Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Gesture and Creating Zones of

Proximal Development for Second


Language Learning
STEVEN G. MCCAFFERTY
Department of Curriculum and Instruction
University of Nevada, Las Vegas
Las Vegas, NV 89154-3005
Email: mccaffes@nevada.edu

This study investigated the role of gesture in and of itself and in conjunction with speech in
creating zones of proximal development (ZPD) for second language learning and teaching. A
university student of English, newly arrived in the United States, was videorecorded once a
week in conversational interaction with an American graduate student, an ESL/EFL teacher,
over two different periods lasting 15 weeks altogether. The view taken in the study of Vygotsky’s
concept of the ZPD follows that of Newman and Holtzman (1993), who argued that it primarily
concerns revolutionary activity, that learning and teaching transforms as a consequence of
interacting in the ZPD, and that this affects all participants. Findings indicate the important
role that gesture played both in promoting language learning and in facilitating positive
interaction between the two participants, helping to create a sense of shared social, symbolic,
physical, and mental space.

DEFINING IN FUNCTIONAL TERMS THE zone Also, as suggested by Newman and Holtzman
of proximal development (ZPD) in relation to (1993), there has been an undue emphasis on the
teaching and learning, Vygotsky (1978) sug- spatial metaphor in considering the ZPD, that is,
gested that “what is in the zone of proximal devel- speaking of what is “in” the zone. Newman and
opment today will be the actual developmental Holtzman argued that the ZPD is not so much a
level tomorrow—that is, what a child can do with “place” as an “activity,” and moreover, it is a revo-
assistance today she will be able to do by herself lutionary activity that can lead to both learning
tomorrow” (p. 87). Within education there has and transformation for all involved. Thus, New-
been a tendency to treat this concept as a mecha- man and Holtzman characterized the ZPD as a
nism that can be enlisted by teachers to measure “tool and result” as opposed to a “tool for result”
an individual’s deficits in relation to the subject (pp. 46–47). It is both process and product at the
matter at hand. This tendency ignores the central same time in much the way that the poet, e. e.
idea that the ZPD is a function of co-construction: cummings, changed the conventions of printed
The identities of learners and their interactants English (i.e., capitalization) in writing his poems.
make a critical difference. Furthermore, the con- That is, he transformed the language in the pro-
texts of interaction—the setting, artifacts, and use cess of using it. Nor of course were such innova-
of symbolic tools—must be included when con- tions arbitrary, they were done to change the
sidering cognitive development (which, as sense (psychological association) of what was rep-
pointed out by Vygotsky, is inseparable from af- resented through the use of script. It is this per-
fective/volitional elements) within activity theory spective of the ZPD in relation to gesture and
(Wertsch, 1998), the framework most suited to second language (L2) learning that I wish to pur-
considering the ZPD within sociocultural theory. sue in this study.
Learning a new language-culture in a country
The Modern Language Journal, 86, ii, (2002) where the L2 is dominant, that is, in naturalistic
0026-7902/02/192–203 $1.50/0 contexts, is itself a transformational process that,
©2002 The Modern Language Journal
as described by Vygotsky for children, moves from
Steven G. McCafferty 193
an interpersonal to an intrapersonal plane of de- interwoven with a more inclusive or comprehen-
velopment. In the case of adult L2 learners this sive process of communication, in naturalistic
entails an individual’s apprenticeship to new ways contexts L2 learning necessarily involves nonver-
of doing and being. In considering the encul- bal features that are both linguistic and nonlin-
turation of children, Vygotsky (1978) argued that guistic (McCafferty & Ahmed, 2000) and that are
“in appropriating the resources of the culture also found intrapersonally (McCafferty, 1998).
through participation in social action and interac- Given this evidence, it can be assumed that inter-
tion, the individual both transforms those re- action in the ZPD also includes these elements.
sources and is transformed in the process”(p. 42). Play and drama also have important roles in
There are many examples of immigrant popula- relation to the ZPD and L2 learning. When inter-
tions who have changed many of their own ways acting with native speakers, nonnative speakers
but at the same time transformed aspects of the can feel quite disconnected from both the lan-
surrounding culture. However, the extent of guage and the setting, as if they are playing a part.
transformation at the level of the individual has In fact, it may be that they are not themselves.1 In
much to do with intentionality and selectively in- terms of L2 development, such a situation is akin
ternalizing aspects of the second culture (Lantolf, to forms of play in which children take on the role
1999; McCafferty, 1997). This perspective gains of adults, dressing up and imitating the words of
further support if we take a view of the ZPD that their parents. Vygotsky (1978) connected this
emphasizes its role in creating meaning, some- form of pretending to the ZPD, suggesting that in
thing that is cognitively at the heart of apprentic- these circumstances a child “always behaves be-
ing to any new realm of understanding and be- yond his average age, above his daily behavior” (p.
coming. Again, this is why the ZPD cannot be 102). In fact, Cohen and MacKeith (1991) sug-
viewed as simply a pedagogical technique, a tool gested that the noun play depicting a theatrical
for result. As a tool and result, Newman and Holtz- production is more reflective of the children’s
man (1993) argued that the ZPD changes the activity than is generally recognized.
