. The quantitative portion of the study explored the relationship
(Venkata and Panicker, 2013) and 15.17% (Mogasale et al., 2012), re- between knowledge about the illness and self-esteem. Approval was ob-
spectively. The 2 disorders present most often as scholastic backward- tained from our institutional review board.
ness in children (Haneesh et al., 2013; Karande and Kulkarni, 2005;
Loe and Feldman, 2007) and frequently occur as comorbid conditions. Subjects
Children with these disorders often recognize that they have a problem; One hundred twenty consecutive children with a diagnosis of
however, they have varying understanding of its causes, effects, and ADHD (n = 60) or SLD (n = 60) between the ages of 8 and 14 years,
treatment (Fox et al., 2008). Studies have identified that the knowl- attending a Child Guidance Clinic in a tertiary health center in urban
edge of ADHD is more limited as compared with SLD among chil- India, were recruited for the study. Only children with the combined
dren (Brook and Boaz, 2005). Contributing to the lack of knowledge type of ADHD were recruited. Care was taken to include participants
is parental stigma arising from their child’s, which in turn can get in- who had only ADHD or only SLD. These children had received treat-
ternalized by the child (Mukolo et al., 2010). Together, self-stigma ment for at least 3 months. Children with IQ of less than 84 on Wechsler
and parental pressure on academics, driven by cultural elements, Intelligence Scale for Children were not included in the study. Appro-
can lead to low self-esteem and eventually other comorbid condi- priate assent to participate was obtained from the children along with
tions (Shmulsky and Gobbo, 2007), such as mood, anxiety, conduct, parental consent. In the Child Guidance Clinic, diagnoses of the spe-
learning, and substance use disorders. cific disorders were made by a multispecialty team using Diagnostic
Understanding the child’s knowledge and attitudes about the dis- and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revi-
order can act as important avenues for secondary intervention to prevent sion criteria and testing, including educational achievement tests such
further disability and improve recovery. Unfortunately, there is only a as Woodcock-Johnson Psychoeducational Battery. All interviews were
modest amount of literature in this area. This deficit inspired us to con- carried out by 1 interviewer (S.M.) in a single sitting.
duct an exploratory study to evaluate knowledge and attitudes toward A semistructured interview was used to obtain demographic data
and details about family history, including a history of ADHD and SLD.
An open-ended questionnaire was developed, which inquired about the
*Department of Psychiatry, Topiwala National Medical College, Mumbai, India;
†Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, SUNY Upstate Medical Uni-
children’s awareness and understanding of their disorder (SLD or
versity; and ‡Hutchings Psychiatric Center, Syracuse, NY. ADHD) and a comparison illness, common cold. The questionnaire
Send reprint requests to Seethalakshmi Ramanathan, MD, Department of Psychiatry was designed to elicit responses regarding awareness about the illness
and Behavioral Sciences, SUNY Upstate Medical University, 650 Madison St, and included questions pertaining to nosology, etiology, and prognosis
Syracuse, NY 13210. E-mail: seetha.ramanathan@omh.ny.gov.
Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.
(included as supplemental material). Specifically, the themes included
ISSN: 0022-3018/16/20406–0458 awareness about the disorder, recognition about the disorder’s medical
DOI: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000524 nature, attributions regarding the etiology of the disorder, perception

458 www.jonmd.com The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 204, Number 6, June 2016

Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 204, Number 6, June 2016 Mental Health Literacy in Children

