Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 54

Journal Pre-proof

Issues, involvement, and influence: Effects of selective exposure and sharing on


polarization and participation

Benjamin K. Johnson, Rachel L. Neo, Marieke E.M. Heijnen, Lotte Smits, Caitrina van
Veen

PII: S0747-5632(19)30367-X
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.09.031
Reference: CHB 6155

To appear in: Computers in Human Behavior

Received Date: 3 June 2019


Revised Date: 27 September 2019
Accepted Date: 30 September 2019

Please cite this article as: Johnson B.K., Neo R.L., Heijnen M.E.M., Smits L. & van Veen C., Issues,
involvement, and influence: Effects of selective exposure and sharing on polarization and participation,
Computers in Human Behavior (2019), doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.09.031.

This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as the addition
of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the definitive version of
record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and review before it is published
in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early visibility of the article. Please note that,
during the production process, errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal
disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

© 2019 Published by Elsevier Ltd.


Running head: EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 1

Issues, Involvement, and Influence: Effects of Selective Exposure and Sharing on

Polarization and Participation

Benjamin K. Johnsona*
Rachel L. Neob
Marieke E. M. Heijnenc
Lotte Smitsc
Caitrina van Veenc

a
University of Florida, Department of Advertising

1885 Stadium Road

Gainesville, FL 32611, USA

benjaminkjohnson@ufl.edu

b
University of Hawaii at Manoa, School of Communications

2550 Campus Road

Honolulu, HI 96822, USA

c
Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Department of Communication Science

De Boelelaan 1081

1081 HV Amsterdam, Netherlands

*Corresponding author.

Author Note: No competing interests exist.


EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 1

Abstract

Although research has amply demonstrated that people exhibit confirmatory biases associated

with exposure and information sharing on social media, there is a lack of research attempting to

parse out the respective effects of selective exposure and sharing on political outcomes,

especially in non-U.S. contexts. In this experiment, we tested the extent of confirmation bias in

Dutch Facebook users’ selection and sharing of opinionated news about three political issues.

The relative contributions of selecting versus sharing pro-attitudinal (and counter-attitudinal)

messages were assessed for their influences on attitude polarization and political participation.

Value- and impression- involvement were considered as moderating factors. Findings indicate

that a confirmation bias is much more consistently observed in selective sharing than in selective

exposure. Second, pro-attitudinal selective sharing is a more robust predictor of political

outcomes than pro-attitudinal selective exposure. Third, the effects of selective sharing and

exposure on political outcomes depend more on value involvement than impression relevant

involvement. Finally, between-topic differences were evident for the extent of confirmation bias

and its effects on political outcomes.

Keywords: selective exposure, selective sharing, polarization, online news, online

participation, involvement, experiment


EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 2

1. Introduction

The existence of a confirmation bias in the selection and sharing of news and information

is well-documented (Hart et al., 2009; Knobloch-Westerwick, 2015). Selective exposure, a

tendency of people to choose and spend more time reading attitude-consistent information, can

yield effects on outcomes such as attitude polarization, feelings toward outgroups, voting

intentions, and political participation intentions (Feezell, 2016; Garrett et al., 2014; Wojcieszak,

Bimber, Feldman, & Stroud, 2016). In addition to attitude-consistent selective exposure, people

may also engage in attitude-consistent selective sharing. The widespread adoption of social

network sites such as Facebook and Twitter have created new platforms for partisan sharing of

news and political messages (Beam, Hutchens, & Hmielowski, 2016; Oeldorf-Hirsch & Sundar,

2015; Weeks, Lane, Kim, Lee, & Kwak, 2017).

Despite increasing scholarly attention devoted to understanding how selective exposure

or selective sharing on social media influence political outcomes (e.g., Shin & Thorson, 2017;

Weeks et al., 2017), existing research on this topic still has several key limitations. First, most

studies have only examined either selective exposure or selective sharing as focal variables, but

not both. Some scholars have argued that selective sharing is a more overtly clarion reflection of

partisan leanings than selective exposure (Shin & Thorson, 2017) and that it involves greater

behavioral commitment (Lane et al., 2019). Our study thus tests the effects of (a) attitudinal

stance on selective exposure versus selective sharing and (b) how both selective exposure and

selective sharing on social media affect political outcomes among the same individuals. This

allows us to assess which of these two confirmation biases is a stronger predictor of political

outcomes.

Second, few studies on selective exposure or sharing have accounted for the moderating
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 3

role of involvement (Liao & Fu, 2013; Wheeless, 1974). Research has shown that involvement

levels affect partisan-based political behaviors (Perloff, 1989). However, not everyone exhibits

deep-seated levels of value involvement toward any given political issue or exhibits similar

levels of concern about others’ evaluations of their political preferences.

Third, most studies on selective exposure (Bakshy, Messing, & Adamic, 2015) or sharing

on social media (e.g., Weeks et al., 2017) have been conducted in the United States. The United

States has a unique two-party political system with an electorate that is heavily polarized along

party lines (Garrett et al., 2014). It is imperative to conduct more experimental research outside

of the U.S. in order to provide stronger evidence for these two aforementioned confirmation

biases, especially in democracies such as the Netherlands with parliamentary systems involving a

fragmented diversity of political parties that must typically work together in coalition to form a

functioning government (Pellikaan, de Lange, & van der Meer, 2018). In the Netherlands,

specifically, there have been well-designed tests of partisan selective exposure using panel data

(e.g., Bos, Kruikemeier, & de Vreese, 2016), a quasi-experimental design (Trilling, van

Klingeren, & Tsfati, 2017), and experiments testing the effects of frames on selective exposure

(Brenes Peralta, Wojcieszak, Lelkes, & de Vreese, 2017; Hameleers, Bos, & de Vreese, 2018).

However, there is little to no evidence to date on selective sharing in Dutch contexts, and more

research is needed on the process of—and potential for—polarization in multi-party systems

such as the Netherlands.

Fourth, most studies have examined selective exposure exclusively in the context of

highly polarized topics (e.g., Westerwick, Johnson, & Knobloch-Westerwick, 2017). In our

study, we use several topics which vary in their existing polarization and salience (cf. Y. M.

Kim, 2009).
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 4

To bridge these research gaps outlined above, this study reports the results of an

experiment in which compares the relative influence of selective exposure and sharing (to pro-

and counter-attitudinal political content on Facebook) on political participation and opinion

polarization. In particular, we investigate these processes in the contexts of three political topics

chosen for their varying relevance and salience for a Dutch sample: the relationship between

Ukraine and the EU; the entry of refugees into the Netherlands; and equal pay for men and

women. Moreover, we investigate two forms of attitudinal involvement—value-involvement and

impression-involvement—as moderators not only of selectivity, but also of selectivity’s effects

on polarization and participation.

2. Literature review

2.1. The effect of information stance on selective exposure

Numerous studies have shown that individuals’ attitudinal stances will guide the

composition and degree of their information exposure (Garrett, 2009a; Hart et al., 2009;

Knobloch-Westerwick, 2015). Notably, confirmation bias describes the tendency where people

will gravitate toward information that aligns with their existing views (Lord, Ross, & Lepper,

1979). The psychological processes underlying confirmation bias can be explained by the theory

of cognitive dissonance (Festinger, 1957), which posits that people strive to minimize

psychological discomfort arising from performing actions that are inconsistent with their values

and beliefs. One such action is that of counter-attitudinal political news consumption (Jeong, Zo,

Lee, & Ceran, 2019). Although people do not avoid counter-attitudinal political information

entirely (Garrett, Carnahan, & Lynch, 2013), they nonetheless spend less time reading counter-

attitudinal than pro-attitudinal news stories (e.g., Frey, 1986; Garrett, 2009a).

Some scholars have argued that the control afforded by online platforms facilitates the
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 5

selective consumption of pro-attitudinal political information (Garrett, 2009b). Likewise, the

sheer volume of content made available by digital platforms may heighten the ability, desire, or

necessity for user selectivity in news use (S. Lee, Lindsey, & Kim, 2017). Indeed, research has

consistently demonstrated robust support for confirmation bias in online contexts such as social

media (e.g., Westerwick et al., 2017).

The term selective exposure is often used to refer to an observed confirmation bias in

information exposure, where pro-attitudinal content is selected at a higher rate (Stroud, 2008) or

even exclusively, in some formulations. We take a broader definition of selective exposure,

following Knobloch-Westerwick (2015), such that it refers to any systematic pattern in media

use. This accounts for a broader set of motivations for media use (including accuracy, impression

management, novelty, entertainment, etc.) as well as the possibility that some individuals, under

certain situations, may exercise a selective preference for counter-attitudinal messages (e.g., if

that information provides more utility; Knobloch-Westerwick & Kleinman, 2012). However, our

starting point for this investigation is a general expectation that a confirmation bias will be

evident in exposure to political information presented in a social media context.

H1: People will spend more time reading pro-attitudinal than counter-attitudinal news

stories on social media.

2.2. The effect of selective exposure on political participation and opinion polarization

Next, we can expect pro- and counter-attitudinal political information consumption to

have effects on variables such as opinion polarization and political participation. Research has

demonstrated a robust positive link between pro-attitudinal news consumption and political

participation (Knobloch-Westerwick & Johnson, 2014; Stroud, 2010). And, the more people

attend to attitude-reinforcing political information, the more they will exhibit strong and even
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 6

extreme attitudes. Such confidence emboldens people to take political action, so that attitude-

affirming news on social media is likely to foster both online and offline political participation

(Beam, Hutchens, & Hmielowski, 2018; Feezell, 2016; Y. Kim & Chen, 2016).