functional structure of consciousness, and that Along these same lines, Newman and Holtz-
one of Vygotsky’s principal concerns was how man (1993) pointed out that we create new roles
such changes reorganize overall development. for ourselves through performing them. With an
Although Vygotsky has been accused of propa- eye to how this happens in interaction, Habermas
gating dualisms within his own theoretical ap- (1984) suggested that people in conversation are
proach (see Wells, 1999; Wertsch, 1991), he none- engaged in “dramaturgical action,” that is, they
theless wrote explicitly of the woes of this kind of “[constitute] a public for one another, before
thinking. In a paper first published in 1925 in whom they present themselves. The actor evokes
which he laid out much of what was to occupy in his public a certain image, or impression of
him intellectually during the remainder of his himself, by more or less purposefully disclosing
short life, Vygotsky (1981) argued that neither his subjectivity” (p. 86). This behavior is of course
subjectivism nor behaviorism (the dominant especially important to establishing a sense of
schools of psychological thought of his day) alone identity in a new language-culture, an area of
could account for human psychology. Nor did he research currently receiving a good deal of atten-
view the attempt simply to merge one with the tion within Applied Linguistics (see Kramsch,
other as tenable, instead arguing for a reconcep- 1995; Norton, 1997; Pavlenko & Lantolf, 2000;
tualization of mind and behavior (Minick, 1997). Siegal, 1996; Toohey, 2000). As pointed out by
The Cartesian mind/body split that has so Leont’ev (1981), and in relation to Vygotsky, it is
dominated Western thought is equally untenable. through play that children attend to becoming
Cognitive science cultivated a representation of adults. In a sense they are mastering their future
the mind as solipsistic, that is, the individual func- roles and activities. Certainly this process pertains
tioning largely independently of others, and as to L2 learners in naturalistic contexts as well.
disembodied as well. In the formal study of lin- Gesture is an important mediational means in
guistics this has led to a view of language as an both play and drama; indeed, Vygotsky (1978)
inventory of grammatical structures that are pointed out in reference to how objects used in
passed back and forth from “one brain to an- play come to represent other things that “It is only
other.” However, a moment’s reflection on the on the basis of . . . indicatory gestures that play-
host of nonverbal features attendant in face-to- things themselves gradually acquire their mean-
face interaction and their significance in making ing” (p. 108). Artigal (1993) used indexical space
meaning reveals this conceptualization for the and play as a primary means to teach a L2 when
illusion that it is. Because language is inextricably working with kindergarten children. He did so by
194 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
marking objects and actions in a narrative this element was of an admittedly limited focus as
through pantomiming actions in culturally recog- only gesture and speech were examined.
nizable spaces, for example, putting cookies in a
cookie jar on a kitchen countertop. In doing so, Participants
Artigal also recognized that the way an artifact or
action is depicted influences how it is understood, The L2 learner in the study, hereafter desig-
that is, there is a direct connection to sense nated as “B,” was in his 20s and had left his country,
through the nonverbal channel. This perspective Taiwan, to come to study English in the United
finds support in work on psychological develop- States. At the beginning of the research, B had
ment. Tomasello (1999) stated that been living in the United States for 1 month and
was attending a large university in the American
When children begin to understand other persons as Southwest, enrolled in two intermediate English
intentional agents, and so to imitatively learn the as a Second Language (ESL) classes. Over the pe-
conventional use of artifacts through them, the world riod of time he was recorded, he resided with his
of cultural artifacts becomes imbued with intentional sister with whom he spoke Taiwanese the majority
affordances to complement their sensory-motor af-
of the time. According to him, outside of the study,
fordances—with children’s very strong tendency to
he engaged in the production of English only in
imitate adult interactions with objects clearly appar-
ent. (p. 86) class or when doing written assignments at home.
He also said that he tried to improve his listening
The use of signs and symbols that accompany comprehension by watching television, which he
such actions with objects is of course equally criti- reported doing some 2 to 4 hours a day. Moreover,
cal. In his account of semiotic mediation, Vygot- he suggested that on occasion he read newspapers
sky (1978) observed that children first connect and magazines in English and used English-
an object with its referent through grasping. language Internet sites to help with his reading in
However, when the child begins to point at ob- addition to doing his homework assignments.
jects which are then brought to him or her by Although initially not considered a participant
others, the child experiences having his or her in the study, because the expected focus was on
intentions understood through the use of signs the dyadic interaction of L2 learners, a graduate
and acted upon, a major step in entering the student, called “J” hereafter, ended up taking an
world of human activity. unanticipated interactional role with B owing to
circumstances that developed in the course of the
study. It is important to point out that J is an
experienced English as a Foreign Language
THE STUDY
(EFL)/ESL instructor, having spent several years
Purpose in both Asia and the United States teaching at the
postsecondary level. This experience clearly had
It is important to reiterate that the use of ges- something to do with his orientation toward in-
ture either by itself or in conjunction with speech teracting with B.
in creating ZPDs for language learning and teach-
ing was examined in this study within an activity Videotaping
framework; that is, the spatial metaphor charac-
terizing what is or is not “in” the zone was consid- Originally, 5 ESL students were involved in the
ered too narrow a perspective to account for the study, although all but B dropped out by the 8th
kind of revolutionary transformations that take week. Once a week various groupings of between 2
shape through this process as described by Vygot- and 4 participants were videorecorded while they
sky. Moreover, the view adopted in this study em- were sitting on benches in a small park-like area
phasizes the importance of co-construction, that not far from their ESL classroom. The benches
the ZPD potentially transforms the participants formed a “v” shape, and the students sat halfway
not only individually but collectively as well and between facing each other and the camera, which
that new ways of thinking and doing develop out was positioned behind the opening of the “v.” The
of the interaction. On this view, it was of course camera was on a tripod and took in most of the
also important to consider the setting and arti- body of each of the participants (sometimes the
facts utilized by the participants in relation to lower legs and feet were not seen). An external
speech and gesture. Finally, one overarching as- microphone was used to enhance the quality of
pect of the study had to do with language learning the audiorecording. J prepared a short list of top-
and teaching as an embodied activity, although ics that he hoped would stimulate discussion for
Steven G. McCafferty 195
most sessions. Sessions generally lasted between 5 Therefore, he never considered himself to be
and 10 minutes. During the early recordings J part of the study, nor, when asked upon comple-
stayed behind the camera and, although he had tion of the recording did he recall ever being
not planned on doing so, found himself engaging aware of his use of gesture. B was never told
the students quite frequently beyond just provid- anything about the purpose of the research be-
ing topics. yond the fact that it had to do with L2 learn-
By the 5th week, B was able to volunteer only ing—gesture was never mentioned.