regarding volitional control over the symptoms, knowledge about treat-


ment modalities, perception about prognosis, and, finally, subjective TABLE 1. Percent of Subjects Responding to the Various Themes
Presented as Proportions
distress and coping styles. The themes were identified in focus groups
conducted with 3 expert psychiatrists and facilitated by one of the coau-
Children Children
thors (S.M.); questions developed based on these themes were further
With ADHD With SLD
reviewed by the 3 experts. Finally, to ensure integrity of the question-
naire, it was translated and back-translated from English into 2 local Common Common
languages and back (by 2 nonmedical professionals who were profi- Themes ADHD Cold SLD Cold
cient in all 3 languages) then compared and modified until uniformity
Awareness of symptoms 80 100 100 100
was confirmed. The responses to the questions were documented verba-
Recognition as a disorder 15 100 25 100
tim to ensure accuracy of data.
A pilot study was carried out with 10 children to determine ap- Accurate attributions 35 1.67 80 23.33
about etiology
propriateness of the questionnaire. The questions were then modified
based on the pilot study. The final questionnaire was used in the study. Perception of volitional 40 63.33 15 61.67
control over symptoms
A visual analog scale (VAS) ranging from 1 to 5 (1 being the best and 5
being the worst) was used to assess distress from the disorder. Hoge and Coping with disorder
McCarthy’s (1983) adaptation of the Rosenberg’s Self-esteem Scale Positive 51.67 100 40 100
was used to assess self-esteem. The scale has a concurrent validity of Negative 1.67 46.67
0.5 to 0.8 and test-retest reliability of 0.7 to 0.9 and has been used for Knowledge about treatment
children, adolescents, and adults (Hoge and McCarthy, 1983). Scores Aware about treatment 53.33 100 40 100
less than 15 are suggestive of low self-esteem. Finally, a VAS was cre- Understand mechanisms 35 73.33 13.33 81.67
ated, which rated symptoms on a scale of 1 to 5, with 1 being the best
and 5 being the worst (illustrated in Appendix 1).
A. Awareness about their disorder (ADHD or SLD)
Data Analysis Awareness of symptoms. Eighty percent of children with ADHD
were aware of their full symptoms (attention deficit and hyperac-
For the purpose of qualitative analysis, responses were classified tivity), 5% were aware only of the attention-deficit symptoms,
into specific themes: awareness of symptoms (identification of their and 15% were not aware of their symptoms; 100% of the SLD
disorder in a dichotomous manner and describing their symptoms), children were aware of their symptoms, significantly more than
recognition as disorder (a narrative of the child’s understanding of the ADHD group ( p < 0.05). Similar to SLD, all the children
their diagnosis), attribution of disorder (discussed in detail in Results), (100%) were aware of the symptoms of common cold.
perception of volitional control over symptoms (a dichotomous re- Description of symptoms. Forty-five percent of children with
sponse), coping with the disorder (distress due to their illness and ADHD described their symptoms to be of behavioral nature de-
various coping strategies), and treatment description (based on the scribing hyperactivity-impulsivity (e.g., “I make too much mis-
available methods of treatment in India). Specifically, the children were chief and irritate everyone”), whereas only 8.33% of the
encouraged to elaborate in their views about their illness in these vari- children described the inattention component appropriately—in
ous categories, including in the questions that would primarily elicit a relation to academic difficulties (“My mind cannot stay on one
dichotomous response to ensure accuracy of the response. These re- thing for long; it wanders, so I can’t finish my homework.”).
sponses were then categorized into appropriate categories as deemed All the children with SLD described their symptoms in terms
by the questions (as an example, correct and incorrect attributions) by of difficulties in academics (e.g., “When I read, the words don’t
2 of the authors, S.M. and H.R.S. Responses were then converted into make sense”). This difference between the 2 groups was statisti-
frequency tables for nonparametric analysis between the 3 groups: cally significant ( p < 0.05). In contrast, all children described
ADHD, SLD, and common cold. For assessing the relationship of the symptoms of common cold correctly, responses being
self-esteem, scores on the Self-esteem Scale were dichotomized as nor- combinations of “runny nose,” “throat pain,” “headache,” “fever,”
mal (≥15) a nd low (<15) and then used in Fisher exact test. This test “coughing,” and “sneezing.”
was chosen because it gives accurate results even with widely varying Existence of a formal disorder name. Of the children with
cell values (0–99 in case of this test), which suited our study well as ADHD and SLD, 63.33% and 78.33%, respectively, said that their
the individual values varied greatly, but none were less than 0 or greater problem had a specific formal name. The SLD group was signif-
than 99. For parametric data, independent-samples t test was done. All icantly more knowledgeable about whether there was specific
analyses were done using SPSS (version 16) (SPSS Inc, 2007). name for their disorder than the ADHD group ( p < 0.05). The
“correct” ADHD group (11.67% of the total ADHD group) gave
fully or partially accurate terminology, that is, “ADHD,” “hyperac-
tivity,” and “attention deficit.” The “incorrect” ADHD group
RESULTS (51.67% of the total ADHD group) gave lay responses, for exam-
One hundred twenty children were studied. The ADHD group ple, “masti,” “vedepana” (mischief and madness, respectively, in
consisted of 54 boys and 6 girls (45% and 5% of the total population, local language); 76.67% of all the SLD children gave “correct” re-
respectively). The SLD group consisted of 45 boys and 15 girls sponses: “LD,” “learning disability,” and “dyslexia/dysgraphia/
(37.5% and 12.5% of the total population, respectively).The average dyscalculia/combination of all of them.” The SLD group gave sig-
age of children in the SLD group (11.81 ± 2.05 years) was significantly nificantly more correct responses compared with the ADHD
higher (p < 0.05) as compared with children in the ADHD group group ( p < 0.05).
(10.63 ± 1.89 years); 11.67% of ADHD children and 31.67% of the Source of awareness/knowledge. Most common sources of
SLD children had family members with similar complaints. awareness were both parents and teachers (86.67% and 90% of
The responses to the various themes in the questionnaire are ADHD and SLD groups, respectively). Other sources included
summarized in Table 1 and detailed in the following list: friends and media. Notably, doctors were not a source of knowl-
1. Understanding of ADHD/SLD (as compared with common cold): edge for any of the children. According to all the children,

