Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

www.nature.

com/scientificreports

OPEN Empathy moderates


the relationship between cognitive
load and prosocial behaviour
Roger S. Gamble *
, Julie D. Henry & Eric J. Vanman

Cognitive load reduces both empathy and prosocial behaviour. However, studies demonstrating these
effects have induced cognitive load in a temporally limited, artificial manner that fails to capture real-
world cognitive load. Drawing from cognitive load theory, we investigated whether naturally occurring
cognitive load from the ongoing COVID-19 pandemic moderated the relationship between empathy
and prosocial behaviour (operationalised as support for public health measures). This large study in an
Australian sample (N = 600) identified negative relationships between pandemic fatigue, empathy for
people vulnerable to COVID-19, and prosocial behaviour, and a positive relationship between empathy
and prosocial behaviour. Additionally, we found that the negative effect of the pandemic on prosocial
behaviour depended on empathy for vulnerable others, with pandemic fatigue’s effects lowest for
those with the highest empathy. These findings highlight the interrelationships of cognitive load and
empathy, and the potential value of eliciting empathy to ease the impact of real-world cognitive load
on prosocial behaviour.

Empathy is the ability to understand (cognitive empathy) and experience (affective empathy) the perspective
and feelings of a­ nother1–3 and has been linked to prosocial behaviour in adults and c­ hildren4–6. However, con-
ventional approaches to studying empathy have relied almost exclusively on relating behavioural or neural
empathic responses to trait-based ­indicators7,8. Such approaches fail to consider state-level variation in empathy
attributable to external factors.
Several recent studies have demonstrated that state empathy reflects a dynamic process, which can be driven
by the motivation to avoid engagement for fear of negative consequences or to approach others to obtain soci-
oemotional ­rewards9. Empathy is also influenced by external variables, including physical ­distance10, target ­race11,
and group ­membership12. Because empathy is dependent on how we cognitively process these external features,
cognitive factors play a crucial role in understanding when and how strongly empathy is elicited.
If cognitive processing is important, then cognitive load should be too. Higher cognitive load decreases
performance on various t­ asks13, including social ones. For instance, people experiencing higher cognitive load
omit potentially important information when making inferences about o ­ thers14. Finite, top-down working mem-
ory resources should also theoretically impose limits on a person’s capacity for empathy. Consistent with this
view, increased cognitive load reduces behavioural and neural empathic responses to emotional ­scenarios15–18.
Additionally, when attention was directed away from a painful image, neural activity related to the component
of the empathic response associated with affect-sharing was unaffected by cognitive load but reduced in the
component believed to reflect cognitive ­evaluation19. Cognitive load also appears to influence the downstream
effects of empathy. Hiraoka and Nomura asked participants to memorise and retain a two- (low cognitive load)
or eight-letter (high load) Latin alphabet string while listening to the sound of a crying i­ nfant20. Cognitive load
decreased caregiving intention and increased intention to neglect the infant, with both these effects mediated
by state affective empathy. These reductions in empathy, reduced caregiving intention, and increased neglect
intention were also more substantial in the high load than the low load condition, suggesting that cognitive load
diminishes prosocial behaviour by depleting cognitive resources. Cognitive load also reduces prosocial behaviour
­ thers21, whereas the use of a cognitively effortful emotion regulation strategy (cognitive
in the form of trust with o
reappraisal) moderates the relationship between empathy and prosocial b ­ ehaviour22.
However, a critical limitation to these cognitive load studies relates to the nature of the cognitive load
manipulation. Typically, cognitive load is manipulated in a temporally limited, artificial fashion—such as by
having participants memorise and then retain an alphanumeric sequence or focus on stimuli unrelated to the
empathy-inducing stimulus. Perhaps more importantly, cognitive load can persist across extended timeframes

School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia. *email: roger.gamble@uq.net.au

Scientific Reports | (2023) 13:824 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-28098-x 1


