Targeting Body Image in Eating Disorder

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

ScienceDirect

Review

Targeting body image in eating disorders


Piers L. Cornelissen and Martin J. Tovée

Abstract of the disorder [4,5]. BID has been suggested to be a


Eating disorders, such as anorexia nervosa, are challenging to predictor of onset and the effectiveness of treatment,
treat successfully and have a very high relapse rate. Body and its persistence is a predictor of relapse [6e10],
image disturbance (BID) is a core component of these eating which has been estimated to be as high as 41% [11,12].
disorders. It is a predictor of onset, treatment outcome and BID is so central to AN such that it has even been
future relapse. However, recent studies suggest that BID can suggested that it be renamed as body image disorder
be improved by an adaptation of cognitive bias training. This [13]. In developing interventions to treat AN, BID
does not target the accuracy of body size judgements, but provides a potential gateway into treating this most
instead focuses on how a body of a particular size is catego- intractable of chronic psychiatric conditions. If BID can
rized by a patient. This recalibration of the categorical be improved, then it is possible that we may also see a
boundary at which bodies are judged as overweight, which more global improvement in symptoms. Therefore, we
challenges a patient's existing preconceptions about which will first define the nature of BID and then discuss how
constitutes an acceptable body size, seems to lead to a more it can be treated and the impact of this treatment on a
general reassessment of eating disordered attitudes and a patient’s psychological profile.
significant improvement in their psychological profile. These
promising findings need further trials to determine the long- Attitudinal and perceptual body image:
term effectiveness of such a targeted intervention, but it same or different?
potentially provides an important additional treatment option. In As per the influential meta-analysis by Cash and Deagle
addition, this cognitive bias training may also be an effective [4], body image comprises two components: (1)
augmentation to treatment in other conditions that feature BID, perceptual body image represents the accuracy with
such as bulimia nervosa and body dysmorphia. which a person can judge the physical dimensions of
their own body (see also the studies by Gardner and
Addresses
Department of Psychology, Northumbria University, Newcastle upon Brown [14] and Skrzypek et al [15]), and (2) attitudinal
Tyne, United Kingdom body image captures the feelings that a person has about
his/her body size and shape. Nevertheless, a number of
Corresponding author: Tovée, Martin J. (martin.j.tovee@northumbria. authors have suggested that body size overestimation in
ac.uk)
conditions such as AN might be explained solely in
terms of changes in attitudinal body image [16e18], and
Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 41:71–77 tasks that purportedly measure perceptual body image
This review comes from a themed issue on Psychopathology
may in fact be visual proxies for estimates of attitudinal
body image. Given the severe psychological and physical
Edited by Peter de Jong and Yannick Boddez
consequences of eating disorders to sufferers, it is
For a complete overview see the Issue and the Editorial important to decide unambiguously whether there is a
Available online 8 April 2021 separable perceptual component to body image.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.03.013
2352-250X/© 2021 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. To address this problem, Cornelissen et al. [19] used a
high-level adaptation paradigm to ask whether perceptual
body image measures are really a proxy for attitudinal body
Keywords
Body image disturbance, Cognitive bias training, Anorexia nervosa,
image, which is usually assessed psychometrically, or
Body image change. whether these are indeed meaningfully separate, statis-
tically independent constructs. Visual adaptation is a
Anorexia nervosa (AN) is a serious mental illness temporary change in perception after prolonged exposure
affecting up to 1% of the female population in Western to a new or intense stimulus. It leads to a lingering after-
societies, where the long-term mortality rate has been effect that may continue once the adapting stimulus is
estimated to be as high as 10% [1e3], being the highest removed. High-level adaptation effects result in after-
for any psychiatric disease. A distorted evaluation of effects that change in the same direction as the adapting
personal body size, or body image disturbance (BID; stimulus. Thus, if the first model for body image were true
DSM-5, 2013), is one of the central diagnostic criteria (i.e. vision as a proxy for attitude), following the logic of
for AN and is also a key element of psychological models social comparison theory [20], the regression of