© 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. www.jonmd.com 459

Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


Mukherjee et al. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 204, Number 6, June 2016

knowledge about common cold is a part of general knowledge (27.27%) ( p < 0.05). Perception of volitional control over symp-
and did not identify a source. toms of common cold was higher and similar between the 2
B. Recognition as a medical disorder groups (ADHD, 63.33%; SLD, 61.67%; p > 0.05).
Only 15% of ADHD and 25% of the SLD children recognized E. Knowledge about treatment
their difficulties as a medical disorder ( p > 0.01). The rest of Treatment modalities. Correct responses for ADHD included
them gave varied responses, of which the 2 most common pharmacotherapy and occupational/behavior therapy and for
were thinking the symptoms were normal or a temporary SLD remedial education with or without certification for educa-
sociobehavioral/academic difficulty. In contrast, 100% of the tional concessions. The ADHD group (53.33%) gave signifi-
sample recognized the common cold to be an illness. cantly more correct responses about treatment as compared
C. Attributions regarding etiology of the disorder with the SLD group (40%) ( p < 0.05). All the children from both
The responses were analyzed and condensed into themes and groups gave correct responses when asked about treatment of
further condensed into “correct” (C) and “incorrect” (I) attribu- common cold.
tions. Thirty-five percent of children with ADHD provided accu- Mechanism of action of treatment. The ADHD group (35%)
rate attributions. Children with SLD (80%), on the other hand, had significantly more ( p < 0.05) accurate knowledge of the
provided significantly more correct attributions ( p < 0.05). Chil- mechanism of the prescribed treatment (example of correct re-
dren with SLD (23.3%) were also more likely to provide cor- sponse: “The medicines calm my brain down and help me fo-
rect attributions for common cold as compared with children cus”) as compared with the SLD group (13.33%) (example of
with ADHD (1.67%) ( p < 0.05). correct response: “it’s specific teaching for my brain because I
can’t learn like everybody else”); 73.33% of the ADHD group
Among children with ADHD, the themes included the following: and 81.67% of the SLD group identified the mechanisms of
1. Hereditary/genetic: for example, “Both my brother and I have it, so it treating common cold accurately ( p < 0.05).
runs in the family.” (C) F. Perceptions regarding prognosis
2. Functional neurological (i.e., attribution of symptoms to neurologi- Significantly more children with SLD (36.67%) felt despondent
cal malfunction): “My brain runs too fast.” (C) about their prognosis as compared with children with ADHD
3. Organic neurological (i.e., attribution of symptoms to structural neu- (3.33%) (p > 0.05). Fifteen percent of ADHD and 70% of SLD
rological deficits): “My brain has not developed properly.” (C) children said it would be a big problem if their symptoms do
4. Congenital: “I was born with it.” (C) not resolve soon (p < 0.05). On the contrary, 70% ADHD and
5. Effect of certain stimulus: “Watching TV makes me hyperactive.” (I) 98.33% SLD children felt it would a big problem if common cold
6. Postillness sequelae: “I have become more mischievous after I got ty- did not resolve fast showing that the SLD group’s perception of
phoid.” (I) common cold as a big problem was also significantly more than
7. Voluntary behavior: “I like doing a lot of masti (mischief ); that’s the ADHD group (Fisher p = 0.016).
why I do it.” (I) G. Subjective distress and coping
8. Normal: “It’s normal for kids to be naughty, so there’s no problem Perception of distress due to symptoms. On the VAS, children
with me.” (I) with ADHD reported less distress as compared with children
with SLD. Furthermore, children with ADHD reported signifi-
Among children with SLD, the themes were as follows: cantly more distress with common cold (4.18 ± 1.15) as com-
1. Hereditary/genetic: for example, “It is a genetic disorder.” (C) pared with their own disorder (2.08 ± 1.14). Children with
2. Functional neurological: “My brain is slow and doesn’t understand SLD (4.36 ± 0.87), on the other hand, reported significantly
properly.” (C) more distress with their own disorder as compared with common
3. Organic neurological: “The calculation area in my brain has not de- cold (3.74 ± 0.70) (Table 2).
veloped properly.” (C) Perceived impairment in aspects of daily life due to disorder. Of
4. Congenital: “I was always this way.” (C) the 6 areas (academics, extracurricular activity, play, and rela-
5. Effect of certain stimulus: “I once played too much of video games tionship with family, friend, and teachers), both ADHD children
and exhausted my mind; that is why it is slow.” (I) (60%) and SLD children (100%) identified academics as the
6. Postillness sequelae: “I developed reading problem because I got most affected area (p < 0.05) followed by relationship with
asthma attack.” (I) teachers, family members, and friends and play and extracurric-
7. Voluntary behavior: “I hate homework—it takes such a long time to ular activities, which were not statistically significant. A signifi-
finish; that’s why I don’t do it.” (I) cantly greater proportion (p < 0.05) of children with SLD
8. Somatic: “My hand writes slowly.” (I) (83.33%) as compared with children with ADHD (48.33%) re-
ported that their disorder affected their lives every day.
Coping strategies to deal with the symptoms. The following
For common cold, themes were as follows:
themes emerged and were classified into “positive” (P) and
1. Environmental: for example, “I can get a cold by drinking cold
“negative” (N) coping strategies:
Pepsi.” (I)
• (P) Constructive self-improvement: for example, ADHD: “I focus
2. Infectious: “Bacteria and viruses can both cause common cold.” (C)
all my energy into 1 hour of sports everyday so that my mind is
3. Both environmental and infectious: “My throat grows bad bacteria in
calm enough to do homework”; SLD: “I put in more effort in
cold weather.” (C)
4. Congenital: “I always had a cold.” (I)
TABLE 2. Comparison of VAS Rating of Perceived Distress Due to
D. Perception of volitional control over disorder Their Own Disorder and Common Cold by Children With ADHD/SLD
Forty percent of the ADHD group and 15% of the SLD group
reported they could control their symptoms whenever they Group ADHD/SLD Common Cold
wanted ( p < 0.05). Furthermore, the ADHD group felt signifi- ADHD distress ratings 2.08 ± 1.14 4.18 ± 1.15 −9.710 (<0.05)
cantly more in control of their own behavior when they did not SLD distress ratings 4.36 ± 0.87 3.74 ± 0.70 4.231 (<0.05)
label it as a disorder (90%) as compared with when they did

460 www.jonmd.com © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.

Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 204, Number 6, June 2016 Mental Health Literacy in Children

school”; common cold: “I put on warm clothes and stay away from children who received a diagnosis of these disorders. Awareness about
cold drinks.” the disorder was higher in children with SLD as compared with ADHD.
• (P) Seeking external help: “I visited the school counselor to ask how This increased awareness may have been driven by the difference in age
to be better at studies.” group; SLD children were older than children with ADHD. However,
• No felt need for active coping: (children emotionally at peace with among children with SLD, age did not seem to affect the responses to
their disorders and feel no need to expend emotional energy over various themes, including awareness. A second explanation could be
them), for example, “Nothing extra needs to be done; I’m happy with the importance of academic performance in the Indian community.
the way my life is right now.” We can speculate that children with SLD were likely being labeled
• (N) Externalizing behavior: “I get angry, scream, and hit people’ as “disabled,” whereas children with ADHD were normalized as
• (N) Avoidance of problem: “I try not to think about it as it makes “mischievous.” This pressure of academic success may also explain
me upset.” the observation that both groups reported more distress with unre-
solved cold as compared with ADHD and SLD. We can speculate
The first 2 were included in “positive” and last 2 in “negative” that this could be because cold keeps the child out of school as
coping strategies, whereas the third was taken as a standalone category. against having ADHD or SLD, which keeps the child in school.
Children with SLD used significantly more negative coping Our findings do not appear to be unique to the Indian population
strategies than did the ADHD children when it came to their disorder and are similar to studies from Western societies (Feurer and Andrews,
( p < 0.05); 28.33% of ADHD children and 18.33% of SLD children felt 2009; Palladino et al., 2000) that have reported that children with SLD
there was no need for active coping. In contrast, for common cold, most are more mindful about their symptoms. Recognition of symptoms as
children of both groups reported positive coping strategies ( p > 0.05). a disorder was similarly low in both groups. According to the partici-
Furthermore, children who labeled their problems as due to SLD used pants, ADHD and SLD do not feel like conventional medical illnesses
significantly more (60%) negative coping mechanisms compared with such as common cold; this belief likely led to the rejection of the theme
those who did not (37.77%) ( p < 0.05). that ADHD and SLD are medical illnesses. Furthermore, the concept of
an “illness” does not crystallize in a child’s mind until the age of 12 to
13 years, so immaturity of cognitive processes may have led to this ob-
2. Relation of understanding of ADHD/SLD with servation (Perrin et al., 2008). Surprisingly, children in our study re-
A. Family members having same disorder ported a high number of biological explanations. This could be the
Significantly more awareness of disorder, less sense of perceived result of an increased focus on biological causes of psychiatric disor-
control, and less felt need for active coping were seen in children ders spilling over into the community (Hinshaw, 2005). It may also
with ADHD who had a family member having similar com- be a mechanism for self-preservation. An organic cause leads to reduc-
plaints than those who did not ( p < 0.05). In comparison, ADHD tion of stigma as the negative social perception attached to being behav-
children who did not have a family member suffering from iorally and academically poor is transferred from the self to an organic
the same disorder used significantly more positive coping. There cause (Hinshaw and Stier, 2008).
