Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

NEURAL PLASTICITY VOL. 7, NOS.

1-2, 2000

GABA and Muscimol as Reversible Inactivation Tools


in Learning and Memory
M. Majchrzak and G. Di Scala

Laboratoire de Neurociences Comportementales et Cognitives


LN2C UMR7521 ULP/CNRS, 12 rue Goethe 67000 Strasbourg, France

ABSTRACT the mechanisms of learning and memory


without a commensurate progress of system
Reversible inactivation of brain areas is a studies showing where and when the cellular
useful method for inferring brain-behavior changes take place.
relationships. Infusion of GABA or of the
In this regard, neuropsychological analysis of
GABA receptor agonist muscimol is considered
brain-injured patients has profoundly influenced
one interesting reversible inactivation method
the present conception of memory systems (for
because it may not affect fibers of passage and
example, Tulving, 1991). In animal studies, new
may therefore be compared to axon-sparing
paradigms have been introduced to explore the
types of lesions. This article reviews the data
multiplicity of the processes underlying these
obtained with this method in learning and
memory systems, and new techniques (for
memory experiments. A critical analysis of
example, expression of immediate early genes)
data, collected in collaboration with Simon
have been adapted to identify the brain networks
Brailowsky, with chronic GABA infusion is
supporting these processes. Lesion techniques in
presented, together with an illustration of data animals have also improved in neuroanatomical
obtained with muscimol-induced inactivation.
selectivity, using excitotoxic compounds (for
example, ibotenate, AMPA [a-amino-3-hydroxy-
5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionic acid], quisqualate)
KEYWORDS
that destroy cell bodies without affecting the
fibers of passage. For instance, in the ongoing
amygdala, GABA, frontal cortex, leaming, debate on the role of the hippocampus in learning
memory, muscimol, nucleus basalis magno- and memory, it has been found that part of the
cellularis
deficits (in particular, some nonspatial learning
deficits), induced by mechanical or electrolytic
lesions, may to be due to damage to neighboring
INTRODUCTION
structures rather than to the hippocampus itself
(for example, Jarrard, 1993). Despite this
Considerable insight into the molecular
progress, the various shortcomings of lesions
mechanisms that are involved in learning and
studies for inferring brain-behavior relationships
memory has been gained in recent years (for must be recognized. One drawback is that
example, Lynch, 1998). Nevertheless, as stated inference about a relation between brain damage
by Bures and Buresova (1990): and a behavioral deficit implicitly supposes that
In spite of its importance, research the undamaged components of the system
specifying plastic phenomena at the continue to function normally (see Jaffard &
microscale cannot lead to understanding of Meunier, 1993; Farah, 1994), which is unlikely

(C)Freund & Pettman, U.K., 2000 19


20 M. MAJCHRZAK AND G. DI SCALA

to be the case. Somewhat related to the Spatial and temporal characteristics of the
preceding, lesion effects are usually tested after a inactivation induced by GABA, administered
necessary recovery period from surgery; during into cortical sites, has been recently reviewed by
that time, various restorative and/or adaptive Hup6 et al. (1999). Repeated injections of small
processes occur that may obscure the primary doses of GABA have been reported (a) to induce
effect of the lesion. For example, several studies a more homogeneous inactivation than a single
have reported partial or complete recovery of injection of larger amounts does, and (b) to
cholinergic markers within the cortex after increase the duration of inactivation (Hup6 et al.,
unilateral excitotoxic lesions of the nucleus 1999).
basalis magno-cellularis (NBM), provided that a Although the short duration of GABA-
sufficient recovery period (3 mo) was respected induced inactivation is not compatible with
(for example, Wenk & Olton, 1984; Gardiner et learning and memory tests, this inconsistency can be
al., 1987; Casamenti et al., 1988). Returning to compensated for by infusing the GABA
Bures and Buresova’s statement, brain lesions constantly over a period of time, using sub-
certainly contribute to our knowledge of where in cutaneous osmotic minipumps (Alzet(R)). Through
the brain plastic changes may occur during the appropriate choice of the minipump model,
learning. Because they are irreversible, however, duration and rate of infusion (for example,
brain lesions rarely indicate when plastic changes 1 laL/h for 7 d using the 2001 model) can be
occur. In this regard, reversible inactivation of chosen to fit the experimental design. In a series
brain areas has proved to be an efficient tool to of studies, Brailowsky and collaborators (1989)
complement lesion studies in various fields of showed that chronic GABA infusion is an
research, including the study of learning and efficient method to inactivate brain regions involved
memory. This article aims to briefly present data in memory processes. Delayed responses depend
obtained with GABA or GABA agonists as on the prefrontal cortex (for example, Kolb,
reversible inactivation tools, with a special 1984). Infusion of GABA (50 tg/ttl) over 7 days
emphasis on data obtained in collaboration with after acquisition of the task was found to impair
Simon Brailowsky. the delayed response in monkeys (Brailowsky et
al., 1989) and rats (Di Scala et al., 1990;
Meneses et al., 1993). This deficit was found to
GABA AND REVERSIBLE INACTIVATION be relatively stable over the treatment period (for
example, Meneses et al., 1993), and rapid
GABA-induced inactivation has a rapid onset recovery of performance occurred upon cessation
and a short duration; its principal use has been in of the treatment. In this cortical area, histological
anesthetized animals. For example, acute examination of the sites of infusion did not
injection of GABA into the anterodorsal reveal clear signs of lesions (at least, not larger
tegrnentum of anesthetized rats (0.25 to 1.0 mg/tL than those after vehicle infusion, Di Scala et al.,
saline, injection volume of 0.1 or 0.2 tL) was 1990; Meneses et al., 1993).
found to block the locomotion that is elicited by The NBM, the main source of cortical
hypo-thalamic stimulation within 5 min of the acetylcholine afferents, has been implicated in
injection, with recovery occurring within 10 to attentional and working memory processes
20 min (Sinnamon & Benaur, 1997). A similar onset (Dunnett et al., 1991; Muir et al., 1993;
and recovery time ofthe reaction time to a stimulus- McDonald & Overmier, 1998). As cholinergic
triggered movement was recently reported by NBM neurons receive a massive GABAergic
Martin and Ghez (1999) after GABA injection innervation (for example, Wood & Richard,
into the magnocellular red nucleus of cats. 1982; Zaborsky et al., 1986; Ingham et al., 1988),
GABA AND MUSCIMOL 21