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 41:71–77


72 Psychopathology

percentage error in body size estimation on the strength of illustrated in Figure 2a, they found that (1) the same
an adapting stimulus should show negative slopes, with a regression of estimated BMI on actual BMI captured the
linear interaction between the strength of the adapting data for both patients with AN and the healthy control
stimuli and body attitude (see Figure 1a). If the second, participants and (2) the slope of this regression line was
‘independence’ model were true, a regression of per- less than 1. This implied that inaccuracies in body size
centage error in body size estimation on the strength of estimation across all participants, whether patients with
the adapting stimulus should have a positive slope. In AN or healthy controls, could be explained by a well-
addition, the intercept of this regression line should be known bias in magnitude estimation called contraction
proportional to attitudinal measures: elevated body image bias, which is perfectly normal [23]. Contraction bias
concerns contribute a fixed amount to body size over- arises when one uses a standard reference or template
estimation (see Figure 1b). Therefore, plots of the for a particular kind of object against which to estimate
adaptation effect in different observers, each of whom the size of other examples of that object. The estimate
obtains a different score on body attitude, should produce is most accurate when estimating the size of an object of
a set of regression lines that have a positive slope and are a similar size to the reference but becomes increasingly
parallel to each other. Cornelissen et al. [19] thus showed inaccurate as the magnitude of the difference between
clear evidence in support of independence. the reference and the object increases. When this hap-
pens, the observer estimates that the object is closer in
Disturbed perceptual body image in AN? size to the reference than it actually is. As a result, an
Having established the validity of an independent object smaller in size than the reference will be over-
perceptual component to body image, the next clinically estimated and an object larger in size will be
important question is whether any differences exist in underestimated.
this attribute when comparing women with AN with
healthy controls. Cornelissen et al. [21] reanalysed a The contraction bias explanation predicts that for both
previous study in which women with AN and healthy healthy controls and women with AN, the accuracy of
controls were asked to estimate their own body size by their body size estimation will be driven by the BMI of
manipulating an image of themselves using a body the participants, in the same way. While reanalysis of
morphing programme [22]. The software allowed par- Cornelissen et al. [21] showed that this was the case,
ticipants to match their perception of the size of their nevertheless, the BMI values of the women with AN in
individual body parts with what they saw on the screen the study by Tovée et al. [22] all actually fell within a
by manipulating slider controls. Cornelissen et al. [21] relatively narrow range of 6.9 BMI units (11.5e18.4).
directly compared participants’ estimates of their own Most of the variation in BMI in this study was based on
body mass indexes (BMIs) with their actual BMIs. As is the responses of the control participants, for whom BMI

Figure 1

(a) Sketch graph showing predicted effects of adaptation for the ‘perception as a proxy for attitude’ hypothesis. The y-axis represents percentage error in
postadaptation body size estimation. Negative values represent underestimation, and positive values represent overestimation. The x-axis corresponds to
the size of the adapting stimulus relative to the body size of the observer. (b) Sketch graph showing predicted effects of adaptation for the ‘independence’
hypothesis. For sketch graphs (a) and (b), the adaptation effects are shown separately for low (circles), medium (triangles) and high (squares) body image
concerns.

Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 41:71–77 www.sciencedirect.com


Body image in eating disorders Cornelissen and Tovée 73

Figure 2

(a) Schematic representation of the results from the study by Cornelissen et al. [21], with women with AN in orange and healthy controls in cyan. (b) The
pattern of body size estimation predicted by the contraction bias model in women with AN, or recovering from AN, who have a wider range of BMI. (c) The
pattern of body size overestimation in women with AN whose perceptual body image is distorted and follows a different trajectory from that predicted by
contraction bias. In (a), (b) and (c), the dashed line represents veridical performance, where body size estimates are perfectly accurate. AN, anorexia
nervosa; BMI, body mass index.

ranged between 14.7 and 36.8 (22.1 BMI units).


