Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 19

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev

The role of serotonin in declarative memory: A systematic review of animal


and human research
Rebecca Coray a, b, *, Boris B. Quednow a, b
a
Experimental and Clinical Pharmacopsychology, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Psychiatric University Hospital Zurich, University of
Zurich, Switzerland
b
Neuroscience Center Zurich, University of Zurich and Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Zurich, Switzerland

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: The serotonergic system is involved in diverse cognitive functions including memory. Of particular importance to
Serotonin daily life are declarative memories that contain information about personal experiences, general facts, and
5-HT events. Several psychiatric or neurological diseases, such as depression, attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder
Declarative memory
(ADHD), and dementia, show alterations in serotonergic signalling and attendant memory disorders. Never­
Learning
Encoding
theless, understanding serotonergic neurotransmission and its influence on memory remained a challenge until
Consolidation today. In this systematic review, we summarize recent psychopharmacological studies in animals and humans
Recognition from a psychological memory perspective, in consideration of task-specific requirements. This approach has the
Fear conditioning advantage that comparisons between serotonin (5-HT)-related neurochemical mechanisms and manipulations
are each addressing specific mnemonic circuits. We conclude that applications of the same 5-HT-related treat­
ments can differentially affect unrelated tasks of declarative memories. Moreover, the analysis of specific
mnemonic phases (e.g., encoding vs. consolidation) reveals opposing impacts of increased or decreased 5-HT
tones, with low 5-HT supporting spatial encoding but impairing the consolidation of objects and verbal mem­
ories. Promising targets for protein synthesis-dependent consolidation enhancements include 5-HT4 receptor
agonists and 5-HT6 receptor antagonists, with the latter being of special interest for the treatment of age-related
decline. Further implications are pointed out as base for the development of novel therapeutic targets for
memory impairment of neuropsychiatric disorders.

1. Introduction decisive contribution of 5-HT to memory formation by increasing the


level of cyclic adenosine 3’,5’-monophosphate (cAMP) in the sensory
The neurotransmitter 5-HT is involved in a wide variety of brain neurons of Aplysia (Cedar and Schwartz, 1972). This in turn activates
functions and behaviours. This includes the control of physiological the cAMP-dependent protein kinase, which facilitates synaptic trans­
activities, such as sleep, stress response, cardiovascular and endocrine mission in terms of sensitization (Brunelli et al., 1976) and, after
functions, the modulation of reward, emotions, mood, and behaviors repeated stimulations, induces protein synthesis-dependent long-term
such as anger, aggression, appetite, pain, sexuality, as well as neuro­ potentiation (LTP) of synaptic strength (Schacher et al., 1988). Since
psychological processes like perception, attention, and memory (Bac­ then, the participation of 5-HT in memory processes has frequently been
qué-Cazenave et al., 2020). Indeed, this broad spectrum of contributions subject of research and up until today, the 5-HT system is considered as
and the anatomical fact that 5-HT axons in the central nervous system interesting target in the development of therapeutic applications to treat
innervate almost every brain region, led to the claim, that – in terms of human memory disorders (Buhot et al., 2000; Chakraborty et al., 2019;
brain functions – 5-HT “is at once implicated in virtually everything, but Meneses, 2013). In addition, dysregulations of the serotonergic system
responsible for nothing” (Jacobs and Fornal, 1995). have been associated with drug addiction and the development of
The specific serotonergic participation in learning and memory was declarative drug memories that maintain addictive behaviors such as
first demonstrated by Eric Kandel in the 1970 s. He demonstrated the drug-seeking behavior. Serotonergic treatments therefore may offer

* Correspondence to: Experimental and Clinical Pharmacopsychology, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Psychiatric University
Hospital Zurich, University of Zurich, Lenggstrasse 31, CH-8032 Zurich, Switzerland.
E-mail address: rebecca.coray@bli.uzh.ch (R. Coray).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104729
Received 11 February 2022; Received in revised form 13 May 2022; Accepted 6 June 2022
Available online 9 June 2022
0149-7634/© 2022 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

great potential to addiction treatments via attenuation and extinction of distinction offered by Tulving in 1972 (Tulving, 1972). An episodic
otherwise resistant drug memories (Müller, 2013; Müller and Homberg, memory trace usually binds together multiple situational aspects of a
2015). unique personal experience, such as the time, place, or the social
There already exist some reviews about 5-HT and memory (Meneses, context. In contrast, semantic memory incorporates memory for generic,
2015; Perez-Garcia and Meneses, 2008; Zhang and Stackman, 2015a, context-free knowledge which is acquired over multiple occasions and is
Van Goethem et al., 2015). Nevertheless, a systematic review with a therefore partly dissociable from the episodic memory system. However,
focus on the serotonergic involvement in the modulation of different in terms of the underlying neuronal substrates, these forms of memory
phases of declarative memories (encoding, consolidation, retrieval) is are closely related and recruit very similar brain structures during their
lacking to date. This overview will thus be grouped into different types formation – at least in a healthy brain (Ryan et al., 2008). (See also Duff
of declarative memories to provide a framework that combines knowl­ et al., 2020 for a comprehensive review about the episodic vs. semantic
edge from cognitive psychology and neuropharmacology. This focus on memory interdependence and distinction).
serotonergic modulation of neuronal circuits required for a specific Declarative memories have extensively been studied in cases of
memory process may provide a basis for deriving new hypotheses that human amnesia (Squire and Zola, 1998). Out of this context, this term
will inspire future research and development of therapeutic targets for originally refers to memories that can be consciously and verbally
disorders of the brain involving memory impairment. stated. Another line of research investigated mnemonic functions of
animals in hippocampal-dependent learning and provided additional
1.1. Anatomy of the serotonergic system insights in fundamental mechanisms of declarative memories across
species (Eichenbaum, 2000; Eichenbaum et al., 1992). As some
Despite its diverse involvement, the ability to synthesize 5-HT is phenomenological aspects of the definition of human declarative
limited to just a few neurons; these account for less than 0.1% of neurons memory are not transferable to animal models (e.g. linguistic-based
in the mammalian brain (Okaty et al., 2019). The cell bodies of sero­ behavioral markers of recollection), a more anatomically and biologi­
tonergic neurons are forming clusters in the brain stem, called raphe cally based framework was established to promote translational
nuclei, and their long and fine graded axons are widely branched research. In this framework, a functional organization of the declarative
throughout the central nervous system. Of particular interest in this memory system was proposed, that considered the medial temporal lobe
review are ascending projections stemming from the dorsal raphe (DR; as a relational processing system whereas parallel “what”, “where” and
B6 and B7) and the median raphe nuclei (MR; B5 and B8). “when” information is integrated within the hippocampus to represent
The rostral DR (B7) mainly projects to the extended amygdala, events in the spatio-temporal context in which they occurred (Dere
striatum, cerebral cortex, and substantia nigra, whereas neurons of the et al., 2008; Eichenbaum, 2001). The consolidation of information into a
caudal DR (B6) innervate the (ventral) hippocampus, locus coeruleus persistent trace relies on the prefrontal cortex areas in addition, which
and entorhinal cortex. MR axons are targeting predominantly midline also supports the recombination and transfer of experiences to novel
structures, including the septo-hippocampal system, (dorsal) hippo­ events in service of successful behavior (Preston and Eichenbaum, 2013;
campus, tegmental nucleus, interpeduncular nucleus, hypothalamus, Zeithamova et al., 2012). This definition encompasses the domain of
and lateral habenula (Hornung, 2003; Jacobs and Azmitia, 1992; Vertes spatial memory (Buzsáki, 2002; Eichenbaum and Cohen, 2014; Has­
et al., 1999). Raphe projections are topographically organized and selmo and Stern, 2014), trace conditioning (Connor and Gould, 2016),
adding the complexity of serotonergic forebrain projections, can be and contextual fear conditioning (Anagnostaras et al., 2001), since those
further subdivided based on neuronal density, morphology, tasks were shown to critically rely on hippocampal system during
co-transmission of other neurotransmitters, genetic signatures, electro­ memory formation.
physiological properties and projection areas (Okaty et al., 2019; Ren In the following, we will first review animal studies investigating
et al., 2019). The axonal shapes of 5-HT vary according to function and spatial memory, object memory, and contextual fear memory, while
target regions, with MR arising axons beeing characterized by large each section is grouped according to effects of lowering and enhancing
beaded varicosities (type M axons) while DR axons are predominantly 5-HT levels and then summarizes the literature on specific 5-HTRs.
very fine with small, pleomorphic varicosities (type D axons) (Kosofsky Subsequently, we will focus on human studies that employed verbal or
and Molliver, 1987). Type M fibres were shown to form direct synaptic visual learning tasks, keeping the same structure for grouping seroto­
contacts, for example to hippocampal GABAergic interneurons (Papp nergic manipulations as in the animal section.
et al., 1999; Varga et al., 2009) in contrast to type D fibres which mainly
innervate cortical regions by volume transmission via extra-synaptic 2. Method
serotonin-containing vesicle release (del Cid-Pellitero and Garzón,
2011). This systematic review was conducted according to the Preferred
5-HT receptors (5-HTRs) are classified into seven types, 5-HT1 Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA)
through 5-HT7. The majority of 5-HTRs are postsynaptic with the statement (Page et al., 2021).
exception of 5-HT1ARs and 5-HT1BRs that additionally function as pre­
synaptic autoreceptors. Except 5-HT3Rs, which are ionotropic, all 5-
HTRs are coupled to G-proteins. This remarkable diversity within the 2.1. Study eligibility
5-HT system enables to carry a variety of information and might
represent the morphological substratum of the diverse processes For an extensive search of literature, we first defined search-terms
modulated by 5-HT (Luchetti et al., 2020). describing forms of learning that address declarative memories in
humans and mammals. Behavioral tasks assessing declarative memories
1.2. Declarative memories and their underlying substrates and had to fulfill following criteria: the acquisition and storage of the
mechanisms memory is reliant on brain structures of the medial temporal lobe, and
further, encoding, consolidation, and retrieval are critical steps of
Declarative memory refers to the acquisition and retrieval of facts, memory processing (Eichenbaum, 2001). Matching task in humans are
events, and episodes allowing the remembered material to be compared verbal or visual tasks including the formation of associations between
and contrasted (Squire, 2004). Representations of declarative memories pairs of stimuli or spatial locations, contextual fear conditioning, and
can be accessed flexibly, enabling logical inferences and generalization pattern or object recognition. Corresponding tasks in animals are novel
of experience. One prominent characterization of declarative memory is object recognition, spatial learning tasks (e.g. reference learning),
that it can be decomposed into episodic and semantic memory, a key contextual fear conditioning, trace conditioning.

2
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

2.2. Literature search 3. Animal studies

Two different search terms were used to identify human and animal 3.1. Spatial learning and memory
studies:
1. For human studies, we restricted our search to healthy participants Spatial learning involves the memorization of landmarks along a
and searched PubMed (all years, 101 results, 21.3.2022) and Embase (all route in a cognitive map and their flexible representation during navi­
years, 36 results, 21.3.2022) using following search strategy: ((serotonin gation (Tolman, 1948). To evaluate rodent spatial memory, maze
OR 5-HT OR serotonergic)("declarative memory" OR "episodic memory" learning tasks are widely used. One of the most popular is the Morris
OR "semantic memory" OR "verbal memory" OR "verbal learning" OR water maze (MWM), which assesses hippocampus-dependent reference
"visual memory" OR "contextual fear conditioning" OR "spatial memory" learning when the animals must search for a hidden platform to escape
OR "spatial learning" OR "associative learning" OR "long-term memory" the maze. It also has a hippocampal independent variant, using a visible
OR "pattern recognition" OR "object recognition")(placebo OR acute OR platform, that assesses cue learning.
challenge OR depletion)) NOT (review) NOT (disease). A filter to select
only studies with humans was applied for both databases. 3.1.1. Lowering or depleting 5-HT centrally and globally
2. This search strategy was slightly adapted to obtain equivalent Common approaches for lowering central 5-HT include nutritional
studies for non-human species: ((serotonin OR 5-HT OR serotonergic) restrictions and chemical agents. Dietary 5-HT deprivation, for instance,
("declarative memory" OR "episodic memory" OR "semantic memory" is carried out by substituting nutrition with a drink that contains a
OR "object recognition" OR "object memory" OR "visual memory" OR mixture of large neutral amino acids but lacks the 5-HT precursor L-
"contextual fear conditioning" OR "trace conditioning" OR "spatial tryptophan (TRP). This prevents 5-HT production in brainstem neurons
memory" OR "spatial learning" OR "associative learning" OR "long-term and results in a short-term and reversible reduction of cerebral 5-HT
memory")(placebo OR vehicle OR acute OR challenge OR depletion OR (Hood et al., 2005; Van Donkelaar et al., 2011; Young et al., 1989).
deficiency)(animal)) NOT (review) NOT (model of schizophrenia) NOT Investigations on spatial memory indicated, that acute or chronic (~40
(model of depression) NOT (model of anxiety) NOT (disease). Like for days) nutritional TRP restriction does not affect task performance in the
humans, PubMed (all years, 191 results, 21.3.2022) and Embase (all MWM (Lieben et al., 2004; Liu et al., 2013; Uchida et al., 2007) and also
years, 103 results, 22.3.2022) have been searched. No additional filters not in a radial arm maze (Stancampiano et al., 1997). In line with this,
were applied. Since for humans the terms "deficiency", "vehicle", and neither the chemical induced attenuation of 5-HT synthesis by the spe­
"trace conditioning" did not yield any additional matching results, they cific and irreversible tryptophan hydroxylase inhibitor p-chlor­
were omitted from the search term. ophenylalanine (PCPA; 300 mg/kg single injection or 400
mg/kg/day×3) nor the serotonergic neurotoxin 5,7-dihydroxytrypt­
2.3. Inclusion criteria amine (5,7-DHT; 150 μg), which selectively destroys 5-HT axons, had
any effect on performance in hippocampus-dependent maze learning
Studies were identified which matched our definition of declarative tasks (Lehmann et al., 2000; Majlessi et al., 2003; Nilsson et al., 1988;
memories and then grouped according to population, intervention types, Richter-Levin and Segal, 1989). Thus, hippocampal-dependent allo­
underlying serotonergic mechanisms of actions and form of memory centric spatial memory was not affected across several studies as well as
before summarization. different procedures applied for central 5-HT depletion.

Fig. 1. PRISMA flow diagram, based on inclusion and exclusion criteria.


3
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

Contrasting results were obtained after repeated 3,4-methylene­ Moreover, suppression of theta, regulation of hippocampal ripple os­
dioxy-N-methylamphetamine (MDMA) intoxication. Although acutely cillations as well as memory consolidation were demonstrated to be
given MDMA stimulates the release of 5-HT, its repeated administration crucially modulated by MR 5-HT neuronal activation (Wang et al.,
within a short time frame inhibits 5-HT synthesis and therefore provokes 2015).
5-HT depletion (Lyles and Cadet, 2003). Such a regimen (15 mg/kg x 4
doses MDMA in one day) caused deficits in the Cincinnati water maze 3.1.3. Enhancement of 5-HT globally
and, to a lesser extent, long-term impairments in an MWM reversal Acutely administered selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs)
learning task (Able et al., 2006).1 block 5-HT reuptake and increase its levels in the extracellular space
We briefly mention some factors that may account to those contra­ (Walker, 2013). Regarding spatial memory, SSRI administration was
dictory outcomes between MDMA and other 5-HT depletion methods. robustly associated with performance deteriorations. Acute dosage with
The Cincinnati water maze, which involves egocentric route-based citalopram (10 and 20 mg/kg) before MWM training caused spatial
learning, as well as the MWM reversal learning task are both sug­ reference memory deficits (Mutlu et al., 2011) and pretraining cit­
gested to rely on the striatum (Braun et al., 2015; Castañé Anna et al., alopram (5 and 10 mg/kg) impaired long-term retention in the Y-maze
2010). By contrast, allocentric learning in the MWM reference memory test (Bridoux et al., 2013). Further, a dose-dependency in the effects of
version is assumed to mainly rely on the dorsal hippocampus (Miyoshi SSRI on maze-learning was revealed. Accordingly, MWM acquisition
et al., 2012; Morris et al., 1982). Able et al. (2006) reported a decrease of and navigation were impaired after pretraining treatment with cit­
dopamine (DA) in the striatum indicating a contribution of dopami­ alopram (4 and 8 mg/kg), fluoxetine (8 and 16 mg/kg) or alaproclate
nergic interactions in MDMA-induced egocentric spatial memory im­ (7.5 and 20 mg/kg), whereby lower doses had no impact (Majlessi and
pairments. A similar pattern was found in adult rodents receiving Naghdi, 2002; Riekkinen et al., 1991). The distinction between acute
chronical MDMA treatment during development (Williams et al., 2003). and chronic administration does not seem to be necessarily important, as
In a further egocentric spatial navigation task, facilitated task acquisi­ a consistent effect was still replicated after 4 weeks of fluoxetine (0.7
tion after selective striatal 5-HT depletion (using 5,7 DHT; 25 μg) was mg/kg per day) treatment (Ampuero et al., 2013).
observed and this effect was abolished by DA antagonists (SCH-23 390
or spiperone) (Anguiano-Rodríguez et al., 2007). This implies that the 3.1.4. 5-HT1A/1BR
deterioration in spatial performance may be mediated by a concomitant The 5-HT1AR is one of the most studied serotonergic receptors to date
influence on striatal DA tone following MDMA-induced 5-HT depletion (e.g., Glikmann-Johnston et al., 2015). There exists strong evidence for
with egocentric learning being more severely affected. 5-HT1AR involvement in spatial memory. When animals had to perform
an Atlantis Water maze, pro-cognitive effects during encoding were
3.1.2. 5-HT depletion in selective regions found after dosing with a selective and silent 5-HT1AR antagonist
Selective depletion of 5-HT from the medial septum (MS)/diagonal (WAY-101 405; 1, 3, 10 mg/kg) (Hirst et al., 2008). In another study,
band of the brocca or from the hippocampus facilitated spatial task however, the 5-HT1AR antagonists NAD-299 (0.05, 0.15, 0.5 and 1.5
acquisition in the Morris water maze (Gutiérrez-Guzmán et al., 2011, mg/kg) and WAY-100 635 (0.3 and 1 mg/kg) failed to alter MWM
2017; Hernández-Pérez et al., 2015). For this, 5,7-DHT was injected into performance (Lüttgen et al., 2005).
the target region and in addition, hippocampal electroencephalography Agonistic 5-HT1AR ligands yielded mixed and dose-dependent re­
(EEG) was measured during task performance. Improved spatial task sults. As such, spatial memory was enhanced following a low dose of 8-
acquisition was accompanied by earlier expression of dominant high OH-DPAT (0.3 mg/kg) but deteriorated after a high dose of 8-OH-DPAT
frequency theta oscillations during training and increased theta (1.0 mg/kg) (Haider et al., 2012). Dose dependent impairments were
coupling coherence between the MS-hippocampus and the also found in other rat strains (Lüttgen et al., 2005). The selective
MS-mammillary nuclei. Theta oscillations are critical for spatial cogni­ infusion of 8-OH-DPAT (0.5 μg) into the MS worsened MWM perfor­
tion, serving the integration of information from separate functional mance (Bertrand et al., 2000) but deficits were reversed by injecting
units during mnemonic processes (Korotkova et al., 2018). Accordingly, WAY-100 635 at the same site. Interestingly, the variation of the time­
in a state of low central 5-HT, facilitated communication between points when infusing 8-OH-DPAT into the MS (pre-training,
task-involved regions could promote information processing. Indeed, post-training, 2 h after training, before probe-trial) unveils, that
spatial encoding was improved, primarily during the initial phase of 5-HT1AR of the MS contribute to a 5-HT-mediated mechanism involved
learning, as expressed in behaviour (Gutiérrez-Guzmán et al., 2011, in encoding and in consolidation, but not retrieval of spatial
2017). Hippocampal theta involvement was thereby specific for spatial hippocampal-dependent knowledge. Specifically, 8-OH-DPAT infusion
reference memory necessitating the use of cognitive maps, whereas cue into the MS hindered encoding and the protein synthesis-dependent,
learning remained unaffected (Olvera-Cortés et al., 2002). late-phase consolidation processes (Koenig et al., 2008). The effect
However, when depleting 5-HT from the supra-mammillary nucleus was suggested to be mediated through an 8-OH-DPAT-induced hyper­
(SUM), inefficient MWM performance was evident. Hippocampal high- polarisation of cholinergic and/or GABAergic and/or glutamatergic
frequency theta was abolished during MWM training and facilitated neurons in the MS-hippocampal neuronal pathway (Bertrand et al.,
MS-hippocampal theta coherence was absent in SUM lesioned animals 2000; Koenig et al., 2011).
(Hernández-Pérez et al., 2015). A participation of the SUM in retention Consequences of persistent alterations in 5-HT1AR expression were
of spatial reference memory was suggested (Aranda et al., 2008; Shahidi investigated in transgenetic mouse models. 5HT1AR – knockout (KO)
et al., 2004). Based on those findings, the authors proposed changes in mice exhibited spatial memory impairments but intriguingly, this was
theta frequency as an operating mechanism by which the hippocampus only obtained in tasks requiring the integrity of hippocampal functions
switches between acquisition and retrieval states. In this respect, (Sarnyai et al., 2000). As contributing factors, the absence of 5-HT1A
encoding of environmental information is supposed to be mainly per­ autoreceptors (Wolff et al., 2004) and/or long-term developmental
formed by the dorsal (in animals) or posterior (in humans) hippocam­ plastic adaptions such as altered hippocampal CA1 GABAergic trans­
pus, which in turn is strongly innervated by MR originating, beaded type mission (Sarnyai et al., 2000) were suggested. Spatial performance of
M serotonergic axons (Freund et al., 1990; Wilson and Molliver, 1991). 5-HT1AR KO seems to be age-dependent, since deficits were obvious in
young adult (3 months) but not older (22 months) 5-HT1AR KO mice.
This is possibly attributable to an age associated compensation of
1
MDMA studies with mice are omitted in this review given that MDMA is reduced cholinergic activity by the 5-HT1AR KO mutation (Wolff et al.,
mainly neurotoxic for the dopamine system instead of the 5-HT system in mice, 2004). Likewise, elevations of hippocampal acetylcholine after acute
which is in contrast to other species (Logan et al., 1988). dosage of WAY-101 405 (10 mg/kg) have been reported (Hirst et al.,