for a time slot when none of the other students
could join him, so J set up the camera and micro- Analysis
phone and sat down to converse with him. Al-
though he followed the same format, the topics The 15 videotaped sessions were examined for
that were introduced generally were discussed at the use of gesture as part of creating ZPDs for L2
greater length and with more depth than they learning and teaching as found in the contexts of
had been with the other participants. There were the study. Included in the analysis were the par-
also many more student-initiated topics, B taking ticipants’ explicit attention to linguistic features
the role of expert, J becoming the learner (al- of the L2, their use of artifacts in the setting as
though not with regard to English). At times the well as the setting itself, and aspects of their inter-
interaction was highly conversational, with sym- personal relationship, such as the establishment
metry if not equality, between B and J (see van of participant roles and intersubjectivity, which
Lier, 1996). Overall, in these recordings J took a Rommetviet (1974) defined as an “immediately
much more active pedagogical stance than in the shared social reality” (p. 30).
previous sessions when he had remained behind In most cases it was not necessary to view the
the camera. He supplied vocabulary, had B re- videotapes in slow motion, but there were in-
peat words and phrases, spelled words out, ex- stances when slow motion provided a needed ex-
plained syntactic structures, and so forth. Gesture actness. However, in all cases multiple viewings
had an important role in these efforts and in and were necessary in order to insure the accuracy of
of itself as well. the transcription for speech, gesture, and combi-
It was hoped that B would be involved in the nations of speech and gesture.
research over a continuous period of time, per- The section headings for the findings and dis-
haps lasting a year. However, after arriving in Sep- cussion are not meant to be completely discrete
tember for the fall semester, he decided to go because clearly there is overlap. Rather, the head-
back to Taiwan by the middle of November. He ings focus on what proved to be significant as-
expected at that time not to return to the United pects of the findings from a functional/activity
States, but in March of the following year, much perspective.
to my surprise, I discovered him in the Student
Union having a meal and asked him if he would
FINDINGS AND DISCUSSION
be willing to continue being recorded, to which
he readily agreed. For the second set of seven Gesture and Lexical Comprehension
recordings the weather had grown cooler, so J
and B moved indoors and used a couple of differ- Because of difficulties with the verbal channel,
ent rooms in the same building on campus. The L2 learners can develop other means for both por-
guidelines established previously for video- traying and comprehending meaning, including
recording procedures continued to be followed, relying more on nonverbal than verbal modes. In
although the participants sat on chairs, and each the 4th recording, J relied on a gesture to guess the
of the rooms included somewhat different fea- meaning for a lexical item. B’s conversational part-
tures. Therefore, all told, 15 sessions were re- ner at the time, also a student from Taiwan, used
corded, the first 8 beginning soon after B’s arrival an iconic gesture (a concrete representation) for
in the United States, and the second sequence, the word statue. When J responded with the word
lasting 7 weeks, starting sometime after he re- statue, the student looked at B as if to convey that
turned from Taiwan where he had stayed for a he was pleased that J had been able to help him.
period of 19 weeks. The total duration of time Although this response to the other participant’s
covered by the study was close to 8 months. use of gesture did not have any effect on B’s ges-
Finally, it is important to mention that J tures at the time, it did set the stage for things to
thought the research had something to do with come. The next session was the first time B inter-
the appropriation of abstract gestures on the part acted solely with J. After setting up the camera and
of L2 learners, a topic he had not examined. microphone, J took a seat on the bench and began
196 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
to engage B in various topics of conversation. Al- hand (a beat) on each of the three syllables, not
most immediately B used an iconic gesture when quite reaching the steady hand. J repeated this
searching for a vocabulary item, looking at J as his same gestural sequence with minor variations
classmate had done the week before. Although J four more times on saying “until now” as embed-
did not respond in this instance (the gesture was ded within utterances of a similar or identical con-
rather vague), this form of gesture, that is, B look- tent. This is a metaphoric gesture (McNeill, 1992)
ing at J and attempting to illustrate a vocabulary in that the moving of the lower hand progressively
item while conducting a lexical search, was re- upward toward the upper hand is an iconic, or
peated in later sessions as well, becoming an estab- concrete, representation of a concept—the pas-
lished part of the praxis created by the interac- sage of time—rather than an object. Although B
tants. Although the gesture in this instance was not said “oh” at the end of J’s intervention, signaling
successful in promoting comprehension, I want to comprehension, this response appears to have
emphasize, following the suggestion of Wells been more out of politeness than understanding
(1999), that it is diversity more than improvement because he made no attempt to continue the con-
per se that should receive emphasis in characteriz- versation from where it had left off before the
ing the ZPD. The example should, therefore, be intervention, despite J’s repeating so far at the end
appreciated for its potential in creating a ZPD. of the intervention. Nonetheless, it is possible to
This form of seeking help with a lexical item see how the gesture might have led to B’s compre-
was also found by Beavin Bavelas, Chovil, Coates, hension because imagistically, the indication of an
and Roe (1995) among native speakers of a lan- end point through the use of the steady hand and
guage; gesture alone was sufficient to stimulate a the gradual movement toward that point, not
response from an addressee. It may be that B also reaching it, is illustrative of the concept so far. J
elicited help in his native language in this way, also used a metaphoric/emblematic gesture dur-
but, in the contexts of the study, there is no re- ing the session, and what is important to note, it
corded use of gesture for this function until after was one that B had used during the previous ses-
he had seen his countryman deploy the statue sion in reference to children: He held his right
gesture and J’s subsequent response. hand out in front of where he was seated, palm
In the next recording, B gesturally illustrated down, about 3 feet above the ground. Both speak-
water being splashed on him by cupping both ers used this gesture in subsequent sessions as
palms and “splashing” himself by pumping his well, although B at times used both hands. The
arms up from a horizontal position on his lap gesture is metaphoric in that it references age
inward toward himself. He did this while relating through height and emblematic because it is a
his experience of watching a killer whale perform culturally prevalent nonverbal form for referring
at Sea World in San Diego. J at once realized that to children, functioning as a lexical item in and of
B did not know the word splash and asked him, itself. Another metaphoric gesture was intro-
“Did the whale splash you?” (emphasizing splash). duced and repeated during this session. J held
When B gave him a quizzical look, he continued, one hand up about chest level with the palm down
“you got wet,” producing a similar splash gesture to signify an upper limit as in the sense of keeping
with the word wet. A bit later J said, “the verb is the price down. The gesture accompanied the word
splash, you know water . . . ” leaving the sentence discount. Later in the recording, B repeated this
verbally unfinished, and supplying a gesture in- gesture, providing the same nonverbal sense to
stead, but this time he brought down his hand, the word.