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol.:(0123456789)
www.nature.com/scientificreports/

in a naturalistic environment because attentional resources are constantly used. Prolonged cognitive load also
induces mental f­ atigue23, yet this has not been considered in research on empathy and cognitive load.
The COVID-19 pandemic has provided a unique, powerful, real-world way to test the prediction that cog-
nitive load might influence prosocial behaviour. In response to the pandemic, which has killed millions and
infected hundreds of millions more g­ lobally24, public health measures such as physical d ­ istancing25, mandated
mask-wearing26, and l­ockdowns27 have been widely deployed. Although such measures effectively reduced the
spread of the ­virus28, they have come at an enormous economic, social, and psychological ­cost29. Consequently,
many countries have reported reduced adherence to public health measures owing to mental ­exhaustion30. The
World Health Organisation has termed this phenomenon ‘pandemic fatigue’28.
In the context of the current study, pandemic fatigue can also be considered a specific, severe form of cognitive
load as defined under cognitive load theory. Working memory is the critical mental workspace that allows us
to hold and manipulate information, and therefore to reason, learn and comprehend. Central to cognitive load
theory is the understanding that because this working memory capacity is limited, greater cognitive load may
negatively impact cognitive function and task ­performance13,31. Although cognitive load theory identifies three
distinct types of cognitive load, most relevant to understanding the deleterious effects of pandemic fatigue on
psychological performance is extraneous load, which refers to unnecessary mental effort that promotes neither
task completion or l­ earning13,31. Importantly too, empirical studies consistently show how cognitive load exacer-
bates the effects of stress. For instance, higher stress-induced cortisol levels following extended periods of mental
activity compromises performance on tests of executive ­function32, while inducing stress in a high cognitive load
condition disrupts attentional ­processing33. Higher levels of cognitive load have also been shown to increase the
proclivity to make riskier d ­ ecisions34. Thus, pandemic fatigue can most parsimoniously be explained under the
cognitive load theory framework. Specifically, the introduction of new ways of performing routine tasks, the
rapidly shifting conditions of the pandemic and the uncertainty about the duration of health measures required
additional mental resources to process and contributed to overall stress l­ evels35.
Cognitive load theory also provides an elegant explanation of why those who are high in information fatigue
choose to reduce their knowledge of COVID information: they engage in avoidant behaviour precisely because
it is so prevalent. Avoidance of COVID-19 information has been linked to perceived information overload in a
German ­setting36, and information avoidance predicted reluctance to engage in COVID public health measures
in a Chinese s­ etting37. Thus, the accumulation of novel tasks and environmental stressors during the pandemic
introduce extraneous cognitive load that depletes working memory capacity, which negatively impacts broader
cognitive function and behaviour,
Importantly, adherence to public health measures should be linked to empathy because it is a form of prosocial
­behaviour38–40. This is because perceptions of civic duty and benefits to others represent prosocial motivations,
and empirical data supports this view. For instance, a recent study using cross-sectional research methods showed
that stronger prosocial motivations in general were associated with increased adherence to physical ­distancing41.
Further, these effects have also been demonstrated using both longitudinal and experimental research methods.
Using group-based trajectory modelling to track adherence to physical distancing over an 8 month period, one
of the most important predictors of poorer adherence trajectories was a lower prosocial attitude towards physi-
cal ­distancing42. Experimental studies have also shown that when physical distancing is framed as a prosocial
behaviour, adherence behaviour is subsequently greater relative to messaging that uses either self-interested or
threatening ­language43,44.
Empathy for people vulnerable to COVID-19 has also been directly linked to greater adherence to physical
distancing and mask-wearing, and inducing empathy promoted greater adherence to these health ­measures45. In
addition, empathy uniquely predicted acceptance of disruptive mandates (e.g., lockdowns) as effective in reduc-
ing virus spread; distress did not influence this ­relationship46. Moreover, higher state empathy and experimental
inducement of empathy each result in increased intention to vaccinate oneself against COVID-1947, with COVID-
19 vaccination status also predicted by prosociality relating to protecting others from d ­ isease48. Such evidence
supports the view that empathy and adherence to COVID-19 health measures are related.
As noted previously, because pandemic fatigue is a specific type of cognitive load, it provides a powerful way
to test the real-life impact of cognitive load on empathy and prosocial behaviour. When this study was conducted,
Australia was ideal for testing this hypothesis because widespread vaccination had not been a­ chieved49, and
repeated, prolonged city-wide and state-wide lockdowns were c­ ommon50,51. Moreover, the extension of meas-
ures such as mandatory mask-wearing, enforcement of physical distancing, and limits on gatherings became
features of life in pandemic-era Australia (additional context for Australia during the COVID-19 pandemic is
in Supplementary Materials)52.
In American and French samples, it has been shown that compliance with public health measures can be
impacted by individual differences in affective dispositions, such that moral values emphasizing care and patho-
gen disgust sensitivity increase public health compliance, and affective pushback against threats to personal
liberty reduce c­ ompliance53. Based on this, the Australian experience of the COVID-19 pandemic and the real
limitations on personal freedom due to the imposition of unpredictable and severe measures might have pre-
dicted heightened, widespread resistance from the population regarding public health measures. However, this
was not generally seen and likely reflects that Australians place less emphasis on personal liberty relative to their
American or British counterparts. During the pandemic, most Australians were willing to comply with prolonged
personal restrictions in the interest of collective protection against the ­virus54–56. Therefore, pandemic fatigue in
the Australian context more strongly reflects a measure of exhaustion with ongoing pandemic information and
restrictions, and not a reaction to infringements on personal liberty.
In light of such chronic and extreme levels of pandemic fatigue among Australian ­residents57,58, this country
provided a perfect location to test the central research question of whether cognitive load influences the relation-
ship between empathy and prosocial behaviour (operationalised here as support for public health measures) in