acting on NBM GABA receptors constitutes a high dose (50 or 100 tg/tL/h) of GABA, but not
means to modify the activity of these neurons. with a smaller dose (10 Ixg/lxL/h), was obtained
Infusion of small doses of GABA (10 tg/tL/h) using a variety of techniques. Infusion of a high
induces a delay-dependent deficit in a previously dose of GABA (100 tg/tL/h) into the NBM
learned win-shift task in a radial maze induced neuronal damage within this nucleus,
(Majchrzak et al., 1990). This effect is consistent which could be observed on Cresyl violet-stained
with the reported deficit after excitotoxic sections (see Fig. 1).
(ibotenate) lesion of the NBM (for example, The loss of magnocellular cholinergic
Bartus et al., 1985). Infusions of higher doses (50 neurons was confirmed by reduced acetyl-
or 100 tg/lxL/h) of GABA induces a delay- cholinesterase (ACHE) and choline acetyltrans-
independent deficit in the win-shift task ferase (CHAT) activities in the frontal and
(Majchrzak et al., 1990), together with profound parietal cortices (see Fig. 2; Will et al., 1988;
sensorimotor impairments (Will et al., 1988; Majchrzak et al., 1990; 1992; Majchrzak, 1992). In
Majchrzak et al., 1990; 1992a). With both doses, another study (Ballough et al., 1992), two well-
the deficits appeared to be reversible because established bioindicators of neurotoxicity, azure
performance recovered shortly after interruption B-RNA and Feulgen-DNA expression, were used
of the infusion. to examine the putative cytopathic effects of
In a series of experiments, possible long- GABA infusion into the basal forebrain. This
lasting (or irreversible) effects of GABA infusion method revealed a reduced neuronal RNA
into the NBM were evaluated. To characterize metabolism shortly (24 h) after infusion, even
the spatial extent of the inactivation induced by when using the small dose. In the latter case,
GABA infusion, Majchrzak et al., (1992b) however, this effect disappeared within an 8-d
measured local cerebral metabolic rates for postinfusion delay.
glucose (CMRglc) after 24-h infusions of Taken together, the data indicate that GABA
GABA. The results showed that despite its injection is an efficient method to inactivate a
efficacy on the memory task, infusion of a small brain area; the data also indicate that the effects
dose (10 tg/tL/h) did not modify (in comparison of chronic GABA infusion may not be reversible,
with saline-treated animals) CMRglc within the mainly when high doses are used. A series of
NBM, in neighboring structures, or in NBM biological parameters (see above), as well as the
cortical projection areas (frontal and parietal mere existence of the GABA-withdrawal syndrome
cortex). In contrast, infusion of high doses of (see Brailowsky et al., 1987; Fukuda et al., 1987;
GABA (100 tg/laL/h) induced a strong reduction of Brailowsky et al., 1988; 1989) are indicative of
CMRglc within the NBM, as well as in plastic and/or degenerative effects.
neighboring structures (for example, globus
pallidus) and NBM projection areas (frontal and
parietal cortex, amygdala, reticular thalamic MUSCIMOL-INDUCED REVERSIBLE
nucleus). Such distant hypometabolic effects of INACTIVATION
GABA infusion may reflect an NBM reduction
of synaptic activity, but also may be due to Muscimol rapidly induces a hyperpolarization
degenerative processes. Indeed, Browne et al. lasting several hours, with the overall duration
(1998) have shown that intra-NBM injections of depending on the dose (see Martin & Ghez,
excitotoxic compounds (NMDA and AMPA) 1993; 1999). In a series of articles, Martin and
rapidly induce a reduction of glucose utilization colleagues (for example, Martin, 1991; Martin &
in interconnected cortical areas. Ghez, 1999) provided a thorough analysis of
Further evidence of irreversible lesion with a muscimol-induced inactivation, together with
22 M. MAJCHRZAK AND G. DI SCALA