4
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

2008). sub-chronic treatment (13 days) with the 5-HT6R antagonist RO-4 368
Overexpression of 5-HT1AR did not affect spatial learning. 8-OH- 554 (3 mg/kg per day) did not yield any effects on spatial cognition
DPAT (0.3 mg/kg) administration in transgenetic mice, however, (Gyertyán et al., 2020). Nevertheless, another investigation pinpointed
resulted in a higher sensitivity for MWM task failures. Those might be the cognition-enhancing effects of the 5-HT6R antagonist (SB-27 104
mediated by the surplus of postsynaptic heteroreceptors in projection 610 or 20 mg/kg per day for 40 days) on the acquisition and recall of a
areas of 5-HT neurons such as the hippocampus and outer cortical layers spatial learning task following chronic drug administration in a popu­
(Bert et al., 2009). lation of aged rats (Foley et al., 2004). Together, 5-HT6R antagonists
There are only two studies on 5-HT1BRs and their role in spatial might still be an interesting target for cognitive enhancement and
memory. Both investigations observed improved long-term performance memory improvement especially in some age-related dysfunctions.
in 5-HT1BR KO mice which was specifically evident in the hippocampus-
dependent reference version of the MWM, but not in the cue-guided 3.1.9. 5-HT7R
visual version (Buhot et al., 2003; Malleret et al., 1999). This effect In 5-HT7R KO mice, long-term memory formation, memory consol­
was stronger at increased task complexity and even persisted with older idation, and memory retrieval of a Barnes maze were unaffected, sug­
age. Those findings indicate pro-cognitive influences of 5-HT1BR genetic gesting that this receptor is not critical for spatial memory in general.
deletion on spatial memory and even a protective effect on age-related However, 5-HT7R KO mice were less efficient in accommodating to
hippocampal-dependent memory decline. changes in spatial arrangement as evident in reversal learning. When
changing the location of the escape hole, 5-HT7R KO mice displayed
3.1.5. 5-HT2AR perseverative behavior. Therefore, a role for the 5-HT7R in the adjust­
Activation of 5-HT2ARs was associated with impaired spatial memory ment of neuronal networks to changes in environments was suggested.
encoding and retrieval. Acute dosing with the 5-HT2AR agonist TCB-2 (1 In line with this, the ability to perform a switch from a striatal-
mg/kg) before the probe trial delayed the initiation of spatial search dependent egocentric cognitive strategy towards a hippocampus-
behavior, which is required to escape the MWM in the reference version, dependent allocentric cognitive strategy while solving a spatial task
without affecting cue memory. In addition, those findings were was reduced (Roberts et al., 2004; Sarkisyan and Hedlund, 2009).
accompanied by diminished long-term stability of hippocampal CA1
place fields especially for the novel environment, but not for pre-existing 3.2. Summary
place cell firing maps. Those effects were prevented by the 5-HT2AR
antagonist MDL 11 939 (0.5 mg/kg) (Zhang et al., 2017). Similar find­ In general, global lowering of 5-HT levels did not affect spatial
ings were obtained using the 5-HT2AR agonistic acting substance psilo­ learning except when using MDMA as depleting agent. Sub-chronic
cin, which impaired encoding in the Carousel maze (1 and 4 mg/kg) and MDMA administration might particularly affect spatial tasks whose
retrieval in the MWM (4 mg/kg) after previous drugfree acquisition performance is additionally modulated by the striatal DA tonus. Global
(Rambousek et al., 2014). Blockade of hippocampal CA1 5HT2A/2 CRs by enhancement of 5-HT by SSRIs worsened spatial performance. Inter­
ritanserin (4 μg) improved learning in spatial version of MWM. Ac­ estingly, the serotonergic tonus in the SUM might be important for
cording to the authors, ritanserin might act on GABA interneurons and memory consolidation. By contrast, high levels of 5-HT in the MS
disinhibit cholinergic transmission and/or prevent 5-HT induced inhi­ hampered encoding/consolidation but low levels supported encoding.
bition on CA1 pyramidal neurons (Naghdi and Harooni, 2005). In line MS 5-HT levels were additionally related to increases in theta frequency
with this finding, no navigational deficits were found in 5-HT2AR KO and coherence suggesting a role for 5-HT as hippocampal theta modu­
mice in several spatial memory tasks (Morici et al., 2015). lator. Studies investigating specific receptors revealed mostly conflicting
results, but acute administration of 5-HT6R antagonists might improve
3.1.6. 5-HT3R spatial learning performance.
The blockade of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neuron 5HT3Rs by
granisetron (0.25 μg) caused impairments in the MWM (Naghdi and 3.3. Object recognition
Harooni, 2005). Another investigation, however, found spatial naviga­
tion being improved after application of the 5-HT3R antagonist ondan­ Novel object recognition is usually assessed by letting the animal
setron (doses between 100 and 1000 μg/kg) (Stäubli and Xu, 1995). deeply explore two objects (sample session) and afterwards, replacing
one object with a new object (test session). Rodents have a natural pro­
3.1.7. 5-HT4R clivity to explore novel, non-threatening objects and therefore, the time
There is one investigation on 5-HT4R signalling providing evidence spent exploring the new object in comparison to the old object is
for a bidirectional impact on memory formation. Accordingly, the considered as valid measurement for novelty recognition (Cohen and
agonist BIMU8 (30 mg/kg) enhanced plasticity on the Schaffer collat­ Stackman, 2015). Although the hippocampus and amygdala were once
erals and improved performance in an MWM. The specific antagonist thought to be critical contributors to object recognition memory, recent
GR125487 (10 mg/kg), on the other hand, impaired synaptic potentia­ systematic studies have revealed the greater importance of temporal
tion and in consequence, spatial memory was attenuated (Teixeira et al., cortical areas, with particular emphasis on perirhinal cortex. While the
2018). hippocampus clearly contributes to the performance of object recogni­
tion tasks under certain, as yet not fully understood conditions, it does
3.1.8. 5-HT6R not appear to be required for the familiarity-based recognition of object
5-HT6R antagonists produced beneficial effects on spatial learning information per se (Winters et al., 2008).
(Da Silva Costa et al., 2008: SB-27 104 610 mg/kg; Rogers and Hagan,
2001: SB-271 046-A or SB-357 134-A, 10 mg/kg). Acute dosage pro­ 3.3.1. Lowering or depleting 5-HT globally
longed the retention MWM reference memory (Rogers and Hagan, 2001) In contrast to in spatial learning, global cerebral serotonin deficiency
and further, improved acquisition and consolidation of spatial recogni­ was quite consistently related to object recognition impairments. A
tion in the Y-maze in young adult mice. In addition, 5-HT6R antagonism bunch of evidence using acute TRP depletion observed object recogni­
counteracted age-related consolidation deficits of spatial recognition tion impairments, that have been evident 2 – 6 h after the first treatment
memory in older mice (18–21 months) (Da Silva Costa et al., 2008; Foley (Jans et al., 2007, 2009, 2010; Lieben et al., 2004, 2005a; Olivier et al.,
et al., 2004). In another examination, Long-Evans rats (2 years old) were 2008; Rutten et al., 2007; van Donkelaar et al., 2008). Similar findings
tested in a clinical trial-like design. For this, a “patient population” was were obtained after 21 days of a TRP-free diet (Jenkins et al., 2010). In
created by increasing the difficulty of the task step-by-step. In this, addition, TRP depletion decreased 5-HT levels in brain structures that

5
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

incorporate a crucial role in memory, such as the hippocampus and the object recognition performance. In this regard, fluoxetine (10 mg/kg)
frontal cortex (Jans et al., 2007; Jenkins et al., 2010; Olivier et al., was demonstrated to increase the extracellular 5-HT concentrations, e.g.
2008). The observed effects of TRP depletion were even more pro­ in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), and impaired performance of
nounced in rodents with pre-existing abnormal 5-HT function (Olivier mice in the object recognition 24 h post-administration (Castañé et al.,
et al., 2008). Importantly, sex and estrous cycle phase were demon­ 2015; Flores-Burgess et al., 2019).
strated to influence the behavioral response to TRP depletion whereby
females in pro-estrus/estrus exhibited the strongest object recognition 3.3.3. 5-HT1A/1BR
deficits after acute TRP depletion (Jans et al., 2007). Notably, a possible A role of 5-HT1ARs in object recognition was pointed out. In this
strain dependence of animals in TRP effects was suggested as well (Jans regard, the treatment with 5-HT1AR antagonists (WAY-101 405: 1.0–10
et al., 2010). mg/kg or WAY-100 635: 1.0 mg/kg) prolonged memory span in object
Those findings were not obtained in two examinations using other recognition up to 48 h (Hirst et al., 2008; Pitsikas et al., 2003). These
chemical agents for cerebral 5-HT depletion. One study applied a sub­ promoting effects were observed regardless of the time of administra­
cutaneous p-chloroamphetamine (PCA) regimen (5 mg/kg) and tested tion, which took place either before the acquisition, storage, or retrieval.
animals in an object recognition task one week later. Object recognition Accordingly, 5-HT1AR antagonists likely restore time-related object
remained intact, even though SERT binding in the hippocampus and recognition deficits overall and not specific to memory phases (Pitsikas
perirhinal cortex was decreased in depleted animals (Belcher et al., et al., 2003). To determine the unique contribution of 5-HT1A autor­
2005). In another investigation repeated MDMA treatment (15 mg/kg x eceptors and heteroreceptors on object recognition, Van Goethem et al.
4 doses in one day) produced substantial reductions of 5-HT and 5-HIAA (2015) used ‘biased agonists’ which preferentially activate only one of
concentrations in the whole brain including the hippocampus and the those subtypes. They challenged mechanisms of hippocampal pattern
prefrontal cortex. Nonetheless, object recognition performance was not separation with an object-in-context recognition version of object
impaired (Able et al., 2006). By contrast, (Morley et al., 2001) compared recognition. The results described a bell-shaped dose response curve for
a high dose MDMA regimen (4 ×5 mg/kg, over 4 h on each of 2 both agents. Activation of postsynaptic 5-HT1A heteroreceptors (with
consecutive days) with a moderate dose regime of MDMA (1 ×5 mg/kg F15599) positively affected object recognition at a midrange dose (0.04
on each of 2 consecutive days). Animals were then tested in a novel mg/kg), whereas higher and lower doses tended to impair performance.
object recognition task 14 weeks after drug administration. After 15 min Activation of 5-HT1A autoreceptors in raphe (with F13714) impaired
retention, deficits were evident in the high dose group. The authors of object recognition at concentrations of 0.0025 or 0.01 mg/kg, with no
this study considered neurotoxic effects due to the high MDMA dosage as effects at other doses. Together, those findings indicate that object
an explanatory factor. recognition performance might benefit from activation of 5-HT1A het­
Object recognition was also impaired in a hypo-serotonergic mouse eroreceptors (Van Goethem et al., 2015). On the contrary, the genetic
model (Pet1KO) at delayed recalls (90 min and 2 h). Measurements of deletion of 5-HT1BRs did not change object recognition performance
field excitatory postsynaptic potential (fEPSP) at Schaffer collaterals (Malleret et al., 1999).
during task performance revealed exaggerated LTP mechanisms in
Pet1KO mice compared to a control group. Increases in fEPSP ampli­ 3.3.4. 5-HT2A/2 CR
tudes were observed directly after training and were even strikingly Systemic 5-HT2AR activation (TCB-2; 1.0 mg/kg) enhanced the
larger after 5 h. To note, no alterations in hippocampal theta or gamma consolidation of object memory. The strongest impact on object recog­
oscillations were found in this study. Further, novel object recognition nition performance was reported when the agent was given directly after
deficits have been restored by increasing global 5-HT levels through object memory acquisition. Pre-treatment with a 5-HT2AR antagonist
repeated administration of a combination of 5-HTP (10 mg/kg) and (MDL 11 939) diminished this effect. Since memory consolidation in its
benserazide (20 mg/kg) while training and before testing. Accordingly, initial stages is thought to start right after learning, this finding suggests
the non-specific strengthening of synapses after object recognition that TCB-2 likely strengthened the nascent object memory (Stackman
acquisition in the CA1 region may occlude the correct formation of et al., 2013). In line with this, activation of 5-HT2AR was associated with
object memory traces, which emphasizes that serotonergic inhibitory increases of extracellular glutamate efflux and facilitated induction of
control over the hippocampal formation is fundamental to normal object synaptic plasticity (Zhang et al., 2016).
memory functions during consolidation. In a subsequent examination of The ability to remember the spatial context or the temporal order of
the same experimental setting, the authors demonstrated that neural objects was demonstrated to rely more heavily on the hippocampus than
activity of the MR is required for encoding/consolidation but not recall the usually applied simple object recognition procedure (Balderas et al.,
of object memory. This was shown by selective blockade of either MR of 2008; Forwood et al., 2005). To challenge hippocampal functions as
DR activity with reversible designer drugs (DREADDs) induced chemo­ such, animals were trained to remember object-in-context combination
genetic inhibition during task performance (Fernandez et al., 2017). or the temporal-order-of objects, that necessitate the recollection of
Overall, a bunch of evidence reliably related acute lowering of 5-HT conjunctive ‘what’ or ‘when’ and ‘which context’ representations at test.
levels by TRP depletion, but not more direct 5-HT depletion methods, to Specific and retrieval associated deficits for object-in-context recogni­
object recognition impairments. We will briefly mention some possible tion were found after the selective blockade of 5-HT2ARs (MDL 11 939)
reasons for the discrepancies reported beyond depletion regimens. In in the mPFC (Bekinschtein et al., 2013) and the same findings were
this, strain specific variations might play a role. For example, Able et al. obtained in 5-HT2AR KO mice (Barre et al., 2016; Morici et al., 2015),
(2006) and Belcher et al. (2005) used Sprague-Dawley rats for their whereas the conventionally applied object recognition procedure
examinations. In contrast to other strains (e.g. Morley et al., 2001), remained unimpaired (see also Castañé et al., 2015). Their role in
Sprague-Dawley rats have been found to be less vulnerable to 5-HT retrieval-related deficits was supposed to be mediated by imbalances of
associated object recognition deficits before (Jans et al., 2010). More­ 5-HT concentrations in the mPFC. A detrimental excess of 5-HT is likely
over, Able et al. (2006) used a rather short retention time (1 h), due to increased 5-HT1AR activation, as observed in the absence of
compared to other studies (e.g. Lieben et al., 2004, 2005b [4 h, 24 h]; 5-HT2AR signalling (Castañé et al., 2015; Morici et al., 2015).
Rutten et al., 2007 [3 h]; Jans et al., 2009 [2 h, 4 h, 6 h]). This pinpoints Prefrontal-hippocampal circuit are essential for the encoding and
a role of 5-HT specifically in late phase consolidation processes. retrieval of declarative memories, and the specific role of 5-HT2ARs
herein was further pinpointed. Bekinschtein et al. (2013) injected rats
3.3.2. Enhancement of 5-HT globally either ipsilateral, contralateral, or bilateral with the 5-HT2AR antagonist
In line with the experimental findings on spatial learning, the acute MDL 11 939 in the mPFC and/or muscimol in the hippocampus. They
increase of central 5-HT tonus by SSRI treatment resulted in worse found that both, the 5-HT2ARs in the mPFC and 5-HT2AR activity in the