Scientific Reports | (2023) 13:824 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-28098-x 2


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol:.(1234567890)
www.nature.com/scientificreports/

a real-world setting. The central hypothesis was that pandemic fatigue would decrease support for public health
measures but that empathy would moderate this relationship with higher empathy buffering the negative impact
of pandemic fatigue on motivation to engage in public health measures.
This study tested whether empathy was related to adherence to health measures and vaccination intention
in a pandemic-fatigued population. Specifically, we hypothesised a positive relationship between empathy, sup-
port for public health measures, and vaccination intention. We also predicted a negative relationship between
pandemic fatigue and empathy, support for public health measures, and vaccination intention. In addition, we
assessed whether increasing empathy for people vulnerable to COVID-19 influences motivation to adhere to
health measures and vaccination intention in a pandemic-fatigued population. We, therefore, hypothesised that
empathy would moderate the effects of pandemic fatigue to increase support for public health measures and
vaccination likelihood.

Results
To test the first hypothesis that there would be a positive relationship between empathy, support for public health
measures, and vaccination intention, and a negative relationship between pandemic fatigue and empathy, sup-
port for public health measures, and vaccination intention, we first conducted a series of correlation analyses
(see Table 1). There was a strong positive relationship between empathy and support for public health measures
(r[598] = 0.515, p < 0.001), a weak positive relationship between empathy and vaccination status (r[598] = 0.128,
p = 0.002), and a weak positive association between vaccination status and support for public health measures
(r[598] = 0.172, p < 0.001). A weak negative relationship was identified between pandemic fatigue and empathy
(r[598] = − 0.204, p < 0.001), with a moderate negative relationship between pandemic fatigue and support for
public health measures (r[598] = − 0.384, p < 0.001) and a weak negative association between pandemic fatigue
and vaccination status (r[598] = − 0.110, p = 0.007). Location information was used to create a lockdown status
index (lockdown and no lockdown). Lockdown status was considered as an alternative predictor of pandemic
fatigue because public health orders are the most restrictive during periods of lockdown (and so pandemic fatigue
would be expected to be higher). This also revealed a weak positive correlation between lockdown status and
empathy (r[598] = 0.089, p = 0.029), a weak positive association between lockdown status and support for public
health measures (r[598] = 0.123, p = 0.002), a weak positive relationship between lockdown status and pandemic
fatigue (r[598] = 0.252, p < 0.001), and a weak positive association between lockdown status and vaccination status
(r[598] = 0.155, p < 0.001). Correlation analyses were conducted for the Study 1 dataset (Table S1).
To test the second hypothesis that empathy would moderate the effect of pandemic fatigue on support for
public health measures and vaccination intention, we ran separate hierarchical multiple regressions for each
outcome variable. A moderated hierarchical linear regression investigating support for public health meas-
ures as the outcome variable was conducted by assessing pandemic fatigue, empathy, and empathy × pandemic
fatigue interaction as predictors (see Table 2). Model 1 incorporating pandemic fatigue explained 14.5% of
the total model variance, with pandemic fatigue decreasing support for public health measures (p < 0.001).
Including empathy in the second model explained an additional 19.7% of the model variance (p < 0.001), with
pandemic fatigue reducing and empathy increasing support for public health measures (p’s < 0.001). Model 3,
which included the empathy × pandemic fatigue interaction, accounted for an additional 3.3% of the variance,
with pandemic fatigue reducing support for public health measures (p < 0.001), empathy × pandemic fatigue
increasing support for public health measures (p < 0.001), and empathy no longer contributing to support for
public health measures (p = 0.777).