palm flat, as if slapping the surface of water.2 This The use of gestures that became established
intervention hinged upon J’s recognition and lexical items and were repeated by both interlocu-
utilization of B’s gestures in conjunction with the tors is, I think, a fine example of the ZPD in rela-
verbal contexts. tion to language learning. Through creating a
In another such intervention, J tried to illus- shared history of signs, the participants scaffolded
trate the meaning of so far by providing both a each other in their efforts to co-construct mean-
semantic equivalent, until now and an accompany- ing and provide the sense they wanted to convey
ing gesture. J held his left hand, palm down and because, clearly, the gestures described consti-
flat, at a level about even with his eyes. His right tuted a significant means of enhancing communi-
hand was directly below at about chest level and cation and facilitating comprehension. Also, this
also with the palm down. The onset of the gesture use of gesture helped to create what appeared to
came just before for in the utterance “It’s like an be a high degree of intersubjectivity between the
idiom for until now.” With until now he brought his participants. Gesture in these instances inhabited
lower hand progressively up with a pulse of the the shared social, physical, symbolic, and mental
Steven G. McCafferty 197
space created by the interlocutors. Furthermore, B (2)
the gestures imparted a dramatic, play-like quality a. [you – then – it can]
to the exchanges. holds both hands close to his chest with
fingers pointed downward
[do]
Illustrator Gestures
pulses hands in same position in a beat
The splash gesture produced by B is both iconic [look the]
and an illustrator given that it is used by the moves right hand into palm up position as
speaker to provide an imagistic realization of the before, representing a drawing
accompanying verbalization. The use of illustra- look a [children draw . . . ]
tors appears to be rather idiosyncratic—some left hand replaces right in same position,
people use them with frequency, while others al- then right hand forms configuration of
most never use them. It has also been found that holding a writing instrument and moves
cultures vary in their use of these gestures. Ac- over the left hand as if drawing
cording to Argyle (1988), “Illustrators add consid-
erably to the amount of information conveyed by B’s use of illustrators before J started sitting
speech, especially about shapes, physical objects, down to talk with him was infrequent at best. Also,
and spatial relations” (p. 195). B did not deploy these gestures in all of the sub-
B’s use of illustrators was quite common. The sequent recordings. It is of course critical to con-
following protocol is a brief example. The partici- sider the specifics of the contexts of interaction,
pants were discussing B’s friend who was studying and, as expressed above, J explicitly used both B’s
art therapy. The segment starts with a question and his own gestures as a way of imparting mean-
from J about the process. In the transcription, ing. This use of gestures leaves open the possibility
brackets indicate the stroke of the gesture (its that, when speaking with J, B utilized illustrators as
beginning to end across time). Utterances are part of what he perceived as the pedagogical con-
subdivided by gesture, each subdivision centering texts. Indeed, unlike the example above, many of
on a single stroke. B’s illustrator gestures accompanied well-formu-
lated utterances; in other words, the gestures were
J (1) not needed to induce J’s comprehension per se,
a. [so, is she] although it may be that B was concerned about
right hand next to face holding virtual conveying the sense of what he wanted to say, feel-
writing instrument ing that his ability at this level of lexical repre-
b. [using] sentation was especially limited. Along these lines,
pulses right hand (same position) in a beat and with regard to native speakers, Beavin Bavelas
[pictures then?] and Chovil (2000) noted that “signs or behaviors”
moves right hand (same initial position) that appear to be redundant in that they mirror
down and away from face with a squiggly the verbal message may in fact function “to reduce
motion as if writing the ambiguity of a message . . . to increase the
B (1) likelihood of a correct decoding of the meaning”
a. no, [she] look (p. 185). It is not difficult to see why a L2 learner
points down with index finger of right hand would utilize as many means as possible to config-
held just above the lap (seems to be ure meaning, that is to say, perhaps even more
referencing the drawing he is about to than when speaking the native language, although
mention) this comparison has yet to be investigated.
[the] Another consideration from a sociocultural
flips right hand over, palm up and flat (this perspective deals with intrapersonal dimensions
is the drawing), eyes focused on the hand for the use of this type of gesture. Although not
[draw . . .] meant as an explanation separate from the two
right hand moves up to chest level and interpersonal aspects discussed above, which op-
assumes a form like that found for J erate concurrently, enacting meaning with the
above, that is, as if holding a writing hands should serve purposes of self-regulation as
instrument, then traces a rectangular frame well as communication. It may be that B found
vertically that his gestures helped him make meaning for
J (2) himself as well as for J, as Vygotsky (1986) sug-
a. [ . . . looks at the drawing, yeah] gested for speech. Moreover, McNeill (1992) ar-
nods head gued that thoughts come together imagistically,
198 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
linguistically, and pragmatically as a single unit, the architectural plan of the campus, on saying
which he termed the growth point. This intercon- the word planning B extended both arms out to
nection lends support for the notion that B was his sides at about chest level, then swept them
concretizing the verbal element through gesture inward in a half circle, palms down, until the
as a way of leading his linguistic efforts. thumbs touched, arms extended in front of him.