6
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

hippocampus are necessary for the correct solution of the HT6Rs on glutamatergic and/or cholinergic neurones whereas antago­
object-in-context task. In addition, contralateral infusion of both agents nists attenuate active serotonergic input to upstream inhibitory
resulted in performance deficits, whereas ipsilateral infusions, which GABAergic neurones, which in turn disinhibit glutamate and/or
leave the more dominant interactions between the mPFC and the hip­ acetylcholine release. Thus, both agents may elevate glutamate and/or
pocampus intact (Churchwell and Kesner, 2011), had no impact. acetylcholine release and augment object memory albeit via distinct
Crucially, the restauration of object-in-context recognition performance mechanisms of action (Kendall et al., 2011).
in 5-HT2AR KO mice was possible using a viral gene-delivery approach.
After the re-expression of 5-HT2ARs at presynaptic sites in the medi­ 3.3.7. 5-HT7R
odorsal thalamus, no more differences to wild-type mice have been A role in novelty detection for 5-HT7Rs was indicated. Accordingly,
evident (Barre et al., 2016). Morici et al. (2015) demonstrated a role for both pharmacological blockade (SB-269 970; 10–15 mg/kg) and genetic
mPFC 5-HT2ARs in the selection of the correct memory trace during inactivation resulted in a lack of behavioral response to novel objects
retrieval. Together, these findings strikingly suggest a necessary and novel environments without altering habituation behavior. In
engagement of 5-HT2ARs in prefrontal-hippocampal circuits, incorpo­ addition, the tendency to actively seek novelty was inversely correlated
rating a role specific to the retrieval of contextual elements in episodic with 5-HT7R gene expression in pivotal areas for information trafficking,
memories. such as thalamo-cortical projection areas and the dorsal hippocampus
Pro-cognitive effects in object recognition were also demonstrated (Ballaz et al., 2007; Sarkisyan and Hedlund, 2009). On the other hand,
after selective blockade of 5-HT2 CRs. Specifically, SB 242 084 (0.63, 2.5 5-HT7R activation LP-211 (0.25 mg kg-1) improved the consolidation of
and 10 mg/kg) prolonged object recognition memory duration up to 24 chamber-shape memories after the sample session. This was evident in
h (Bertaina-Anglade et al., 2011). the subsequent novelty-preference test where ameliorated chamber
recognition memory was observed that persisted up to 24 h (Beaudet
3.3.5. 5-HT4R et al., 2017).
Systemic activation of 5-HT4Rs yielded beneficial effects on object
memory. In general, 5-HT4Rs have been shown to improve acquisition 3.4. Summary
and consolidation in several mnemonic domains (e.g. Hagena and
Manahan-Vaughan, 2017), which also applies to object recognition Global 5-HT lowering or elevation impaired object memory, except
tasks. Acute and sub-chronic activation (14 days) of the 5-HT4R with the in two studies where MDMA or PCA, respectively, was administered to
partial agonist RS-67 333 extended memory traces in the novel object achieve central depletion. 5-HT1AR and 5-HT2 CR antagonists prolonged
recognition task. This was suggested to result from a functional inter­ the memory of the object unspecific to any memory phase. 5-HT2AR, 5-
action between the cholinergic and serotonergic system, which is HT4R and possibly 5-HT7R agonists as well as 5-HT6R ligands enhanced
mediated by the nucleus basalis magnocellularis (NBM). Regarding this, its acquisition and/or consolidation. Concordantly, blockade of 5-
the local intra-NBM infusion of RS-67 333 enhanced the acquisition and HT2ARs worsened object memory and underlined the involvement of 5-
the consolidation of place recognition memory but did not affect recall. HT2ARs in hippocampal - prefrontal circuits as modulators of synaptic
These effects were reversed by pre-treatment with the selective 5-HT4R plasticity and memory consolidation.
antagonist RS-39 604 (Orsetti et al., 2003; Quiedeville et al., 2015).
3.5. Contextual fear conditioning
3.3.6. 5-HT6R
Evidence of 5-HT6R on object recognition revealed mixed results, but Contextual fear conditioning can be described as a type of associative
quite a bunch of studies reported prolongations of object memory after learning that involves a strong emotional arousal due to the sudden
treatments with 5-HT6R ligands. Acute dosage with 5-HT6R antagonists application of a painful stimulus in a specific spatial context. Contextual
(Ro 04–6790 and SB-271 046; both: 10 mg/kg) shortly before, or fear conditioning accounts for an episodic-like memory in its extended
directly after the object recognition sample session, improved object definition, since successful learning and retrieval can be inferred from
memory acquisition and/or consolidation and prolonged the duration of behavioral expressions referring to the content (what), place (where),
the memory (up to 4 h). No effects were evident when the administration and temporal context (sequence of events) (Pause et al., 2013). It re­
took place before the test session (Kendall et al., 2011; King et al., 2004, quires the normal function of the hippocampus, where the contextual
2009). Similar findings where obtained after 14 days sub-chronic element is stored, and the amygdala. Cued fear conditioning instead
antagonistic treatments (Mitchell et al., 2009: Ro4368554 [5 mg/kg]; builds an associative memory between a tone and fear, it requires
Quiedeville et al., 2015: SB-271 046 [10 mg/kg]). In addition, normal function of the amygdala only (Kim and Fanselow, 1992). While
Ro4368554 (3 mg/kg or 10 mg/kg) was able to reverse object recog­ the former is dependent on a specific context (place) as a conditioned
nition retention deficits within 1 h delay in a serotonergic deficiency stimulus for fear induction, the latter is context-independent in the sense
model (TRP-restriction regimen) (Lieben et al., 2005a). Those beneficial that the cuing stimulus triggers fear behaviour no matter where it is
effects on object recognition were suggested to be mediated via MR perceived.
innervated brain regions. Accordingly, the selective lesioning of the MR
– but not the DR – completely abolished the effects of a 5-HT6R antag­ 3.5.1. Lowering or depleting 5-HT globally
onist (Ro 04–679 010 mg/kg) (King et al., 2009). However, it is worth Some studies obtained contextual fear conditioning impairments
mentioning that together, the abovementioned enhancing effects on after acute central 5-HT depletion that were evident in reduced freezing
retention were rather short-lived and absent with longer inter-trial in­ behavior of rodents at the test session. It was thereby irrelevant, whether
tervals (up to 24 h) (Gyertyán et al., 2020; Lieben et al., 2005b) or 5,7-DHT (25 mg/kg) (Matsumoto et al., 2004) or TRP dietary restriction
non-evident within a very short inter-trial time interval (1 min)(Woolley (Uchida et al., 2007) was applied as depleting compound. Contextual
et al., 2003). fear conditioning deficits were apparent 2 h, 24 h, and 3 weeks after
At first glance, it might be paradoxical, that agonistic ligands such as acquisition. No behavioral alterations have been obtained during
E-6801 (5 or 10 mg/kg) and EMD-386 088 (10 mg/kg) were shown to training and 30 min afterwards at the first test session, indicating that
enhance object recognition memory as well. This was true, even when 5-HT deficiency did not affect contextual fear conditioning acquisition
given together with sub-effective dose of the 5-HT6R antagonist SB-271 or baseline anxiety levels. Crucially, animals did also not differ in a cued
046. The closer inspection on this phenomenon revealed distinct con­ fear conditioning paradigm, where – as mentioned above - the
tributions of agonistic and antagonistic agents to object recognition. One hippocampal-dependent processing of the contextual component is
plausible explanation is, that agonists directly amplify the action of 5- lacking (Uchida et al., 2007). Taken together, those findings suggest that

7
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

acute central 5-HT deficiency might impair the hippocampal-dependent, electrophysiological baseline activity of fimbria-CA3 synaptic efficacy
but not the amygdala-dependent, processes that are involved in the and furthermore, when animals where re-exposed to the fear context
consolidation of contextual fear memory. after successful extinction training, fear learning potentiation was sup­
Contrasting results were obtained in mice with a constitutive lack of pressed. This finding suggests a protective effect of fluoxetine on
central 5-HT. Such mice were generated, for example, by a region- conditioned fear and stress-induced disturbances of hippocampal plas­
specific conditional KO of the transcription factor Lmx1B and conse­ ticity (Spennato et al., 2008).
quently, those mice are lacking essentially all central 5-HT neurons and
5-HT levels are decreased to a level of < 10%. Another approach is the 3.5.3. 5-HT1A/1BR
targeted inactivation of the neuronal tryptophan hydroxylase-2 (Tph2) During stressful conditions, protective functions of endogenous 5-HT
gene. Such Tph2 null mutant (Tph2 − /− ) mice lack the capability to were shown, by decreasing the firing activity of hippocampal CA1 py­
synthesize 5-HT specifically in the brain (Gutknecht et al., 2009). Both ramidal neurons (McEwen and Magarinos, 1997). The 5-HT1AR antag­
variants of genetic manipulations in mice provoked greatly enhanced onists WAY-100 635 (0.2 mg/kg) and WAY-100 135 (5 mg/kg)
contextual fear memory. Tph2 − /− mice displayed a faster acquisition abolished the decrease in firing frequency during conditioning, yet
of conditioned fear, as well as enhanced contextual representation of the without affecting contextual conditioning-induced freezing behavior at
fear memory, but normal functional extinction of the context (Gutknecht retrieval (Tada et al., 2004). Congruently, the selective intra­
et al., 2015; Waider et al., 2019). In conditional KO mice, exaggerated hippocampal activation of postsynaptic 5-HT1AR by injection of flesi­
contextual fear memory was diminished after injecting 5-HT into the noxan (3 μg) reduced the expression of conditioned fear (Li et al., 2006).
lateral ventricles or by administration of the 5-HT1AR agonist 8-OH-D­ Moreover, the MR – hippocampal pathway was associated with reduced
PAT (Dai et al., 2008). The examination of immediate-early gene fear responses evoked by contextual but not acoustic sensory stimuli.
expression (c-Fos imaging) and electrophysiological recordings in the This finding was obtained after selective 8-OH-DPAT microinjection into
dorsal hippocampus (dHip) of Tph2 − /− mice emphasized that lifelong the MR (1 μg/0.5 μl saline), which underlines the involvement of
5-HT deficiency renders the dHip hyperexcitable in fear conditioning 5-HT1ARs during the process of memory formation of conditioning fear
(Waider et al., 2019). These observations are in accordance with the associated with aversive contexts (Avanzi and Brandão, 2001).
finding of exaggerated LTP at Schaffer Collaterals after high-frequency The one investigation on 5-HT1BR KO mice revealed no alterations of
stimulation in the 5-HT deficient Pet1KO mouse model (Fernandez contextual fear conditioning compared to wild-type controls (Malleret
et al., 2017). In this sense, exaggerated context-dependent fear memory et al., 1999).
likely involves dysfunctional raphe-hippocampal 5-HT innervation,
which may result in the failure of 5-HT receptor-mediated inhibition of 3.5.4. 5-HT2AR
hippocampal circuitries. Of note, female Tph2 − /− mice, in contrast to Stimulation of 5HT2A was related to facilitated consolidation and
males, displayed increased stress vulnerability and depression-like extinction of fear memories in a trace fear conditioning task. Trace fear
behavior, suggesting additional sex-hormone-dependent interactions conditioning demands the CS and the US to be associated over a
between female and male behavioral responses to central 5-HT defi­ stimulus-free interval known as the “trace” and are thereby thought to
ciency (Gutknecht et al., 2015). In sum, the abovementioned results be functionally and morphologically incorporated by hippocampal
support the view of a direct relationship between 5-HT levels and neurons. It was found that post-conditioning activation of 5-HT2ARs by
contextual fear memory. However, whereas in the case of a chronic 5-HT TCB-2 (1 mg/kg) enhanced consolidation of the fear memory, whereas
deficit since birth, contextual fear memory was exaggerated, the oppo­ post-conditioning blockade of 5-HT2ARs by MDL 11 939 (0.5 mg/kg)
site was true in case of an acutely induced deficit by 5-HT depletion, yielded a tendency towards less freezing. Both ligands had no effects
which specifically deteriorated the storage of the contextual element in when administered right before encoding or retrieval (Stackman et al.,
the hippocampus. Yet, under both conditions, cued fear conditioning 2013). 5-HT2AR activation induced facilitation in fear condition, was
remained unaffected (Fernandez et al., 2017; Uchida et al., 2007), this further shown in cued and Pavlovian eyeblink conditioning, indicating
was also true when using 5,7-DHT for central 5-HT deficiency induction that 5-HT2AR activation is not specifically related to the contextual
(Romano et al., 2006). element but to associative learning (Romano et al., 2006). In addition,
dosing animals with TCB-2 (1.0 mg/kg) before training facilitated the
3.5.2. Enhancement of 5-HT globally extinction of the non-hippocampus-dependent cued delay fear memory
Acute or chronic SSRI treatment before contextual fear conditioning and by contrast, MDL 11 939 (0.5 mg/kg) delayed the recovery of
training revealed mixed behavioral responses. Escitalopram 1 mg/kg freezing in mice (Stackman et al., 2013). Taken together, reported
(but not 0.5 and 5 mg/kg) given 30 min before training increased studies indicate a rather general role for 5-HT2AR in fear memory
freezing time in the retention test 24 h after acquisition. However, when consolidation and extinction. Nonetheless, it is conceivable that
escitalopram 1 mg/kg (but not 0.5 and 5 mg/kg) was administered the 5-HT2AR activation facilitates hippocampal glutamate transmission
next day after acquisition, 30 min before the test session, reduced (Zhang and Stackman, 2015b), thereby promoting the information
freezing time was evident. To elucidate the effect on memory consoli­ transmission and retention in mnemonic circuits in support of consoli­
dation, escitalopram (5 mg/kg; but not 0.5 and 1 mg/kg) was given dation including the hippocampus, lateral amygdala and mPFC.
immediately after the acquisition phase which induced augmentation of
freezing at the retention test held on next day. Of note, escitalopram did 3.5.5. 5-HT7R
not affect baseline freezing behavior at any dose tested. This demon­ In 5-HT7R KO mice, a specific impairment of contextual fear condi­
strates that contextual fear conditioning is affected differently and dose- tioning was found, sparing other forms of learning such as spatial
sensitively by central 5-HT tonus depending on the mnemonic phase learning, cued and operant conditioning (Roberts et al., 2004). The
(Montezinho et al., 2010). In another investigation, rodents were treated dissociation between contextual-based learning and the cued fear con­
sub-chronically (21 days; 7 mg/kg; 18 doses) after having been sub­ ditioning indicates, that more complex integrative learning mechanism
jected to contextual fear conditioning. Contextual fear conditioning involving contextual processing, may rely on 5-HT7Rs. Dissociations
acquisition (over 3 days) took place in a box with the contextual element between spatial learning and contextual fear condition have been pre­
being represented by a specific odour und using eyelid-shocks as an viously reported (Cho et al., 1998; Clark et al., 2008). This com­
unconditioned stimulus. Following three weeks of fluoxetine treatment plementing finding of Roberts et al. (2004) might indicate a deficit in
(7 mg/kg), they were re-exposed to the conditioning context. In this place learning that is distinct from spatial processing and navigation,
investigation, SSRI treatment failed to abolish conditioned freezing to with 5-HT7Rs playing a role.
contextual stimuli. However, fluoxetine treatment restored

8
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

3.6. Summary The most popular tests following this procedure are the Rey Auditory
Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) and the California Verbal Learning Test
Central or hippocampal 5-HT elevations may protect against hypersen­ (Strauss et al., 2006).
sitivity to contextual fear learning in cases of innate 5-HT deficiency by
reducing pyramidal cell firing in the CA1 region. 5-HT2ARs exert hippo­ 4.1.1. Lowering 5-HT globally
campus independent influences on contextual fear memory consolidation. 5- TRP depletion as a pharmacological 5-HT deficiency model in
HT7Rs may be involved in more complex processing of contextual elements. humans, commonly results in impairments of declarative memory. In
Specifically, activation of 5-HT2ARs or inhibition of 5-HT7Rs appears to verbal learning tests, the effects were most prominent at delayed recall,
facilitate contextual memory processes in a cortico-limbic circuit. with fewer words recollected after TRP depletion (Amin et al., 2006;
Borghans et al., 2017; Harrison et al., 2004; Klaassen et al., 1999;
3.7. 5-HT and interactions with other neurotransmitters McAllister-Williams et al., 2002; Riedel et al., 1999a, 1999b; Sambeth
et al., 2007, 2009; Schmitt et al., 2000). There are only two studies,
Interactions between 5-HT and the cholinergic system have often which at least partly, failed to replicate this finding (Evers et al., 2005;
been addressed. The potential of serotonergic agents to change levels of Hughes et al., 2003). In another examination on verbal memory,
cholinergic transmission is of great interest for research on novel ther­ whereas prose paragraph had to be recalled, no verbal memory deficits
apeutic targets treating disorders such as age-related cognitive decline have been observed after using chronic fenfluramine (20 mg PO/day)
or dementia (Lanctô et al., 2001). application for 5-HT depletion (Meador et al., 2008).
It was demonstrated, that the combined cholinergic and serotonergic In contrast to verbal learning, immediate verbal recall was mostly
denervation of the forebrain in rodents produces severe spatial memory either unaffected (Harrison et al., 2004; Klaassen et al., 1999; Riedel
impairments, whereas 5-HT depletion alone had no effect (Lehmann et al., 1999a, 1999b; Schmitt et al., 2000), or only females were found to
et al., 2000; Nilsson et al., 1988; Richter-Levin and Segal, 1989; be impaired (Borghans et al., 2017; Helmbold et al., 2013; Mace et al.,
Richter-Levin et al., 1994). Further, the administration of the SSRI 2008; Sambeth et al., 2007). The specific greater vulnerability of females
alaproclate (20 mg/kg) together with scopolamine (0.8 mg/kg) or the to the effects of TRP restriction likely involves interactions with estro­
combination of 8-OH-DPAT and scopolamine at subthreshold doses (30 gen. Evidence for this was provided by estrogen treatments in meno­
µg/kg and 100 µg/kg, respectively) likewise induced a far greater place pausal woman, which leaded to augmented verbal memory performance
navigation deficit than scopolamine alone (Riekkinen et al., 1995, (Amin et al., 2006) and increased 5-HT turnover (Lippert et al., 1996).
1991). This entails, that changes in central 5-HT levels are capable to Moreover, recovered depressed participants receiving a low-dose TRP
magnify the functional defects induced by cholinergic blockade, leading diet were more vulnerable to deteriorations in cognitive function. In this
to a severe failure of spatial performance. study, impairments were specific to verbal short-term memory deficits,
On the other hand, scopolamine induced deficits in contextual fear without showing changes in word encoding, long-term memory, and
conditioning and object recognition tasks where reversed by pretraining subjective mood (Hayward et al., 2005).
administration of 5-HT1AR antagonists (WAY-101 405 [1 mg/kg]; Regarding specific memory phases, a TRP depletion-related consol­
Ro4368554 [10 mg/kg or 30 mg/kg]; WAY 100 635[1 mg/kg]; Ro 04–6790 idation impairment was pointed out. Consolidation is a process
[10 or 30 mg/kg]) (Hirst et al., 2008; Lieben et al., 2005a, b; Pitsikas et al., involving several stages (McGaugh, 2000), and in relation to TRP
2003; Woolley et al., 2003). The memory improving effects were thereby depletion, mostly immediate consolidation, which begins as early as
associated with increases in hippocampal acetylcholine levels (Hirst et al., during encoding up to 30 min after acquisition, was reduced (Harrison
2008). A possible underlying mechanism of action could involve opposite et al., 2004; Riedel et al., 1999a, 1999b; van der Veen et al., 2006). EEG
interactions between 5-HT1AR antagonists and scopolamine at the level of measurements during a verbal learning task revealed alterations of the
pyramidal hippocampal neurons. Accordingly, the blockade of hyper­ N400 event-related potential component after acute TRP depletion,
polarizing actions by endogenous 5-HT may compensate the loss of cholin­ which is consistent to consolidation deficits (Borghans et al., 2017). No
ergic input, favouring the action of other excitatory neurotransmitters on event-related potential changes were found at retrieval (McAllis­
pyramidal cells (Carli et al., 1997; Pitsikas et al., 2003). ter-Williams et al., 2002). One study, applying task-based fMRI after
In a similar vein, scopolamine induced object recognition deficits TRP depletion, observed attenuated neuronal activity in the right hip­
were counteracted by 5-HT6R ligands. The agonistic compound E-6801 pocampus during encoding of a visual-verbal memory task. No brain
(2.5 or 5 mg/kg) alone or in combination with subthreshold doses of the activity differences in comparison to a control group were detectable
antagonistic compounds EMD386088, as well as the antagonistic com­ during retrieval (van der Veen et al., 2006).
pound Ro4368554 (3 mg/kg or 10 mg/kg) restored object recognition Effects of TRP depletion on word recognition were less consistent.
performance (Kendall et al., 2011; Woolley et al., 2003). Furthermore, Recognition deficits were reported to be most pronounced after some
the acute and prolonged (21-day) administration of both, the selective hours delay (Riedel et al., 1999a, 1999b; Rubinsztein et al., 2001;
5-HT6R agonist WAY-181 187 (1 and 3 mg/kg) and the antagonist Sambeth et al., 2007; Schmitt et al., 2000; van der Veen et al., 2006).
SB-745 427 (3 mg/kg) prevented memory impairments and alterations Evers et al. (2005) found increased reaction times in delayed visual word
in BDNF signaling induced by MK-801 in rats (Rychtyk et al., 2019). recognition, however, other examinations failed to find differences in
comparison to control conditions (Borghans et al., 2017; Harrison et al.,
4. Human studies 2004; Helmbold et al., 2013; Klaassen et al., 1999; Sambeth et al., 2009).
Interestingly, the cognitive effects of TRP depletion were differentially
4.1. Verbal learning and memory affected by genotypes at the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism (ss vs. ll geno­
type). Regarding this, the ss genotypes showed deficits in an affective
Verbal learning tests in humans usually consist of a list of words that verbal directed forgetting task after TRP depletion, in contrast to the ll
has to be memorized by the subject being tested. During the learning genotype group (Roiser et al., 2007).
phase, words are presented verbally or visually and immediately
retrieved afterwards, a procedure that is repeated several times (5 times 4.1.2. Enhancement of 5-HT globally
in general). This is followed by delayed recall procedures including the Acute treatment with SSRIs, which causes a global increase in the
recognition of words from a list. Delayed recalls are performed after the serotonergic tonus, had fairly inconsistent effects on verbal learning
presentation of an interference list (without recognition), after 2 h delay across studies. Citalopram (10 mg IV) was found to improve long-term
and after 24 h delay (to assess protein-dependent long-term memory). recall and recognition in the RAVLT in female volunteers, without