Pandemic Vaccination Vaccination


Age Empathy Public health fatigue status willingness Lockdown
Age –
− 0.044 –
Empathy
[− .123, .036]
− .148*** .515*** –
Public health
[− .225, − .126] [.453, .571]
− 0.054 − .204*** − .384*** –
Pandemic fatigue
[− .133, .027] [− .28, − .126] [− .45, − .314]
Vaccination 0.046 .128** .172*** − .11** –
­status1 [− .034, .126] [.048, .206] [.093, .248] [− .189, − .03]
Vaccination − 0.044 .106** .201** − 0.066 − .10* –
willingness [− .124, .036] [.026, .185] [.123, .277] [− .146, .014] [− .179, − .02]
− .155*** .089* .123** .252*** .155*** 0.005 –
Lockdowna
[− .232, − .076] [.009, .168] [.043, .201] [.176, .326] [.075, .232] [− .075, .085]

Table 1.  Correlation matrix of age, empathy, support for public health measures, pandemic fatigue,
vaccination status (yes/no), vaccination willingness and lockdown status (no lockdown, lockdown). Pearson’s r,
with 95% confidence intervals shown in brackets. a  = point biserial correlation was calculated. *p < .05, **p < .01,
***p < .001.

Scientific Reports | (2023) 13:824 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-28098-x 3


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol.:(0123456789)
www.nature.com/scientificreports/

95% Confidence
Interval
Variable β Lower Upper t p ΔR2 AIC
Block 1 .145 1202.558
Intercept 64.779 < .001***
PF − .382 [− .457, − .308] − 10.088 < .001***
Block 2 .197 1047.591
Intercept 16.354 < .001***
PF − .292 [− .359, − .226] − 8.62 < .001***
Empathy .453 [.387, .52] 13.365 < .001***
Block 3 .033 1019.002
Intercept 12.006 < .001***
PF − .284 [− .349, − .219] − 7.13 < .001***
Empathy .405 [.338, .472] − 0.284 .777
Empathy × PF .16 [.104, .216] 5.584 < .001***

Table 2.  Hierarchical linear regression of support for public health measures predicted by pandemic fatigue,
empathy, and empathy × pandemic fatigue interaction. PF pandemic fatigue, AIC akaike information criterion.
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.

Comparing the models’ AICs indicated that Model 3 was the best predictive model. Estimated marginal
means analysis indicated that state empathy moderated the pandemic fatigue effect on support for public health
measures, such that support for public health measures was stronger in high-empathy participants regardless of
pandemic fatigue and support for public health measures was weaker in low-empathy participants who had high
pandemic fatigue compared to low-empathy participants with low pandemic fatigue (see Fig. 1). A hierarchical
binomial logistic regression was then conducted to determine the sequential effect of pandemic fatigue, state
empathy and empathy × pandemic fatigue interaction on the likelihood that participants were vaccinated against
COVID-19 (see Table 3). Model 1 incorporating pandemic fatigue was significant (p = 0.007), accounting for
1.9% of the total variance, with pandemic fatigue decreasing the likelihood of being vaccinated (p = 0.007). In
Model 2, including empathy was significant (p = 0.001), accounting for 3.4% of the total variance, with empathy
increasing (p = 0.035) and pandemic fatigue decreasing (p = 0.011) vaccination probability. Model 3 incorporating
empathy × pandemic fatigue was not significantly different from Model 2 (p = 0.557). Assessing the AICs showed
that Model 2, which included state empathy and pandemic fatigue separately, was the most predictive model.
Finally, exploratory analyses investigating the effect of vaccination status on state empathy, support for public
health measures, and pandemic fatigue using three two-tailed Mann–Whitney U tests while using the Benja-
mini–Hochberg ­procedure59 to control the false discovery rate (FDR) revealed significant effects of vaccina-
tion status on state empathy (Yes: M = 4.43, Mdn = 4.667, SD = 0.696; No: M = 4.211, Mdn = 4.333, SD = 0.754;
Public Health Support

Pandemic Fatigue

Empathy

Figure 1.  Estimated marginal means graph of state empathy plotted against support for public health measures,
divided by low (− 1SD), average and high (+ 1SD) pandemic fatigue in Study 2. When empathy was high, there
was no significance difference of pandemic fatigue on support for public health measures. However, when
empathy was low, increasing pandemic fatigue decreased support for public health measures and decreasing
pandemic fatigue increased support for public health measures.