Fig. 1: Photomicrographs of coronal sections of (A) the NBM of a rat after 24-h GABA (100 lag/tL/h) infusion, and
(B) the contralateral noninfused NBM. The brain was processed for Cresyl violet staining 8 days aer the
infusion. The arrows in B indicate the magnocellular neurons that are lacking in A; note the strong gliotic
reaction in A. Scale bars: 100 tm.
GABA AND MUSCIMOL 23

comparisons with other reversible inactivating lidocaine induced the same type of inactivation.
agents (GABA, lidocaine). These experiments Muscimol-induced inactivation has been used
showed that autoradiographic measurement of in diverse species in a variety of behavioral
[4C]glucose uptake, following injection of experiments (for example, Di Scala et al., 1983;
muscimol (1 lg/tL) into the cerebral cortex of rats, Martin & Ghez, 1993; Gallese et al., 1994;
revealed a small area (1 mm) of strong hypo- Mason et al., 1998), including learning and
metabolism, surrounded by an area of milder memory experiments (for example, Matsumara et
hypometabolism. This hypometabolic area al., 1991; Hardiman et al., 1996; Krupa et al.,
exceeded the spread of the drug that was 1996; Ramnani & Yeo, 1996; Milak et al., 1997;
evaluated with [3H] muscimol injection, Baunez & Robbins, 1999). The following section
indicating that the area of hypometabolism may does not attempt to provide an exhaustive review
be due to reduced synaptic activity in inter- of these experiments, but rather illustrates some
connected neurons. In this regard, muscimol and questions that can be addressed with this type of

Choline acetyltransferase Acetylchol inesterase


140

saline (n=8)
120 GABA10 (n=8)
GABA100 (n=6)

100
o

E 8o-

o
EE 6o-

o 40-

frontal cortex parieta cortex frontal cortex parietal cortex

Fig. 2: The effect of GABA infusion into the NBM on cholinergic markers in frontal and parietal cortices. Saline or
GABA (10 or 100 lag/tL/h) were infused over 24 h. Fourteen days later, the rats were sacrificed, and the
frontal and parietal cortices were rapidly dissected. Choline acetyltransferase and acetylcholinesterase
activities were assayed using enzymatic methods (Fonnum, 1975; Ellman et al., 1961). The highest
concentration of GABA induced a significant reduction of both cholinergic markers as compared with saline
(* p<0.05; ** p<0.01; Newman-Keuls post-hoc comparisons).
24 M. MAJCHRZAK AND G. DI SCALA