9
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

affecting immediate recall 15 min earlier. This finding is consistent with activation, or MDMA administration reported prominent deficits in
studies suggesting reduced memory consolidation after TRP depletion, verbal memory overall. The effects of 5-HT2AR activation in humans
thus indicating reciprocal effects of low and high 5-HT levels in long- may not be easy to interpret given that the psychedelics commonly used
term memory processes in humans (Harmer et al., 2002). Yet, other induced hallucinogenic effects which also affect attentional processes
studies did not find group differences in verbal memory after acute (Vollenweider et al., 2007; Quednow et al., 2012). The 5-HT4R might be
citalopram ingestion (20 mg oral) (Sambeth et al., 2015; Wingen et al., an interesting candidate for enhancing cognitive performance.
2007) or by contrast, they observed impaired verbal memory perfor­
mance (Heckman et al., 2019a). Chronic escitalopram treatment (10–20 5. Visuo-spatial learning and memory
mg/day) for 15 days did not affect visual-verbal memory, which was
tested on several days during this period (Wingen et al., 2006). 5.1. Lowering 5-HT globally
Acute administration of the serotonin and noradrenaline releaser
MDMA causes a strong increase of extracellular 5-HT and noradrenaline A simple spatial learning task involved the pictorial presentation of a
levels by promoting non-exocytotic release of these neurotransmitters house with nine windows, yet only four of those windows had a light
from storage vesicles (Rothman et al., 2001). Acute MDMA worsens turned on. The memory for the positions of those illuminated windows
verbal memory during the intoxication phase at immediate and delayed after a short delay was not different between an TRP depletion group
recall (Haijen et al., 2018; Kuypers et al., 2013, 2016; Kuypers and and controls, indicating no visuo-spatial learning alterations. To note,
Ramaekers, 2005). The effects were strongest when MDMA was given reducing dopamine availability resulted in short-term memory impair­
before encoding, but also present when MDMA was given before ments in the same task (Harrison et al., 2004). Furthermore, no influ­
retrieval (Doss et al., 2017). Verbal memory recognition scores were not ence of TRP depletion was found on spatial pattern – location
affected by acute MDMA intoxication (Kuypers et al., 2013; Kuypers and recognition or in visuo-spatial learning tests from neurocognitive as­
Ramaekers, 2005; Van Wel et al., 2011), yet deficits have been obtained sessments (Amin et al., 2006; Evers et al., 2005; Hughes et al., 2003;
in a pictorial recognition task (Kuypers et al., 2013). Some research Mace et al., 2008; Park et al., 1994; Rubinsztein et al., 2001; Scarnà
points towards a specific role of 5-HT2ARs in MDMA induced memory et al., 2005). In an object relocation task, ten objects were presented
impairments (Reneman et al., 2001; Van Wel et al., 2011). shortly to participants in a square frame. Afterwards, the positions of the
objects were marked, and the objects had to be assigned to their previous
4.1.3. 5-HT1AR positions. In this specific task, TRP depletion was found cause perfor­
In male volunteers, the regional binding density of 5-HT1ARs ([11C] mance deficits in delayed recall taking place 4 h later. However, for the
WAY-100 635) was negatively associated with verbal memory perfor­ same task, there was a separate condition, demanding the placement of a
mance exclusively in the hippocampus. 5-HT1AR binding density in set of new objects in their original position as accurately as possible,
other memory relevant regions (e.g. ant. cingulate cortex, PFC, temporal without pre-marked dots. Interestingly, the TRP depletion group’s per­
cortex) was not related to verbal or visual memory. Further, the 5-HT1AR formance was superior to controls in placing objects to their exact co­
agonist tandospirone (60 mg, but not 30 mg) selectively impaired verbal ordinate position. According to the authors, the assignment of objects to
memory in the RAVLT in several indices without affecting other cogni­ a relative position likely incorporates a verbal component, as opposed to
tive domains, such as working memory or the ability to inhibit cognitive the remembrance of the more precise, metrical position of an object
interference (Stroop task). The authors suggested a postsynaptically whereas the metrical displacement to the correct position is measured
mediated mechanism, which inhibits hippocampal pyramidal neurons (Sambeth et al., 2009). Similarly, TRP depletion was found to augment
(Yasuno et al., 2003). In accordance, the combination of the 5-HT1AR spatial pattern recognition (Scarnà et al., 2005).
agonist pindolol (10 mg) and escitalopram (20 mg) exacerbated deficits
in immediate verbal memory recall (Wingen et al., 2007). Pindolol binds 5.1.1. Enhancement of 5-HT globally
at presynaptic 5-HT1A autoreceptors and hence, likely contributes to the Acute administration of the SSRI citalopram (20 mg) did not affect
general 5-HT increase caused by escitalopram (Rasmussen et al., 2004). spatial memory or visuo-spatial recognition, neither in a spatial test
based on object relocation, nor in a continuous pattern recognition test
4.1.4. 5-HT2AR (Heckman et al., 2019b; Park et al., 1994; Sambeth et al., 2015). In
Psilocybin (10, 20 and 30 mg/70 kg) and LSD (100 micrograms) are accordance, no effects on memory outcomes, white matter plasticity
both acting as 5-HT2AR (and a weaker 5-HT1AR) agonists and their (measured by diffusion tensor imaging), or structural changes
application decreased the free recall of words in a verbal memory task. (T1-weighted MRI data) were found after chronic escitalopram treat­
This effect was dose-dependent for free recall and recognition, with the ment for 3 weeks (10 mg/day) during a visual face – object associate
lowest dose of psilocybin (10 mg/70 kg) and a lower dose of LSD (50 relearning task (Vanicek et al., 2022). However, functional activation
micrograms) having no effect on word recognition (Barrett et al., 2018). changes while relearning were found from the right insula to both the
Similarly, object recognition and recall were affected after LSD appli­ anterior cingulate and right angular gyrus in the same sample (Reed
cation (100 micrograms) (Jarvik et al., 1955). et al., 2022). In contrast to these previous studies, the combined treat­
ment of citalopram (20 mg) together with anodal transcranial direct
4.1.5. 5-HT4R current stimulation, entailed beneficial effects on short-term memory in
The memory enhancing effects of 5-HT4R agonism observed in ani­ an object-location recognition task. This applied to both, younger and
mal studies could be demonstrated in humans as well. The 5-HT4R older adults (Prehn et al., 2016).
agonist prucalopride (1 mg) increased recall of words in a verbal Acute MDMA induced pronounced impairments of visuo-spatial
learning task and increased the accuracy of recall and recognition of memory. Neither pretreatment with the 5-HT1AR antagonist pindolol
words in an incidental emotional memory task. This successful trans­ (20 mg) nor with the 5-HT2AR antagonist ketanserin (50 mg) prevented
lation from animal to human studies suggest 5-HT4R agonist as prom­ or altered MDMA induced visuo-spatial memory impairments (Hasler
ising treatment for cognitive disturbances (Murphy et al., 2020a). et al., 2009; Van Wel et al., 2011).

4.2. Summary 5.2. Summary

Central 5-HT deficits mainly impaired delayed recall in verbal Interestingly, spatial memory for accurate placements might even
learning tasks, suggesting poor consolidation. But also, studies that slightly benefit from 5-HT deficiency induced by TRP depletion. This
induced overstimulation of the 5-HT system through SSRI, 5-HT1AR contrasts with the apparent impairments in verbal memory using the

10
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

same psychopharmacological manipulation. memory performance has been discussed, but in most of the studies
showing memory effects after TRP depletion, mood was not altered
6. General discussion (Ruhé et al., 2007).
Evidence stands that partially segregated networks are recruited for
6.1. Low global 5-HT spatial navigation and object memories even though they all do account
as medial temporal lobe dependent memories (Chao et al., 2016; Hales
In the majority studies, TRP depletion induced deficits in human et al., 2014; Rauchs et al., 2008). Most interestingly, according to the
verbal memory or object recognition in animals while sparing spatial findings reviewed here, cognitive subroutines underlying spatial navi­
memory. Across species, females were more vulnerable to TRP gation, object memories, and storage of the contextual element in
depletion-induced deficits, especially before ovulation when estrogen contextual fear conditioning, seem to be differentially affected by cen­
levels are rising. To mention at this point are findings that document a tral 5-HT deficiency. This suggests dissociable processes underlying
higher 5-HT synthesis overall in males than in females, which could storage of objects and contexts as opposed to the formation of cognitive
relate to the abovementioned finding (Chugani and Muzik, 2000). In maps during allocentric navigation. Research on such topics remained
addition, conditions associated with less efficient 5-HT functions, such sparse to date, especially with regard to 5-HT. The retrosplenial cortex
as psychiatric disorders in humans or their mimicry in genetically and the MS are critically involved in spatial navigation (Kubik et al.,
modified animals, enhanced the effect of TRP depletion (Alhaj et al., 2012), yet storage of objects relies on extrahippocampal structures such
2012; Merens et al., 2008). TRP depletion was further associated with as the perirhinal cortex in particular (Burwell et al., 2004; Miranda and
slight increases in attentional processes and perceptual processing Bekinschtein, 2018). Considering this, further research might take up
(Schmitt et al., 2000), whereas dopamine depletion primarily affected findings describing different molecular mechanisms in the respective
spatial working memory (Harrison et al., 2004). Concerning human brain regions that process and store different types of information
studies a contribution of lowered mood which negatively affects (Mitchnick et al., 2015; Natale et al., 2020), and relate them to the 5-HT

Table 1
Main effects animal studies. Superscript arrow symbols refer to directions of effects found in the respective studies. ↑: memory improvement. ↓: memory deterioration.
→: no effect.

11
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

Table 2
Main effects human studies. Superscript arrow symbols refer to directions of effects found in the respective studies. ↑: memory improvement. ↓: memory deterioration.
→: no effect.

system. A further interesting topic to pursue are theta wave changes modulates the output in dorsal and ventral hippocampus in opposite
found to occur after modulating 5-HT levels. For example, Kragel et al. directions, causing cell firing attenuation in the dorsal and facilitation in
(2020) suggested that encoding and retrieval happens at different pha­ the ventral segment (Mlinar and Corradetti, 2018). In response to
ses of hippocampal theta oscillations, an observation which may provide aversive stimuli, 5-HT level of dorsal hippocampus increases, serving
indications why a lower central 5-HT tone supported encoding under protective functions (Almada et al., 2013; Dai et al., 2008) and likely
some conditions (e.g. spatial learning). counteract the consolidation of the contextual component in stressful
In addition, another intriguing issue to be addressed is the fact that 5- memories (Graeff et al., 1996; Maren et al., 2013). At the same time,
HT might differently influence distinct forms of synaptic plasticity. In ventral hippocampal facilitation likely boosts the signal to its down­
this respect, it was found that various forms of LTP contribute to stream targets, such the amygdala, that are involved in emotional
different aspects of memory storage with theta frequency and CaMKII- regulation (Mlinar and Corradetti, 2018; Ohmura et al., 2010; Twining
(calmodulin-dependent protein kinase) dependent LTP being particu­ et al., 2020). The failure in retrieving fear-related contextual memory
larly necessary for spatial navigation (Bach et al., 1995; Rotenberg et al., occurs by not activating the subsequent fear circuit that induces freezing
1996). This was indicated by the observation, that CaMKII mutant mice behavior (Tada et al., 2004). Such a modulation was lacking in chron­
exhibit a selective loss of hippocampal LTP in the range of the theta ically 5-HT deficient mice (Waider et al., 2019). Since innate anxiety
frequency and displayed impairments in spatial memory but not in behaviors were reduced in chronic-5-HT deficient animals (Dai et al.,
contextual memory storage (Bach et al., 1995; Silva et al., 1992). 2008; Mosienko et al., 2014), albeit inconsistently (Gutknecht et al.,
Remarkably, TRP depletion-related results are consistent to in­ 2015), exaggerated contextual fear conditioning is plausibly a conse­
vestigations in recurrent MDMA users (human 5-HT lesion model). quence of altered synaptic plasticity and circuitry in the hippocampus
Accordingly, MDMA-induced 5-HT deficiency was most pronounced in resulting from lifelong 5-HT deficiency, and rather not due to a more
the domain of verbal memory, but not in the visual domain (Bhattachary anxious phenotype. Abovementioned considerations pinpoint the MR as
and Powell, 2001; Burgess et al., 2011; Kalechstein et al., 2007; Qued­ crucial in fear-conditioning and this might further be a highly relevant
now et al., 2006). basis for exploring differences in terms of raphe nuclei and
Some other mechanism of actions underlying TRP depletion than material-specific consolidation (Lieben et al., 2006; Lin et al., 2021;
purely serotonergic ones have been suggested (see van Donkelaar et al., Netto et al., 2002).
2010 for an extensive review). One of the most debated is the possible
interaction between TRP depletion and the enzyme nitric oxide synthase 6.2. High global 5-HT
(NOS). Yet, NOS inhibitors have been demonstrated to severely impair
both, spatial learning and object recognition (Gocmez et al., 2015; Spatial navigation and object memories were both impaired after
Majlessi et al., 2008; Pitsikas, 2015). It is therefore interesting that TRP either acute SSRI or acute MDMA treatment in animals, yet mixed effects
depletion consistently deteriorated object recognition, but not spatial were obtained for contextual fear conditioning. Acute SSRI adminis­
memory and navigation in animals. Two studies revealed very similar tration in humans yielded inconsistent results so far. The global eleva­
findings in humans. This argues rather against NOS as a critical mediator tions in extracellular 5-HT concentrations trigger non-specific increases
in TRP depletion induced object recognition deficits.(Tables 1 and 2). in the activity of autoreceptors and heteroreceptors, which impede
Potential reasons for the contrasting effects between acute and efficient information processing (Fuller, 1995). Hence, resulting mem­
chronic 5-HT deficiency specifically in contextual fear conditioning can ory alterations might involve complex interactions and currently, it is
just be discussed briefly here, as their elaboration would exceed the not known which receptors are indeed involved in the reduced memory
scope of this review. 5-HT incorporates an influential role in circuit performance after elevation of 5-HT and whether different phases of
formation and the refinement of neuronal connections during develop­ memory are affected differently.(Fig. 1).
ment and therefore, alterations of the 5-HT system concomitant adap­
tations may have behavioral consequences (e.g. Daubert and Condron,
2010; Mosienko et al., 2014). Normally, MR – hippocampal 5-HT input