J used this gesture in referring to the buildings on
Environmental Affordances campus as well, but later in the session the ges-
ture took on the more general meaning of every-
Both participants referenced various objects in thing. This transformation of meaning is of par-
the immediate environment as well as sites in the ticular interest in connection to the ZPD because
distance through the use of deictic gestures (refer- it is a clear case of dialogic interanimation.3 The
ential, mainly pointing). Some examples from the meaning of the gesture could have changed only
recordings follow. B pointed at the ground, index through its evolving sense within the co-con-
finger extended, when saying “soil.” J patted the structed contexts of its use. Also, it is important
wooden benches they were sitting on just before to take into consideration the role that imitation
saying “structure.” B also struck the bench with the plays in this case—that it is creative and transfor-
flat of his hand when saying, “We sit the chair.” B mational,4 as will be discussed in the next section.
pointed to the ground when referring to the name Overall, in Artigal’s (1993) terms, the interac-
of the local city and tapped his index finger on the tants established “a semiotic territory that is rec-
arm of his chair in one of the rooms on campus, ognizable, i.e., shared and co-present for all par-
again when indicating the city (“here”). In addi- ticipants” (pp. 15–16). However, for Artigal, as
tion, B often pointed to or put his hand on his described above, the assigning of space is a delib-
chest when referencing himself and also pointed erate practice in his approach to L2 pedagogy. In
to J at times when referring to him (“you” in most the case of the current data, the assigning of
cases). He also used reflexive gestures when refer- space arose through J and B’s “framing” the envi-
ring to personal attributes, for example, when ronment as part of the “stage set” for their con-
speaking of his native language. versations. Some of these practices became con-
Both B and J also pointed to a number of places ventions; indeed, most of the nonverbal features
that were not within their immediate visual field. listed in this section were found in both the first
These references included buildings on campus, and second set of recordings, despite changes of
points of interest in the city, people’s houses, a location.
national park in another state, a city in that same It is of course important to keep in mind that
state, and other features of the landscape. In all “Any line of action that a person is pursuing is
cases, the participants either used an index finger always carried out in relation to a specific envi-
or thumb to point in the direction of the place ronment” (Kendon, 1990, p. 247). How individu-
indicated. What is perhaps particularly notewor- als mediate understanding, create a sense of
thy about this use of gesture is that it happened identity, attempt self-regulation, and so forth,
with regularity, and in all cases, close attention then, has much to do with what is available to
was paid to the cardinal direction. them (see van Lier, 2000). In connection to this
There are also instances in the data when deic- notion, it is critical to realize that “people in in-
tic pointing was extended to include virtual or teraction become environments for each other”
far-off contexts. For example, when asked about (Erickson & Schultz, 1997, p. 22, citing McDer-
what he would do when buying his first home, B mott, 1976). Also, the background or larger con-
pointed to the ground while responding, “I will texts influence activity. In the current research,
buy carpet,” deicticly situating himself in concord the setting was a university campus, B was asked
with the hypothetical location. Also, in reference to participate in a study concerning L2 learning,
to his hometown in Taiwan, B said, “It’s south of J was identified to B as a research assistant and
Taiwan,” moving his forearm down across his EFL teacher, the seating arrangement was speci-
chest. This gesture clearly originated from a pri- fied, the activity was limited to the visual field of
vate point of reference, seemingly an image of the camera, and so forth. All of these elements
Taiwan as represented on a map. played a part in leading to the creation of ZPDs.
In one instance, the meaning of a gesture However, it would be misleading, I believe, to
transformed over the course of the session, going think of them as “factors.” The outcome of the
from referencing things in the more immediate convergence of these elements may be more akin
environment to include distal elements as well. At to what happens with complexity theory because
the beginning of the session, when speaking of “both natural and human sciences deal not only
Steven G. McCafferty 199
with universal laws, but also with the realms of persons in this way, I internalize not only their com-
unstable, unpredictable, creative, and unique municative intention (their intention to get me to
processes” (Lektorsky, 1999, p. 69). Finally, the share their attention) but also the specific perspective
use of gesture cited in this section is a particu- they have taken. (p. 128)
larly good example of how the ZPD can trans- And with explicit regard to gesture, he stated:
form the tools, the setting, and the activity “Even in the case of nonlinguistic gestures, if the
(Wells, 1999). learning process involves the understanding of
communicative intentions and the execution of
Imitation role-reversal imitation inside a joint attentional
scene, the product will be a communicative sym-
Vygotsky (1978) argued that imitation is a key bol” (p. 106).
aspect of learning, “a person can imitate only that Also, it has been demonstrated that nonverbal
which is within her developmental level” (p. 88). imitation can be key to creating a sense of shared
Furthermore, Vygotsky (1986) stated: context and interpersonal rapport. For example,
mimicry, which is more literal than imitation in
In the child’s development . . . imitation and instruc-
tion play a major role. They bring out the specifically the sense that it is meant to be an “exact duplica-
human qualities of mind and lead the child to new tion” (Donald, 1991, p. 168), according to Hess,
developmental levels. In learning to speak, as in Philippot, and Blairy (1999) “may increase coher-
learning school subjects, imitation is indispensable. ence between interaction partners by making
What the child can do in cooperation today he can them more similar to one another” (p. 215). Fur-
do alone tomorrow. Therefore the only good kind of thermore, with regard to imitation in childhood,
instruction is that which marches ahead of develop- “imitating another’s nonverbal actions is a core
ment and leads it: it must be aimed not so much at
behavioral strategy for achieving social coordina-
the ripe as at the ripening functions. (p. 188)
tion during the developmental period preceding
In explicating this view, Newman and Holtz- reliance on verbal communication” (p. 221).
man (1993) stated that, for Vygotsky, imitation in Also, and within a tutorial setting, Allen and Feld-
childhood is “the predominant joint revolution- man (1976) found that young children imitated
ary activity that occurs in the ZPD,” and that as their tutors’ gestures and other nonverbal expres-
such, it is “fundamental to the unity {meaning- sions to a high degree.