reversible inactivation. studies. Using tetrodotoxine (TTX), lidocaine (or


As mentioned earlier, GABA receptors, novocaine), and muscimol have largely contributed
located on NBM cholinergic neurons, may to the identification of these processes (for
constitute a target to modify the activity of these example, Gallo et al., 1992; Willner et al., 1993;
neurons. Muscimol injection into the NBM was Jerusalinsky et al., 1994; Muller et al., 1997;
found to impair the performance of rats in Ambrogi-Lorenzini et al., 1999). In this regard,
attentional tasks, such as two- and five-choice conditioned food-aversion procedures have been
reaction time (Muir et al., 1992; Pang et al., considered particularly appropriate to realize a
1993) or conditioned discrimination (Dudchenko "chronometric analysis" of the various processes
& Sarter, 1991). The effects were similar to those that are involved in learning and memory, with
obtained with excitotoxic lesions of the NBM, the aid of reversible inactivation (see Bures, 1990;
including AMPA lesion, which is thought to have Bures & Buresova, 1990). In these procedures,
a preferential effect on cholinergic neurons intake of a food by the rat (a drinking solution,
(Muir et al., 1995; Everitt et al., 1987; Robbins et which may be identified by its taste or its odor) is
al., 1989). Working memory deficits in a double followed by intoxication, induced by injection of
Y-maze were reported after intra-NBM injections lithium chloride, resulting in avoidance of the
of small doses of muscimol (0.1 tg; Beninger et food upon subsequent encounter. Using TTX to
al., 1992; DeSousa et al., 1994), whereas both inactivate a variety of brain structures at specific
working and reference memory deficits were phases of a conditioned taste aversion, Bures and
obtained after injecting higher doses (1 tg collaborators (Bures, 1990; Gallo et al., 1992)
Beninger et al., 1992). have exquisitely documented the involvement of
Similar effects, confined to working memory, the connections between the parabrachial
were obtained after quisqualate lesion (Biggan et nucleus, the amygdala, and the gustatory cortex
al., 1991; Beninger et al., 1994), which induces in this learning. In a recent series of experiments,
restricted NBM lesions (Dunnett et al., 1987) we used muscimol to study the neuroanatomical
when compared with the working and reference substrate that is involved in a particular instance
memory deficits that are obtained after ibotenate of conditioned food aversion, which is
lesions (see Dunnett et al., 1991). The data conditioned odor aversion (COA). Conditioned
support the idea that muscimol injection induces odor aversion is the avoidance of a tasteless,
a reversible inactivation, the effects of which are odorized solution, the ingestion of which has
similar to those of excitotoxic lesions. Moreover, preceded toxicosis; COA differs from the classic
in the NBM, such effects may be related to an conditioned taste aversion (CTA) in that it does
action on cholinergic neurons, as excitotoxic not tolerate long interstimulus intervals (ISI)
compounds having some selectivity for these between the solution intake and the induction of
neurons have similar effects. Nonetheless, the toxicosis (Hankins et al., 1973). Nonetheless,
deficits induced by intra-NBM injection of the evidence exists of COA that is acquired despite
selective cholinergic neurons toxin, 9IgG- long ISis when the odor is presented together
saporin, are smaller than those induced by with a taste during acquisition; this procedure is
muscimol in a variety of tests (Torres et al., called Taste-Potentiated Odor Aversion (TPOA).
1994; Wenk et al., 1994; Baxter et al., 1995), TPOA depends on the baso-lateral nucleus of the
suggesting that the behavioral deficits induced by amygdala (BLA), as electrolytic or excitotoxic
muscimol may depend also on its effects on lesions of this nucleus were found to disrupt it
noncholinergic NBM neurons. (Bermudez-Rattoni et al, 1986; Hatfield et al., 1992;
The amygdala is involved at various stages of Ferry et al., 1995). Muscimol-induced inactivation
learning and memory, and reversible inactivation of the BLA during the acquisition phase, but not
GABA AND MUSCIMOL 25

during the retrieval phase, of the procedure was methods is not possible. The issue of reversibility
found to be effective, suggesting that this nucleus is particularly important when animals are tested
is involved in the former process. Furthermore, anew after the reversible inactivation, as it is
to be effective, muscimol could be injected either then considered that the system is fully
before or after presentation of the odor-taste functional again. In this regard, little empirical
stimulus, suggesting that neuronal activity in the evidence demonstrating complete reversibility of
BLA is necessary after the sensory processing of any pharmacological treatment is available.
the composite stimulus, that is for a memory Another concern with reversible inactivation
process (Ferry et al., 1995). In this regard, in learning tasks is state-dependent retrieval
muscimol injection differs from other reversible (SDR). State-dependent retrieval means that
inactivation compounds in that injection of information learned in a given state may not be
novocaine into the amygdala impairs TPOA if it (or may be poorly) retrieved when the subject is
is administered before, but not after, presentation in a different state. Evidence for SDR has been
of the odor-taste stimulus (Bermudez-Rattoni et obtained with systemic pharmacological treatments
al., 1983). Conversely, it is noticeable that given at various stages (acquisition, extinction) of
whether injected before or after presentation of learning tasks (for example, Colpaert, 1990; Bouton
the odor-taste stimulus, muscimol selectively et at, 1990; Oberling et at, 1996) and has been taken
affects COA without affecting CTA (tested into account when discussing reversible inactivation
separately), which develop in parallel. This result data (for example, Muller et al., 1997). It is unknown
is consistent with those of Gallo et al. (1992) to what extent drug injections into brain areas can
showing that inactivating the amygdala by TTX produce SDR, and studies addressing this problem
before taste presentation does not impair CTA, would certainly be welcome.
whereas inactivating the amygdala after taste As a final comment, reversible inactivation
presentation only attenuates CTA. The data, if techniques significantly contribute to the
suggestive of subtle differences between different knowledge of "where and when" (Bures &
methods of reversible inactivation, do not support Buresova, 1990) neuronal events for learning and
a clear-cut distinction between these methods. memory take place in the brain. As first stated in
this article, knowledge about the nature of such
neuronal events has considerably progressed in
DISCUSSION recent years. Using research strategies similar to
reversible inactivation, antisense oligonucleotide
The data briefly reviewed above clearly techniques offer the opportunity to interfere with
indicate that GABA or GABA-receptor agonists neuronal events in precise locations in the brain
constitute valuable reversible inactivation tools and at chosen phases of a task (for example,
for studying learning and memory. Our critical Lamprecht et al., 1997; Guzowski & McGaugh,
analysis of the data obtained with chronic 1997; Ma et al., 1998). There is little doubt that
infusions of GABA, one of Simon Brailowsky’s such techniques will shortly complement the
favorite tools, suggests that depending on the pharmacological analysis of brain systems of
dose used and the targeted area, some effects learning and memory.
may not be reversible. To our knowledge, little
attention has been given to possible long-lasting
REFERENCES
effects of other compounds that are used for
reversible inactivation (see however, Hemandez Ambrogi-Lorenzini CG, Baldi E, Bucherelli C,
& Schallert, 1990) and hence, comparison of the Saccheti B, Tassoni G. Neural topography and
advantages and drawbacks of the various chronology of memory consolidation: a review
26 M. MAJCHRZAK AND G. DI SCALA