12
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

6.3. 5-HT receptors spatial navigation might even benefit from low serotonergic tone in
support of encoding. Central and non-specific pharmacologically
For all forms of declarative memories investigated, procognitive ef­ induced 5-HT elevations may be beneficial for consolidation in some
fects of 5-HT1AR antagonists were demonstrated, while 5-HT1AR ago­ cases, but further studies are clearly needed for clarification. 5-HT1AR
nists as well as changes in their expression profile yielded mixed results. antagonists, 5-HT4R agonists, and 5-HT6R ligands might comprise
Improving effects on memory are likely mediated via stimulation of promising targets for declarative memory impairments.
postsynaptic receptors, yet overstimulation of the serotonergic system
might produce the opposite suggesting that associated pro-cognitive Conflict of interest
effects may follow a bell-shaped function. There is further evidence
indicating that more efficient pattern separation in hippocampal CA1 is The authors declare no conflict of interest.
accountable for improved memory due to increased inhibitory seroto­
nergic tone (Fernandez et al., 2017). 5-HT1AR interact with the cholin­ Acknowledgement
ergic and glutamatergic system and thus, might serve the treatment of
age-related cognitive disorders. Up until today, however, cognition RC was supported by a grant of the Novartis Foundation for Medical-
improving effects of 5-HT1AR partial agonists applied for treating Biological Research (#19C167).
schizophrenia-related cognitive impairments in humans (e.g. buspirone
and tandospirone) has not been unambiguously demonstrated yet (Buoli References
and Altamura, 2015).
Acute blockade of 5-HT2AR impaired recognition of objects in Able, J.A., Gudelsky, G.A., Vorhees, C.V., Williams, M.T., 2006. 3,4-
Methylenedioxymethamphetamine in adult rats produces deficits in path integration
context and reduced the conditioned response in contextual fear con­ and spatial reference memory. Biol. Psychiatry 59 (12), 1219–1226. https://doi.org/
ditioning, yet contrastingly, improved spatial reference memory 10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2005.09.006.
retrieval. Further investigations revealed that 5-HT2ARs in the mPFC Alhaj, H.A., Selman, M., Jervis, V., Rodgers, J., Barton, S., McAllister-Williams, R.H.,
2012. Effect of low-dose acute tryptophan depletion on the specificity of
play an important role in controlling contextual retrieval of concurrent
autobiographical memory in healthy subjects with a family history of depression.
memories by selecting the correct memory trace and suppressing com­ Psychopharmacology 222 (2), 285–292. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-012-
petitors in complex tasks. In rodents – but not in humans – 5-HT2AR 2644-X.
Almada, R.C., Albrechet-Souza, L., Brandão, M.L., 2013. Further evidence for
activation enhanced consolidation of object memory and contextual fear
involvement of the dorsal hippocampus serotonergic and γ-aminobutyric acid
memory as well as the reconsolidation of object memories in the peri­ (GABA)ergic pathways in the expression of contextual fear conditioning in rats.
rhinal cortex, especially when administered directly after learning, J. Psychopharmacol. 27 (12), 1160–1168. https://doi.org/10.1177/
while pre-training activation facilitated extinction of fear memories. In 0269881113482840.
Amin, Z., Gueorguieva, R., Cappiello, A., Czarkowski, K.A., Stiklus, S., Anderson, G.M.,
spatial learning, however, agonistic ligands produced impairments. Naftolin, F., Epperson, C.N., 2006. Estradiol and tryptophan depletion interact to
Accordingly, effects of 5-HT2AR on declarative memories could differ modulate cognition in menopausal women. Neuropsychopharmacology 31 (11),
across mnemonic modalities. There is also an impairing side effect of 2489–2497. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301114.
Ampuero, E., Stehberg, J., Gonzalez, D., Besser, N., Ferrero, M., Diaz-Veliz, G.,
hallucinogenic agents on spatial navigation that merits consideration Wyneken, U., Rubio, F.J., 2013. Repetitive fluoxetine treatment affects long-term
(Zhang et al., 2017). Further studies with regard to possible memories but not learning. Behav. Brain Res. 247, 92–100. https://doi.org/
cognition-enhancing effects of 5-HT2 CR antagonists could certainly be 10.1016/J.BBR.2013.03.011.
Anagnostaras, S.G., Gale, G.D., & Fanselow, M.S. (2001). Hippocampus and Contextual
beneficial (Bertaina-Anglade et al., 2011). Fear Conditioning: Recent Controversies and Advances. 〈https://doi.org/10.1002/10
Although the effects of 5-HT3R antagonism on spatial learning were 98–1063〉.
inconclusive in healthy animal populations, it may represent a potential Anguiano-Rodríguez, P.B., Gaytán-Tocavén, L., Olvera-Cortés, M.E., 2007. Striatal
serotonin depletion facilitates rat egocentric learning via dopamine modulation. Eur.
treatment for cholinergic dysfunction (Passani and Blandina, 1998).
J. Pharmacol. 556 (1–3), 91–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.EJPHAR.2006.10.042.
Pro-cognitive effects on acquisition and/or consolidation were also Aranda, L., Begega, A., Sánchez-López, J., Aguirre, J.A., Arias, J.L., Santín, L.J., 2008.
induced by 5-HT4R agonists and on the other hand, impairments were Temporary inactivation of the supramammillary area impairs spatial working
memory and spatial reference memory retrieval. Physiol. Behav. 94 (3), 322–330.
observed when blocking this receptor. Positive effects also occurred in
https://doi.org/10.1016/J.PHYSBEH.2008.01.024.
humans, as evidenced by increased precision in recall and recognition of Avanzi, V., Brandão, M.L., 2001. Activation of somatodendritic 5-HT1A autoreceptors in
verbal memory, concepting 5-HT4R agonists as a potential target for the the median raphe nucleus disrupts the contextual conditioning in rats. Behav. Brain
treatment of cognitive deficits (Murphy et al., 2020b). Res. 126 (1–2), 175–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00254-6.
Bach, M.E., Hawkins, R.D., Osman, M., Kandel, E.R., Mayford, M., 1995. Impairment of
5-HT6Rs constitute a further promising therapeutic objective spatial but not contextual memory in CaMKII mutant mice with a selective loss of
particularly for the prevention of age-related deficits due to its in­ hippocampal ltp in the range of the θ frequency. Cell 81 (6), 905–915. https://doi.
teractions with the cholinergic system. Interestingly, both agonistic and org/10.1016/0092-8674(95)90010-1.
Bacqué-Cazenave, J., Bharatiya, R., Barrière, G., Delbecque, J.-P., Bouguiyoud, N., Di
antagonistic ligands enhanced declarative memories in a similar vein, Giovanni, G., Cattaert, D., De Deurwaerdère, P., 2020. Serotonin in animal cognition
yet via distinct neuronal mechanism of action (Kendall et al., 2011). and behavior. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 21 (5), 1649. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms21051649.
5-HT7R were of special relevance for the detection of novelty in Balderas, I., Rodriguez-Ortiz, C.J., Salgado-Tonda, P., Chavez-Hurtado, J., McGaugh, J.
L., Bermudez-Rattoni, F., 2008. The consolidation of object and context recognition
object recognition without having direct behavioral impacts in spatial memory involve different regions of the temporal lobe. Learn. Mem. 15 (9),
learning. Interestingly, KO models provided an example of dissociations 618–624. https://doi.org/10.1101/LM.1028008.
between egocentric and allocentric spatial learning strategies. Specific Ballaz, S.J., Akil, H., Watson, S.J., 2007. The 5-HT7 receptor: role in novel object
discrimination and relation to novelty-seeking behavior. Neuroscience 149 (1),
deficits in contextual learning were uncovered, assumingly involving a
192–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2007.07.043.
cognitive subroutine that is less important for egocentric spatial- Barre, A., Berthoux, C., De Bundel, D., Valjent, E., Bockaert, J., Marin, P., Bécamel, C.,
navigational processes with 5-HT7Rs playing a role (Sarkisyan and 2016. Presynaptic serotonin 2A receptors modulate thalamocortical plasticity and
associative learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 113 (10), E1382–E1391. https://doi.
Hedlund, 2009).
org/10.1073/PNAS.1525586113/-/DCSUPPLEMENTAL.
Barrett, F.S., Carbonaro, T.M., Hurwitz, E., Johnson, M.W., Griffiths, R.R., 2018. Double-
7. Conclusion blind comparison of the two hallucinogens psilocybin and dextromethorphan: Effects
on cognition. Psychopharmacology 235 (10), 2915. https://doi.org/10.1007/
S00213-018-4981-X.
Taken together, the findings reviewed here indicate that acutely Beaudet, G., Paizanis, E., Zoratto, F., Lacivita, E., Leopoldo, M., Freret, T., Laviola, G.,
lowered brain 5-HT tonus most likely impairs protein synthesis- Boulouard, M., Adriani, W., 2017. LP-211, a selective 5-HT7 receptor agonist,
dependent consolidation in humans and rodents. Considering task- increases novelty-preference and promotes risk-prone behavior in rats. Synapse 71
(12), e21995. https://doi.org/10.1002/SYN.21995.
related cognitive processes unveils material-specify for object-related Bekinschtein, P., Renner, M.C., Gonzalez, M.C., Weisstaub, N., 2013. Role of medial
and verbal memory content being particularly affected. By contrast, prefrontal cortex serotonin 2a receptors in the control of retrieval of recognition

13
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

memory in rats. J. Neurosci. 33 (40), 15716–15725. https://doi.org/10.1523/ Churchwell, J.C., Kesner, R.P., 2011. Hippocampal-prefrontal dynamics in spatial
JNEUROSCI.2087-13.2013. working memory: Interactions and independent parallel processing. Behav. Brain
Belcher, A.M., O’Dell, S.J., Marshall, J.F., 2005. Impaired object recognition memory Res. 225 (2), 389–395. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BBR.2011.07.045.
following methamphetamine, but not p-chloroamphetamine- or d-amphetamine- Clark, P.J., Brzezinska, W.J., Thomas, M.W., Ryzhenko, N.A., Toshkov, S.A., Rhodes, J.S.,
induced neurotoxicity. Neuropsychopharmacology 30 (11), 2026–2034. https://doi. 2008. Intact neurogenesis is required for benefits of exercise on spatial memory but
org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300771. not motor performance or contextual fear conditioning in C57BL/6J mice.
Bert, B., Voigt, J.P., Kusserow, H., Theuring, F., Rex, A., Fink, H., 2009. Increasing the Neuroscience 155 (4), 1048–1058. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.
number of 5-HT(1A)-receptors in cortex and hippocampus does not induce NEUROSCIENCE.2008.06.051.
mnemonic deficits in mice. Pharmacol., Biochem., Behav. 92 (1), 76–81. https://doi. Cohen, S.J., Stackman, R.W., 2015. Assessing rodent hippocampal involvement in the
org/10.1016/J.PBB.2008.10.014. novel object recognition task. A review. Behav. Brain Res. 285, 105–117. https://doi.
Bertaina-Anglade, V., Drieu-La-Rochelle, C., Mocaër, E., Seguin, L., 2011. Memory org/10.1016/J.BBR.2014.08.002.
facilitating effects of agomelatine in the novel object recognition memory paradigm Connor, D.A., Gould, T.J., 2016. The role of working memory and declarative memory in
in the rat. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 98 (4), 511–517. https://doi.org/10.1016/J. trace conditioning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 134 (Part B), 193–209. https://doi.org/
PBB.2011.02.015. 10.1016/J.NLM.2016.07.009.
Bertrand, F., Lehmann, O., Lazarus, C., Jeltsch, H., Cassel, J.C., 2000. Intraseptal Da Silva Costa, V., Duchatelle, P., Boulouard, M., Dauphin, F., 2008. Selective 5-HT6
infusions of 8-OH-DPAT in the rat impairs water-maze performances: effects on receptor blockade improves spatial recognition memory and reverses age-related
memory or anxiety? Neurosci. Lett. 279 (1), 45–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304- deficits in spatial recognition memory in the mouse. Neuropsychopharmacology 34
3940(99)00948-9. (2), 488–500. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2008.94.
Bhattachary, S., Powell, J.H., 2001. Recreational use of 3,4-methylenedioxymetham­ Dai, J.X., Han, H.L., Tian, M., Cao, J., Xiu, J.B., Song, N.N., Huang, Y., Xu, T., Le, Ding, Y.
phetamine (MDMA) or ‘ecstasy’: evidence for cognitive impairment. Psychol. Med. Q., Xu, L., 2008. Enhanced contextual fear memory in central serotonin-deficient
31 (4), 647–658. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291701003828. mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105 (33), 11981–11986. https://doi.org/10.1073/
Borghans, L.G.J.M., Blokland, A., Sambeth, A., 2017. Effects of biperiden and acute PNAS.0801329105.
tryptophan depletion and their combination on verbal word memory and EEG. Daubert, E.A., Condron, B.G., 2010. Serotonin: a regulator of neuronal morphology and
Psychopharmacology 234 (7), 1135–1143. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-017- circuitry. Trends Neurosci. 33 (9), 424–434. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.
4549-1/TABLES/3. TINS.2010.05.005.
Braun, A.A., Amos-Kroohs, R.M., Gutierrez, A., Lundgren, K.H., Seroogy, K.B., del Cid-Pellitero, E., Garzón, M., 2011. Medial prefrontal cortex receives input from
Skelton, M.R., Vorhees, C.V., Williams, M.T., 2015. Dopamine depletion in either the dorsal raphe nucleus neurons targeted by Hypocretin1/OrexinA-containing axons.
dorsomedial or dorsolateral striatum impairs egocentric Cincinnati water maze Neuroscience 172, 30–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.10.058.
performance while sparing allocentric Morris water maze learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Dere, E., Zlomuzica, A., Huston, J.P., Silva, M.A.D.S., 2008. Animal episodic memory.
Mem. 118, 55–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NLM.2014.10.009. Handb. Behav. Neurosci. 18, 155–184.
Bridoux, A., Laloux, C., Derambure, P., Bordet, R., Monaca Charley, C., 2013. The acute Doss, M.K., Weafer, J., Gallo, D.A., De Wit, H., 2017. MDMA impairs both the encoding
inhibition of rapid eye movement sleep by citalopram may impair spatial learning and retrieval of emotional recollections. Neuropsychopharmacology 43 (4),
and passive avoidance in mice. J. Neural Transm. 120 (3), 383–389. https://doi.org/ 791–800. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2017.171.
10.1007/S00702-012-0901-0/FIGURES/3. Duff, M.C., Covington, N.V., Hilverman, C., Cohen, N.J., 2020. Semantic memory and the
Brunelli, M., Castellucci, V., Kandel, E.R., 1976. Synaptic facilitation and behavioral hippocampus: revisiting, reaffirming, and extending the reach of their critical
sensitization in Aplysia: Possible role of serotonin and cyclic AMP. Science 194 relationship. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13, 471. https://doi.org/10.3389/
(4270), 1178–1181. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.186870. FNHUM.2019.00471/BIBTEX.
Buhot, M.C., Martin, S., Segu, L., 2000. Role of serotonin in memory impairment. In: Eichenbaum, H., 2000. A cortical–hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nat. Rev.
Annals of Medicine, Vol. 32. Royal Society of Medicine Press Ltd, pp. 210–221. Neurosci. 1 (1), 41–50. https://doi.org/10.1038/35036213.
https://doi.org/10.3109/07853890008998828. Eichenbaum, H., 2001. The hippocampus and declarative memory: cognitive
Buhot, M.C., Wolff, M., Savova, M., Malleret, G., Hen, R., Segu, L., 2003. Protective effect mechanisms and neural codes. Behav. Brain Res. 127 (1–2), 199–207. https://doi.
of 5-HT1B receptor gene deletion on the age-related decline in spatial learning org/10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00365-5.
abilities in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 142 (1–2), 135–142. https://doi.org/10.1016/ Eichenbaum, H., Cohen, N.J., 2014. Can we reconcile the declarative memory and spatial
S0166-4328(02)00400-X. navigation views on hippocampal function? Neuron 83 (4), 764–770. https://doi.
Buoli, M., Altamura, A.C., 2015. May non-antipsychotic drugs improve cognition of org/10.1016/J.NEURON.2014.07.032.
schizophrenia patients? Pharmacopsychiatry 48 (2), 41–50. https://doi.org/ Eichenbaum, H., Otto, T., Cohen, N.J., 1992. The hippocampus-what does it do? Behav.
10.1055/S-0034-1396801. Neural Biol. 57 (1), 2–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-1047(92)90724-I.
Burgess, A.P., Venables, L., Jones, H., Edwards, R., Parrott, A.C., 2011. Event related Evers, E.A.T., Tillie, D.E., Van Der Veen, F.M., Lieben, C.K., Jolles, J., Deutz, N.E.P.,
potential (ERP) evidence for selective impairment of verbal recollection in abstinent Schmitt, J.A.J., 2005. Effects of a novel method of acute tryptophan depletion on
recreational methylenedioxymethamphetamine (“Ecstasy”)/polydrug users. plasma tryptophan and cognitive performance in healthy volunteers.
Psychopharmacology 216 (4), 545–556. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-011- Psychopharmacology 178 (1), 92–99. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-004-2141-
2249-9/TABLES/4. Y/FIGURES/2.
Burwell, R.D., Saddoris, M.P., Bucci, D.J., Wiig, K.A., 2004. Corticohippocampal Fernandez, S.P., Muzerelle, A., Scotto-Lomassese, S., Barik, J., Gruart, A., Delgado-
contributions to spatial and contextual learning. J. Neurosci. 24 (15), 3826–3836. Garciá, J.M., Gaspar, P., 2017. Constitutive and acquired serotonin deficiency alters
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0410-04.2004. memory and hippocampal synaptic plasticity. Neuropsychopharmacology 42 (2),
Buzsáki, G., 2002. Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. In: Neuron, Vol. 33. Cell Press, 512–523. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2016.134.
pp. 325–340. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00586-X. Flores-Burgess, A., Millón, C., Gago, B., García-Durán, L., Cantero-García, N.,
Carli, M., Bonalumi, P., Samanin, R., 1997. WAY 100635, a 5-HT1A receptor antagonist, Coveñas, R., Narváez, J.A., Fuxe, K., Santín, L., Díaz-Cabiale, Z., 2019. Galanin
prevents the impairment of spatial learning caused by intrahippocampal (1–15)-fluoxetine interaction in the novel object recognition test. Involvement of 5-
administration of scopolamine or 7-chloro-kynurenic acid. Brain Res. 774 (1–2), HT1A receptors in the prefrontal cortex of the rats. Neuropharmacology 155,
167–174. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(97)81700-3. 104–112. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROPHARM.2019.05.023.
Castañé, A., Kargieman, L., Celada, P., Bortolozzi, A., Artigas, F., 2015. 5-HT2A receptors Foley, A.G., Murphy, K.J., Hirst, W.D., Gallagher, H.C., Hagan, J.J., Upton, N., Walsh, F.
are involved in cognitive but not antidepressant effects of fluoxetine. Eur. S., Regan, C.M., 2004. The 5-HT(6) receptor antagonist SB-271046 reverses
Neuropsychopharmacol. 25 (8), 1353–1361. https://doi.org/10.1016/J. scopolamine-disrupted consolidation of a passive avoidance task and ameliorates
EURONEURO.2015.04.006. spatial task deficits in aged rats. Neuropsychopharmacol. Off. Publ. Am. Coll.
Castañé Anna, A., Theobald, D.E.H., Robbins, T.W., 2010. Selective lesions of the Neuropsychopharmacol. 29 (1), 93–100. https://doi.org/10.1038/SJ.NPP.1300332.
dorsomedial striatum impair serial spatial reversal learning in rats. Behav. Brain Res. Forwood, S.E., Winters, B.D., Bussey, T.J., 2005. Hippocampal lesions that abolish spatial
210 (1), 74–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BBR.2010.02.017. maze performance spare object recognition memory at delays of up to 48 h.
Cedar, H., Schwartz, J.H., 1972. Cyclic adenosine monophosphate in the nervous system Hippocampus 15 (3), 347–355. https://doi.org/10.1002/HIPO.20059.
of Aplysia calffornica: II. efect of serotonin and dopamine. J. Gen. Physiol. 60 (5), Freund, T.F., Gulyas, A.I., Acsady, L., Gorcs, T., Toth, K., 1990. Serotonergic control of
570–587. https://doi.org/10.1085/jgp.60.5.570. the hippocampus via local inhibitory interneurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 87 (21),
Chakraborty, S., Lennon, J.C., Malkaram, S.A., Zeng, Y., Fisher, D.W., Dong, H., 2019. 8501–8505. https://doi.org/10.1073/PNAS.87.21.8501.
Serotonergic system, cognition, and BPSD in Alzheimer’s disease. In: Neuroscience Fuller, R.W., 1995. Serotonin uptake inhibitors: uses in clinical therapy and in laboratory
Letters, Vol. 704. Elsevier Ireland Ltd., pp. 36–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. research. Prog. Drug Res. 45, 167–204. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-0348-7164-
neulet.2019.03.050 8_5.
Chao, O.Y., Huston, J.P., Li, J.S., Wang, A.L., de Souza Silva, M.A., 2016. The medial Glikmann-Johnston, Y., Saling, M.M., Reutens, D.C., Stout, J.C., 2015. Hippocampal 5-
prefrontal cortex—lateral entorhinal cortex circuit is essential for episodic-like HT1A receptor and spatial learning and memory. Front. Pharmacol. 6, 289. https://
memory and associative object-recognition. Hippocampus 26 (5), 633–645. https:// doi.org/10.3389/FPHAR.2015.00289/BIBTEX.
doi.org/10.1002/HIPO.22547. Gocmez, S.S., Yazir, Y., Sahin, D., Karadenizli, S., Utkan, T., 2015. The effect of a
Cho, Y.H., Friedman, E., Silva, A.J., 1998. Ibotenate lesions of the hippocampus impair selective neuronal nitric oxide synthase inhibitor 3-bromo 7-nitroindazole on spatial
spatial learning but not contextual fear conditioning in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 98 learning and memory in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 131, 19–25. https://doi.
(1), 77–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00054-0. org/10.1016/J.PBB.2015.01.013.
Chugani, D.C., Muzik, O., 2000. α[C-11]methyl-L-tryptophan PET maps brain serotonin Graeff, F.G., Guimarães, F.S., De Andrade, T.G.C.S., Deakin, J.F.W., 1996. Role of 5-HT in
synthesis and Kynurenine pathway metabolism. J. Cereb. Blood Flow. Metab. 20 (1), stress, anxiety, and depression. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 54 (1), 129–141.
2–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004647-200001000-00002. https://doi.org/10.1016/0091-3057(95)02135-3.