Therefore, if perceptual body image is indeed normal in
AN, a sample of women with AN and a much wider BMI Figure 3
range (including recovering patients to expand the
range) should mirror the responses of healthy control
participants. This is illustrated in Figure 2b, where the
gold arrow shows how the regression of estimated BMI
on actual BMI in these women should track up along the
same regression line as in Figure 2a. Alternatively, if
perceptual body image is disturbed in AN, we should see
a very different slope for the regression of estimated
BMI on actual BMI, as illustrated in Figure 2c.

To test this hypothesis, Cornelissen et al. [24] asked


women with AN and controls to estimate their body size.
In the analysis, they subdivided their groups into those
with higher and lower body concerns. For the controls,
both high- and low-concern groups show evidence of
contraction bias (Figure 3). Statistically, the slope of these
two lines is the same, so that means that the perceptual
component d that is, the contraction bias effect d is the
same for both groups. The difference between the two
lines is driven solely by body attitudes. Thus, this means
that whatever your actual BMI is, having negative atti-
tudes to your body increases the size that you think you
are. By comparison, women with a history of AN are The relationship between participants' BMI (x-axis) and their subjective
estimate of body size (the point of subjective equality or PSE) separately
different. The slope relating estimated size to actual size for women with a history of AN (white) and healthy controls (black). The
is much steeper than for controls (Figure 3). Although, dotted black line represents the line of equality, where PSE matches BMI
low BMI women with AN are actually quite accurate as perfectly. The impact of psychometric performance on these relationships
the body weight of anorexics increases, so the tendency is illustrated by the separate lines for each group, that is, the data are
for them to overestimate body size systematically in- plotted for PSYCH (a latent variable derived from a principal
component analysis of questionnaires assessing attitudes to body shape,
creases as a linear function of their own actual BMI. This eating, depression and self-esteem) at +1 SD (dashed lines) and –1 SD
is important because persistent body size overestimation (solid lines). AN, anorexia nervosa; BMI, body mass index; SD, standard
predicts relapse [6,25], and this overestimation gets deviation.