of functional inactivation findings. Neurobiol GABAB receptor site distribution in rat central
Learning and Memory 1999; 71" 1-18. nervous system. Neurosci 1987; 20: 365-383.
Ballough G, Majchrzak M, Strauss J, Kan R, Bouton ME, Kenney FA, Rosengard C. State-
Anthony A, Will B. Cytophotometric analysis of dependent fear extinction with two benzo-
magnocellular azure B-RNA and Feulgen-DNA diazepine tranquilizers. Behav Neurosci 1990;
following chronic GABA infusion into the nucleus 104: 44-55.
basalis of rats. Life Sci 1992; 50:1299-1310. Brailowsky S, Kunimoto M, Menini C, Silva-Barrat
Bartus RT, Flicker C, Dean RL, Pontecorvo M, C, Riche D, Naquet R. The GABA-withdrawal
Figueiredo JC, Fisher SK. Selective memory loss syndrome: a new model of focal
following nucleus basalis lesions: long term epileptogenesis. Brain Res 1988; 442" 175-179
behavioral recovery despite persistent cholinergic Brailowsky S, Menini C, Silva-Barrat C, Naquet R.
deficiencies. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1985; Epileptogenic gamma-aminobutyric-acid withdrawal
23: 125-135. syndrome after chronic, inWacortical infusion into
Baunez C, Robbins TW. Effects of transient baboons. Neurosci Lett 1987; 74: 75-80.
inactivation of the subthalamic nucleus by local Bmilowsky S, Silva-Barmt C, Menini C, Riche D,
muscimol and APV infusions on performance in Naquet R. Effects of localized, chronic GABA
the five-choice serial reaction time task in rats. infusions into different cortical areas of the photo-
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1999; 141: 57-65. sensitive baboon, Papio papio. Electroencephalogr
Baxter MG, Bucci DJ, Gorman LK, Wiley RG, Clin Neurophysiol 1989;72: 147-156.
Gallagher M. Selective immunotoxic lesions of Browne SE, Muir JL, Robbins TW, Page KJ,
basal forebrain cholinergic ceils: effects on Everitt BJ, McCulloch J. The cerebral metabolic
learning and memory in rats. Behav Neurosci effects of manipulating glutamatergic systems
1995; 109:714-722. within the basal forebrain in conscious rats. Eur
Beninger RJ, Ingles JL, Mackenzie PJ, Jhamandas J Neurosci 1998; 10" 649-663.
K, Boegman ILl. Muscimol injections into the Bures, J. Neurobiology of memory: the significance
nucleus basalis magnocellularis of rats: selective of anomalous findings. In: McGaugh JL, Wein-
impairment of working memory in the double berger NM, Lynch G, eds., Brain Organization and
Y-maze. Brain Res 1992; 597: 66-73. Memory: Cells, Systems, and Circuits. New York:
Beninger RJ, Kuhnemann S, Ingles JL, Jhamandas Oxford University Press, 1990: 3-19.
K, Boegman ILl. Mnemonic deficits in the Bures J, Buresova O. Reversible lesions allow
double Y-maze are related to the effects of reinterpretation of system level studies of brain
nucleus basalis injections of ibotenic and mechanisms of behavior. Concepts Neurosci
quisqualic acid on choline acetyltransferase in 1990; 1: 69-89.
the rat amygdala. Brain Res Bull 1994; 35: 147- Casamenti F, DiPatre PL, Bartolini L, Pepeu G.
152. Unilateral and bilateral nucleus basalis lesions:
Bermudez-Rattoni F, Rusiniak KW, Garcia J. differences in neurochemical and behavioral
Flavor-illness aversions: potentiation of odor by recovery. Neurosci 1988; 24: 209-215.
taste is disrupted by application of novocaine Chu DCM, Albin RL, Young AB, Penney JB.
into amygdala. Behav Neural Biol 1983; 37: Distribution and kinetics of GABAa binding sites
61-75. in rat central nervous system: a quantitative auto-
Bermudez-Rattoni F, Grijalva CV, Kiefer SW, Garcia radiographic study. Neurosci 1990; 34:341-357.
J. Flavor-illness aversions: the role of the Colpaert FC. Amnesic trace locked into the ben-
amygdala in the acquisition of taste-potentiated zodiazepine state of memory. Psychopharma-
odor aversions. Physiol Behav 1986; 38: 503- cology 1990; 102: 28-36.
508. DeSousa NJ, Beninger RJ, Jhamandas K, Boegman RJ.
B iggan SL, Beninger ILl, Cockhill J, Jhamandas K, Stimulation of GABAB receptors in the basal
Boegman ILl. Quisqualate lesions of rat NBM: forebrain selectively impairs working memory of rots
selective effects on working memory in a double in the double Y-maze. Brain Res 1994; 641" 29-38.
Y-maze. Brain Res Bull 1991; 26:613-616. Di Scala G, Schmitt P and Karli P. Unilateral
Bormann J. Electrophysiology of GABAA and injection of GABA agonists in the superior
GABAB receptors subtypes. Trends Neurosci colliculus: asymetry to tactile stimulation.
1988; 11:112-116. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1983; 19: 284-285.
Bowery NG, Hudson AL, Price GW. GABAA and Di Scala G, Meneses S, Brailowsky S. Chronic
GABA AND MUSCIMOL 27