Scientific Reports | (2023) 13:824 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-28098-x 4


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol:.(1234567890)
www.nature.com/scientificreports/

95% Confidence
Interval
Variables OR Lower Upper Z p ΔRN2 (χ2Sig) AIC
Block 1 .019 (.007**) 631.501
Intercept 6.643 < .001***
PF 0.829 [0.724, 0.951] − 2.682 .007**
Block 2 .015 (.012*) 627.188
Intercept 0.598 .55
PF 0.861 [0.748, 0.989] − 2.11 .035*
Empathy 1.395 [1.079, 1.803] 2.543 .011*
Block 3 .001 (.557) 628.842
Intercept − 0.291 .771
PF 1.049 [0.534, 2.06] 0.14 .889
Empathy 1.719 [0.819, 3.608] 1.433 .152
Empathy × PF 0.954 [0.816, 1.116] − .588 .557

Table 3.  Hierarchical binomial logistic regression of vaccination status predicted by pandemic fatigue,
empathy, and empathy × pandemic fatigue interaction. The outcome variable is the probability of being
vaccinated against COVID-19. OR odds ratio, PF pandemic fatigue, AIC akaike information criterion, RN2
nagelkerke’s R2. *p < .05, **p < .01, *** p < .001.

U[NYes = 467, NNo = 133] = 24,694.50, p < 0.001, rrb = 0.205), support for public health measures (Yes: M = 4.37,
Mdn = 4.5, SD = 0.668; No: M = 4.075, Mdn = 4.333, SD = 0.819; U[NYes = 467, NNo = 133] = 23,693.50, p < 0.001,
rrb = 0.237), and pandemic fatigue (Yes: M = 3.782, Mdn = 3.833, SD = 1.437; No: M = 4.162, Mdn = 4.167,
SD = 1.385; U[NYes = 467, NNo = 133] = 26,442.0, p = 0.009, rrb = 0.149). These analyses indicated that vaccinated
participants had higher empathy for COVID-vulnerable others, were more supportive of public health measures
and experienced less pandemic fatigue than unvaccinated participants.

Discussion
This study provided partial support for our hypotheses. In support of our first hypothesis, pandemic fatigue
was negatively associated with empathy, support for public health measures, and vaccination status. Addition-
ally, consistent with prior ­research45,46, there was a strong positive correlation between empathy and support
for public health measures. The presence of lockdowns was also associated with increased pandemic fatigue,
empathy for COVID-vulnerable others, support for public health measures, and an increased likelihood of being
vaccinated. In support of our second hypothesis, state empathy moderated the relationship between pandemic
fatigue and support for public health measures by lessening the negative impact of pandemic fatigue on support
for public health measures while maintaining this relationship in low-empathy participants. For vaccination
status, empathy and pandemic fatigue contributed to the likelihood of being vaccinated, with the interaction
of empathy × pandemic fatigue not affecting the probability of being vaccinated. These results provided partial
support for our second hypothesis.
Of note in this study was the finding that state affective empathy moderated the relationship between natu-
ralistic cognitive load and prosocial behaviour. Specifically, although cognitive load due to pandemic fatigue
reduced prosocial behaviour, higher state empathy mitigated this detrimental effect of cognitive load on proso-
cial behaviour, with cognitive load having its most severe impact on prosocial behaviour in those with lower
empathy. However, this study also revealed that specific prosocial behaviours like vaccination likelihood were
influenced by pandemic fatigue and state empathy, suggesting that empathy and cognitive load contributed to
specific prosocial action.
Prior research assessing adherence to COVID-19 public health measures have noted a range of factors that
include but are not limited to point to trust in government, interpersonal trust, autonomous motivation, political
ideology in the United States, social dominance orientation, trust in science, and psychological ­reactance53,60–64.
Together, these highlight the complexity in attributing support for public health measures solely to prosocial
intent or behaviour. However, our finding that empathy modulated support for prosocial behaviour is consistent
with other findings on empathy and prosocial behaviour during the COVID-19 ­pandemic45,47. These data are
also compatible with the altruism hypothesis of empathy, in which a central tenet is that empathy, specifically
empathic concern for others, plays a causal role in altruistic motivation that then influences prosocial b­ ehaviour4.
Importantly, this hypothesis identifies two paths whereby cognitive processing may influence prosocial behaviour,
namely during the initial processing of affective empathy and evaluating the necessity of altruistic ­responding65.
Given the central role of cognitive processing in this model, empathy may be susceptible to interference from
cognitive load.
As revealed in our best-fitting model, state empathy positively predicted, and cognitive load negatively pre-
dicted, vaccination likelihood, the former finding similar to P ­ fattheicher47. Past research has also revealed that
prosociality increased the intention to vaccinate against COVID-19, whereas religiosity and belief in conspir-
acy theories reduced vaccination i­ntention48. The current results combined with prior literature highlight the