making/learning-leading development} because As indicated in the findings above, imitation
and as the child does what she/he is not yet was a prominent aspect of B’s nonverbal interac-
capable of doing” (p. 151). This marks imitation tion with J. It is also crucial that J both initiated
as a transformational process (internalization); the use of gesture and imitated B’s gestures as
that is, enactment on the social or interpersonal well. Had he not done so, the interpersonal dy-
plane leads to development on the psychological namic may well have been more static, lessening
or intrapersonal plane. B’s sense of involvement in the language learning
Tomasello (1999) also found the developmen- experience afforded him, decreasing interaction
tal impact of imitation remarkable: “During the and the ZPD. Indeed, with monolingual children,
period from one to three years old, young chil- Ward and von Raffler-Engel (1980) found that
dren are virtual ‘imitation machines’ as they seek imitation was not a two-way proposition, that the
to appropriate the cultural skills and behaviors of teacher initiated all of the gestures that were used
the mature members of their social groups” (p. jointly. This finding is an interesting contrast to
159). If we invoke Frawley and Lantolf’s (1985) what unfolded in the present study. Although it is
principle of continuous access (that adults go back not known at this time, it may be that other expe-
to the ways of “knowing” [psychological tools] rienced L2 or foreign language teachers mimic or
developed during childhood) then of course it imitate the gestures and other nonverbal ele-
would not be surprising to find that L2 learners ments of their students—a possibility worthy of
in naturalistic contexts utilize imitation in rela- further research. It may also prove upon further
tion to both speech and gesture and that it serves study that without the presence of these features
transformational purposes. In further support of in face-to-face communication with a native
why this would likely be so, Tomasello (1999) speaker, L2 learners simply conclude that the in-
contended: terlocutor is unwilling to make the kinds of ac-
I learn to use the symbolic means that other persons commodations necessary to facilitate conversa-
have used to share attention with one another. In tion, which John-Steiner (1985) found was true in
imitatively learning a linguistic symbol from other the case of verbal adjustments.
200 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
Synchrony pany speech are in place before the language
itself is acquired (Waylie, 1985). B may also have
In nonverbal interaction, synchrony refers to been trying to capture the rhythms of English by
the “mirroring” of “rhythm [and] movement, and synchronizing with J. Finally, as pointed out by
the meshing of interaction” (Argyle, 1988, p. 34). Newman and Holtzman (1993), acting is “copy-
Kendon (1990) contended that ing, mimicking, repeating without being ahead of
To move with another is to show that one is “with” him oneself” (p. 153). However, in the case of L2
in one’s attentions and expectancies. Coordination learning, the creation of oneself dramaturgically
of movement in interaction may thus be of great is a different, transformational process, one in
importance since it provides one of the ways in which which mimicry, imitation, and synchrony may be
two people signal they are “open” to one another. (p. leading development.
114)

In an interesting illustration, McGuire (1980) CONCLUSION


found that an American child, while watching a
I believe that the role of gesture in relation to
videotaped version of himself, would mirror his
B’s language learning in this study supports a
posture and expressions when he agreed with
view of the ZPD as revolutionary activity: that J
what his screen image was saying but would not do
and B utilized each other, the setting, and arti-
so when he did not agree with his image. It is also
facts through gesture to transform teaching and
noteworthy that research has demonstrated that a
learning. Wells (1999) identified four basic forms
lack of such coordination can lead to a sense of
of transformation in relation to the ZPD, all of
frustration on the part of an interlocutor in the
which apply to the findings for this study.
case of native speakers of a language (Bernieri &
The first form is transformation of the learner’s
Rosenthal, 1991).
identity in relation to “his or her capacity to effec-
There were many instances, in addition to
tively participate in future actions of a related
those already described, when the two partici-
kind” (pp. 327–328). The amount of effort that
pants mirrored each other’s movements and ges-
both B and J expended in trying to communicate
tures. A further quotation from Kendon (1990)
effectively through the use of gesture suggests its
suggests the relationship of interactional syn-
interpersonal importance. Gesture is also consid-
chrony to the ZPD:
ered to have helped B gain self-regulation in both
Interactional synchrony is best regarded as an developing and conveying his thoughts in the L2.
achievement of the interactants that is attained when B’s use of gesture, then, proved a vital feature of
the participants come to govern their behavior in his conversations with J, and although his interac-
relation to one another in respect to a commonly tions with other people in future will of course
shared frame or joint plan of action. Interactants prove to be different in any number of ways, it is
come to be able to behave together as if they share a
still reasonable to expect gesture to play a facilitat-
common musical score and this can make possible a
ing role at times.
very high degree of temporal coordination between
them. (p. 115) The second form of transformation and the
ZPD that Wells mentioned is the “invention of
Wells (1999) used learning to dance as an anal- new tools and practices” or the “modification of
ogy for what happens in “learning and teaching existing ones” that lead to a transformation of
in the ZPD” (pp. 322–323). He argued that the “the culture’s tool-kit and its repertoire for prob-
cultural historicity and inherent structure of the lem solving” (p. 328). Certainly gesture operated
activity provide contexts in which novices must at as a form of problem-solving for B and J, however,
first be guided by those who know the steps but there was no cultural tool kit per se for them to
which eventually lead them not only to be able to rely on. Rather, coming from different language-
partner others but to innovate as well. For B and culture backgrounds, the interactants had to co-
J, learning and teaching were also bound up with construct their own tool kit and practices, which
a kind of “dancing,” interactive synchrony having included the use of gesture to reference artifacts
been described in just these terms (Kendon, and other features in the environment. It is im-
1990). Of course, this may also have something to portant to note that for B, words in the new
do with B’s learning English as an embodied lan- language had little discursive history outside the
guage, as I have argued elsewhere. classroom; this study was the first time he had
An additional consideration stems from the ever interacted with a native speaker of English
study of child language acquisition where there is for any real length of time. Therefore, much of
evidence that the body movements that accom- B’s vocabulary lacked “sense.” This may have
Steven G. McCafferty 201
been instrumental in why gesture emerged as it ments. In order to do this, however, students
did; that is, it was needed by both participants to would first need the opportunity to examine the
shore up this missing dimension, although clearly use of gesture in the L2 by watching videotaped
this was more of a necessity for B than for J. interactions of various sorts as well as by explicitly
The third form is the “transformation of the talking about gesture when performing role plays,
activity setting brought about by the problem- scenes, skits, and scenarios, for example.