Social media provide ease of access to content, coupled with the presence of social peers,

making it an important context for interacting with news and political information. As such,

social media appear to be especially consequential for political participation (Bode, Vraga,

Borah, & Shah, 2014; Boulianne, 2015). Furthermore, consuming attitude-affirming news

increases the salience of a person’s political identity, thereby causing a person to think and

behave in ways that signify their party affiliation (Stroud, 2010). For instance, selective exposure

causes people to develop more polarized opinions on controversial issues that align with their

party’s values (Y. Kim, 2015; Stroud, 2010; Westerwick, Johnson, & Knobloch-Westerwick,

2017). From this, we can expect that accessing pro-attitudinal news in a social media context will

increase participation and opinion polarization. We hypothesize that:

H2: The amount of time spent reading pro-attitudinal news stories will be positively

associated with (a) online political participation, (b) offline participation, and (c) opinion

polarization.

By contrast, counter-attitudinal information consumption can cause people to challenge

the views espoused by pro-attitudinal information (Garrett et al., 2014). People might gain a

better understanding of their political opponents’ issue positions through exposure to counter-

attitudinal political information (Mutz, 2002). As such, counter-attitudinal information

consumption might make people hold ambivalent issue attitudes and fewer intentions to engage

in participatory political activities (Mutz, 2002).

In some circumstances, exposure to counter-attitudinal information via social media


EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 7

might increase participation intentions (Y. Kim & Chen, 2016), especially if incivility is present

(Hwang, Kim, & Huh, 2014). And, some research on the relationship between counter-attitudinal

information exposure and opinion polarization shows null or nuanced results, such that exposure

to ideologically diverse content online might not influence polarization outcomes (J. Lee, Choi,

Kim, & Kim, 2014) or might yield a mix of polarization and depolarization effects, depending on

the context and particular political issue (Y. Kim, 2015).

Yet, evidence generally shows that contact with counter-attitudinal information

diminishes political behavior (Matthes, 2012) and weakens (i.e., depolarizes) attitudes

(Westerwick et al., 2017). In the social media context, recent surveys with Americans found that

counter-attitudinal discussion on social media was linked to less political participation (Lu,

Heatherly, & Lee, 2016) and counter-attitudinal news exposure on Facebook produced

depolarization over time (Beam et al., 2018). To that end, the evidence suggests that counter-

attitudinal political information exposure will reduce the degree to which issue opinions are

polarized along partisan lines. The following hypothesis is proposed:

H3: The amount of time spent reading counter-attitudinal news stories will be negatively

associated with (a) online political participation, (b) offline participation, and (c) opinion

polarization.

2.3. The effect of information stance on selective sharing

The presence of an expressive goal has been shown to enhance confirmation bias in

message exposure (Smith, Fabrigar, Powell, & Estrada, 2007). This need to present one’s

identity and opinions are even more pertinent when it comes to selective sharing, as the sharing

is often directed at a large audience (if not public) and plays a role in individual self-presentation

(Coppini et al., 2017). In the context of social media, news sharing encompasses outwardly
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 8

expressive activities such as recommending, posting, or forwarding various forms of news and

political information to the members of one’s social network (Kümpel, Karnowski, & Keyling,

2015), plus other interactions such as comments or likes which signal engagement with news.

Network analyses of social media data have consistently shown that people selectively

share attitude-affirming political information such as hyperlinked blogposts (Adamic & Glance,

2005) or tweets (Barbera, Jost, Nagler, Tucker, & Bonneau, 2015; Colleoni, Rozza, &

Arvidsson, 2014). However, such big data analyses have certain limitations. For instance, big

data analyses are often restricted to publicly available data, potentially yielding unrepresentative

samples (Garrett, 2013). Thus, it is equally important to corroborate findings from analyses of

big data with research carried out using other types of methodologies such as surveys or

experiments (Garrett, 2013). Furthermore, survey and experimental designs allow for greater

inferences about mental processes and causal steps, and have unique strengths with regard to

selective exposure research (Clay, Barber, & Shook, 2013). As such, this study uses an online

experiment embedded within a web-based behavior-tracking study of social media news use to

examine whether people are more likely to share pro-attitudinal social media news items over

counter-attitudinal news items.

Furthermore, some scholars have argued that pro-attitudinal information sharing is a

much more consistently observed phenomenon on social media platforms than selective

exposure (Shin & Thorson, 2017). People do not always fully attune themselves to pro-

attitudinal information (Garrett et al., 2013). There are times when they will deliberately attend

to counter-attitudinal political information, e.g., because they want to develop arguments against

opposing viewpoints (Knobloch-Westerwick & Kleinman, 2012). In contrast, sharing counter-

attitudinal information could create conflict or misunderstanding with one’s network. Pro-
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 9

attitudinal information sharing on social media represents an overt reflection of one’s partisan

values (Shin & Thorson, 2017). Notably, Shin and Thorson (2017) found that people are more

likely to re-tweet fact-checking messages that favor the in-party over those that favor the out-

party as an overt statement of loyalty and support for their in-party. All in all, people may be less

likely to share counter-attitudinal information than to read counter-attitudinal information.

H4: People will be more likely to share pro-attitudinal than counter-attitudinal news

stories on social media.

RQ1: Is the sharing of pro- versus counter-attitudinal news stories on social media a

stronger pattern than exposure to pro- versus counter-attitudinal news stories?

2.4 The effect of selective sharing on political participation and political polarization

Current research has mostly focused on establishing the positive relationship between

generalized political information sharing and political participation (Gil de Zúñiga, Molyneux, &

Zheng, 2014; Vaccari et al., 2015). Online information sharing increases political participation

by increasing issue knowledge and enabling people to co-organize political events (Kwak,

Williams, Wang, & Lee, 2005; F. Lee, Chen, & Chan, 2017; Valenzuela, 2013). Given that

selective sharing of attitude-affirming information is a bold, outward expression of one’s partisan

leanings (Shin & Thorson, 2017), it is likely that such selective sharing will make one’s partisan

identity even more cognitively salient than selective exposure alone. Although there is some

cross-sectional evidence that news sharing on social media may increase network heterogeneity

(i.e., contact with others with divergent opinions; Choi & Lee, 2015), it can foster relationships

with like-minded others. News sharing can also produce feelings of certainty and efficacy

(Oeldorf-Hirsch & Sundar, 2015). It is a logical inference that the sharing of pro-attitudinal

information will have stronger effects than pro-attitudinal news exposure on empowering people
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 10

to collaborate with other in-party members on events or causes benefiting their political party.

Relatedly, the sharing of pro-attitudinal information is likely to be a more consistent

predictor of opinion polarization than pro-attitudinal news consumption. As an outward display

of issue stance, the sharing of pro-attitudinal information is arguably a much more effective

catalyst of partisan beliefs and affiliation than selective exposure (Shin & Thorson, 2017). When

people engage in such partisan-based social categorization, opinion polarization occurs as they

develop extreme views about group norms toward issue positions (Hogg, 2014). Panel survey

data suggest that social media use contributes to political engagement and subsequent

polarization of attitudes (C. Lee, Shin, & Hong, 2018). This study proposes that:

H5: The sharing of pro-attitudinal news stories on social media will be positively

associated with (a) online political participation, (b) offline political participation, and (c)

opinion polarization.

RQ2: Is the sharing of pro-attitudinal news stories on social media a more consistently

positive predictor of (a) online political participation, (b) offline political participation,

and (c) opinion polarization than exposure to pro-attitudinal news stories?

By contrast, the effects of sharing counter-attitudinal social media news on political

participation and opinion polarization are less understood. As outlined above, people are more

likely to share pro-attitudinal over counter-attitudinal information (Shin & Thorson, 2017).

However, scholars have pointed out that the act of sharing counter-attitudinal political

information indicates an outward openness toward embracing, or at the very least reflecting

upon, alternative political views (Lane et al., 2019). This suggests that people who openly share

counter-attitudinal political information are willing to openly admit that they are politically

undecided, and will consequently be less likely to take concrete political action.
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 11

Furthermore, when people are unabashedly willing to consider the validity of alternative

viewpoints (Mutz, 2002), it is likely that they will not hold extreme issue positions that conform

to partisan values. In addition, it takes considerably more commitment to share counter-

attitudinal political information on social media than consume counter-attitudinal political

information (Lane et al., 2019). It is thus plausible that sharing counter-attitudinal political

information will be even more effective than consuming counter-attitudinal political information

at reducing political participation and attitude extremity. As such, we propose:

H6: The sharing of counter-attitudinal news stories on social media will be negatively

associated with (a) online political participation, (b) offline political participation, and (c)

opinion polarization.

RQ3: Is the sharing of counter-attitudinal news stories on social media a more

consistently negative predictor of (a) online political participation, (b) offline political

participation, and (c) opinion polarization than exposure to counter-attitudinal news

stories?

2.5. The moderating role of involvement

The construct of involvement has played a crucial role in explaining how message

processing affects judgments across many domains of media and communication research (e.g.,

Brown & Basil, 1995; Kwak, 1999; Slater & Rouner, 1992, 1996). Scholars have identified three

main types of involvement: value relevant, outcome relevant, and impression relevant (Cho &

Boster, 2005). Although all three types of involvement trigger attitudes related to one’s self-

concept, each type of involvement results in different persuasive outcomes (Cho & Boster,

2005). To date, few studies have examined how these specific types of involvement affect the

degree to which selective exposure and sharing behaviors predict political outcomes. This study
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 12

focuses on examining two types of involvement, value-relevant and impression-relevant

involvement, as moderators of the effects outlined in the aforementioned hypotheses.

2.5.1. Value-relevant involvement

This dimension of involvement is defined as “the psychological state that is created by

the activation of attitudes that are linked to important values” (Johnson & Eagly, 1989, p. 290).