14
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

Gutiérrez-Guzmán, B.E., Hernández-Pérez, J.J., González-Burgos, I., Feria-Velásco, A., Hornung, J.P., 2003. The human raphe nuclei and the serotonergic system. J. Chem.
Medina, R., Guevara, M.Á., López-Vázquez, M.Á., Olvera-Cortés, M.E., 2011. Neuroanat. 26 (4), 331–343. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jchemneu.2003.10.002.
Hippocampal serotonin depletion facilitates place learning concurrent with an Hughes, J.H., Gallagher, P., Stewart, M.E., Matthews, D., Kelly, T.P., Young, A.H., 2003.
increase in CA1 high frequency theta activity expression in the rat. Eur. J. The effects of acute tryptophan depletion on neuropsychological function.
Pharmacol. 652 (1–3), 73–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.EJPHAR.2010.11.014. J. Psychopharmacol. 17 (3), 300–309. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Gutiérrez-Guzmán, B.E., Hernández-Pérez, J.J., Olvera-Cortés, M.E., 2017. Serotonergic 02698811030173012.
modulation of septo-hippocampal and septo-mammillary theta activity during Jacobs, B.L., Azmitia, E.C., 1992. Structure and function of the brain serotonin system.
spatial learning, in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 319, 73–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/J. Physiol. Rev. Vol. 72 (Issue 1), 165–230. https://doi.org/10.1152/
BBR.2016.11.017. physrev.1992.72.1.165.
Gutknecht, L., Kriegebaum, C., Waider, J., Schmitt, A., Lesch, K.P., 2009. Spatio- Jacobs, B.L., Fornal, C.A., 1995. Serotonin and behavior: a general hypothesis.
temporal expression of tryptophan hydroxylase isoforms in murine and human Psychopharmacol. Fourth Gener. Prog. 461–469.
brain: convergent data from Tph2 knockout mice. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 19 Jans, L.A.W., Lieben, C.K.J., Blokland, A., 2007. Influence of sex and estrous cycle on the
(4), 266–282. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.EURONEURO.2008.12.005. effects of acute tryptophan depletion induced by a gelatin-based mixture in adult
Gutknecht, L., Popp, S., Waider, J., Sommerlandt, F.M.J., Göppner, C., Post, A., Reif, A., Wistar rats. Neuroscience 147 (2), 304–317. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.
Van Den Hove, D., Strekalova, T., Schmitt, A., Colaço, M.B.N., Sommer, C., NEUROSCIENCE.2007.04.028.
Palme, R., Lesch, K.P., 2015. Interaction of brain 5-HT synthesis deficiency, chronic Jans, L.A.W., Lieben, C.K.J., Smits, L.T., Blokland, A., 2009. Pharmacokinetics of acute
stress and sex differentially impact emotional behavior in Tph2 knockout mice. tryptophan depletion using a gelatin-based protein in male and female Wistar rats.
Psychopharmacology 232 (14), 2429–2441. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-015- Amino Acids 37 (2), 349–357. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00726-008-0160-4/
3879-0/FIGURES/5. FIGURES/4.
Gyertyán, I., Kassai, F., Kozma, K., Kitka, T., Ernyey, A.J., 2020. Procognitive profiling of Jans, L.A.W., Korte-Bouws, G.A.H., Korte, S.M., Blokland, A., 2010. The effects of acute
a serotonin 5-HT6 receptor antagonist in a complex model system in rats: a novel tryptophan depletion on affective behaviour and cognition in Brown Norway and
translational approach for clinical prediction. Brain Res. Bull. 165, 238–245. https:// Sprague Dawley rats. J. Psychopharmacol. 24 (4), 605–614. https://doi.org/
doi.org/10.1016/J.BRAINRESBULL.2020.10.014. 10.1177/0269881108099424.
Hagena, H., Manahan-Vaughan, D., 2017. The serotonergic 5-HT4 receptor: a unique Jarvik, M.E., Abramson, H.A., Hirsch, M.W., 1955. Lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD-25):
modulator of hippocampal synaptic information processing and cognition. VI. Effect upon recall and recognition of various stimuli. J. Psychol. 39 (2), 443–454.
Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 138, 145–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/J. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1955.9916194.
NLM.2016.06.014. Jenkins, T.A., Elliott, J.J., Ardis, T.C., Cahir, M., Reynolds, G.P., Bell, R., Cooper, S.J.,
Haider, S., Khaliq, S., Tabassum, S., Haleem, D.J., 2012. Role of somatodendritic and 2010. Tryptophan depletion impairs object-recognition memory in the rat: Reversal
postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors on learning and memory functions in rats. by risperidone. Behav. Brain Res. 208 (2), 479–483. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.
Neurochem. Res. 37 (10), 2161–2166. https://doi.org/10.1007/S11064-012-0839- BBR.2009.12.030.
5/FIGURES/2. Kalechstein, A.D., De La Garza, R., Mahoney, J.J., Fantegrossi, W.E., Newton, T.F., 2007.
Haijen, E., Farre, M., de la Torre, R., Pastor, A., Olesti, E., Pizarro, N., Ramaekers, J.G., MDMA use and neurocognition: a meta-analytic review. Psychopharmacology 189
Kuypers, K.P.C., 2018. Peripheral endocannabinoid concentrations are not (4), 531–537. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-006-0601-2/TABLES/3.
associated with verbal memory impairment during MDMA intoxication. Kendall, I., Slotten, H.A., Codony, X., Burgueño, J., Pauwels, P.J., Vela, J.M., Fone, K.C.
Psychopharmacology 235 (3), 709–717. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-017- F., 2011. E-6801, a 5-HT6 receptor agonist, improves recognition memory by
4787-2/FIGURES/2. combined modulation of cholinergic and glutamatergic neurotransmission in the rat.
Hales, J.B., Schlesiger, M.I., Leutgeb, J.K., Squire, L.R., Leutgeb, S., Clark, R.E., 2014. Psychopharmacology 213 (2–3), 413–430. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-010-
Medial entorhinal cortex lesions only partially disrupt hippocampal place cells and 1854-3/TABLES/4.
hippocampus-dependent place memory. Cell Rep. 9 (3), 893–901. https://doi.org/ Kim, J.J., Fanselow, M.S., 1992. Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science
10.1016/J.CELREP.2014.10.009. 256 (5057), 675–677. https://doi.org/10.1126/SCIENCE.1585183.
Harmer, C.J., Bhagwagar, Z., Cowen, P.J., Goodwin, G.M., 2002. Acute administration of King, M.V., Sleight, A.J., Woolley, M.L., Topham, I.A., Marsden, C.A., Fone, K.C.F., 2004.
citalopram facilitates memory consolidation in healthy volunteers. 5-HT6 receptor antagonists reverse delay-dependent deficits in novel object
Psychopharmacology. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-002-1151-X. discrimination by enhancing consolidation–an effect sensitive to NMDA receptor
Harrison, B.J., Olver, J.S., Norman, T.R., Burrows, G.D., Wesnes, K.A., Nathan, P.J., antagonism. Neuropharmacology 47 (2), 195–204. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.
2004. Selective effects of acute serotonin and catecholamine depletion on memory in NEUROPHARM.2004.03.012.
healthy women. J. Psychopharmacol. 18 (1), 32–40. https://doi.org/10.1177/ King, M.V., Spicer, C.H., Sleight, A.J., Marsden, C.A., Fone, K.C.F., 2009. Impact of
0269881104040225. regional 5-HT depletion on the cognitive enhancing effects of a typical 5-ht(6)
Hasler, F., Studerus, E., Lindner, K., Ludewig, S., Vollenweider, F.X., 2009. Investigation receptor antagonist, Ro 04-6790, in the Novel Object Discrimination task.
of serotonin-1A receptor function in the human psychopharmacology of MDMA. Psychopharmacology 202 (1–3), 111–123. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-008-
J. Psychopharmacol. 23 (8), 923–935. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 1334-1.
0269881108094650. Klaassen, T., Riedel, W.J., Deutz, N.E.P., Van Someren, A., Van Praag, H.M., 1999.
Hasselmo, M.E., Stern, C.E., 2014. Theta rhythm and the encoding and retrieval of space Specificity of the tryptophan depletion method. Psychopharmacology 141 (3),
and time. NeuroImage 85, 656–666. https://doi.org/10.1016/J. 279–286. https://doi.org/10.1007/S002130050835.
NEUROIMAGE.2013.06.022. Koenig, J., Cosquer, B., Cassel, J.C., 2008. Activation of septal 5-HT1A receptors alters
Hayward, G., Goodwin, G.M., Cowen, P.J., Harmer, C.J., 2005. Low-dose tryptophan spatial memory encoding, interferes with consolidation, but does not affect retrieval
depletion in recovered depressed patients induces changes in cognitive processing in rats subjected to a water-maze task. Hippocampus 18 (1), 99–118. https://doi.
without depressive symptoms. Biol. Psychiatry 57 (5), 517–524. https://doi.org/ org/10.1002/HIPO.20368.
10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2004.11.016. Koenig, J., Lecourtier, L., Cosquer, B., Pereira, P.M., Cassel, J.C., 2011. Spatial memory
Heckman, P.R.A., Blokland, A., Sambeth, A., 2019a. No interaction between rivastigmine alterations by activation of septal 5HT1A receptors: No implication of cholinergic
and citalopram on memory and novelty processing in healthy human volunteers. septohippocampal neurons. Psychopharmacology 214 (2), 437–454. https://doi.
J. Psychopharmacol. 33 (2), 210–218. https://doi.org/10.1177/ org/10.1007/S00213-010-2049-7/FIGURES/10.
0269881118796816. Korotkova, T., Ponomarenko, A., Monaghan, C.K., Poulter, S.L., Cacucci, F., Wills, T.,
Heckman, P.R.A., Blokland, A., Sambeth, A., 2019b. No interaction between rivastigmine Hasselmo, M.E., Lever, C., 2018. Reconciling the different faces of hippocampal
and citalopram on memory and novelty processing in healthy human volunteers. theta: The role of theta oscillations in cognitive, emotional and innate behaviors.
J. Psychopharmacol. 33 (2), 210–218. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 85, 65–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.
0269881118796816. NEUBIOREV.2017.09.004.
Helmbold, K., Bubenzer, S., Dahmen, B., Eisert, A., Gaber, T.J., Habel, U., Konrad, K., Kosofsky, B.E., Molliver, M.E., 1987. The serotoninergic innervation of cerebral cortex:
Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Zepf, F.D., 2013. Influence of acute tryptophan depletion on different classes of axon terminals arise from dorsal and median raphe nuclei.
verbal declarative episodic memory in young adult females. Amino Acids 45 (5), Synapse 1 (2), 153–168. https://doi.org/10.1002/syn.890010204.
1207–1219. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00726-013-1582-1/FIGURES/6. Kragel, J.E., VanHaerents, S., Templer, J.W., Schuele, S., Rosenow, J.M., Nilakantan, A.
Hernández-Pérez, J.J., Gutiérrez-Guzmán, B.E., López-Vázquez, M., Olvera-Cortés, M.E., S., Bridge, D.J., 2020. Hippocampal theta coordinates memory processing during
2015. Supramammillary serotonin reduction alters place learning and concomitant visual exploration. Elife 9, e52108. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.52108.sa2.
hippocampal, septal, and supramammillar theta activity in a Morris water maze. Kubik, S., Miyashita, T., Kubik-Zahorodna, A., Guzowski, J.F., 2012. Loss of activity-
Front. Pharmacol. 6 (OCT), 250. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPHAR.2015.00250/ dependent Arc gene expression in the retrosplenial cortex after hippocampal
BIBTEX. inactivation: interaction in a higher-order memory circuit. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.
Hirst, W.D., Andree, T.H., Aschmies, S., Childers, W.E., Comery, T.A., Dawson, L.A., 97 (1), 124–131. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NLM.2011.10.004.
Day, M., Feingold, I.B., Grauer, S.M., Harrison, B.L., Hughes, Z.A., Kao, J., Kelly, M. Kuypers, K.P.C., De La Torre, R., Farre, M., Pujadas, M., Ramaekers, J.G., 2013.
G., Van Der Lee, H., Rosenzweig-Lipson, S., Saab, A.L., Smith, D.L., Sullivan, K., Inhibition of MDMA-induced increase in cortisol does not prevent acute impairment
Rizzo, S.J.S., Schechter, L.E., 2008. Correlating efficacy in rodent cognition models of verbal memory. Br. J. Pharmacol. 168 (3), 607–617. https://doi.org/10.1111/
with in vivo 5-hydroxytryptamine1a receptor occupancy by a novel antagonist, (R)- J.1476-5381.2012.02196.X.
N-(2-methyl-(4-indolyl-1-piperazinyl)ethyl)-N-(2-pyridinyl)-cyclohexane Kuypers, Kim P.C., Ramaekers, J.G., 2005. Transient memory impairment after acute
carboxamide (WAY-101405). J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 325 (1), 134–145. https:// dose of 75 mg 3.4- Methylenedioxymethamphetamine. J. Psychopharmacol. 19 (6),
doi.org/10.1124/JPET.107.133082. 633–639. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881105056670.
Hood, S.D., Bell, C.J., Nutt, D.J., 2005. Acute tryptophan depletion. Part I: rationale and Kuypers, Kim P.C., Theunissen, E.L., Van Wel, J.H.P., De Sousa, E.B., Perna, F.,
methodology. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 39 (7), 558–564. https://doi.org/10.1111/ Linssen, A., Sambeth, A., Schultz, B.G., Ramaekers, J.G., 2016. Verbal memory
j.1440-1614.2005.01627.x.