infusions of GABA into the medial frontal rat on acetylcholine release in cerebral cortex:
cortex of the rat induce a reversible delayed time course of compensatory events. Brain Res
spatial alternation deficit. Behav Brain Res 1987; 407: 263-271.
1990; 40: 81-84. Guzowski JF, McGaugh JL. Antisense oligodeoxy-
Dudchenko P, Sarter M. GABAergic control of nucleotide-mediated disruption of hippocampal
basal forebrain cholinergic neurons and cAMP response element binding protein levels
memory. Behav Brain Res 1991; 42: 33-41. impairs consolidation of memory for water maze
Dunnett SB, Everitt B J, Robbins TW. The basal training. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1997; 94:
forebrain-cortical cholinergic system: 2693-2698.
interpreting the functional consequences of Hankins WG, Garcia J, Rusiniak KW. Dissociation
excitotoxic lesions. Trends Neurosci 1991; 14" of odor and taste in bait shyness. Behav Biol
494-501 1973; 8:407-4 19.
Dunnett SB, Whishaw IQ, Jones GH, Bunch ST. Hardiman MJ, Ramnani N, Yeo CH. Reversible
Behavioural, biochemical and histochemical inactivations of the cerebellum with muscimol
effects of different neurotoxic amino acids prevent the acquisition and extinction of
injected into nucleus basalis magnocellularis of conditioned nictitating membrane responses in
rats. Neuroscience 1987; 20: 653-669. the rabbit. Exp Brain Res 1996;110:235-247.
Ellman GL, Courtney KD, Andres VJr, Hatfield T, Graham PW, Gallagher M. Taste-
Featherstone RM. A new and rapid colorimetric potentiated odor aversion learning: role of the
determination of acetylcholinesterase activity. amygdaloid basolateral complex and central
Biochem Pharmacol 1961; 7: 88-95. nucleus. Behav Neurosci 1992; 106: 286-293.
Everitt HJ, Robbins TW, Evenden JL, Marston HM, Hemandez TD, Schallert T. Long-term impairment
Jones GH, Sirki/t TE. The effects of excitotoxic of behavioral recovery from cortical damage can
lesiom of the substantia innominata, ventral and be produced by short-term GABA-agonist
dorsal globus pallidus on the acquisition and infusion into adjacent cortex. Restor Neurol
retention of a conditional visual discrimination: Neurosci 1990; 1" 323-330.
implications for cholinergic hypothesis of learning Hup6 JM, Chouvet G, Bullier J. Spatial and
and memory. Neurosci 1987; 22: 441-469. temporal parameters of cortical inactivation by
Farah MJ. Neuropsychological inference with an GABA. J Neurosci Meth 1999; 86: 129-143.
interactive brain: a critique of the "locality" Ingham CA, Bolam JP, Smith AD. GABA-
assumption. Behav Brain Sci 1994; 17 43-104. immuno-reactive synaptic boutons in the rat
Ferry B, Sandner G, Di Scala G. Neuroanatomical and basal forebrain: comparison of neurons that
functional specificity of the basolateral amygdaloid project to the neocortex with pallidosubthalamic
nucleus in taste-potentiated odor aversion. neurons. J Comp Neurol 1988; 273:263-282
Neurobiol Learn Mem 1995; 64:169-180. Iversen LL, Kelly JC. Uptake and metabolism of
Fonnum F. A rapid radiochemical method for the ,-aminobutyric acid by neurons and glial cells.
determination of choline acetyltransferase. J Biochem Pharmacol 1975; 24: 933-938.
Neurochem 1975; 24: 407-409. Jaffard R, Meunier M. Role of the hippocampal
Fukuda H, Brailowsky S, Menini C, Silva-Barrat C, formation in learning and memory. Hippocampus
Riche D, Naquet R. Anticonvulsant effect of 1993; 3: 203-217.
intracortical, chronic infusion of GABA in Jarrard LE. On the role of the hippocampus in
kindled rats: focal seizures upon withdrawal. learning and memory in the rat. Behav Neural
Exp Neurol 1987; 98: 120-129. Biol 1993; 60: 9-26.
Gallese V, Murata A, Kaseda M, Niki N, Sakata H. Jerusalinsky D, Quillfeldt JA, Walz R, Da Silva RC,
Deficit of hand preshaping after muscimol Bueno e Silva M, Bianchin M, et al. Effect of the
injection in monkey parietal cortex. Neuroreport infusion of the GABA-A receptor agonist, musci-
1994; 5: 1525-1529. mol, on the role of the entorhinal cortex, amygdala,
Gallo M, Roldan G, Bures J. Differential involvement and hippocampus in memory processes. Behav
of gustatory insular cortex and amygdala in the Neural Biol 1994; 61" 132-138.
acquisition and retrieval of conditioned taste Kolb B. Functions of the frontal cortex of the rat: a
aversion in rats. Behav Brain Res 1992; 52:91-97. comparative review. Brain Res 1984; 320: 65-98.
Gardiner IM, De Belleroche J, Premi BK, Hamilton Krogsgaard-Larsen P. GABA synaptic mechanisms:
MH. Effects of lesion of the nucleus basalis of stereochemical and conformational requirements.
28 M. MAJCHRZAK AND G. DI SCALA