A person’s political identity and values are integral to their self-concept (Sherif, Sherif, &

Nebergall, 1965), and will inform their judgments and political information consumption

behaviors (Perloff, 1989). When value-relevant involvement is high, a person’s range of

acceptable beliefs narrows, and they tend to reject a wide set of attitude positions (Cho & Boster,

2005; Fazio, Zanna, & Cooper, 1977). Under such conditions, these types of people will be prone

to engaging in behaviors or exhibiting attitudes that align with their values or beliefs, and

correspondingly just as likely to avoid adopting value-inconsistent behaviors and attitudes (Fazio

et al., 1977).

Given the above, we can predict that individuals with high levels of value involvement

will be especially likely to engage in informational consumption or expressive behaviors that

affirm their political values, such as selectively consuming or sharing pro-attitudinal social

media news. In addition, people with high levels of value involvement are particularly likely to

be politically active, and tend to have very extreme opinions (Perloff, 1989). It is plausible that

such pro-attitudinal political information consumption and sharing behaviors will translate into

political action and opinion polarization at high levels of value involvement. By contrast,

consuming and sharing counter-attitudinal political information are behaviors that ostensibly

conflict with one’s political values (e.g., Lane et al., 2019). People with high levels of value

involvement are more defensively-oriented and unlikely to allow counter-attitudinal information


EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 13

to shape their political opinions or decisions to partake in political activities. As such, we predict

that the negative effects of counter-attitudinal news exposure and sharing on political

participation and opinion polarization will be weakest when value involvement is high.

H7: People with higher levels of value involvement will be more likely to (a) spend time

reading and (b) share pro-attitudinal versus counter-attitudinal news stories on social

media.

H8: People with higher levels of value involvement will show stronger positive effects of

pro-attitudinal news exposure on (a) online political participation, (b) offline political

participation, and (c) opinion polarization.

H9: People with higher levels of value involvement will show weaker negative effects of

counter-attitudinal news exposure on (a) online political participation, (b) offline political

participation, and (c) opinion polarization.

H10: People with higher levels of value involvement will show stronger positive effects

of pro-attitudinal news sharing on (a) online political participation, (b) offline political

participation, and (c) opinion polarization.

H11: People with higher levels of value involvement will show weaker negative effects

of counter-attitudinal news sharing on (a) online political participation, (b) offline

political participation, and (c) opinion polarization.

2.5.1. Impression-driven involvement

According to Johnson and Eagly (1989), impression involvement is a form of attitudinal

involvement that gauges the degree to which individuals inherently care about the social

ramifications of their opinions. Unlike value involvement, which focuses on an individual’s

deep-seated values or issue attitudes, impression-driven involvement centers on an individual’s


EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 14

concern about how others perceive them (Cho & Boster, 2005). Thus, people who exhibit high

levels of impression involvement tend to deliberately engage in public displays of behavior that

conform to expectations of important referent groups (Leippe & Elkin, 1987).

Sharing pro-attitudinal social media news can be construed as a public demonstration of

in-party solidarity in front of an imagined audience (Liu, Rui, & Cui, 2017; Shin & Thorson,

2017). Furthermore, participatory political activities can be regarded as outward indications of

the degree to which a person embodies the values of their political affiliation, or seeks greater

affiliation (Huddy, Mason, & Aarøe, 2015). With this in mind, it is plausible that those with high

levels of impression involvement will be most likely to share pro-attitudinal over counter-

attitudinal social media news, and in turn become more politically active after selectively sharing

pro-attitudinal news as explicit signs that they conform to in-party norms. By contrast, sharing

counter-attitudinal political news is antithetical to partisan group norms (Lane et al., 2019).

Consequently, individuals who exhibit high levels of impression involvement might be reluctant

to openly share counter-attitudinal political news and allow such news items to undermine their

desire to participate in political activities. When they do share such content and make a semi-

public commitment to counter-attitudinal information, it is especially likely to shift their

opinions and dampen their participation intentions. As such, the negative effects of counter-

attitudinal social media news exposure on online and offline political participation is likely to be

strongest among those with high levels of impression involvement.

Additionally, holding polarized issue opinions is arguably a more inward manifestation of

one’s political values than political participation (Huddy et al., 2015). Regardless of opinion

extremity, people can choose to keep their opinions to themselves without letting others know

(Cho & Boster, 2005). However, shared opinions are likely to lead to commitment and
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 15

reinforcement (Lane et al., 2019). Thus, the moderating role of impression involvement on the

relationship between attitudinal stance and opinion polarization via selective sharing is different

from that of value involvement. Those who desire to make a positive impression on their social

network (Smith et al., 2007) should shift their beliefs and behaviors in the direction of the news

they have consumed, and especially the content they have shared. The following hypotheses are

proposed:

H12: People with higher levels of impression involvement will be more likely to (a)

spend time reading and (b) share pro-attitudinal versus counter-attitudinal news stories on

social media.

H13: People with higher levels of impression involvement will show stronger positive

effects of pro-attitudinal news exposure on (a) online political participation, (b) offline

political participation, and (c) opinion polarization.

H14: People with higher levels of impression involvement will show stronger negative

effects of counter-attitudinal news exposure on (a) online political participation, (b)

offline political participation, and (c) opinion polarization.

H15: People with higher levels of impression involvement will show stronger positive

effects of pro-attitudinal news sharing on (a) online political participation, (b) offline

political participation, and (c) opinion polarization.

H16: People with higher levels of impression involvement will show stronger negative

effects of counter-attitudinal news sharing on (a) online political participation, (b) offline

political participation, and (c) opinion polarization.

The study’s hypotheses and research questions are visualized in Figure 1, which illustrates the

within-subjects differences in pro- and counter-attitudinal exposure and sharing with brackets on
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 16

the left-hand side of the model, the between-subjects effects of exposure and sharing on the

dependent variables with paths on the right-hand side of the model, and the moderating effects of

value and impression involvement on main effects indicated with the moderator variables at the

bottom of the figure.


EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 17

Figure 1. Conceptual model with hypotheses and research questions. Brackets represent within-

subjects effects (tested with paired-sample t-tests, by topic) and paths to dependent variables

represent between-subjects effects (tested with a regression model for each DV, by topic; results

appear in Tables 1 and 2). Value and impression involvement were tested with subsequent

ANCOVAs and regression models. H7a tests moderation of H1, H7b tests moderation of H4,

H8a tests moderation of H2a, H8b tests moderation of H2b, and so on.
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 18

3. Method

An online experiment was conducted, using a 3 (issue: Ukraine vs. refugees vs. equal

pay) x 2 (stance: pro- vs. counter-attitudinal) within-subjects manipulation of topic and stance. A

free-choice paradigm (Hastall & Knobloch-Westerwick, 2013) allowed participants to freely

select articles to read and subsequently express intentions to share. The study data, syntax for

analysis, and questionnaire are available at: https://osf.io/p9uzw/.

3.1. Participants

A convenience sample of adult Dutch Facebook users was recruited via invitations

distributed through university students’ social media networks and flyers in campus and public

settings. As an incentive, participants were entered into a drawing for €5 giftcards. A total of 399

individuals completed the questionnaire. Three cases were removed because of invalid self-

reported age, another eight were removed because elapsed time to submit the survey was +/-3 SD

(i.e., > 5 hrs), and a further four were removed because total stimuli browsing time was +/-3 SD

(i.e., > 1 hr). This left a final sample for hypothesis testing of N = 384. This sample was 62.5%

women, and ranged in age from 18 to 68 (M = 29.72, SD = 10.59). The majority (96.6%) had

Dutch nationality and were born in the Netherlands (93.5%). The majority (63.4%) had earned a

college degree (HBO or WO in the Dutch system).

3.2. Procedure

Data were collected from 21 March to 5 April 2016, prior to a referendum on the EU’s

relationship with Ukraine. After completing baseline attitudes and distractors, participants were

presented with an overview page of news articles, from which they could select one or more

articles to read. Participants reported sharing intentions, post-exposure attitudes, participation

intentions, involvement, and other variables. In general, participants reported moderate attitudes
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 19

toward the referendum, polarized attitudes toward refugees, and positive attitudes toward pay

equality (see details in Section 3.4). Neutral individuals on the three topics were omitted from

analysis.

3.3. Stimuli

Eight news articles were adapted from Dutch newspaper stories. Six reflected opposing

stances on the three target issues of (a) affirming the EU-Ukraine association in a referendum,

(b) accepting more migrant refugees to the Netherlands, and (c) ensuring pay equality for men

and women. The pro- or anti- stances of the articles was confirmed with a pre-test in which 20

Dutch university students rated headlines for their stance on the issue. The full articles were

rewritten and edited for length and to match the position and tone of their headlines. Two

distractor stories were also included, on soft news topics (single parenthood and monogamy).

Each article preview consisted of a headline and lead (word count: M = 32.75, SD =

3.65), a byline with name and timestamp (e.g., “N. Meyer, yesterday 11:03”). These eight

previews were presented (in a randomized order) in two columns and four rows on an overview

page, from which participants could select articles for further reading (they were instructed to

choose at least one). If a particular article was selected for reading, they were directed to a full

page that presented the entire article (word count: M = 651.5, SD = 43.83). When multiple

articles were selected, the presentation order of articles was varied. Time spent reading

individual articles was unobtrusively recorded by the questionnaire platform (Hastall &

Knobloch-Westerwick, 2013).

To enhance ecological validity, each article (and article preview) was assigned a number

of Facebook likes and reactions, and each article was assigned a number of Facebook comments

that appeared after the articles. The valence of reactions and comments was intended as a
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 20

manipulation of opinion climate, but did not yield effects and is not analyzed further.

3.4. Measures

3.4.1. T1 attitudes

Attitudes were measured with 101-point thermometers ranging from 0 = completely

against to 100 = completely in favor. Toward the Ukraine referendum, “the association

agreement between Ukraine and the EU,” M = 48.39, SD = 25.41; 5.7% did not report an

attitude, 30.2% reported a negative attitude, 31.8% reported a neutral attitude at the midpoint of

50, and 32.3% reported a positive attitude. Toward refugees, “allowing more migrants in the

Netherlands,” M = 64.14, SD = 26.45; 4.4% did not report an attitude, 22.4% reported a negative

attitude, 10.4% reported a neutral attitude at the midpoint, and 62.8% reported a positive attitude.