were no significant findings regarding these issues when it Compared with children with SLD, children with ADHD report
came to the SLD group ( p > 0.05). more perceived control over their illness and have more knowledge
B. Self-esteem about treatment modalities. Children with SLD showed distress about
The mean scores on Rosenberg’s Self-esteem Scale for the their illness and also demonstrated more despondence about the prog-
ADHD and SLD groups were 22.33 ± 3.86 and 15.02 ± 2.76, nosis as compared with children with ADHD. Interestingly, among chil-
respectively. Children with ADHD had significantly higher self- dren with SLD, the perceived distress was higher for common cold, as
esteem than did SLD children (independent-samples t = 11.935; well as compared with children with ADHD. One possible explanation
p < 0.01). In addition, we found that significantly more ( p < for these observations could be learned helplessness and lack of ability
0.01) ADHD children (90.90%) with normal self-esteem felt in children with SLD (Shmulsky and Gobbo, 2007; Tominey, 1996). It
in control of their disorder than the SLD group (37.78%), and could also be the result of a general negativistic attitude among children
in both groups, children with low self-esteem felt signifi- with SLD (Shmulsky and Gobbo, 2007) secondary to the development
cantly more (Fisher p < 0.01) lack of control over their dis- of depressive symptoms. This was also reflected in the lower self-
order (ADHD, 100%; SLD, 91.67%) as compared with those esteem, the perception of poor prognosis, and the use of more negative
with normal self-esteem (ADHD, 9.01%; SLD, 62.22%). coping strategies among children with SLD. In contrast, children with
C. Age ADHD appeared to use more positive coping strategies and had a
Because of the difference in ages that was noted between the 2 higher self-esteem. The importance of mental health literacy in the chil-
groups, we further examined the influence of age on the various dren’s responses can be noted in the differential responses among chil-
themes. For this purpose, the children were divided into 2 groups dren with family members with similar diagnoses; children with ADHD
based on Piaget’s stages of cognitive development (8–11 and who also have family members with ADHD demonstrated a better ad-
11–14 years). A significant difference ( p < 0.05) was noted only justment to their illness and reported as compared with children without
among children with ADHD, with older children reporting family members with the diagnosis.
more correct attributions about their disorder as compared with Mental health literacy (Jorm et al., 1997) and stigma have been
younger children. Younger children also tended to “normalize” identified as important barriers to help-seeking behaviors (Gulliver
their disorder as compared with older children; however, this et al., 2010). Our study illustrates some of the qualitative elements in-
was not statistically significant. No statistically significant differ- volved in the association between mental health literacy and stigma
ence was noted in responses to any of the other themes between and dysfunctional perceptions about mental illnesses among children.
the 2 age groups. The dysfunctional perceptions noted in the study may be driven by var-
ious cultural elements such as the relative comfort of accepting medical
DISCUSSION explanations as compared with psychiatric labels (Kermode et al.,
Our study explored knowledge and understanding about 2 com- 2009). Thus, in India, a more medical explanation of psychiatric ill-
mon childhood disorders, ADHD and SLD, among children identified nesses may help improve help-seeking behaviors. An interesting aspect
as having the disorders. As hypothesized, we found significant differ- of the study is that school performance was a significant contributor
ences in knowledge and attitudes about ADHD and SLD among to the perceptions and attitudes among the children. This in turn may