solving action, which, in turn, opens up further In closing, the findings of this study strongly
possibilities for action” (p. 328). The first use of support the idea that the ZPD is embodied. This
gesture came as an explicit pedagogical tool in area of research appears to hold a good deal of
the form of eliciting vocabulary from J. In and of promise at this early juncture for teachers and
itself this transformed the activity setting. How- researchers alike.
ever, other forms of utilizing gesture quickly fol-
lowed, and furthermore, over the course of the
study took on a variety of functions. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Fourth, in conjunction with transformation of
the participation of the individual participants, is I would like to thank Chris Iddings, George Jacobs,
“transformation in the social organization of the Gary Palmer, Gale Stam, and Gordon Wells for their
ground and in the ways in which the members valued comments and suggestions on earlier drafts of
relate to each other”(p. 328). The use of gesture this article. I take full responsibility for all shortcomings.
impacted the social ground primarily through its
role in helping to create intersubjectivity. All of
NOTES
the functions of gesture discussed in this article
contributed to developing a sense of shared physi-
1 See Harre and Gillette (1994) who argued that in a
cal, symbolic, psychological, and social space for
very real sense we are our discourse.
the participants. In face-to-face interaction peo- 2 Upon viewing this gesture after the study, J sug-
ple are almost always communicating whether or
gested that it stemmed from his many years at sea—that
not they are speaking or even whether or not they the sense of the gesture represented the force and solid-
intend to do so, as gaze, posture, and so forth, all ity of water as it splashes against a person or object.
carry meaning. Also, although the present study 3 Interanimation is a Bakhtinian term that portrays one
focused only on gesture, and therefore is of lim- speaker’s ability to add to or change the meaning of
ited scope, it nonetheless provides an example of another’s words. The discourse of dictatorship offers a
the significance that nonverbal forms can have in counterexample because no one is allowed to interani-
defining participant roles. mate such a voice.
4 I would like to thank one of the reviewers for point-
However, it is essential to recognize that it was
ing this out.
B’s willingness to interact with J in the second
language-culture that allowed what took place in
the study to occur. Putting oneself in this position REFERENCES
is a daunting prospect for anyone—essentially B
had to find a way of re-mediating understanding
Allen, V. L., & Feldman, R. S. (1976). Studies on the role
and re-representing himself. The circumstances
of tutor. In V. Allen (Ed.), Children as teachers: The-
in which he engaged, however, appear to have
ory and research on tutoring (pp. 113–128). New
been well suited to these purposes because they York: Academic Press.
provided him with the tutorship of a language Argyle, M. (1988). Bodily communication (2nd ed.). Madi-
teacher familiar with Asian cultures and with a son, CT: International Universities Press.
context in which he learned his new role through Artigal, J. M. (1993). The L2 kindergarten teacher as a
performing it. territory maker. In J. E. Alatis (Ed.), Georgetown
In applying these findings to pedagogical con- University Roundtable, 1993 (pp. 1–24). Washing-
cerns, it would seem that L2 students exposed to ton, DC: Georgetown University Press.
naturalistic contexts might benefit from becom- Beavin Bavelas, J. B., & Chovil, N. (2000). Visible acts of
meaning. An integrated message model of lan-
ing aware of the use of gesture as part of the
guage in face-to-face dialogue. Journal of Language
overall process of making meaning in the L2. Not
and Social Psychology, 19, 163–194.
only might this awareness help them both to com- Beavin Bavelas, J. B., Chovil, N., Coates, L., & Roe, L.
prehend the language and to express themselves (1995). Gestures specialized for dialogue. Personal-
in it, but also it might help them consider how ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 394–405.
they want to portray themselves by using or resist- Bernieri, F. J., & Rosenthal, R. (1991). Interpersonal co-
ing specific gestures and other nonverbal ele- ordination: Behavior matching and interactional
202 The Modern Language Journal 86 (2002)
sychrony. In R. S. Feldman & B. Rime (Eds.), Funda- McDermott, R. P. (1976). Kids make sense: An ethno-
mentals of nonverbal behavior (pp. 401–432). New graphic account of the interactional management of
York: Cambridge University Press. success and failure in one first-grade classroom. Un-
Cohen, S., & MacKeith, S. A. (1991). The development of published doctoral dissertation, Stanford Univer-
imagination. The private worlds of childhood. London: sity, California.
Routledge. McGuire, M. (1980). Metakinesic behaviour—Some
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind. Cam- theoretical considerations. In W. von Raffler-Engel
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Ed.), Aspects of nonverbal communication (pp.
Erickson, F., & Schultz, J. (1997). When is a context? 125–137). Bath, UK: Pitman Press.
Some issues and methods in the analysis of social McNeill, D. (1985). So you think gestures are nonver-
competence. In M. Cole, Y. Engestrom, & O. Vas- bal? Psychological Review, 92, 350–371.
quez (Eds.), Mind, culture and activity: Seminal pa- McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind. Chicago: University
pers from the laboratory of comparative human cognition of Chicago Press.
(pp. 22–31). Cambridge: Cambridge University Minick, N. (1997). The early history of the Vygotskian
Press. school: The relationship between mind and activ-
Frawley, W., & Lantolf, J. P. (1985). Second language ity. In M. Cole, Y. Engestrom, & O. Vasquez (Eds.),
discourse: A Vygotskyan perspective. Applied Lin- Mind, culture and activity: Seminal papers from the labo-
guistics, 6, 19–44. ratory of comparative human cognition (pp. 117–127).