Toward equal pay, “pay equality between men and women,” M = 83.07, SD = 31.39; 4.2% did

not report an attitude, 14.1% reported a negative attitude, 3.9% reported a neutral attitude at the

midpoint, and 77.9% reported a positive attitude. In addition to these scalar attitudes, scores were

trichotomized for the purpose of classifying exposure and sharing as pro-attitudinal or counter-

attitudinal (excluding neutrals by topic).

3.4.2. Selective exposure

Participants selected an average of 2.85 (SD = 1.53) of the eight available articles: six

political articles about the three target issues, plus two lifestyle news distractors. They spent M =

160.16 seconds, SD = 310.47, reading political articles and M = 45.58 seconds, SD = 77.45,

reading distractor articles. The time spent on political articles, by issue, and by stance, was

recoded to reflect whether the stance of the article was pro-attitudinal or counter-attitudinal for

the individual participant (based on their trichotomized attitudes). This procedure yielded

measures of time spent reading pro-attitudinal and counter-attitudinal articles for Ukraine,
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 21

refugees, and pay equality.

3.4.3. News sharing intentions

After the browsing opportunity, participants were presented with the headline for each

article, one at a time, and asked three questions: “How likely is it that you would ‘like’ this

article on Facebook?” “How is it likely that you would ‘share’ this article on your timeline on

Facebook?” and “How is it likely that you would post a comment under this article on

Facebook?” Participants indicated their likelihood, from 1 = not likely to 7 = very likely. These

mean scores for these three items—liking, sharing, and commenting—were taken as an index of

sharing intentions (cf. Alhabash, Almutairi, Lou, & Kim, 2019), with α’s ranging from .689 to

.874 for all six political articles. Sharing intentions were variable but relatively low, with a grand

mean across articles of 1.61 (SE = .06). The least shared article type was counter-attitudinal

Ukraine (M = 1.31, SD = 0.75) and the most shared article type was pro-attitudinal refugees (M =

2.04, SD = 1.30).

3.4.4. T2 attitudes and polarization

Attitudes were re-administered for each issue with 101-point thermometers. Polarization

was measured by subtracting t1 attitudes from t2 attitudes. This difference was multiplied by -1

for those with initially negative attitudes, so that a positive score on polarization indicated

greater extremity in the direction of the original attitude, and a negative score indicated

movement in the opposite direction (depolarization). On average, attitudes slightly depolarized

from t1 to t2 for all three issues: Ukraine, M = -6.31, SD = 15.95, refugees, M = -1.39, SD =

12.87, and equal pay, M = -14.61, SD = 27.94.

3.4.5. Offline and online political participation

Participants indicated, regarding politics in general, “how willing you are to…” engage in
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 22

different behaviors, 1= not at all willing to 7 = very willing. Three items (adapted from

Kruikemeier, van Noort, Vliegenthart, & de Vreese, 2014) measured online participation (“Sign

a political petition online,” “Send an e-mail to someone to influence him or her politics,” and

“Have a discussion about politics online”), α = .671, M = 2.83, SD = 1.31, and five items

(adapted from Shah et al., 2007) measured offline participation (“Sign a political petition,”

“Participate in a political protest,” “Volunteer for a political party/issue/protest group,” and

“Have a face-to-face discussion about politics,” and “Place a poster or sticker or wear a button

with a political message”), α = .807, M = 3.20, SD = 1.33. Online and offline participation were

strongly correlated, r = .685, p < .001.

3.4.6. Voting intention

Participants were asked “If the Ukraine referendum were held today, would you go

vote?” A majority of 53.9% said yes, 45.8% said no, and only one participant declined to answer.

3.4.7. Involvement

Items were adopted from the Cho and Boster (2005) measure of attitudinal involvement,

and worded with regard to political attitudes. Six items measured value involvement (e.g., “My

life would change if my political views were different”), α = .778, M = 3.88, SD = 1.18, and five

items measured impression involvement (“People could judge me on the basis of my political

convictions”), α = .671, M = 3.53, SD = 0.98. These two dimensions of involvement were

moderately correlated, r = .323, p < .001.

3.4.8. Other measures

Other variables included in the dataset but not included in these analyses are Facebook

use, news use, public opinion perceptions, perceived knowledge, affect, political interest,

ideology, political affiliation, group identification, willingness to self-censor, and conflict


EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 23

avoidance.

3.5. Analysis plan

Given the use of multiple topics, and the mix of within-subjects and between-subjects

effects predicted, as well as the focus on three dependent variables, the data were analyzed in a

series of tests. Each test was conducted separately for each topic, owing to different patterns of

individuals holding neutral attitudes (e.g., an individual with a pro- or anti-refugee attitude might

have a neutral attitude on pay equality).

First, the within-subjects effects of pro- versus counter-attitudinal exposure (H1) and

sharing (H4) were tested with paired-sample t-tests. Then, the effects of exposure and sharing

were modeled as predictors in regression models for the outcomes of online participation, offline

participation, and opinion polarization. These regression models test the between-subjects main

effects outlined in H2, H3, H5, and H6. Next, value involvement and impression involvement

were tested as moderators of these main effects. Within-subjects ANCOVA tested whether value

(H7) and impression (H12) involvement moderated pro-attitudinal versus counter-attitudinal (a)

exposure and (b) sharing. Interaction effects were probed with the MEMORE macro (Montoya &

Hayes, 2017). Then, to test the two involvement variables as moderators of the effects of

exposure and sharing on the dependent variables (H8-11 and H13-16), regression models

introduced involvement as moderating variables. In these between-subjects analyses, the

PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2018) was used to probe interactions with the Johnson-Neyman

technique. Finally, Research Questions 1, 2, and 3 are addressed by comparing the relative

strength of the main effects of exposure versus sharing.

4. Results

4.1. Within-Subjects main effects


EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 24

To test the basic confirmation bias hypotheses of H1 and H4, paired-sample t-tests

compared pro-attitudinal and counter-attitudinal exposure and sharing for each topic. Topics

were analyzed separately owing to the exclusion of attitude-neutrals. For selective exposure,

there was a significant effect for the refugee topic alone, t(326) = 3.52, p < .001, d = .195, where

more seconds were spent reading pro-attitudinal (M = 33.89, SD = 65.24) than counter-

attitudinal (M = 16.75, SD = 63.99) articles. No effect was evident for exposure to the

referendum (p = .730, d = .022) or pay equality (p = .584, d = .041) topics. H1 was partially

supported. For selective sharing, all three topics showed pro-attitudinal effects: the referendum,

t(231) = 4.26, p < .001, d = .280, refugees, t(310) = 8.92, p < .001, d = .506, and pay equality,

t(337) = 3.48, p < .001, d = .189. H4 was fully supported, and RQ1 was answered: selective

sharing was stronger and more consistently observed than selective exposure.
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 25

Table 1

Relationships of Selective Exposure and Sharing with Online and Offline Participation

UA Ref Refugees Equal Pay UA Ref Refugees Equal Pay


→Online →Online →Online →Offline →Offline →Offline
β β β β β β
Model 1: Main effects ∆R2=.198 ∆R2=.268 ∆R2=.242 ∆R2=.315 ∆R2=.384 ∆R2=.349
Pro-Att Exposure .016 -.028 .005 .007 .009 .017
Counter-Att Exposure .054 -.012 .029 .042 -.042 .078
Pro-Att Sharing .232** .286*** .065 .204** .261*** .125*
Counter-Att Sharing .051 .085 .259*** -.011 -.049 .084
Value Involvement .298*** .314*** .348*** .461*** .481*** .506***
Impression Involvement .038 .066 .062 .076 .064 .063
Model 2: Value ∆R =.050 ∆R =.012 ∆R =.021 ∆R =.027 ∆R =.007 ∆R2=.016
2 2 2 2 2

Involvement Interactions
Value*Pro Exposure .649* .121 .344 .487 .238 .413*
Value*Counter Expo. .417 -.337 .450 .163 -.143 -.048
Value*Pro Sharing -.332 -.203 .040 -.262 .013 -.167
Value*Counter Sharing -.112 .332 .313 -.390 .247 .288
Model 3: Impression ∆R2=.027 ∆R2=.002 ∆R2=.005 ∆R2=.018 ∆R2=.005 ∆R2=.004
Involvement Interactions
Impress*Pro Exposure .095 -.130 .059 .135 -.196 .037
Impress*Counter Expo. .429 -.039 .270 .656* .176 -.269
Impress*Pro Sharing -.492 .179 .260 -.269 -.001 .312
Impress*Counter Shar. .964* -.048 -.019 .222 -.118 -.286
N= 231 310 336 231 310 336
Note. Standardized coefficients. UA Ref = Ukraine-EU association referendum. *p < .05, **p <
.01, ***p < .001. Model 2 and Model 3 each build on the main effects in Model 1; Model 2
tested all value-involvement interactions simultaneously, and Model 3 tested all impression-
involvement interactions simultaneously.
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 26

4.2. Between-Subjects main effects

Next, regression models for each topic (pro-attitudinal and counter-attitudinal exposure

and sharing as predictors) tested effects on online and offline participation as well as opinion

polarization, controlling for value and impression involvement (main effect models at the top of

Tables 1 and 2). Intention to share the pro-attitudinal article was associated with more online

participation intentions and offline participation intentions for the Ukraine referendum and

refugee topics (Table 1, columns 1, 2, 4 and 5). Sharing pro-attitudinal articles about equal pay

was positively associated with offline participation (Table 1, column 6). For the refugee and pay

equality topics, pro-attitudinal sharing was positively linked to polarization, and attitude-

inconsistent sharing was linked to depolarization (Table 2, columns 2 and 3).


EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 27

Table 2

Effects of Selective Exposure and Sharing on Opinion Polarization

UA Ref Refugees Equal Pay


β β β
2 2 2
Model 1: Main effects ∆R = .025 ∆R = .121 ∆R = .125
Pro-Att Exposure -.008 .067 -.012
Counter-Att Exposure -.119 .010 .025
Pro-Att Sharing .074 .128* .279***
Counter-Att Sharing -.116 -.328*** -.378***
Value Involvement .023 -.008 -.077
Impression Involvement -.022 .109 -.117*
Model 2: Value Involvement ∆R2 = .036 2
∆R = .014 ∆R2 = .021
Interactions
Value*Pro Exposure -.507 .002 -.179
Value*Counter Exposure .196 .270 -.185
Value*Pro Sharing .149 .202 .190
Value*Counter Sharing -1.093* -.407 -.556*
Model 3: Impression ∆R2 = .015 ∆R2 = .010 ∆R2 = .009
Involvement Interactions
Impress*Pro Exposure .375 -.160 .130
Impress*Counter Exposure -.254 .330 -.290
Impress*Pro Sharing -.371 -.269 .504
Impress*Counter Sharing .653 -.113 -.379
N= 229 310 333
Note. Standardized coefficients. UA Ref = Ukraine-EU association referendum. *p < .05, **p <
.01, ***p < .001. Model 2 and Model 3 each build on the main effects in Model 1; Model 2
tested all value-involvement interactions simultaneously, and Model 3 tested all impression-
involvement interactions simultaneously.
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 28

From these main effect findings outlined above, H5a-c were largely supported: the

sharing of pro-attitudinal social media news stories was positively associated with offline and

online political participation, as well as opinion polarization. By contrast, H2a-c, which predicted

positive effects of pro-attitudinal information exposure on political participation and

polarization, were not supported at all. These discrepancies in findings answer RQ2, which

questioned whether sharing of pro-attitudinal social media news stories will be a more

consistently positive predictor of political participation and polarization than exposure to pro-

attitudinal social media news stories, which was indeed the case. H3a-c, which specified

relationships between the amount of time spent reading counter-attitudinal news stories and both

political participation and polarization, proved untenable. However, H6c, which hypothesized

that sharing counter-attitudinal social media news stories will be negatively associated with

opinion polarization was supported for the equal pay and refugee issues. Based on this finding,

RQ3c was answered, such that the sharing of counter-attitudinal news stories was a stronger

predictor of issue depolarization than exposure to counter-attitudinal news stories (except in the

case of the Ukraine issue).

In addition, sharing counter-attitudinal articles about pay equality was positively linked to

online participation (Table 1, column 3). Contrary to H6a, this finding provides some evidence

that counter-attitudinal information sharing was positively rather than negatively associated with

online political participation. This result also addresses RQ3a: Counter-attitudinal sharing was

not a strong negative predictor (in the case of pay equality was even a positive predictor) relative

to counter-attitudinal exposure. Furthermore, there was no association between counter-

attitudinal sharing and offline political participation across all three issues, lending no support to

H6b. This also addresses RQ3b regarding the relative contribution of exposure and sharing.
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 29

An additional regression model (not in table) tested whether exposure to and sharing of

Ukraine articles were associated with the intention to vote in the referendum (an outcome

specific to this topic). Only pro-attitudinal sharing was a (positive) predictor, β = .174, p = .035.

4.3. Within-Subjects interaction effects

Within-subjects ANCOVA with value and impression involvement as covariates

identified moderating effects of value involvement on selective (pro- vs. counter-attitudinal)

sharing for stories on the referendum, F(1, 229) = 7.74, p = .006, ηp2 = .033, refugees, F(1, 308)

= 12.33, p < .001, ηp2 = .038, and equal pay, F(1, 334) = 3.91, p = .049, ηp2 = .012. Probing these

interactions with Johnson-Neyman technique via the MEMORE macro (Montoya & Hayes,

2017) found that moderate and high levels of value involvement (≥ 3.16 for Ukraine, ≥ 2.25 for

refugees, and ≥ 3.16 for pay equality, where M = 3.88) were linked to more pro-attitudinal over

counter-attitudinal message sharing, whereas those low on value involvement show no difference

in sharing pro-attitudinal versus counter-attitudinal articles. H7b was supported. Value

involvement fell short of moderating selective exposure to referendum (p = .074), refugee (p =

.080), and equal pay (p = .169) articles. H7a was not supported.

Impression involvement did not moderate selective sharing, (ps > .25), lending no

support to H12b. With regard to H12a, neither did impression involvement moderate the bias in

pro-attitudinal versus counter-attitudinal exposure (ps > .35).

4.4. Between-Subjects interaction effects

Then, regression models were extended to test value involvement and impression

involvement as moderators. One set of models tested value involvement as a simultaneous

moderator of all focal variables (pro-attitudinal exposure, counter-attitudinal exposure, pro-

attitudinal sharing, and counter-attitudinal sharing) on the DVs of interest (Model 2 in Tables 1
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 30

and 2). Alternatively, impression involvement was tested as a simultaneous moderator of focal

variables in Model 3 (bottom of Tables 1 and 2). A number of significant interactions emerged,

as indicated in the Tables. The PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2018) was used to probe and interpret

these interactions with the Johnson-Neyman method.

There was partial support for H8a-b which hypothesized that pro-attitudinal information

exposure will have positive effects on offline and online participation at high levels of value

involvement. People high on value involvement (the 13.79% at or above a score of 5.306) had a

positive effect of Ukraine pro-attitudinal exposure on online participation. Also, people high on

value involvement (the 14.54% at or above a score of 5.012) had a positive effect of equal pay

pro-attitudinal exposure on offline participation. Although not hypothesized, at low levels of

value involvement (the 1.72% at or below a score of 1.824) there was a negative effect of

Ukraine pro-attitudinal exposure on online participation. Similarly, people low on value

involvement (the 7.42% at or below a score of 2.003) had a negative effect of equal pay pro-

attitudinal exposure on offline participation.

People high on value involvement (the 39.57% at or above a score of 4.058) had a

negative effect of Ukraine counter-attitudinal sharing on their polarization. Similarly, people

with moderate to high on value involvement (the 89.52% at or above a score of 2.486) had a

negative effect of counter-attitudinal sharing on their polarization for the issue of equal pay,

where higher involvement led to stronger depolarization effects. In all, these findings run counter

to H11c, which hypothesized that the negative effect of counter-attitudinal information sharing

on opinion polarization is weakest among individuals with high levels of value involvement.

With regard to H14b, the relationship between counter-attitudinal information exposure

on offline participation depended on impression involvement. However, the effect was in an


EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 31

unexpected direction. Specifically, people high on impression involvement (the 21.12% at or

above a score of 4.374) had a positive effect of Ukraine counter-attitudinal exposure on offline

participation. Next, people low on impression involvement (the 6.90% at or below a score of

2.062) had a negative effect of Ukraine counter-attitudinal sharing on online participation, while

people high on impression involvement (the 21.12% at or above a score of 4.328) had a positive

effect of Ukraine counter-attitudinal sharing on online participation. This finding ran counter to

H16a, which hypothesized that the negative effects of counter-attitudinal information sharing on

online political participation will be stronger among people high on impression involvement.

In addition, value and impression involvement did not moderate any effects of exposure

or sharing on voting intentions in the Ukraine-EU referendum.

None of the other moderation effect hypotheses or research questions were supported

with statistically significant results. With regard to H8c, H9c, and H10c, which hypothesized that

the respective effects of information exposure and pro-attitudinal information sharing on opinion

polarization would depend on value involvement, there was no support. Furthermore, regarding

H13c, H14c, H15c, and H16c, impression involvement did not moderate the effects of either

information sharing or exposure on opinion polarization.

Also, the effects of pro-attitudinal information sharing on the two types of political

participation did not depend on value involvement. Additionally, moderation analyses showed

that the effects of counter-attitudinal exposure and sharing on both types of political participation

did not depend on value involvement. H9a-b, H10a-b, and H11a-b received no support. With

respect to H13a-b, the relationship between pro-attitudinal information exposure and both types

of political participation also did not depend on impression involvement. The relationship

between counter-attitudinal information exposure and online political participation also did not
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 32

depend on impression involvement (H14a). The effects of pro-attitudinal information sharing on

both types of political participation did not depend on impression involvement, lending no

support to H15a-b. Finally, the effects of counter-attitudinal information sharing on offline

political participation did not depend on impression involvement, lending no support to H16b.

In short, H1 was supported (partial, by topic), H4 was supported, H5 was supported, H6c

was supported (partial, by topic), H7b was supported, and H8a-b was supported (partial, by

topic). Participants had confirmation bias in their exposure and sharing. Sharing stories affected

polarization; pro-attitudinal sharing also related to participation. Value involvement heightened

pro-attitudinal sharing as well as the influence of pro-attitudinal exposure on participation.

5. Discussion

In sum, this study’s findings are multi-faceted and complex. In terms of main effects,

results provide partial support for the hypothesis that people will spend more time reading pro-

attitudinal than counter-attitudinal social media news articles. The presence of a confirmation

bias was contingent on the issue: the refugee issue was the only topic to produce more pro-

attitudinal than counter-attitudinal exposure, and it also produced greater selective sharing than

the other two topics. The topic of refugees was the most polarized (at baseline) of the issues

examined in this study, which likely contributes to this difference with the other two issues.

Furthermore, pro- and counter-attitudinal information exposure did not have any main effects on

opinion polarization and political participation.