© 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. www.jonmd.com 461

Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


Mukherjee et al. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 204, Number 6, June 2016

be driven by the emphasis on academic performance in traditional REFERENCES


Indian society.
Brook U, Boaz M (2005) Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and
Considering the importance of academics, school-based inter-
learning disabilities (LD): Adolescents perspective. Patient Educ Couns. 58:
vention programs may be an ideal form of intervention in terms of im-
187–191.
proving mental health literacy and addressing stigma. School-based
mental health programs have been implemented in a number of coun- Calear AL, Christensen H (2010) Systematic review of school-based prevention and
tries including the United States (Wahl et al., 2011). A recent meta- early intervention programs for depression. J Adolesc. 33:429–438.
analysis (Calear and Christensen, 2010) noted that approximately half Feurer DP, Andrews JJ (2009) School-related stress and depression in adolescents with
the studies (14 of 27) reported significant changes in stigmatizing atti- and without learning disabilities: An exploratory study. Alberta J Educ Res. 55:92.
tudes and greater help seeking. A study of a school-based mental health Fox C, Buchanan-Barrow E, Barrett M (2008) Children’s understanding of mental ill-
program done in a culturally similar society, Pakistan, noted a signifi- ness: An exploratory study. Child Care Health Dev. 34:10–18.
cant change in stigmatizing attitudes among the students and the com-
munity (Rahman et al., 1998). Greenhill LLHL (2009) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. In Sadock BJ, Sadock
An important limitation of the study is its exploratory nature. VA, Ruiz P (Eds), Kaplan and Sadock’s comprehensive textbook of psychiatry
While exploratory studies provide detailed insights into the mecha- (9th ed, pp 3560–3571). Philadelphia, PA: Williams and Wilkins.
nisms that may be involved, they do not draw definite conclusions. This Gulliver A, Griffiths KM, Christensen H (2010) Perceived barriers and facilita-
limitation is mitigated by our employment of quantitative methods and tors to mental health help-seeking in young people: A systematic review.
the use of a wide range of questions that address similar issues, thus BMC Psychiatry. 10:113.
giving a more complete and nonrandom view. Another potential limita- Haneesh K, Krishnakumar P, Sukumaran SK, Riyaz A (2013) Risk factors for scholas-
tion of the study is that it uses a novel questionnaire that has not been tic backwardness in children. Indian Pediatr. 50:655–658.
previously tested for reliability and validity. However, the questionnaire
Hinshaw SP (2005) The stigmatization of mental illness in children and parents:
was constructed based on discussions with a panel of experts in child
Developmental issues, family concerns, and research needs. J Child Psychol
psychiatry, who are also familiar with Indian society, thus ensuring Psychiatry. 46:714–734.
content validity.
A second limitation is that the study was conducted in a special- Hinshaw SP, Stier A (2008) Stigma as related to mental disorders. Annu Rev Clin
ized clinic that caters to children who received a diagnosis of ADHD Psychol. 4:367–393.
and SLD. The responses provided by the children in our sample could Hoge DR, McCarthy JD (1983) Issues of validity and reliability in the use of real–ideal
have been inadvertently influenced by their treatment teams, and they discrepancy scores to measure self-regard. J Pers Soc Psychol. 44:1048.
may not be generalizable to community samples. Jorm AF, Korten A, Jacomb P, Christensen H, Rodgers B, Pollitt P (1997) Mental
Another important factor that could have driven a number of our health literacy. A survey of the public’s ability to recognise mental disorders and
findings is the difference in age, which may have resulted in increased their beliefs about the effectiveness of treatment. Med J Aust. 166:182–186.
awareness, and lower self-esteem among children with SLD as com-
pared with children with ADHD. Finally, we were unable to obtain de- Karande S, Kulkarni M (2005) Poor school performance. Indian J Pediatr. 72:
961–967.
tails about the severity of ADHD and SLD (including functioning in
school), which would have provided us a more accurate representation Kermode M, Bowen K, Arole S, Pathare S, Jorm AF (2009) Attitudes to people with
of real versus perceived distress. Nevertheless, the findings of our study mental disorders: A mental health literacy survey in a rural area of maharashtra,
clearly demonstrate an important public health need to educate children india. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 44:1087–1096.
about the various disorders in different settings, including schools. Loe IM, Feldman HM (2007) Academic and educational outcomes of children with
ADHD. J Pediatr Psychol. 32:643–654.