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 41:71–77


74 Psychopathology

worse as women’s BMI increases towards normal and even trials (i.e. 3 presentations of each of the 15 images), and no
higher levels. feedback about the participants’ decisions was given.
Based on their responses, the baseline thin/fat boundary
for that day’s session was calculated. During the training
Can we treat this problem? phase, the participants then carried out a further 6 blocks
Currently, the available treatments for eating disorders of 31 trials each, and these include feedback after every
are based primarily on specially adapted cognitive trial, for example, ‘Incorrect, that body was thin’ or ‘Cor-
behavioural approaches [26,27], and these have shown rect, that body was fat’. During the post-training mea-
limited effectiveness [28e30]. A recent systematic surement, the same sequence of trials as the baseline
review and meta-analysis suggests that once corrections measurement was presented, again without feedback.
for biases in the data are applied, the effect sizes of Therefore, in each training session, the extent of any shift
these treatments are small and strongly suggest the in the thin/fat category boundary could be calculated by
need for new therapies [31]. The role of BID as a core comparing the post-training measurement with that
feature of AN and as a predictor of both onset and session’s baseline measurement. The key element of the
relapse suggests the need to develop specific therapies training that appears to cause the shift in the categorical
that target distorted body image as part of their treat- boundary was the confrontational nature of the feedback
ment [6,25,32]. that the participants received in the training phase. In the
intervention group, the feedback was ‘inflationary’ and
Recent laboratory evidence has shown that a yes\no forced designed to move the boundary towards heavier bodies.
choice cognitive bias modification paradigm that focuses For example, if the preintervention baseline measure-
on thin ideals and social comparison processes leads to ment gave a thin/fat boundary at image 7/15, then the
reduced concerns about eating (indexed by standardized ‘inflationary’ feedback was given as if that boundary was
psychometric assessment) and improvements in body really at image 9/15. Therefore, with respect to images
image [33,34]. However, systematic randomized that the participants had decided were fat during the
controlled studies for these effects in clinical samples are baseline measurement, they now had to accept as thin to
still required. The interventions have used adapted get a ‘correct’ response. By comparison, in the control
cognitive bias modification techniques that were origi- group, the feedback merely maintained the boundary
nally developed in the field of anxiety [35,36]. Gledhill identified during the baseline measurement (i.e. ‘main-
et al. [33] adapted this paradigm for body size estimation tenance’ feedback). Thus, if the preintervention baseline
in the light of evidence that individuals show a clear cat- measurement gave a thin/fat boundary at image 7/15, now
egorical boundary when judging bodies [37]. Using the ‘maintenance’ feedback was also given for a boundary
computer-generated imagery (CGI) avatars representing at image 7/15.
a continuum of body sizes from emaciated to obese,
Gledhill et al. [33] first identified the baseline measure- Gledhill et al. [33] showed that the increase in the size
ment of the thin vs fat boundary in individual participants. of a body that was classified as ‘fat’, as a result of
To do this, the participants locate the point along the training, occurred for those participants who received
continuum that represents their subjective judgement inflationary feedback, but not for those who received
for the transition from a ‘thin’ to a ‘fat’ body (i.e. the maintenance feedback (Figure 4b and c). This change in
categorical boundary). Gledhill et al. [33] then applied the way bodies were categorized was accompanied by a
the training procedure to shift an individual’s categorical significant reduction of the participants’ own concerns
boundary towards heavier bodies (see Figure 4a). They about body shape/weight and eating (Figure 4d and e).
ran this training paradigm in (1) a sample of adult healthy When tested at 1-month follow-up, these reductions
women with high body image concerns and (2) a sample of were retained. In a similar study, Szostak [38] also used
women with atypical AN. Participants sat in front of a the same training paradigm to manipulate participants’
computer screen on which images of a female CGI body interpretation of body size and to encourage their
were presented one at a time. A total of 15 images were interpretation of thinness over heaviness in normal-
used representing the change in BMI from emaciated to sized bodies, in women with heightened body shape,
obese. In each trial, the participant was asked to classify weight and eating concerns. The training successfully
whether the image on the screen was thin or fat. Training shifted the categorical boundary towards a heavier body,
was carried out over 4 sessions. Each session and the shift was retained at a 2-week follow-up. Most
comprised (1) a pretraining baseline measurement; (2) an recently, Irvine et al. [34] replicated the study by
intervention training or control training, depending on Gledhill et al. [33], but this time by using longer stim-
the group assignment, and (3) a post-training measure- ulus presentations presented in a virtual reality envi-
ment. The pretraining baseline measurement used 45 ronment. In sum, two key outcomes resulted from these

Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 41:71–77 www.sciencedirect.com


Body image in eating disorders Cornelissen and Tovée 75

Figure 4

(a) The middle row shows part of the body sequence varying in BMI. The top row illustrates the results from a baseline assessment and the position of the
categorical boundary before training. The bottom row illustrates the results from the post-training test session, showing that the categorical boundary has
shifted relative to the pretraining result. (b) A plot of the mean value of BMI at the categorical boundary (predicted from the statistical modelling) as a
function of measurement day. Magenta and red circles represent control group (Con) pretraining and post-training thresholds, respectively. Cyan and blue
circles represent intervention group (Int) pretraining and post-training thresholds, respectively. (c) A plot of the predicted differences between pretraining
and post-training categorical thresholds, with 95% CIs, as a function of training day. CIs that straddle zero are not significant at p <.05. Blue circles
represent the intervention group, and red circles represent the control group. (d) A plot of predicted global eating disorder examination questionnaire
(EDE-Q) scores (reported as z-scores) as a function of measurement day. Blue circles represent the intervention group, and red circles represent the
control group. (e) A plot of the predicted differences in global EDE-Q z-scores between the control and training groups as a function of measurement day,
with 95% CIs, as a function of training day. CIs that straddle zero are not significant at p <.05. BMI, body mass index; CI, confidence interval.