In contrast, findings showed support for the hypothesis that people will be more likely to

share pro-attitudinal over counter-attitudinal news articles on social media. As hypothesized,

pro-attitudinal information sharing was positively associated with online and offline political

participation for issues pertaining to the Ukrainian referendum and refugees. However, there was
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 33

a positive association between counter-attitudinal information sharing on online participation for

the issue of equal pay. Consistent with predictions, the sharing of pro-attitudinal news articles

about refugees and equal pay had positive effects on opinion polarization. Also, the sharing of

counter-attitudinal news articles about refugees and equal pay were negatively associated with

opinion polarization.

Taken together, the mostly significant positive associations between pro-attitudinal

information sharing and political participation across issue types provide fairly strong support for

the argument that pro-attitudinal information sharing is a much stronger sign of one’s

commitment to their political views than pro-attitudinal partisan-based information exposure (cf.

Shin & Thorson, 2017). When people overtly express their identification with specific political

values or opinions in the form of pro-attitudinal information sharing, such information sharing is

likely to translate into political action. However, it is important to note that the link between

selectivity and participation is essentially cross-sectional, as participation was only measured

post-test. In contrast, the effects on attitudinal polarization may be characterized as causal, as

they control for baseline attitudes. The effects of sharing on polarization are absent for the

Ukraine referendum, which was the least salient and least contentious issue of the three. Given

the confusion or apathy among the Dutch electorate regarding the vote (cf. Van der Brug, van der

Meer, & van der Pas, 2018), sharing information about it on social media would involve less of

an identity-defining statement or catalyst for change.

With regard to interaction effects, there was some modest evidence that the positive

association between pro-attitudinal information exposure and political participation depends on

value involvement. Specifically, a positive effect of pro-attitudinal information exposure on both

online and offline political participation tended to be strongest among those with high levels of
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 34

value involvement for the issues of Ukraine referendum and equal pay. This set of findings

provides support for the assumption that people tend to be more politically defensive at high

levels of value involvement. When people are politically closed-minded, they are likely to

engage in pro-attitudinal information consumption behaviors that translate into participatory

action. However, these findings only provide one part of the overall picture: Although not

hypothesized, there were also negative associations between pro-attitudinal news exposure and

political participation when value involvement levels were low for the issues of the Ukraine

referendum and equal pay. Perhaps, at low levels of value involvement, people are more

amenable to allowing opposing political viewpoints to influence their political decisions. And

when people are willing to consider alternative political views, they could experience attitude

ambivalence and develop weakened political attitudes that result in lower levels of political

participation. Alternatively, low-involvement individuals who read pro-attitudinal content may

believe that an active majority agrees with them and that their own political participation is not

needed because others will participate in their place.

In addition, the effects of counter-attitudinal news sharing on opinion polarization

depended on value involvement, for the issues of Ukrainian referendum and equal pay. However,

these findings ran counter to the study’s hypotheses. Specifically, the negative influence of

counter-attitudinal news sharing on opinion polarization was strongest when value involvement

in the issue of the Ukrainian referendum was high. Likewise, the negative effect of counter-

attitudinal news sharing on opinion polarization regarding equal pay was strongest when value

involvement was high. This finding is surprising because people tend to be closed to alternate

viewpoints at high levels of value involvement. It would appear, however, that those high on

value involvement were especially prone to persuasion (i.e., depolarization) if they engaged
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 35

with—and shared—news they disagreed with.

Generally speaking, value-relevant involvement was a more consistent moderator of the

relationships that information exposure and sharing have with political participation and

polarization than is impression-driven involvement. Nevertheless, there was a negative

association between counter-attitudinal news sharing on online participation among those with

low levels of impression involvement on the issue of the Ukrainian referendum. This finding

provides qualified evidence for the idea that people with low levels of impression involvement

are unlikely to be strongly committed toward publicly performing actions that align with their

partisan values. When impression involvement levels are low, people seem alright with explicitly

letting their social media followers know that they are considering alternative political stances.

Also, there was a positive association between counter-attitudinal news sharing and online

participation among those with high levels of impression involvement on the referendum issue.

Perhaps, people with high levels of impression involvement openly share counter-attitudinal

news with the goal of openly deriding such information. Such denigration of counter-attitudinal

news in front of an imagined social media audience allows them to explicitly prove that they are

committed to their partisan values, strengthening their resolve to take political action.

5.1. Implications

In terms of theoretical contributions, this study builds upon existing research on political

information consumption and self-expression patterns on social media in the following ways.

First, this study parses out the respective effects of selective exposure and selective sharing on

political outcomes. We demonstrate that people are much more likely to engage in selective

sharing than selective exposure on social media and that selective sharing on social media is a

much more robust predictor of political outcomes than selective exposure. This underscores the
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 36

importance of delineating between exposure and sharing when examining how selectivity in

information consumption and political self-expression influence political outcomes. Theories of

selective exposure such as the SESAM (Knobloch-Westerwick, 2015) would therefore benefit

from distinguishing between exposure, sharing, and other modes of content consumption and

engagement. Second, scholars have stressed that patterns of selectivity in information

consumption and self-expression on social media can have serious political consequences (Beam

et al., 2018; Lane et al., 2019). This study is thus one of few that examines the effects of both

selective exposure and sharing on different types of political outcomes, specifically participation

and opinion polarization. Our consistent result that sharing is more consequential than exposure

is consistent with theories of public commitment such as identity shift (Lane et al., 2019). People

might harbor polarized issue attitudes internally, but such attitudes are not synonymous with

explicit political action (e.g., Miller & Conover, 2015). For instance, our study shows that pro-

attitudinal information sharing was positively associated with political participation whereas

counter-attitudinal sharing was negatively associated with polarized issue attitudes. This pattern

of findings clearly illustrates that political participation and opinion polarization are conceptually

distinct outcomes. More theoretical development is needed to articulate different processes for

different outcomes in online political communication.

Third, it seems intuitively obvious that highly involved individuals will have very

selective political information consumption and expressive behaviors (Cho & Boster, 2005). Yet

few studies on political social media use have actually tested involvement as a moderator of

selectivity and its effects. Our study addressed this research gap by examining whether effects of

selective sharing and exposure on political outcomes depended on involvement levels. Further

theoretical refinement could articulate conditions under which involvement is consequential, and
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 37

future research should include accuracy involvement (Hart et al., 2009; Johnson & Eagly, 1989).

Fourth, many experiments examining online-based selective exposure (for reviews see

Clay et al., 2013 and Knobloch-Westerwick, 2015) have been conducted in the context of news

websites. However, little experimental work has employed social media contexts (but see Kaiser,

Keller, & Kleinen-von Königslöw, in press; Mothes & Ohme, 2019), even though large

majorities of individuals have become reliant on Facebook and other social media for news

rather than online news sites (Bakshy et al., 2015; Settle, 2018). As such, this study improves

upon previous study designs by using a free choice paradigm to examine partisan selective

exposure and sharing in the context of a Facebook news feed.

This study’s findings have practical implications for harnessing social media platforms

for political engagement. Given that this study’s findings largely show that pro-attitudinal news

sharing is positively associated with political participation, campaign practitioners and

politicians can encourage their voter base to share political messages that favor their in-party

through their online social networks. Such selective sharing not only helps to keep other people

politically informed, but also makes people themselves more likely to take tangible political

action. Furthermore, messages tailored to match peoples’ partisan leanings and levels of value

involvement can be disseminated through online social networks to foster political engagement.

Based on this study’s findings, campaign planners can identify voters from their own camp with

high levels of value involvement on social media and increase political participation levels

among these individuals by encouraging them to share pro-attitudinal news stories. Furthermore,

research has shown that Facebook news use can lead to depolarization (Beam et al., 2018).

Similarly, our study’s findings show that sharing counter-attitudinal political information in a

Facebook context leads to less polarized issue attitudes, regardless of value involvement levels.
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 38

With this in mind, social media platforms can devise strategies to promote counter-attitudinal

information sharing, e.g., by designing advertisements to encourage people from all types of

value involvement levels to like, share, or comment on counter-attitudinal (or perhaps balanced,

cf. Brenes Peralta et al., 2017) news. Ultimately, such counter-attitudinal news sharing on

Facebook is likely to be beneficial to democracy by causing even people with higher levels of

value involvement to hold less extreme attitudes and embrace alternative issue viewpoints.

5.2. Limitations and future research

In terms of study limitations, this study did not measure political participation levels prior

to stimuli exposure. This prevents us from making causal claims about how selective exposure

and selective sharing affect political participation. Nevertheless, this study still provides

consistent evidence for the association between selective sharing and political participation.

Furthermore, this study operationalized selective sharing by using items gauging the likelihood

of commenting on the news article or “liking” the news article. On the one hand, these items can

be regarded as generalized indicators of expressive social media political use (e.g., Macafee &

De Simone, 2012). Future research can explore differences in the affordances of social media

sharing (e.g., liking vs. sharing). Moreover, measuring actual sharing (i.e., participants’ sharing

behavior in their own networks rather than sharing intentions in a questionnaire), perhaps in an

unobtrusive manner, can address limitations associated with self-reported intentions. Also, this

study did not examine mechanisms related to confirmation bias such as cognitive dissonance or

motivated reasoning. Future research examining partisan selectivity on social media should

incorporate such mechanisms into their study design, to better explain the links between

selectivity and its effects on political outcomes. A further limitation is the focus on three specific

topics (although varied in salience and baseline polarization) and variation that might exist in
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 39

stimuli (e.g., in argument strength). Future work should continue to include more variation in

topics and message design. Nevertheless, this study provides strong evidence of selective

information consumption and self-expression behaviors in Netherlands, a country with a multi-

party system, and makes a particularly unique contribution by juxtaposing exposure and sharing.