CONCLUSIONS Martin PY, Turner BA (1986) Grounded theory and organizational research. J Appl
Behav Sci. 22:141–157.
This study provides important insights into the level and signif-
icance of mental health literacy and stigma among children who re- Mayes SD, Calhoun SL, Crowell EW (2000) Learning disabilities and ADHD: Over-
ceived a diagnosis of mental illnesses and their family members. It lapping spectrum disorders. J Learn Disabil. 33:417–424.
also illustrates the need for individual and group psychoeducation for McMenamy JM, Perrin EC (2008) The impact of experience on children’s understand-
children with ADHD and SLD. More importantly, the study identified ing of ADHD. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 29:483–492.
a larger need for studies exploring the efficacy educational programs
Mogasale VV, Patil VD, Patil NM, Mogasale V (2012) Prevalence of specific learning
enhancing mental health literacy and stigma reduction campaigns at a
disabilities among primary school children in a south Indian city. Indian J Pediatr.
school/community level in India. The study also identifies some impor- 79:342–347.
tant public health issues including promoting a medical model about the
illness, which promotes treatment and hope and is less stigmatizing. It Mukolo A, Heflinger CA, Wallston KA (2010) The stigma of childhood mental disor-
also highlights the clinical need for psychoeducation among children ders: A conceptual framework. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 49:92–103.
with ADHD and SLD as improved understanding about the illness Palladino P, Poli P, Masi G, Marcheschi M (2000) The relation between metacognition
can prevent or alleviate comorbidity arising from low self-esteem. and depressive symptoms in preadolescents with learning disabilities: Data in sup-
port of Borkowski’s model. Learn Disabil Res Pract. 15:142–148.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT Perrin JS, Herve PY, Leonard G, Perron M, Pike GB, Pitiot A, Paus T (2008) Growth
The authors thank the Head of the Department of Psychiatry, of white matter in the adolescent brain: Role of testosterone and androgen receptor.
Topiwala National Medical College, Dr Ravindra Kamath, for his sup- J Neurosci. 28:9519–9524.
port of their work and Dr Stephen Faraone, SUNY Upstate Medical Rahman A, Mubbashar M, Gater R, Goldberg D (1998) Randomised trial of impact of
University, for his review of the manuscript. They thank the children school mental-health programme in rural Rawalpindi, Pakistan. Lancet. 352:
and parents who participated in the study. 1022–1025.
Shmulsky S, Gobbo K (2007) Explanatory style and college students with ADHD and
DISCLOSURE LD. J Atten Disord. 10:299–305.
The authors declare no conflict of interest. SPSS Inc (2007) SPSS for Windows, version 16.0. Chicago, IL: SPSS Inc.