laboratory studies: Inflationary feedback succeeded in categorized as ‘thin’ after training. The second key
changing the participants’ judgements about other outcome was the one that is clinically most important:
people’s body size. Avatars that the participants would all these studies found a significant change in the par-
have described as ‘fat’ before training were later ticipants’ attitudes about their own body. Specifically,

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 41:71–77


76 Psychopathology

psychological concerns they may have had about their treatment, which turn predicts BMI and score on the eating Disorder
inventory at 12 months post-treatment.
own body shape and weight, as well as their own eating
habits, were significantly reduced. This effect persisted 7. Bluett EJ, Lee EB, Simone M, Lockhart G, Twohig MP, Lense-
grav-Benson T, Quakenbush-Roberts B: The role of body image
for around one-month after training. psychological flexibility on the treatment of eating disorders
in a residential facility. Eat Behav 2016, 23:150–155, https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.10.002.
Conclusions 8. Calugi S, Dalle Grave R: Body image concern and treatment
BID is a key component of AN and a predictor of the outcomes in adolescents with anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat
onset, treatment outcome and future relapse after Disord 2019, 52:582–585, https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.23031.
treatment. BID can be modified by targeted in- 9. Lee EB, Ong CW, Twohig MP, Lensegrav-Benson T, Quaken-
terventions that do not alter accuracy of body size ** bush-Roberts B: Increasing body image flexibility in a resi-
dential eating disorder facility: correlates with symptom
judgements, instead change how a body of a particular improvement. Eat Disord 2018, 26:185–199, https://doi.org/
size is categorized. This recalibration of the categorical 10.1080/10640266.2017.13662.
This study measured body image concerns and flexibility as predictors
boundary, challenging participants’ existing perceptions of treatment outcome in 103 female in-patients with an eating disorder.
about themselves, seems to lead to a more global reas- Changes in body image predicted changes in a range of measure-
ments in psychological profile (controlling for BMI, depression and
sessment of eating disordered attitudes and a significant anxiety) and researchers conclude that their results show that body
improvement in the psychological profile. These image is an important target for treatment.
promising findings obviously need further trials to 10. Pellizzer ML, Waller G, Wade TD: Body image flexibility: a
determine the intervention’s long-term effectiveness, predictor and moderator of outcome in transdiagnostic
outpatient eating disorder treatment. Int J Eat Disord 2018, 51:
but it potentially provides a tool to treat a particularly 368–372, https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22842.
intractable chronic condition. Although the application
11. Berends T, Boonstra N, van Elburg A: Relapse in anorexia
of this intervention has been focused on women with nervosa: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Curr Opin
AN, it may also be effective in other conditions that Psychiatr 2018, 31:445–455, https://doi.org/10.1097/
YCO.0000000000000453.
feature BID, most obviously bulimia nervosa and body
dysmorphia disorder. 12. Carter JC, Blackmore E, Sutandar-Pinnock K, Woodside DB:
Relapse in anorexia nervosa: a survival analysis. Psychol
Med 2004, 34:671 – 679, https://doi.org/10.1017/
S0033291703001168.
Conflict of interest statement
Nothing declared. 13. Phillipou A, Castle DJ, Rossell SL: Anorexia nervosa: eating
disorder or body image disorder? Aus NZ J Psychiatr 2018, 52:
13–14, https://doi.org/10.1177/0004867417722640.
References 14. Gardner RM, Brown DL: Body size estimation in anorexia
Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review,
have been highlighted as: nervosa: a brief review of findings from 2003 through 2013.
Psychiatr Res 2014, 219:407–410, https://doi.org/10.1016/
* * of outstanding interest j.psychres.2014.06.029.
15. Skrzypek S, Wehmeier PM, Remschmidt H: Body image
1. Berkman ND, Lohr KN, Bulik CM: Outcomes of eating disor- assessment using body size estimation in recent studies on
ders: a systematic review of the literature. Int J Eat Disord anorexia nervosa. A brief review. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatr
2007, 40:293–309, https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20369. 2001, 10:215–221, https://doi.org/10.1007/s007870170010.