Med Res Rev 1988; 8: 27-56. micro-injection of lidocaine and muscimol in


Krupa DJ, Weng J, Thompson RF. Inactivation of the rat. Neurosci Lett 1991; 127: 160-164.
brainstem motor nuclei blocks expression but Martin JH, Ghez C. Differential impairments in
not acquisition of the rabbit’s classically reaching and grasping produced by local
conditioned eyeblink response. Behav Neurosci inactivation within the forelimb representation
110: 219-227. of the motor cortex in the cat. Exp Brain Res
Kuhar MJ. A GABA transporter eDNA has been 1993; 94: 429-443.
cloned. Trends Neurosci 1990; 13" 473-474. Martin JH, Ghez C. Pharmacological inactivation in
Iamaprecht R, Hazvi S, Dudai Y. cAMP respome the analysis of the central control of movement.
element-binding protein in the amygdala is required J Neurosci Meth 1999; 86:145-159.
for long- but not short-term conditioned taste Mason CR, Miller LE, Baker JF, Houk JC.
aversion memory. J Neurosci 1997; 17: 8443-8450. Organization of reaching and grasping
Lomber SG. The advantages and limitations of movements in the primate cerebellar nuclei as
permanent or reversible deactivation techniques revealed by focal muscimol inactivations. J
in the assessment of neural function. J Neurosci Neurophysiol 1998; 79: 537-554.
Meth 1999; 86:109-117. Matsumura M, Sawaguchi T, Oishi T, Ueki K,
Lynch G. Memory and the brain: unexpected Kubota K. Behavioral deficits induced by local
chemistries and a new pharmacology. Neurobiol injection of bicuculline and muscimol into the
Learn Mem 1998; 70: 82-100. primate motor and premotor cortex. J
Ma YL, Wang HL, Wu HC, Wei CL, Lee EH. Neurophysiol 1991; 65" 1542-53.
Brain-derived neurotrophic factor antisense Meneses S, Galicia O, Brailowsky S. Chronic
oligonucleotide impairs memory retention and infusions of GABA into the medial prefrontal
inhibits long-term potentiation in rats. cortex induce spatial alternation deficits in aged
Neuroscience 1998; 82: 957-967. rats. Behav Brain Res 1993; 57" 1-7.
McDonald MP, Overmier JB. Present imperfect: a Milak MS, Shimansky Y, Bracha V, Bloedel JR.
critical review of animal models of the Effects of inactivating individual cerebellar
mnemonic impairments in Alzheimer’s disease. nuclei on the performance and retention of an
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1998; 22" 99-120. operantly conditioned forelimb movement. J
Majchrzak M, Brailowsky S, Will B. Chronic Neurophysiol 1997; 78: 939-959.
infusion of GABA and saline into the nucleus Mugnaini E, Oertel WH. An atlas of the distribution
basalis magnocellularis of rats" II. Cognitive of GABAergic neurons and terminals in the rat
impairments. Behav Brain Res 1990; 37: 45-56. CNS as revealed by GAD immunohisto-
Majchrzak M. L’infusion chronique intrac6r6brale chemistry. In: Bj/Srklund A and H6kfelt T. eds.
de GABA comme mod61e d"6tude du r61e du Handbook of Chemical Neuroanatomy, Vol 4:
noyau basal magnocellulaire dans l’expression GABA and Neuropeptides in the CNS, Part I.
des fonctions cognitives et sensorimotrices chez Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1985; 436-608.
le Rat. Th,se de l’Universit6 Louis Pasteur Muir JL, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Disruptive
(sp6cialit6 Neurosciences), Strasbourg, France, effects of muscimol infused into the basal
1992; 1-259. forebrain on conditional discrimination and
Majchrzak M, Brailowsky S, Will B. Chronic visual attention: differential interactions with
infusion of GABA into the nucleus basalis cholinergic mechanisms. Psychopharmacology
magnocellularis or frontal cortex of rats: a 1992; 107: 541-550.
behavioral and histological study. Exp Brain Muir JL, Page KJ, Sirinathsinghji DJ, Robbins TW,
Res 1992a; 88:531-540. Everitt BJ. Excitotoxic lesions of basal
Majchrzak M, Nehlig A, Will B. Local cerebral forebrain cholinergic neurons: effects on
glucose utilization during chronic infusion of learning, memory and attention. Behav Brain
GABA into the nucleus basalis magnocellularis Res 1993, 57: 123-131.
of rats. Exp Neurol 1992b; 116: 256-263. Muir JL, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Reversal of visual
Malpeli JD. Reversible inactivation of subcortical attentional dysfunction following lesions of the
sites by drug injection. J Neurosci Methods cholinergic basal forebmin by physostigmine and
1999; 86:119-128. nicotine but not by the 5-HT3 receptor antagonist,
Martin JH. Autoradiographic estimation of the ondanetron. Psychopharmacology 1995; 118: 82.
extent of reversible inactivation produced by Muller J, Corodimas KP, Fridel Z, LeDoux JE.
GABA AND MUSCIMOL 29