Habermas, J. (1984). The theory of communicative action: Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Vol. 1. Reason and the rationalization of society. Bos- Newman, F., & Holtzman, L. (1993). Lev Vygotsky: Revo-
ton: Beacon Press. lutionary scientist. New York: Routledge.
Harre, R., & Gillette, G. (1994). The discursive mind. Norton, B. (1997). Language, identity, and the owner-
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage . ship of English. TESOL Quarterly, 31, 409–429.
Hess, U., Philippot, P., & Blairy, S. (1999). Mimicry: Pavlenko, A., & Lantolf, J. P. (2000). Second language
Facts and fiction. In P. Philippot, R. S. Feldman, & learning as participation and the (re)construction
E. J. Cotes (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal of selves. In J. P. Lantolf (Ed.), Sociocultural theory
behavior (pp. 213–241). Cambridge: Cambridge and second language learning (pp. 155–178). Ox-
University Press. ford: Oxford University Press.
John-Steiner, V. (1985). The road to competence in an Rommetveit, R. (1974). On message structure: A framework
alien land: A Vygotskian perspective on bilingual- for the study of language and communication. New
ism. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), Culture, communication York: Wiley.
and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives (pp. 348–372). Siegal, M. (1996). The role of learner subjectivity in
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. second language sociolinguistic competency:
Kendon, A. (1990). Conducting interaction. Cambridge: Western women learning Japanese. Applied Lin-
Cambridge University Press. guistics, 17, 56–82.
Kramsch, C. (1995). The cultural component in lan- Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cogni-
guage teaching. Language, Culture and Curriculum, tion. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
8, 83–92. Toohey, K. (2000). Learning English at school: Identity,
Lantolf, J. P. (1999). Second culture acquisition: Cogni- social relations and classroom practice. Bristol, PA:
tive considerations. In E. Hinkel (Ed.), Culture in Multilingual Matters.
second language teaching and learning (pp. 28–46). van Lier, L. (1996). Interaction in the language curriculum:
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Awareness, autonomy, and authenticity. London:
Lektorsky, V. A. (1999). Activity theory in a new era. In Longman.
M. Cole, Y. Engestrom, & O. Vasquez (Eds.), Per- van Lier, L. (2000). From input to affordance: Social-in-
spectives on activity theory (pp. 65–69). Cambridge: teractive learning from an ecological perspective.
Cambridge University Press. In J. P. Lantolf (Ed.), Sociocultural theory and second
Leont’ev, A. N. (1981). The problem of activity in psy- language learning (pp. 245–259). Oxford: Oxford
chology. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), The concept of ac- University Press.
tivity in Soviet psychology (pp. 37–71). Armonk, NY: Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA:
M. E. Sharpe. Harvard University Press.
McCafferty, S. G. (1997, March). Speaking in tongues: Vygotsky, L. S. (1981). The instrumental method in psy-
Multivocality and biculturalism. Paper presented at chology. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), The concept of activ-
the American Association for Applied Linguistics, ity in Soviet psychology (pp. 134–143). Armonk, NY:
Orlando, FL. M. E. Sharpe.
McCafferty, S. G. (1998). Nonverbal expression and L2 Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge,
private speech. Applied Linguistics, 19, 73–96. MA: MIT Press.
McCafferty, S. G., & Ahmed, M. (2000). The appropria- Ward, L., & von Raffler-Engel, W. (1980). The impact of
tion of gestures of the abstract by L2 learners. In nonverbal behavior on foreign language teaching.
J. P. Lantolf (Ed.), Sociocultural theory and second In W. von Raffler-Engel (Ed.), Aspects of nonverbal
language learning (pp. 199–218). Oxford: Oxford communication (pp. 287–304). Bath, UK: Pitman
University Press. Press.
Steven G. McCafferty 203
Waylie, L. (1985). Language learning and communica- Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the mind: A sociocultural
tion. The French Review, 53, 777–785. approach to meditated action. Cambridge, MA: Har-
Wells, G. (1999). Dialogic inquiry: Toward a sociocultural vard University Press.
practice and theory of education. Cambridge: Cam- Wertsch, J. V. (1998). Mind as action. Oxford: Oxford
bridge University Press. University Press.

Two Journals on Language Learning


LANGUAGE LEARNING JOURNAL of the UK-based Association for Language Learning encourages
articles that focus upon major issues influencing the foreign language teaching profession, that exam-
ine everyday classroom practice, or that draw on theoretical and empirical developments in modern
languages and adjoining disciplines.

The journal, which has an international Editorial Advisory Committee, is fully refereed and features
contributions of high academic and scholarly quality. At the same time, it provides a forum for
professional debate.

The editors strive to make the journal interesting, accessible, and useful to its readers. To this end, in
addition to regular academic/professional articles, which make up the majority of the journal, the
journal features an occasional “Bulletin Board,” which accommodates shorter articles of a more practical
or less academic nature. The section “Review Articles” allows close examination of important publica-
tions in the field of foreign language learning and teaching. In the column “Reflections,” prominent
figures from the world of foreign languages are invited to express their personal views on aspects of
foreign language teaching, in a style and form rather different from those of other articles. The section
“In Other Journals” presents and comments upon interesting and important contributions to other
foreign language periodicals. http://www.all-languages.org.uk/all_language_learning_journal.htm

GERMAN AS A FOREIGN LANGUAGE is a fully-refereed academic journal that aims to promote research
and teaching in the field of German language. It publishes articles in English and German on all aspects
of German language teaching and learning, including the intercultural aspects involved in this process
and the specific perspectives on learning and teaching German outside the German-speaking countries.

GFL features theoretical and applied contributions to the following areas: approaches and methods in
language teaching and learning; intercultural aspects of language teaching and learning; German
in English-speaking countries; and concepts and examples in language teaching and learning.
http://www.gfl-journal.de

You might also like