15
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

impairment in polydrug ecstasy users: a clinical perspective. PLOS ONE 11 (2), Meador, K.J., Loring, D.W., Hendrix, N., Nichols, M.E., Oberzan, R., & Moore, E.E.
e0149438. https://doi.org/10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0149438. (2008). Synergistic anticholinergic and antiserotonergic effects in humans. 〈Http://
Lanctô, K.L., Herrmann, N., Mazzotta, P., 2001. Special articles role of serotonin in the Dx.Doi.Org/10.1080/01688639508405149〉, 17(4), 611–621. 〈https://doi.org/10
behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. .1080/01688639508405149〉.
Neurosci. 13 (1), 5. https://doi.org/10.1176/jnp.13.1.5. Meneses, A., 2013. 5-HT systems: emergent targets for memory formation and memory
Lehmann, O., Jeltsch, H., Lehnardt, O., Pain, L., Lazarus, C., Cassel, J.C., 2000. Combined alterations. Rev. Neurosci. 24 (6), 629–664. https://doi.org/10.1515/revneuro-
lesions of cholinergic and serotonergic neurons in the rat brain using 192 IgG- 2013-0026.
saporin and 5,7-dihydroxytryptamine: neurochemical and behavioural Meneses, A., 2015. Serotonin, neural markers and memory. Front. Pharmacol. 6 (Jun),
characterization. Eur. J. Neurosci. 12 (1), 67–79. https://doi.org/10.1046/J.1460- 143. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphar.2015.00143.
9568.2000.00881.X. Merens, W., Booij, L., Haffmans, P.M.J., Van Der Does, A.J.W., 2008. The effects of
Li, X.B., Inoue, T., Abekawa, T., Weng, S.M., Nakagawa, S., Izumi, T., Koyama, T., 2006. experimentally lowered serotonin function on emotional information processing and
5-HT1A receptor agonist affects fear conditioning through stimulations of the memory in remitted depressed patients. J. Psychopharmacol. 22 (6), 653–662.
postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors in the hippocampus and amygdala. Eur. J. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881107081531.
Pharmacol. 532 (1–2), 74–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.EJPHAR.2005.12.008. Miranda, M., Bekinschtein, P., 2018. Plasticity mechanisms of memory consolidation and
Lieben, C.K.J., Oorsouw, K., Van, Deutz, N.E.P., Blokland, A., 2004. Acute tryptophan reconsolidation in the perirhinal cortex. Neuroscience 370, 46–61. https://doi.org/
depletion induced by a gelatin-based mixture impairs object memory but not 10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2017.06.002.
affective behavior and spatial learning in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 151 (1–2), Mitchell, E.S., McDevitt, R.A., Neumaier, J.F., 2009. Adaptations in 5-HT receptor
53–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BBR.2003.08.002. expression and function: implications for treatment of cognitive impairment in
Lieben, C.K.J., Blokland, A., Şik, A., Sung, E., Van Nieuwenhuizen, P., Schreiber, R., aging. J. Neurosci. Res. 87 (12), 2803–2811. https://doi.org/10.1002/JNR.22100.
2005a. The selective 5-HT6 receptor antagonist Ro4368554 restores memory Mitchnick, K.A., Creighton, S., O’Hara, M., Kalisch, B.E., Winters, B.D., 2015. Differential
performance in cholinergic and serotonergic models of memory deficiency in the rat. contributions of de novo and maintenance DNA methyltransferases to object
Neuropsychopharmacol. Off. Publ. Am. Coll. Neuropsychopharmacol. 30 (12), memory processing in the rat hippocampus and perirhinal cortex – a double
2169–2179. https://doi.org/10.1038/SJ.NPP.1300777. dissociation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 41 (6), 773–786. https://doi.org/10.1111/
Lieben, C.K.J., Blokland, A., Şik, A., Sung, E., Van Nieuwenhuizen, P., Schreiber, R., EJN.12819.
2005b. The selective 5-HT6 receptor antagonist Ro4368554 restores memory Miyoshi, E., Wietzikoski, E.C., Bortolanza, M., Boschen, S.L., Canteras, N.S., Izquierdo, I.,
performance in cholinergic and serotonergic models of memory deficiency in the rat. Da Cunha, C., 2012. Both the dorsal hippocampus and the dorsolateral striatum are
Neuropsychopharmacol. Off. Publ. Am. Coll. Neuropsychopharmacol. 30 (12), needed for rat navigation in the Morris water maze. Behav. Brain Res. 226 (1),
2169–2179. https://doi.org/10.1038/SJ.NPP.1300777. 171–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BBR.2011.09.011.
Lieben, C.K.J., Steinbusch, H.W.M., Blokland, A., 2006. 5,7-DHT lesion of the dorsal Mlinar, B., Corradetti, R., 2018. Differential modulation of CA1 impulse flow by
raphe nuclei impairs object recognition but not affective behavior and corticosterone endogenous serotonin along the hippocampal longitudinal axis. Hippocampus 28
response to stressor in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 168 (2), 197–207. https://doi.org/ (3), 217–225. https://doi.org/10.1002/HIPO.22825.
10.1016/J.BBR.2005.11.003. Montezinho, L.P., Miller, S., Plath, N., Jensen, N.H., Karlsson, J.-J., Witten, L., Mørk, A.,
Lin, R., Liang, J., Luo, M., 2021. The raphe dopamine system: roles in salience encoding, 2010. The effects of acute treatment with escitalopram on the different stages of
memory expression, and addiction. Trends Neurosci. 44 (5), 366–377. https://doi. contextual fear conditioning are reversed by atomoxetine. Psychopharmacology 212
org/10.1016/J.TINS.2021.01.002. (2), 131–143. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-010-1917-5. LK - https://uzb.
Lippert, T.H., Filshie, M., Mück, A.O., Seeger, H., Zwirner, M., 1996. Serotonin swisscovery.slsp.ch/openurl/41SLSP_UZB/41SLSP_UZB:UZB?sid=Elsevier:
metabolite excretion after postmenopausal estradiol therapy. Maturitas 24 (1–2), EMBASE&sid=EMBASE&issn=00333158&id=doi:10.1007%2Fs00213-010-1917-
37–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-5122(95)00998-1. 5&atitle=The+effects+of+acute+treatment+with+escitalopram+on+the+
Liu, H., Zhou, J., Fang, L., Liu, Z., Fan, S., Xie, P., 2013. Acute tryptophan depletion different+stages+of+contextual+fear+conditioning+are+reversed+by+
reduces nitric oxide synthase in the rat hippocampus. Neurochem. Res. 38 (12), atomoxetine&stitle=Psychopharmacology&title=Psychopharmacology&
2595–2603. https://doi.org/10.1007/S11064-013-1177-Y/FIGURES/4. volume=212&issue=2&s131&epage=143&aulast=Montezinho&aufirst=Liliana+
Logan, B.J., Laverty, R., Sanderson, W.D., Yee, Y.Ben, 1988. Differences between rats and P.&auinit=L.P.&aufull=Montezinho+L.P.&coden=PSCHD&isbn=&pa.
mice in MDMA (methylenedioxymethylamphetamine) neurotoxicity. Eur. J. Morici, J.F., Ciccia, L., Malleret, G., Gingrich, J.A., Bekinschtein, P., Weisstaub, N.V.,
Pharmacol. 152 (3), 227–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/0014-2999(88)90717-0. 2015. Serotonin 2a receptor and serotonin 1a receptor interact within the medial
Luchetti, A., Bota, A., Weitemier, A., Mizuta, K., Sato, M., Islam, T., McHugh, T.J., prefrontal cortex during recognition memory in mice. Front. Pharmacol. 6 (DEC)
Tashiro, A., Hayashi, Y., 2020. Two functionally distinct serotonergic projections https://doi.org/10.3389/FPHAR.2015.00298.
into hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 40 (25), 4936–4944. https://doi.org/10.1523/ Morley, K.C., Gallate, J.E., Hunt, G.E., Mallet, P.E., McGregor, I.S., 2001. Increased
JNEUROSCI.2724-19.2020. anxiety and impaired memory in rats 3 months after administration of 3,4-methyl­
Lüttgen, M., Elvander, E., Madjid, N., Ögren, S.O., 2005. Analysis of the role of 5-HT1A enedioxymethamphetamine (“ecstasy”). Eur. J. Pharmacol. 433 (1), 91–99. https://
receptors in spatial and aversive learning in the rat. Neuropharmacology 48 (6), doi.org/10.1016/S0014-2999(01)01512-6.
830–852. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROPHARM.2005.01.007. Morris, R.G.M., Garrud, P., Rawlins, J.N.P. al, O’Keefe, J., 1982. Place navigation
Lyles, J., Cadet, J.L., 2003. Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, Ecstasy) impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature 297 (5868), 681–683.
neurotoxicity: cellular and molecular mechanisms. Brain Res. Rev. 42 (2), 155–168. Mosienko, V., Matthes, S., Hirth, N., Beis, D., Flinders, M., Bader, M., Hansson, A.C.,
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0173(03)00173-5. Alenina, N., 2014. Adaptive changes in serotonin metabolism preserve normal
Mace, J., Porter, R., O’Brien, J., Gallagher, P., 2008. Cognitive effects of acute behavior in mice with reduced TPH2 activity. Neuropharmacology 85, 73–80.
tryptophan depletion in the healthy elderly. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 20 (2), 78–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROPHARM.2014.05.015.
https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1601-5215.2008.00272.X. Müller, C.P., 2013. Episodic memories and their relevance for psychoactive drug use and
Majlessi, N., Naghdi, N., 2002. Impaired spatial learning in the Morris water maze addiction. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 0 (APR 2013), 34. https://doi.org/10.3389/
induced by serotonin reuptake inhibitors in rats. Behav. Pharmacol. 13 (3), 237–242. FNBEH.2013.00034/BIBTEX.
https://doi.org/10.1097/00008877-200205000-00007. Müller, C.P., Homberg, J.R., 2015. The role of serotonin in drug use and addiction.
Majlessi, N., Kadkhodaee, M., Parviz, M., Naghdi, N., 2003. Serotonin depletion in rat Behav. Brain Res. 277, 146–192. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BBR.2014.04.007.
hippocampus attenuates l-NAME-induced spatial learning deficits. Brain Res. 963 Murphy, S.E., Wright, L.C., Browning, M., Cowen, P.J., Harmer, C.J., 2020a. A role for 5-
(1–2), 244–251. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(02)03987-2. HT4 receptors in human learning and memory. Psychol. Med. 50 (16), 2722–2730.
Majlessi, N., Choopani, S., Bozorgmehr, T., Azizi, Z., 2008. Involvement of hippocampal https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719002836.
nitric oxide in spatial learning in the rat. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 90 (2), 413–419. Murphy, S.E., Wright, L.C., Browning, M., Cowen, P.J., Harmer, C.J., 2020b. A role for 5-
https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NLM.2008.04.010. HT4 receptors in human learning and memory. Psychol. Med. 50 (16), 2722–2730.
Malleret, G., Hen, R., Guillou, J.L., Segu, L., Buhot, M.C., 1999. 5-HT1B receptor knock- https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719002836.
out mice exhibit increased exploratory activity and enhanced spatial memory Mutlu, O., Ulak, G., Çelikyurt, I.K., Akar, F.Y., Erden, F., 2011. Effects of citalopram on
performance in the Morris water maze. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 19 (14), cognitive performance in passive avoidance, elevated plus-maze and three-panel
6157–6168. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-14-06157.1999. runway tasks in naïve rats. Chin. J. Physiol. 54 (1), 36–46. https://doi.org/10.4077/
Maren, S., Phan, K.L., Liberzon, I., 2013. The contextual brain: implications for fear CJP.2011.AMK077.
conditioning, extinction and psychopathology. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14 (6), 417–428. Naghdi, N., Harooni, H.E., 2005. The effect of intrahippocampal injections of ritanserin
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3492. (5HT2A/2C antagonist) and granisetron (5HT3 antagonist) on learning as assessed in
Matsumoto, M., Togashi, H., Ohashi, S., Tachibana, K., Yamaguchi, T., Yoshioka, M., the spatial version of the water maze. Behav. Brain Res. 157 (2), 205–210. https://
2004. Serotonergic modulation of psychological stress-induced alteration in synaptic doi.org/10.1016/J.BBR.2004.06.024.
plasticity in the rat hippocampal CA1 field. Brain Res. 1022 (1–2), 221–225. https:// Natale, S., Anzilotti, S., Petrozziello, T., Ciccone, R., Serani, A., Calabrese, L.,
doi.org/10.1016/J.BRAINRES.2004.06.078. Severino, B., Frecentese, F., Secondo, A., Pannaccione, A., Fiorino, F., Cuomo, O.,
McAllister-Williams, R., Massey, A., Rugg, M., 2002. Effects of tryptophan depletion on Vinciguerra, A., D’Esposito, L., Sadile, A.G., Cabib, S., Di Renzo, G., Annunziato, L.,
brain potential correlates of episodic memory retrieval. Psychopharmacology 160 Molinaro, P., 2020. Genetic up-regulation or pharmacological activation of the Na+/
(4), 434–442. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-001-0996-8. Ca2+ exchanger 1 (NCX1) enhances hippocampal-dependent contextual and spatial
McEwen, B.S., Magarinos, A.M., 1997. Stress effects on morphology and function of the learning and memory. Mol. Neurobiol. 57 (5), 2358–2376. https://doi.org/10.1007/
hippocampusa. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 821 (1), 271–284. https://doi.org/10.1111/ S12035-020-01888-4/FIGURES/10.
J.1749-6632.1997.TB48286.X. Netto, S.M., Silveira, R., Coimbra, N.C., Joca, S.R.L., Guimarães, F.S., 2002. Anxiogenic
McGaugh, J.L., 2000. Memory - a century of consolidation. Science 287 (5451), effect of median raphe nucleus lesion in stressed rats. Prog. Neuro Psychopharmacol.
248–251. https://doi.org/10.1126/SCIENCE.287.5451.248/ASSET/708969E9- Biol. Psychiatry 26 (6), 1135–1141. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-5846(02)
5128-42DA-AB08-0CCB17B17BC0/ASSETS/GRAPHIC/SE0108182002.JPEG. 00248-8.

16
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

Nilsson, O.G., Strecker, R.E., Daszuta, A., Björklund, A., 1988. Combined cholinergic and in an associative relearning network during task and rest. NeuroImage 249, 118887.
serotonergic denervation of the forebrain produces severe deficits in a spatial https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2022.118887.
learning task in the rat. Brain Res. 453 (1–2), 235–246. https://doi.org/10.1016/ Ren, J., Isakova, A., Friedmann, D., Zeng, J., Grutzner, S.M., Pun, A., Zhao, G.Q.,
0006-8993(88)90163-1. Kolluru, S.S., Wang, R., Lin, R., Li, P., Li, A., Raymond, J.L., Luo, Q., Luo, M.,
Ohmura, Y., Izumi, T., Yamaguchi, T., Tsutsui-Kimura, I., Yoshida, T., Yoshioka, M., Quake, S.R., Luo, L., 2019. Single-cell transcriptomes and whole-brain projections of
2010. The serotonergic projection from the median raphe nucleus to the ventral serotonin neurons in the mouse dorsal and median raphe nuclei. ELife 8. https://doi.
hippocampus is involved in the retrieval of fear memory through the corticotropin- org/10.7554/eLife.49424.
releasing factor type 2 receptor. Neuropsychopharmacol. Off. Publ. Am. Coll. Reneman, L., Endert, E., De Bruin, K., Lavalaye, J., Feenstra, M.G., De Wolff, F.A.,
Neuropsychopharmacol. 35 (6), 1271–1278. https://doi.org/10.1038/ Booij, J., 2001. The acute and chronic effects of MDMA (“Ecstasy”) on cortical 5-
NPP.2009.229. HT2A receptors in rat and human brain, 2002 26:3 Neuropsychopharmacology 26
Okaty, B.W., Commons, K.G., Dymecki, S.M., 2019. Embracing diversity in the 5-HT (3), 387–396. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0893-133x(01)00366-9.
neuronal system. In: Nature Reviews Neuroscience, Vol. 20. Nature Publishing Richter-Levin, G., Segal, M., 1989. Spatial performance is severely impaired in rats with
Group., pp. 397–424. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-019-0151-3 combined reduction of serotonergic and cholinergic transmission. Brain Res. 477
Olivier, J.D.A., Jans, L.A.W., Korte-Bouws, G.A.H., Korte, S.M., Deen, P.M.T., Cools, A.R., (1–2), 404–407. https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(89)91437-6.
Ellenbroek, B.A., Blokland, A., 2008. Acute tryptophan depletion dose dependently Richter-Levin, Gal, Greenberger, V., Segal, M., 1994. The effects of general and restricted
impairs object memory in serotonin transporter knockout rats. Psychopharmacology serotonergic lesions on hippocampal electrophysiology and behavior. Brain Res. 642
200 (2), 243–254. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-008-1201-0/FIGURES/3. (1–2), 111–116. https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(94)90911-3.
Olvera-Cortés, E., Cervantes, M., González-Burgos, I., 2002. Place-learning, but not cue- Riedel, W.J., Klaassen, T., Deutz, N.E.P., van Someren, A., van Praag, H.M., 1999a.
learning training, modifies the hippocampal theta rhythm in rats. Brain Res. Bull. 58 Tryptophan depletion in normal volunteers produces selective impairment in
(3), 261–270. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0361-9230(02)00769-4. memory consolidation. Psychopharmacology 141 (4), 362–369. https://doi.org/
Orsetti, M., Dellarole, A., Ferri, S., Ghi, P., 2003. Acquisition, retention, and recall of 10.1007/s002130050845.
memory after injection of RS67333, a 5-HT(4) receptor agonist, into the nucleus Riedel, Wim J., Klaassen, T., Deutz, N.E.P., Van Someren, A., Van Praag, H.M., 1999b.
basalis magnocellularis of the rat. Learn. Mem. 10 (5), 420–426. https://doi.org/ Tryptophan depletion in normal volunteers produces selective impairment in
10.1101/LM.67303. memory consolidation. Psychopharmacology 141 (4), 362–369. https://doi.org/
Page, M.J., McKenzie, J.E., Bossuyt, P.M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T.C., Mulrow, C.D., 10.1007/S002130050845.
Shamseer, L., Tetzlaff, J.M., Akl, E.A., Brennan, S.E., Chou, R., Glanville, J., Riekkinen, M., Sirvi6, J., Toivanen, T., Riekkinen, E., 1995. Combined treatment with a
Grimshaw, J.M., Hróbjartsson, A., Lalu, M.M., Li, T., Loder, E.W., Mayo-Wilson, E., 5HTIA receptor agonist and a muscarinic acetylcholine receptor antagonist disrupts
McDonald, S., Moher, D., 2021. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline water maze navigation behavior. Psychopharmacology 122, 137–146.
for reporting systematic reviews. PLOS Med. 18 (3), e1003583 https://doi.org/ Riekkinen, P., Jäkälä, P., Sirviö, J., Riekkinen, P., 1991. The effects of increased
10.1371/JOURNAL.PMED.1003583. serotonergic and decreased cholinergic activities on spatial navigation performance
Papp, E.C., Hájos, N., Acsády, L., Freund, T.F., 1999. Medial septal and median raphe in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 39 (1), 25–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/0091-
innervation of vasoactive intestinal polypeptide-containing interneurons in the 3057(91)90392-F.
hippocampus. Neuroscience 90 (2), 369–382. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4522 Roberts, A.J., Krucker, T., Levy, C.L., Slanina, K.A., Sutcliffe, J.G., Hedlund, P.B., 2004.
(98)00455-2. Mice lacking 5-HT7 receptors show specific impairments in contextual learning. Eur.
Park, S.B., Coull, J.T., McShane, R.H., Young, A.H., Sahakian, B.J., Robbins, T.W., J. Neurosci. 19 (7), 1913–1922. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1460-9568.2004.03288.
Cowen, P.J., 1994. Tryptophan depletion in normal volunteers produces selective X.
impairments in learning and memory. Neuropharmacology 33 (3–4), 575–588. Rogers, D.C., Hagan, J.J., 2001. 5-HT6 receptor antagonists enhance retention of a water
https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3908(94)90089-2. maze task in the rat. Psychopharmacology 158 (2), 114–119. https://doi.org/
Passani, M.B., Blandina, P., 1998. Cognitive implications for H3 and 5-HT3 receptor 10.1007/S002130100840.
modulation of cortical cholinergic function: a parallel story. Methods Find. Exp. Clin. Roiser, J.P., Müller, U., Clark, L., Sahakian, B.J., 2007. The effects of acute tryptophan
Pharmacol. 20 (8), 725–733. https://doi.org/10.1358/MF.1998.20.8.487510. depletion and serotonin transporter polymorphism on emotional processing in
Pause, B.M., Zlomuzica, A., Kinugawa, K., Mariani, J., Pietrowsky, R., Dere, E., 2013. memory and attention. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 10 (4), 449–461. https://doi.
Perspectives on episodic-like and episodic memory. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7 (APR org/10.1017/S146114570600705X.
2013) https://doi.org/10.3389/FNBEH.2013.00033. Romano, A.G., Quinn, J.L., Liu, R., Dave, K.D., Schwab, D., Alexander, G., Aloyo, V.J.,
Perez-Garcia, G., Meneses, A., 2008. Memory formation, amnesia, improved memory and Harvey, J.A., 2006. Effect of serotonin depletion on 5-HT2A-mediated learning in
reversed amnesia: 5-HT role. In: Behavioural Brain Research, Vol. 195. Elsevier, the rabbit: evidence for constitutive activity of the 5-HT2A receptor in vivo.
pp. 17–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2007.11.027. Psychopharmacology 184 (2), 173–181. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-005-0245-
Pitsikas, N., 2015. The role of nitric oxide in the object recognition memory. Behav. Brain 7. LK - https://uzb.swisscovery.slsp.ch/openurl/41SLSP_UZB/41SLSP_UZB:UZB?
Res. 285, 200–207. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BBR.2014.06.008. sid=Elsevier:EMBASE&sid=EMBASE&issn=00333158&id=doi:10.1007%
Pitsikas, N., Rigamonti, A.E., Cella, S.G., Muller, E.E., 2003. The 5-HT1A receptor 2Fs00213-005-0245-7&atitle=Effect+of+serotonin+depletion+on+5-HT2A-
antagonist WAY 100635 improves rats performance in different models of amnesia mediated+learning+in+the+rabbit%3A+Evidence+for+constitutive+activity+of+
evaluated by the object recognition task. Brain Res. 983 (1–2), 215–222. https://doi. the+5-HT2A+receptor+in+vivo&stitle=Psychopharmacology&title=Psychophar
org/10.1016/S0006-8993(03)03091-9. macology&volume=184&issue=2&s173&epage=181&aulast=Romano&aufirst=A.
Prehn, K., Stengl, H., Grittner, U., Kosiolek, R., Ölschläger, A., Weidemann, A., Floël, A., G.&auinit=A.G.&aufull=Romano+A.G.&coden=PSCHD&isbn=&pages=17.
2016. Effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation and serotonergic Rotenberg, A., Mayford, M., Hawkins, R.D., Kandel, E.R., Muller, R.U., 1996. Mice
enhancement on memory performance in young and older adults. expressing activated CaMKII lack low frequency LTP and do not form stable place
Neuropsychopharmacology 42 (2), 551–561. https://doi.org/10.1038/ cells in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Cell 87 (7), 1351–1361. https://doi.org/
npp.2016.170. 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81829-2.
Preston, A.R., Eichenbaum, H., 2013. Interplay of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in Rothman, R.B., Baumann, M.H., Dersch, C.M., Romero, D.V., Rice, K.C., Carroll, F.I.,
memory. Curr. Biol. 23 (17), R764–R773. https://doi.org/10.1016/J. Partilla, J.S., 2001. Amphetamine-type central nervous system stimulants release
CUB.2013.05.041. norepinephrine more potently than they release dopamine and serotonin. Synapse
Quednow, B.B., Jessen, F., Kühn, K.U., Maier, W., Daum, I., Wagner, M., 2006. Memory 39, 32–41. https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-2396.
deficits in abstinent MDMA (ecstasy) users: Neuropsychological evidence of frontal Rubinsztein, J.S., Rogers, R.D., Riedel, W.J., Mehta, M.A., Robbins, T.W., Sahakian, B.J.,
dysfunction. J. Psychopharmacol. 20 (3), 373–384. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 2001. Acute dietary tryptophan depletion impairs maintenance of “affective set” and
0269881106061200. delayed visual recognition in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology 154 (3),
Quednow, B.B., Kometer, M., Geyer, M.A., Vollenweider, F.X., 2012. Psilocybin-induced 319–326. https://doi.org/10.1007/S002130000655.
deficits in automatic and controlled inhibition are attenuated by ketanserin in Ruhé, H.G., Mason, N.S., Schene, A.H., 2007. Mood is indirectly related to serotonin,
healthy human volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology 37 (3), 630–640. https://doi. norepinephrine and dopamine levels in humans: a meta-analysis of monoamine
org/10.1038/npp.2011.228. depletion studies, 2007 12:4 Mol. Psychiatry 12 (4), 331–359. https://doi.org/
Quiedeville, A., Boulouard, M., Hamidouche, K., Da Silva Costa-Aze, V., Nee, G., 10.1038/sj.mp.4001949.
Rochais, C., Dallemagne, P., Fabis, F., Freret, T., Bouet, V., 2015. Chronic activation Rutten, K., Lieben, C., Smits, L., Blokland, A., 2007. The PDE4 inhibitor rolipram reverses
of 5-HT4 receptors or blockade of 5-HT6 receptors improve memory performances. object memory impairment induced by acute tryptophan depletion in the rat.
Behav. Brain Res. 293, 10–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BBR.2015.07.020. Psychopharmacology 192 (2), 275–282. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-006-
Rambousek, L., Palenicek, T., Vales, K., Stuchlik, A., 2014. The effect of psilocin on 0697-4.
memory acquisition, retrieval, and consolidation in the rat. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8 Ryan, L., Hoscheidt, S., Nadel, L., 2008. Perspectives on episodic and semantic memory
(MAY), 180. https://doi.org/10.3389/FNBEH.2014.00180/BIBTEX. retrieval. Handb. Behav. Neurosci. 18, 5–616.
Rasmussen, K., McCreary, A.C., Shanks, E.A., 2004. Attenuation of the effects of Rychtyk, J., Partyka, A., Gdula-Argasińska, J., Mysłowska, K., Wilczyńska, N.,
fluoxetine on serotonergic neuronal activity by pindolol in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 355 Jastrzębska-Więsek, M., Wesołowska, A., 2019. 5-HT6 receptor agonist and
(1–2), 1–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEULET.2003.10.039. antagonist improve memory impairments and hippocampal BDNF signaling
Rauchs, G., Orban, P., Balteau, E., Schmidt, C., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., Maquet, P., alterations induced by MK-801. Brain Res. 1722, 146375 https://doi.org/10.1016/J.
Peigneux, P., 2008. Partially segregated neural networks for spatial and contextual BRAINRES.2019.146375.
memory in virtual navigation. Hippocampus 18 (5), 503–518. https://doi.org/ Sambeth, A., Riedel, W.J., Tillie, D.E., Blokland, A., Postma, A., Schmitt, J.A.J., 2009.
10.1002/HIPO.20411. Memory impairments in humans after acute tryptophan depletion using a novel
Reed, M.B., Klöbl, M., Godbersen, G.M., Handschuh, P.A., Ritter, V., Spurny-Dworak, B., gelatin-based protein drink. J. Psychopharmacol. 23 (1), 56–64. https://doi.org/
Unterholzner, J., Kraus, C., Gryglewski, G., Winkler, D., Seiger, R., Vanicek, T., 10.1177/0269881108089577.
Hahn, A., Lanzenberger, R., 2022. Serotonergic modulation of effective connectivity