Functional inactivation of the lateral and basal selective immunolesions in discrete regions of
nuclei of the amygdala by muscimol infusion the basal forebrain cholinergic system. Neurosci
prevents fear conditioning to an explicit 1994; 63: 95-122.
conditioned stimulus and to contextual stimuli. Tulving E. Concepts of human memory. In: Squire
Behav Neurosci 1997; 111" 683-691. LR, Weinberger NM, Lynch G, McGaugh JL,
Oberling P, Di Scala G, Sandner G. Ketamine acts eds, Memory: Organization and Locus of
as an internal contextual stimulus that facilitates Change. Oxford University Press, 1991; 3-32.
the retrieval of weak associations in conditioned Wenk GL, Olton DS. Recovery of neocortical
taste aversion. Psychobiol 1996; 24: 300-305. acetyltransferase activity following ibotenic acid
Pang K, Williams MJ, Egeth H, Olton DS. Nucleus injection into the nucleus basalis of Meynert in
basalis magnocellularis and attention: effects of rats. Brain Res 1984; 293: 184-186.
muscimol infusions. Behav Neurosci 1993; 107: Wenk GL, Stoehr JD, Quintana G, Mobley S,
1031-1038. Wiley RG. Behavioral, biochemical, histo-
Ramnani N, Yeo CH. Reversible inactivations of logical, and electrophysiological effects of 192
the cerebellum prevent the extinction of IgG-saporin injections into the basal forebrain
conditioned nictitating membrane responses in of rats. J Neurosci 1994; 14: 5986-5995.
rabbits. J Physiol (Lond) 1996; 495: 159-168. Will BE, Toniolo G, Brailowsky TD. Unilateral infusion
Robbins TW, Everitt B J, Marston HM, Wilkinson of GABA and saline into the nucleus basalis of rats:
J, Jones GH, Page KJ, Comparative effects of 1. Effects on motor function and brain morphology.
ibotenic acid and quisqualic acid-induced Behav Brain Res 1988; 27: 123-129.
lesions of the substantia innominata on Willner P, Bianchin M, Walz R, Bueno e Silva M,
attentional function in the rat: Further Zanatta MS, Izquierdo I. Muscimol infused into
implications for the role of the cholinergic the entorhinal cortex prior to training blocks the
neurons of the nucleus basalis in cognitive involvement of this area in post-training memory
processes. Behav Brain Res 1989; 35: 221-240. processing. Behav Pharmacol 1993; 4: 95-99.
Sinnamon HM, Benaur M. GABA injected into the Wood PL, Richard J. GABAergic regulation of the
anterior dorsal tegmentum (ADT) of the substantia innominata-cortical cholinergic path-
midbrain blocks stepping initiated by way. Neuropharmacol 1982; 21: 969-972.
stimulation of the hypothalamus. Brain Res Zaborsky L; Heimer L, Eckenstein F, Leranth C.
1997; 766: 271-275. GABAergic input to cholinergic forebrain
Torres EM, Perry TA, B lockland A, Wilkinson LS, neurons: An ultrastructural study using retrograde
Wiley RG, Lappi DA, Dunnet SB. Behavioural, tracing of HRP and double immunolabeling. J
histohemical and biochemical consequences of Comp Neurol 1986; 250: 282-295.

You might also like