With regard to directions for research, future studies can examine how social media cues

such as the number of “likes” or “reactions” moderate the effects of selective exposure and

selective sharing on political outcomes. Furthermore, traditional public opinion theories such as

the spiral of silence posit that people will assess opinion congruence by paying attention to cues

in their social environments. If people perceive their opinions to be in the minority, they will

self-censor by lapsing into silence on socio-political issues (Noelle-Neumann, 1974). It is

important to examine whether the tenets of such theories receive support in the context of online

social environments (Liu et al., 2017). Future studies can thus examine the respective effects of

selective exposure and selective sharing on other outcome variables such as political efficacy or

specific opinion expressions in social media. In addition, cross-national comparisons can help to

validate study findings and examine differences across political systems and cultures. Future

research should examine how the present predictions and findings will fare in other countries.

5.3. Conclusion

In sum, this study builds upon existing research on selective exposure and sharing in the

following ways: First, this study uses a free-choice experimental design to provide much stronger

evidence for the effects of information stance on selective sharing than on selective exposure.

Second, selective sharing is a much more consistent predictor of political outcomes than

selective exposure. Third, the effects of selective sharing and exposure on political outcomes

depend more on value involvement than impression involvement. Fourth, issues matter. By using
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 40

a variety of topics that varied in their preexisting salience and polarization, we introduced

variation that may explain the differences seen in confirmation bias and its effects. Future

research should even more systematically vary issue characteristics to test how they shape

selectivity effects. As social media platforms continue to play an important role in the political

process, more research needs to be conducted to understand how individual level predispositions

and socio-environmental factors can exacerbate or attenuate the effects of partisan selective

exposure and sharing behaviors on key political outcomes.


EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 41

References

Adamic, L. A., & Glance, N. (2005). The political blogosphere and the 2004 US election:

Divided they blog. In J. Adibi, M. Grobelnik, D. Mladenic, & P. Pantel (Eds.), LinkKKD

’05: Proceedings of the 3rd International Workshop on Link Discovery (pp. 36-43). New

York, NY, USA: ACM Press. doi:10.1145/1134271.1134277

Alhabash, S., Almutairi, N., Lou, C., & Kim, W. (2019). Pathways to virality:

Psychophysiological responses preceding likes, shares, comments, and status updates on

Facebook. Media Psychology, 22, 196-216. doi:10.1080/15213269.2017.1416296

Barbera, P., Jost, J. T., Nagler, J., Tucker, J. A., & Bonneau, R. (2015). Tweeting from left to

right: Is online political communication more than an echo chamber? Psychological

Science, 26, 1531-1542. doi:10.1177/0956797615594620

Bakshy, E., Messing, S., & Adamic, L. A. (2015). Exposure to ideologically diverse news and

opinion on Facebook. Science, 348, 1130-1132. doi:10.1126/science.aaa1160

Beam, M. A., Hutchens, M. J., & Hmielowski, J. D. (2016). Clicking vs. sharing: The

relationships between online news behaviors and political knowledge. Computers in

Human Behavior, 59, 215-220. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.02.013

Beam, M. A., Hutchens, M. J., & Hmielowski, J. D. (2018). Facebook news and (de)polarization:

Reinforcing spirals in the 2016 US election. Information, Communication, & Society, 21,

940-958. doi:10.1080/1369118X.2018.1444783

Bode, L., Vraga, E. K., Borah, P., & Shah, D. V. (2014). A new space for political behavior:

Political social networking and its democratic consequences. Journal of Computer-

Mediated Communication, 19, 414-429. doi:10.1111/jcc4.12048

Bos, L., Kruikemeier, S., & de Vreese, C. (2016). Nation binding: How public service
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 42

broadcasting mitigates political selective exposure. PLoS One, 11(5), e0155112.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0155112

Boulianne, S. (2015). Social media use and participation: A meta-analysis of current research.

Information, Communication, & Society, 18, 524-538.

doi:10.1080/1369118X.2015.1008542

Brenes Peralta, C., Wojcieszak, M., Lelkes, Y., & de Vreese, C. (2017). Selective exposure to

balanced content and evidence type: The case of issue and non-issue publics about

climate change and health care. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 94, 833-

861. doi:10.1177/1077699016654681

Brown, W. J., & Basil, M. D. (1995). Media celebrities and public health: Responses to “Magic”

Johnson’s HIV disclosure and its impact on AIDS risk and high risk behaviors. Health

Communication, 7, 345-370. doi:10.1207/s15327027hc0704_4

Cho, H., & Boster, F. J. (2005). Development and validation of value-, outcome-, and

impression-relevant involvement scales. Communication Research, 32, 235-264.

doi:10.1177/0093650204273764

Choi, J., & Lee, J. K. (2015). Investigating the effects of news sharing and political interest on

social media network heterogeneity. Computers in Human Behavior, 44, 258-266.

doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.029

Clay, R., Barber, J. M., & Shook, N. J. (2013). Techniques for measuring selective exposure: A

critical review. Communication Methods and Measures, 7, 147-171.

doi:10.1080/19312458.2013.813925

Colleoni, E., Rozza, A., & Arvidsson, A. (2014). Echo chamber or public sphere? Predicting

political orientation and measuring political homophily in Twitter using Big Data.
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 43

Journal of Communication, 64, 317-332. doi:10.1111/jcom.12084

Coppini, D., Duncan, M. A., McLeod, D. M., Wise, D. A., Bialik, K. E., & Wu, Y. (2017). When

the whole world is watching: A motivations-based account of selective expression and

exposure. Computers in Human Behavior, 75, 766-774. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2017.04.020

Fazio, R. H., Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1977). Dissonance and self-perception: An integrative

view of each theory's proper domain of application. Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology, 13, 464-479. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(77)90031-2

Feezell, J. T. (2016). Predicting online political participation: The importance of selection bias

and selective exposure in the online setting. Political Research Quarterly, 69, 495-509.

doi:10.1177/1065912916652503

Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA, USA: Stanford University

Press.

Frey, D. (1986). Recent research on selective exposure to information. Advances in Experimental

Social Psychology, 19, 41-80. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60212-9

Garrett, R. K. (2009a). Echo chambers online?: Politically motivated selective exposure among

Internet news users. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 14, 265-285.

doi:10.1111/j.1083-6101.2009.01440.x

Garrett, R. K. (2009b). Politically motivated reinforcement seeking: Reframing the selective

exposure debate. Journal of Communication, 59, 676-699. doi:10.1111/j.1460-

2466.2009.01452.x

Garrett, R. K. (2013). Selective exposure: New methods and new directions. Communication

Methods and Measures, 7, 247-256. doi:10.1080/19312458.2013.835796

Garrett, R. K., Carnahan, D., & Lynch, E. K. (2013). A turn toward avoidance? Selective
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 44

exposure to online political information, 2004-2008. Political Behavior, 35, 113-134.

doi:10.1007/s11109-011-9185-6

Garrett, R. K., Dvir Gvirsman, S., Johnson, B. K., Tsfati, Y., Neo, R., & Dal, A. (2014).

Implications of pro- and counter-attitudinal information exposure for affective

polarization. Human Communication Research, 40, 309-332. doi:10.1111/hcre.12028

Gil de Zúñiga, H., Molyneux, L., & Zheng, P. (2014). Social media, political expression, and

political participation: Panel analyses of lagged and concurrent relationships. Journal of

Communication, 64, 612-634. doi:10.1111/jcom.12103

Hameleers, M., Bos, L., & de Vreese, C. H. (2018). Selective exposure to populist

communication: How attitudinal congruence drives the effects of populist attributions of

blame. Journal of Communication, 68, 51-74. doi:10.1093/joc/jqx001

Hart, W., Albarracín, D., Eagly, A. H., Brechan, I., Lindberg, M. J., & Merrill, L. (2009). Feeling

validated versus being correct: A meta-analysis of selective exposure to information.

Psychological Bulletin, 135, 555-588. doi:10.1037/a0015701

Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A

regression-based approach (2nd ed.). New York, NY, USA: Guilford Press.

Hogg, M. A. (2014). From uncertainty to extremism: Social categorization and identity

processes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 338-342.

doi:10.1177/0963721414540168

Hwang, H., Kim, Y., & Huh, C. U. (2014). Seeing is believing: Effects of uncivil online debate

on political participation and expectations of deliberation. Journal of Broadcasting &

Electronic Media, 58, 621-633. doi:10.1080/08838151.2014.966365

Jeong, M., Zo, H., Lee, C. H., & Ceran, Y. (2019). Feeling displeasure from online social media
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 45

postings: A study using cognitive dissonance theory. Computers in Human Behavior, 97,

231-240. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2019.02.021

Kaiser, J., Keller, T. R., & Kleinen-von Königslöw, K. (in press). Incidental news exposure on

Facebook as a social experience: The influence of recommender and media cues on news

selection. Communication Research. doi:10.1177/0093650218803529

Kim, Y. (2015). Does disagreement mitigate polarization? How selective exposure and

disagreement affect political polarization. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly,

92, 915-937. doi:10.1177/1077699015596328

Kim, Y., & Chen, H.-T. (2016). Social media and online political participation: The mediating

role of exposure to cross-cutting and like-minded perspectives. Telematics and

Informatics, 33, 320-330. doi:10.1016/j.tele.2015.08.008

Kim, Y. M. (2009). Issue publics in the new information environment: Selectivity, domain

specificity, and extremity. Communication Research, 36, 254-284.

doi:10.1177/0093650208330253

Knobloch-Westerwick, S. (2015). Choice and preference in media use: Advances in selective

exposure theory and research. New York, NY, USA: Routledge.