2. Fichter MM, Quadflieg N: Mortality in eating disorders—results 16. Mölbert SC, Thaler A, Mohler BJ, Streuber S, Romero J,
of a large prospective clinical longitudinal study. Int J Eat * * Black MJ, Giel KE: Assessing body image in anorexia nervosa
Disord 2016, 49:391–401, https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22501. using biometric self-avatars in virtual reality: attitudinal
components rather than visual body size estimation are
3. Khalsa SS, Portnoff LC, McCurdy-McKinnon D, Feusner JD: distorted. Psychol Med 2018, 48:642–653, https://doi.org/
What happens after treatment? A systematic review of 10.1017/S0033291717002008.
relapse, remission, and recovery in anorexia nervosa. J Eat This interesting study uses 3D scans of its participants to effectively
Disord 2017, 5:1–12, https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-017-0145-3. create a 3D “photograph” of each of them, to which the investigators
then applied a morph to simulate an increase or decrease in weight by
4. Cash TF, Deagle EA: The nature and extent of body-image up to 20%. Although this range of weight change is limited at this stage,
disturbances in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: a the application of this kind of personalised, highly realistic stimuli is a
meta-analysis. Int J Eat Disord 1997, 22:107–126, https:// clear pointer to the future of body perception research.
doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199709)22:2%3C107::AID-
EAT1%3E3.0.CO;2-J. 17. Mergen J, Keizer A, Koelkebeck K, van den Heuvel MRC,
Wagner H: Women with anorexia nervosa do not show altered
5. Fairburn CG, Cooper Z, Shafran R: Cognitive behaviour therapy tactile localization compared to healthy controls. Psychiatr
for eating disorders: a “transdiagnostic” theory and treat- Res 2018, 267:446–454, https://doi.org/10.1016/
ment. Behav Res Ther 2003, 41:509–528, https://doi.org/ j.psychres.2018.06.007.
10.1016/s0005-7967(02)00088-8.
18. Smeets MAM: The rise and fall of body size estimation
6. Junne F, Wild B, Resmark G, Giel KE, Teufel M, Martus P, et al.: research in anorexia nervosa: a review and reconceptuali-
** The importance of body image disturbances for the outcome zation. Eur Eat Disord Rev 1997, 5:75–95, https://doi.org/
of outpatient psychotherapy in patients with anorexia nerv- 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0968(199706)5:2<75::AID-
osa: results of the ANTOP-study. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2019, 27: ERV190>3.0.CO;2-A.
49–58, https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2623.
This study tests whether body image predicts the outcome of treatment 19. Cornelissen KK, Widrington H, McCarty K, Pollet TV, Tovée MJ,
in an out-patient cohort of 148 women with AN. Path analysis shows Cornelissen PL: Are attitudinal and perceptual body image the
that negative body image at the beginning of treatment predicts stress same or different? Evidence from high-level adaptation. Body
during treatment, which in turn predicts negative mood at the end of Image 2019, 31:35–47, https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.bodyim.2019.08.001.

Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 41:71–77 www.sciencedirect.com