462 www.jonmd.com © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.

Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.


The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 204, Number 6, June 2016 Mental Health Literacy in Children

Tannock R (2009) Learning disorders. In Sadock BJ, Sadock VA, Ruiz P (Eds), B. If Yes:
Kaplan and Sadock’s comprehensive textbook of psychiatry (9th ed, pp 3475–3500). i. How can you help yourself get better?
Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins. ii. How can others help you?
Tominey MF (1996) Attributional style as a predictor of academic success for Medicines/talking to the doctors about your problems/school
students with learning disabilities and/or attention deficit disorder in post- teachers/special teachers/others ____________________ (specify)
secondary education. iii. How will this treatment (ii.) help you?
Venkata JA, Panicker AS (2013) Prevalence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
in primary school children. Indian J Psychiatry. 55:338. 13. How long do you think it will take for you to get better?
Wahl OF, Susin J, Kaplan L, Lax A, Zatina D (2011) Changing knowledge and at- 14. Do you think you will get completely better?
titudes with a middle school mental health education curriculum. Stigma Res 15. If it takes a long time, is it going to be a really big problem for you
Action. 1:44–53. or is it going to be okay?
16. Rate on the following scale how bad is it according to you to have
this problem?

APPENDIX 1: QUESTIONNAIRE

Part I
1. Why do you have to come to this hospital?
2. Do you think you have a problem? Yes/No 17. How would you deal with it on a daily basis?
3. What do you think is the problem? Academic/behavioral/others Part II
(specify) __________ 1. Do you know what common cold is? Yes/No
4. A. Does it have a name? Yes/No 2. Have you ever had it? Yes/No
If Yes: 3. What happens when you get a common cold?
What is it called? _________________________ 4. A. Is it any different from the disorder that you have? Yes/No
B. Have you heard of it before? Yes/No B. If Yes: then how?
If Yes: C. If No: why not?
How did you come to know? 5. How do you catch a cold?
Parents/other family members or relatives/neighbors/teachers/ 6. Can you catch a cold if you want to or does it happen by itself?
friends/media (Radio/TV/public awareness posters)/doctor/others How?
(specify) __________________ 7. How does it affect your:
If No: A. Studies?
C. Have you heard your parents/friends/teachers talk about it? B. Extracurricular activities?
If Yes: C. Play?
What do they call it? D. Relationship with parents and siblings?
5. Why do you think you have it? E. Relationship with friends?
6. How did it begin? F. Relationship with teachers?
7. Do you think it is a disease? Yes/No G. Others? ________________________________ (specify)
If No: what is it?
8. How does this problem affect you? 8. Which of the above aspects of your life gets affected the worst
A. Studies? because of it?
B. Extracurricular activities? 9. Can you make a common cold go away? Yes/No
C. Play? If Yes, how will you do that?
D. Relationship with parents and siblings? If No, what can be done to make a common cold go away?
E. Relationship with friends? Medicines/taking rest/others _________________________ (specify)
F. Relationships with teachers? 10. How will the treatment you are talking about work?
G. Others? _________________________ (specify) 11. How long does it take for a common cold to get better?
12. If it doesn’t get better in that time, does it become a really big
9. Which of the above aspects of your life is affected the worst? problem for you or is it okay?
10. How often does this problem affect you? 13. Rate on the following scale how bad is it according to you to have
11. Do you think that this problem can occur on its own? this problem
12. Do you have any control over this problem? Yes/No
If Yes:
A. Can it occur and can it go away when you wish it to?
B. How do you do that?
If No:
A. Do you think something can be done to help you get better?
Yes/No 14. How do you deal with it?

© 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved. www.jonmd.com 463

Copyright © 2016 Wolters Kluwer Health, Inc. All rights reserved.

You might also like