Knobloch-Westerwick, S., & Kleinman, S. B. (2012). Preelection selective exposure:

Confirmation bias versus informational utility. Communication Research, 39, 170-193.

doi:10.1177/0093650211400597

Kruikemeier, S., van Noort, G., Vliegenthart, R., & de Vreese, C. H. (2014). Unraveling the

effects of active and passive forms of political Internet use: Does it affect citizens’

political involvement?. New Media & Society, 16, 903-920.

doi:10.1177/1461444813495163
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 46

Kümpel, A. S., Karnowski, V., & Keyling, T. (2015). News sharing in social media: A review of

current research on news sharing users, content, and networks. Social Media + Society,

1(2), article 23. doi:10.1177/2056305115610141

Kwak, N. (1999). Revisiting the knowledge gap hypothesis: Education, motivation, and media

use. Communication Research, 26, 385-413. doi:10.1177/009365099026004002

Kwak, N., Williams, A. E., Wang, X., & Lee, H. (2005). Talking politics and engaging politics:

An examination of the interactive relationships between structural features of political

talk and discussion engagement. Communication Research, 32, 87-111.

doi:10.1177/0093650204271400

Hastall, M. R., & Knobloch-Westerwick, S. (2013). Caught in the act: Measuring selective

exposure to experimental online stimuli. Communication Methods and Measures, 7, 94-

105. doi:10.1080/19312458.2012.761190

Huddy, L., Mason, L., & Aarøe, L. (2015). Expressive partisanship: Campaign involvement,

political emotion, and partisan identity. American Political Science Review, 109, 1-17.

doi:10.1017/S0003055414000604

Johnson, B. T., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Effects of involvement on persuasion: A meta-analysis.

Psychological Bulletin, 106, 290-314. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.106.2.290

Knobloch-Westerwick, S., & Johnson, B. K. (2014). Selective exposure for better or worse: Its

mediating role for online news’ impact on political participation. Journal of Computer-

Mediated Communication, 19, 184-196. doi:10.1111/jcc4.12036

Lane, D. S., Lee, S. S., Liang, F., Kim, D. H., Shen, L., Weeks, B. E., & Kwak, N. (2019). Social

media expression and the political self. Journal of Communication, 69, 49-72.

doi:10.1093/joc/jqy064
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 47

Lee, C., Shin, & Hong, A. (2018). Does social media use really make people political polarized?

Direct and indirect effects of social media use on political polarization in South Korea.

Telematics and Informatics, 35, 245-254. doi:10.1016/j.tele.2017.11.005

Lee, F. L. F., Chen, H.-T., & Chan, M. (2017). Social media use and university students’

participation in a large-scale protest campaign: The case of Hong Kong’s Umbrella

Movement. Telematics and Informatics, 34, 457-469. doi:10.1016/j.tele.2016.08.005

Lee, J. K., Choi, J., Kim, C., & Kim, Y. (2014). Social media, network heterogeneity, and

opinion polarization. Journal of Communication, 4, 702-722. doi:10.1111/jcom.12077

Lee, S. K., Lindsey, N. J., & Kim, K. S. (2017). The effects of news consumption via social

media and news information overload on perceptions of journalistic norms and practices.

Computers in Human Behavior, 75, 254-263. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2017.05.007

Leippe, M. R., & Elkin, R. A. (1987). When motives clash: Issue involvement and response

involvement as determinants of persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 52, 269-278. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.2.269

Liao, Q. V., & Fu, W.-T. (2013). Beyond the filter bubble: Interactive effects of perceived threat

and topic involvement on selective exposure to information. In W. E. Mackay, S.

Brewster, & S. Bødker (Eds.), CHI ’13: Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on

human factors in computing systems (pp. 2359-2368). New York, NY, USA: ACM.

doi:10.1145/2470654.2481326

Liu, Y., Rui, J. R., & Cui, X. (2017). Are people willing to share their political opinions on

Facebook? Exploring roles of self-presentational concern in spiral of silence. Computers

in Human Behavior, 76, 294-302. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2017.07.029

Lord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 48

effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 37, 2098-2109. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.37.11.2098

Lu, Y., Heatherly, K. A., & Lee, J. K. (2016). Cross-cutting exposure on social networking sites:

The effects of SNS discussion disagreement on political participation. Computers in

Human Behavior, 59, 74-81. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.01.030

Macafee, T., & De Simone, J. J. (2012). Killing the bill online? Pathways to young people's

protest engagement via social media. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social

Networking, 15, 579-584. doi:10.1089/cyber.2012.0153

Matthes, J. (2012). Exposure to counterattitudinal news coverage and the timing of voting

decisions. Communication Research, 39, 147-169. doi:10.1177/0093650211402322

Miller, P. R., & Conover, P. J. (2015). Red and blue states of mind: Partisan hostility and voting

the United States. Political Research Quarterly, 68, 225-239.

doi:10.1177/1065912915577208

Montoya, A. K., & Hayes, A. F. (2017). Two condition within-participant statistical mediation

analysis: A path-analytic framework. Psychological Methods, 22, 6-27.

doi:10.1037/met0000086

Mothes, C., & Ohme, J. (2019). Partisan selective exposure in times of political and

technological upheaval: A social media field experiment. Media and Communication, 7,

42-53. doi:10.17645/mac.v7i3.2183

Mutz, D. C. (2002). The consequences of cross-cutting networks for political participation.

American Journal of Political Science, 46, 838-855. doi:10.2307/3088437

Noelle-Neumann, E. (1974). The spiral of silence: A theory of public opinion. Journal of

Communication, 24, 43-51. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1974.tb00367.x


EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 49

Oeldorf-Hirsch, A., & Sundar, S. S. (2015). Posting, commenting, and tagging: Effects of

sharing news stories on Facebook. Computers in Human Behavior, 44, 240-249.

doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.024

Pellikaan, H., de Lange, S. L., & van der Meer, T. W. G. (2018). The centre does not hold:

Coalition politics and party system change in the Netherlands, 2002-2012. Government

and Opposition, 53, 231-255. doi:10.1017/gov.2016.20

Perloff, R. M. (1989). Ego-involvement and the third person effect of televised news

coverage. Communication Research, 16, 236-262. doi:10.1177/009365089016002004

Settle, J. E. (2018). Frenemies: How social media polarizes America. Cambridge, UK:

Cambridge University Press.

Shah, D. V., Cho, J., Nah, S., Gotlieb, M. R., Hwang, H., Lee, N. J., Scholl, R. M., & McLeod,

D. M. (2007). Campaign ads, online messaging, and participation: Extending the

communication mediation model. Journal of Communication, 57, 676-703.

doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2007.00363.x

Sherif, C. W., Sherif, M., & Nebergall, R. E. (1965). Attitude and attitude change: The social

judgment-involvement approach. Philadelphia, PA, USA: W. B. Saunders.

Shin, J., & Thorson, K. (2017). Partisan selective sharing: The biased diffusion of fact-checking

messages on social media. Journal of Communication, 67, 233-255.

doi:10.1111/jcom.12284

Slater, M. D., & Rouner, D. L. (1992). Confidence in beliefs about social groups as an outcome

of message exposure and its role in belief change persistence. Communication Research,

19, 597-617. doi:10.1177/009365092019005003

Slater, M. D., & Rouner, D. L. (1996). Value-affirmative and value-protective processing of


EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 50

alcohol education messages that include statistical or anecdotal evidence. Communication

Research, 23, 210-235 doi:10.1177/009365096023002003

Smith, S. M., Fabrigar, L. R., Powell, D. M., & Estrada, M.-J. (2007). The role of information-

processing capacity on goals in attitude-congruent selective exposure effects. Personality

and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 948-960. doi:10.1177/0146167207301012

Stroud, N. J. (2008). Media use and political predispositions: Revisiting the concept of selective

exposure. Political Behavior, 30, 341-366. doi:10.1007/s11109-007-9050-9

Stroud, N. J. (2010). Polarization and partisan selective exposure. Journal of Communication,

60, 556-576. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2010.01497.x

Trilling, D., van Klingeren, M., & Tsfati, Y. (2016). Selective exposure, political polarization,

and possible mediators: Evidence from the Netherlands. International Journal of Public

Opinion Research, 29, 189-213. doi:10.1093/ijpor/edw003

Vaccari, C., Valeriani, A., Barberá, P., Bonneau, R., Jost, J. T., Nagler, J., . . . Tucker, J. A.

(2015). Political expression and action on social media: Exploring the relationship

between lower- and higher-threshold political activities among twitter users in Italy.

Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 20, 221-239. doi:10.1111/jcc4.12108

Valenzuela, S. (2013). Unpacking the use of social media for protest behavior: The roles of

information, opinion expression, and activism. American Behavioral Scientist, 57, 920-

942. doi:10.1177/0002764213479375

Van der Brug, W., van der Meer, T., & van der Pas, D. (2018). Voting in the Dutch “Ukraine-

referendum”: A panel study on the dynamics of party preference, EU-attitudes, and

referendum-specific considerations. Acta Politica, 53, 496-516. doi:10.1057/s41269-018-

0107-z
EXPOSURE AND SHARING EFFECTS 51

Weeks, B. E., Lane, D. S., Kim, D. H., Lee, S. S., & Kwak, N. (2017). Incidental exposure,

selective exposure, and political information sharing: Integrating online exposure patterns

and expression on social media. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 22, 363-

379. doi:10.1111/jcc4.12199

Westerwick, A., Johnson, B. K., & Knobloch-Westerwick, S. (2017). Confirmation biases in

selective exposure to political online information: Source bias versus content bias.

Communication Monographs, 84, 343-364. doi:10.1080/03637751.2016.1272761

Wheeless, L. R. (1974). The effects of attitude, credibility, and homophily on selective exposure

to information. Speech Monographs, 41, 329-338. doi:10.1080/03637757409375857

Wojcieszak, M., Bimber, B., Feldman, L., & Stroud, N. J. (2016). Partisan news and political

participation: Exploring mediated relationships. Political Communication, 33, 241-260.

doi:10.1080/10584609.2015.1051608
Research Highlights

• An experiment showed Dutch adults were more selective in sharing than exposure.

• Selective sharing was most predictive of political polarization and participation.

• Some selectivity effects were strengthened by value or impression involvement.

• The findings also varied by issue, which diverged in pre-existing polarization.

You might also like