Body image in eating disorders Cornelissen and Tovée 77

20. Festinger L: A theory of social comparison processes. Hum 31. van den Berg E, Houtzager L, de Vos J, Daemen I, Katsaragaki G,
Relat 1954, 7:117–140. 10.1177%2F001872675400700202. Karyotaki E, Cuijpers P, Dekker J: Meta-analysis on the efficacy
of psychological treatments of anorexia nervosa. Eur Eat
21. Cornelissen PL, Johns A, Tovée MJ: Body size over-estimation Disord Rev 2019, 27:331–351, https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2683.
in women with anorexic nervosa is not qualitatively different
from female controls. Body Image 2013, 10:103–111, https:// 32. Castro J, Gila A, Puig J, Rodriguez S, Toro J: Predictors of
doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-15339-z. rehospitalization after total weight recovery in adolescents
with anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 2004, 36:22–30, https://
22. Tovée MJ, Benson PJ, Emery JL, Mason SM, Cohen-Tovée EM: doi.org/10.1002/eat.20340.
Measurement of body size and shape perception in eating-
disordered and control observers using body shape soft- 33. Gledhill LJ, Cornelissen KK, Cornelissen PL, Penton-Voak IS,
ware. Br J Psychol 2003, 94:501–516, https://doi.org/10.1348/ * * Munafo MR, Tovée MJ: An interactive training programme to
000712603322503060. treat body image disturbance. Br J Health Psychol 2016, https://
doi.org/10.1111/bjhp.12217.
23. Poulton EC: Bias in quantifying judgements. Hove, UK: Erlbaum; This paper details the application of a cognitive bias program to reca-
1989. librate how participants categorize body weight. In the first study they
reported a 2-arm RCT in which women with high body image concerns
24. Cornelissen KK, Bester A, Cairns P, Tovee MJ, Cornelissen PL:
were randomly allocated into intervention and control conditions. The
The influence of personal BMI on body size estimations and
cohort that received the active intervention showed not only significant
sensitivity to body size change in anorexia spectrum disor-
improvement in body image, but also a general improvement in psy-
ders. Body Image 2015, 13:75–85.
chological profile. In a second study this positive outcome is replicated
25. Liechty JM: Body image distortion and three types of weight in a cohort of women with atypical AN.
loss behaviors among non-overweight girls in the United
34. Irvine KR, Irvine A, Maalin N, McCarty K, Cornelissen KK,
States. J Adolesc Health 2010, 47:176–182, https://doi.org/
Toveé MJ, Cornelissen PL: Using immersive virtual reality to
10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.01.004.
modify body image. Body Image 2020, 33:232–234, https://
26. Fairburn CG, Bailey-Straebler S, Basden S, Doll HA, Jones R, doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2020.03.007.
Murphy R, et al.: A transdiagnostic comparison of enhanced
35. Penton-Voak IS, Thomas J, Gage SH, McMurran M, McDonald S,
cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT-E) and interpersonal psy-
Munafὸ MR: Increasing recognition of happiness in ambig-
chotherapy in the treatment of eating disorders. Behav Res
uous facial expressions reduces anger and aggressive
Ther 2015, 70:64–71, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2015.04.010.
behaviour. Psychol Sci 2013, 24:688–697, https://doi.org/
27. Hay PJ, Claudino AM, Touyz S, Abd Elbaky G: Individual psy- 10.1177/0956797612459657.
chological therapy in the outpatient treatment of adults with
36. Hakamata Y, Lissek S, Bar-Haim Y, Britton JC, Fox NA,
anorexia nervosa. The Cochrane Library; 2015.
Leibenluft E, et al.: Attention bias modification treatment: a
28. Murray SB, Quintana DS, Loeb KL, Griffiths S, Le Grange D: meta-analysis toward the establishment of novel treatment
Treatment outcomes for anorexia nervosa: a systematic for anxiety. Biol Psychiatr 2010, 68:982–990, https://doi.org/
review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.021.
Psychol Med 2019, 49:535–544, https://doi.org/10.1017/
37. Tovée MJ, Edmonds L, Vuong QC: Categorical perception of
s0033291718002088.
human female physical attractiveness and health. Evol Hum
29. Khalsa SS, Portnoff LC, McCurdy-McKinnon D, Feusner JD: Behav 2012, 33:85–93, https://doi.org/10.1016/
What happens after treatment? A systematic review of j.evolhumbehav.2011.05.008.
relapse, remission, and recovery in anorexia nervosa. J Eat
38. Szostak NM: Negative body image and cognitive biases to body
Disord 2017, 5:20, https://doi.org/10.1186/s40337-017-0145-3.
size. Doctoral thesis. 2018. Retrieved from, https://hydra.hull.ac.
30. Zipfel S, Giel KE, Bulik CM, Hay P, Schmidt U: Anorexia nerv- uk/resources/hull:16444.
osa: aetiology, assessment, and treatment. Lancet Psychiatr
2015, 2:1099–1111, https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(15)
00356-9.

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Psychology 2021, 41:71–77

You might also like