17
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

Sambeth, A., Riedel, W.J., Klinkenberg, I., Kähkönen, S., Blokland, A., 2015. Biperiden memory deficit induced by acute tryptophan depletion. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 600
selectively induces memory impairment in healthy volunteers: no interaction with (1–3), 98–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.EJPHAR.2008.10.027.
citalopram. Psychopharmacology 232 (11), 1887–1897. https://doi.org/10.1007/ van Donkelaar, Eva L., Blokland, A., Lieben, C.K.J., Kenis, G., Ferrington, L., Kelly, P.A.
s00213-014-3822-9. T., Steinbusch, H.W.M., Prickaerts, J., 2010. Acute tryptophan depletion in C57BL/6
Sambeth, Anke, Blokland, A., Harmer, C.J., Kilkens, T.O.C., Nathan, P.J., Porter, R.J., mice does not induce central serotonin reduction or affective behavioural changes.
Schmitt, J.A.J., Scholtissen, B., Sobczak, S., Young, A.H., Riedel, W.J., 2007. Sex Neurochem. Int. 56 (1), 21–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUINT.2009.08.010.
differences in the effect of acute tryptophan depletion on declarative episodic Van Donkelaar, E.L., Blokland, A., Ferrington, L., Kelly, P.A.T., Steinbusch, H.W.M.,
memory: a pooled analysis of nine studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 516–529. Prickaerts, J., 2011. Mechanism of acute tryptophan depletion: is it only serotonin?,
https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2006.11.009. 2011 16:7 Mol. Psychiatry 16 (7), 695–713. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2011.9.
Sarkisyan, G., Hedlund, P.B., 2009. The 5-HT7 receptor is involved in allocentric spatial Van Goethem, N.P., Schreiber, R., Newman-Tancredi, A., Varney, M., Prickaerts, J.,
memory information processing. Behav. Brain Res. 202 (1), 26–31. https://doi.org/ 2015. Divergent effects of the ‘biased’ 5-HT1A receptor agonists F15599 and F13714
10.1016/J.BBR.2009.03.011. in a novel object pattern separation task. Br. J. Pharmacol. 172 (10), 2532–2543.
Sarnyai, Z., Sibille, E.L., Pavlides, C., Fenster, R.J., McEwen, B.S., Tóth, M., 2000. https://doi.org/10.1111/BPH.13071.
Impaired hippocampal-dependent learning and functional abnormalities in the Van Wel, J.H.P., Kuypers, K.P.C., Theunissen, E.L., Bosker, W.M., Bakker, K.,
hippocampus in mice lacking serotonin1A receptors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 97 (26), Ramaekers, J.G., 2011. Blockade of 5-HT2 receptor selectively prevents MDMA-
14731–14736. https://doi.org/10.1073/PNAS.97.26.14731. induced verbal memory impairment. Neuropsychopharmacology 36 (9), 1932–1939.
Scarnà, A., McTavish, S.F.B., Cowen, P.J., Goodwin, G.M., Rogers, R.D., 2005. The effects https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2011.80.
of a branched chain amino acid mixture supplemented with tryptophan on Vanicek, T., Reed, M.B., Unterholzner, J., Klöbl, M., Godbersen, G.M., Handschuh, P.A.,
biochemical indices of neurotransmitter function and decision-making. Spurny-Dworak, B., Ritter, V., Gryglewski, G., Kraus, C., Winkler, D.,
Psychopharmacology 179 (4), 761–768. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00213-004- Lanzenberger, R., Seiger, R., 2022. Escitalopram administration, relearning, and
2105-2/FIGURES/1. neuroplastic effects: a diffusion tensor imaging study in healthy individuals.
Schacher, S., Castellucci, V.F., Kandel, E.R., 1988. cAMP evokes long-term facilitation in J. Affect. Disord. 301, 426–432. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JAD.2021.12.135.
Aplysia sensory neurons that requires new protein synthesis. Science 240 (4859), Varga, V., Losonczy, A., Zemelman, B.V., Borhegyi, Z., Nyiri, G., Domonkos, A.,
1667–1669. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2454509. Hangya, B., Holderith, N., Magee, J.C., Freund, T.F., 2009. Fast synaptic subcortical
Schmitt, J.A.J., Jorissen, B.L., Sobczak, S., Van Boxtel, M.P.J., Hogervorst, E., Deutz, N.E. control of hippocampal circuits. Science 326 (5951), 449–453. https://doi.org/
P., Riedel, W.J., 2000. Tryptophan depletion impairs memory consolidation but 10.1126/science.1178307.
improves focussed attention in healthy young volunteers. J. Psychopharmacol. 14 Vertes, R.P., Fortin, W.J., Crane, A.M., 1999. Projections of the median raphe nucleus in
(1), 21–29. https://doi.org/10.1177/026988110001400102. the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 407 (4), 555–582. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1096-
Shahidi, S., Motamedi, F., Naghdi, N., 2004. Effect of reversible inactivation of the 9861(19990517)407:4<555::AID-CNE7>3.0.CO;2-E.
supramammillary nucleus on spatial learning and memory in rats. Brain Res. 1026 Vollenweider, F.X., Csomor, P.A., Knappe, B., Geyer, M.A., Quednow, B.B., 2007. The
(2), 267–274. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BRAINRES.2004.08.030. effects of the preferential 5-HT2A agonist psilocybin on prepulse inhibition of startle
Silva, A.J., Stevens, C.F., Tonegawa, S., Wang, Y., 1992. Deficient hippocampal long- in healthy human volunteers depend on interstimulus interval.
term potentiation in α-calcium-calmodulin kinase II mutant mice. Science 257 Neuropsychopharmacology 32 (9), 1876–1887. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.
(5067), 201–206. https://doi.org/10.1126/SCIENCE.1378648. npp.1301324.
Spennato, G., Zerbib, C., Mondadori, C., Garcia, R., 2008. Fluoxetine protects Waider, J., Popp, S., Mlinar, B., Montalbano, A., Bonfiglio, F., Aboagye, B., Thuy, E.,
hippocampal plasticity during conditioned fear stress and prevents fear learning Kern, R., Thiel, C., Araragi, N., Svirin, E., Schmitt-Böhrer, A.G., Corradetti, R.,
potentiation. Psychopharmacology 196 (4), 583–589. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Lowry, C.A., Lesch, K.P., 2019. Serotonin deficiency increases context-dependent
S00213-007-0993-7/FIGURES/3. fear learning through modulation of hippocampal activity. Front. Neurosci. 13
Squire, L.R., 2004. Memory systems of the brain: a brief history and current perspective. (APR), 245. https://doi.org/10.3389/FNINS.2019.00245/BIBTEX.
Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 82 (3), 171–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/J. Walker, F.R., 2013. A critical review of the mechanism of action for the selective
NLM.2004.06.005. serotonin reuptake inhibitors: Do these drugs possess anti-inflammatory properties
Squire, L.R., Zola, S.M., 1998. Episodic memory, semantic memory, and amnesia. and how relevant is this in the treatment of depression? Neuropharmacology 67,
Hippocampus 8, 205–211. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:3. 304–317. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROPHARM.2012.10.002.
Stackman, R.W., Zhang, G., Sgeirsdóttir, H.N., Cohen, S.J., Munchow, A.H., Barrera, M. Wang, D.V., Yau, H.J., Broker, C.J., Tsou, J.H., Bonci, A., Ikemoto, S., 2015. Mesopontine
P., 2013. Stimulation of serotonin 2A receptors facilitates consolidation and median raphe regulates hippocampal ripple oscillation and memory consolidation.
extinction of fear memory in C57BL/6J mice. Neuropharmacology 64, 403–413. Nat. Neurosci. 18 (5), 728–735. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3998.
https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROPHARM.2012.06.007. Williams, M.T., Morford, L.R.L., Wood, S.L., Rock, S.L., McCrea, A.E., Fukumura, M.,
Stancampiano, R., Cocco, S., Melis, F., Cugusi, C., Sarais, L., Fadda, F., 1997. The Wallace, T.L., Broening, H.W., Moran, M.S., Vorhees, C.V., 2003. Developmental 3,4-
decrease of serotonin release induced by a tryptophan-free amino acid diet does not methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) impairs sequential and spatial but not
affect spatial and passive avoidance learning. Brain Res. 762 (1–2), 269–274. cued learning independent of growth, litter effects or injection stress. Brain Res. 968
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00506-4. (1), 89–101. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(02)04278-6.
Stäubli, U., Xu, F.B., 1995. Effects of 5-HT3 receptor antagonism on hippocampal theta Wilson, M.A., Molliver, M.E., 1991. The organization of serotonergic projections to
rhythm, memory, and LTP induction in the freely moving rat. J. Neurosci. 15 (3), cerebral cortex in primates: Regional distribution of axon terminals. Neuroscience 44
2445–2452. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-03-02445.1995. (3), 537–553. https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4522(91)90076-Z.
Strauss, E., Sherman, E.M., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Wingen, M., Langer, S., Ramaekers, J.G., 2006. Verbal memory performance during
Administration, norms, and commentary, 3rd ed. - PsycNET. 〈https://psycnet.apa.org/ subchronic challenge with a selective serotonergic and a mixed action
record/2006–04736-000〉. antidepressant. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 21 (7), 473–479. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Tada, K., Kasamo, K., Suzuki, T., Matsuzaki, Y., Kojima, T., 2004. Endogenous 5-HT HUP.800.
inhibits firing activity of hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons during conditioned Wingen, M., Kuypers, K.P.C., Ramaekers, J.G., 2007. Selective verbal and spatial memory
fear stress-induced freezing behavior through stimulating 5-HT1A receptors. impairment after 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptor blockade in healthy volunteers pre-
Hippocampus 14 (2), 143–147. https://doi.org/10.1002/HIPO.10178/FORMAT/ treated with an SSRI. J. Psychopharmacol. 21 (5), 477–485. https://doi.org/
PDF. 10.1177/0269881106072506.
Teixeira, C.M., Rosen, Z.B., Suri, D., Sun, Q., Hersh, M., Sargin, D., Dincheva, I., Winters, B.D., Saksida, L.M., Bussey, T.J., 2008. Object recognition memory:
Morgan, A.A., Spivack, S., Krok, A.C., Hirschfeld-Stoler, T., Lambe, E.K., neurobiological mechanisms of encoding, consolidation and retrieval. Neurosci.
Siegelbaum, S.A., Ansorge, M.S., 2018. Hippocampal 5-HT input regulates memory Biobehav. Rev. 32 (5), 1055–1070. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.
formation and schaffer collateral excitation. Neuron 98 (5), 992–1004. https://doi. NEUBIOREV.2008.04.004.
org/10.1016/J.NEURON.2018.04.030. Wolff, M., Costet, P., Gross, C., Hen, R., Segu, L., Buhot, M.C., 2004. Age-dependent
Tolman, E.C., 1948. Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol. Rev. 55 (4), 189–208. effects of serotonin-1A receptor gene deletion in spatial learning abilities in mice.
https://doi.org/10.1037/H0061626. Mol. Brain Res. 130 (1–2), 39–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.
Tulving, E., 1972. 12. Episodic and semantic memory. In: Tulving, E., Donaldson, W. MOLBRAINRES.2004.07.012.
(Eds.), Organization of memory/. Academic Press, NY, pp. 381–403. Woolley, M.L., Marsden, C.A., Sleight, A.J., Fone, K.C.F., 2003. Reversal of a cholinergic-
Twining, R.C., Lepak, K., Kirry, A.J., Gilmartin, M.R., 2020. Ventral hippocampal input induced deficit in a rodent model of recognition memory by the selective 5-HT6
to the prelimbic cortex dissociates the context from the cue association in trace fear receptor antagonist, Ro 04-6790. Psychopharmacology 170 (4), 358–367. https://
memory. J. Neurosci. 40 (16), 3217–3230. https://doi.org/10.1523/ doi.org/10.1007/S00213-003-1552-5/FIGURES/3.
JNEUROSCI.1453-19.2020. Yasuno, F., Suhara, T., Nakayama, T., Ichimiya, T., Okubo, Y., Takano, A., Ando, T.,
Uchida, S., Umeeda, H., Kitamoto, A., Masushige, S., Kida, S., 2007. Chronic reduction in Inoue, M., Maeda, J., Suzuki, K., 2003. Inhibitory effect of hippocampal 5-HT1A
dietary tryptophan leads to a selective impairment of contextual fear memory in receptors on human explicit memory. Am. J. Psychiatry 160 (2), 334–340. https://
mice. Brain Res. 1149 (1), 149–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/J. doi.org/10.1176/APPI.AJP.160.2.334/ASSET/IMAGES/LARGE/L319F3.JPEG.
BRAINRES.2007.02.049. Young, S.N., Ervin, F.R., Pihl, R.O., Finn, P., 1989. Biochemical aspects of tryptophan
van der Veen, F.M., Evers, E.A.T., van Deursen, J.A., Deutz, N.E.P., Backes, W.H., depletion in primates, 1989 98:4 Psychopharmacology 98 (4), 508–511. https://doi.
Schmitt, J.A.J., 2006. Acute tryptophan depletion reduces activation in the right org/10.1007/BF00441950.
hippocampus during encoding in an episodic memory task. NeuroImage 31 (3), Zeithamova, D., Dominick, A.L., Preston, A.R., 2012. Hippocampal and ventral medial
1188–1196. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2006.01.014. prefrontal activation during retrieval-mediated learning supports novel inference.
van Donkelaar, Eva L., Rutten, K., Blokland, A., Akkerman, S., Steinbusch, H.W.M., Neuron 75 (1), 168–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEURON.2012.05.010.
Prickaerts, J., 2008. Phosphodiesterase 2 and 5 inhibition attenuates the object Zhang, G., Stackman, R.W., 2015a. The role of serotonin 5-HT2A receptors in memory
and cognition. Front. Pharmacol. Vol. 6 https://doi.org/10.3389/fphar.2015.00225.

18
R. Coray and B.B. Quednow Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 139 (2022) 104729

Zhang, G., Stackman, R.W., 2015b. The role of serotonin 5-HT2A receptors in memory mice. Neuropharmacology 109, 332–340. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.
and cognition. Front. Pharmacol. 6 (OCT) https://doi.org/10.3389/ NEUROPHARM.2016.04.033.
FPHAR.2015.00225. Zhang, G., Cinalli, D., Stackman, R.W., 2017. Effect of a hallucinogenic serotonin 5-HT2A
Zhang, G., Cinalli, D., Cohen, S.J., Knapp, K.D., Rios, L.M., Martínez-Hernández, J., receptor agonist on visually guided, hippocampal-dependent spatial cognition in
Luján, R., Stackman, R.W., 2016. Examination of the hippocampal contribution to C57BL/6J mice. Hippocampus 27 (5), 558–569. https://doi.org/10.1002/
serotonin 5-HT2A receptor-mediated facilitation of object memory in C57BL/6J HIPO.22712.

19

You might also like