Scientific Reports | (2023) 13:824 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-28098-x 5


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol.:(0123456789)
www.nature.com/scientificreports/

importance of considering empathy, prosocial behaviour measures, and individual difference characteristics
when investigating prosocial action contributors.
At the same time, our study extends the literature on cognitive load interactions with empathic processing
in several meaningful ways. It is the first to assess the relationship between empathy and prosocial behaviour
when subjected to a naturalistic cognitive load. Because we successfully replicated findings from previous studies
investigating the effect of cognitive load on e­ mpathy16–19 and prosocial b ­ ehaviour20, we showed that cognitive
load could be induced from real-world circumstances. Our findings also align with recent research into cognitive
load effects on empathy. For instance, Bajouk found that cognitive load selectively reduced empathy for pain,
negative emotions, and positive emotions in people with low trait ­empathy15.
However, another study revealed that, when listening to a crying infant, people under cognitive load were
more likely to neglect a crying infant if they had high trait affective empathy than those with low trait affective
­empathy66. This latter finding appears inconsistent with our own that high affective empathy negated the effect
of cognitive load on prosocial behaviour compared to low empathy but may reflect a function of the “online
cognitive load” paradigm used to induce cognitive load in H ­ iraoka66, which required participants to memorise
an alphanumeric sequence while presented with a target. This is very different from the approach taken in our
study, which used an “offline cognitive load” design, where the empathic task was conducted after the cognitive
load ­task67. It seems likely that the method used here may have produced weaker cognitive load effects than
an online cognitive load paradigm. Thus, using an online design to investigate the moderating effect of trait
empathy on the relationship between cognitive load and prosocial behaviour would be a valuable approach to
test this possibility directly.
In addition to the theoretical value, our results also have methodological implications for cognitive load
research. They suggest that working memory resources in day-to-day life may carry over into experimental set-
tings. Participants in past cognitive load research were assumed to begin from a non-cognitively loaded baseline,
which had not accounted for individual variations in working memory capacity. Researchers in future cognitive
load studies should attend more closely to whether participants are cognitively loaded at baseline or, if not pos-
sible, monitor individual cognitive load before the study begins.
The findings from this study also highlight the importance of understanding cognitive processing in con-
junction with empathy. A recent neuroimaging meta-analysis suggested that affective empathy is one compo-
nent of the hierarchical multicomponent construct of social cognition and identified an intermediate state that
co-activated neural regions involved in affective and cognitive ­empathy68. These findings are consistent with a
model of empathy that conceptualises the affective and cognitive components of empathy not as a dichotomy
but as an overlapping spectrum in which affect-sharing with others is modulated by cognitive processing, and
perspective-taking is influenced by empathic ­concern69. Indeed, there is a causal link between metacognitive
ability such as self-concept clarity and affective empathy that influences helping b ­ ehaviour70, thus underlining
the necessity of higher-order processes in emotional empathy.
It should be noted that our study examined empathy specifically toward people who were vulnerable to
COVID-19. Although most empathy research focuses on observer traits, it is important to emphasise that empa-
thy is an interpersonal process between an observer and a target, and features of the target may meaningfully
impact on empathy that an observer feels for a ­target71,72. For example, targets who are perceived as vulnerable
on the basis of physical features or socioeconomic status elicit more empathy from observers than targets with
more similarity to the ­observer73–75. If empathy in our study was defined alternatively to focus on a target from
a different group (such as those most affected economically or socially by COVID public health measures), this
may have impacted our results and interpretation. Thus, the way empathy is framed is an important considera-
tion in the interpretation of our findings that empathy for COVID-vulnerable people moderates the relationship
between cognitive load and prosocial behaviour.
Finally, it is worth noting that after the data were collected for this study, Hajek reported the results of a similar
COVID-19 project that examined demographic contributors to trait empathy and trait ­altruism76. The results
revealed that trait altruism, but not trait empathy, was associated with vaccination against COVID-19. Although
these findings are important in understanding factors contributing to vaccination likelihood, a key difference
between the two studies is that our study assessed state empathy as a predictor variable and specific prosocial
behaviours in the form of support for public health measures and vaccination against COVID-19 as outcome
variables. Recent research has revealed an important divergence between state and behavioural indicators of
­empathy77, which are often only negligibly correlated.
Some limitations of this study need to be acknowledged. Firstly, owing to its cross-sectional nature, it is
not possible to draw conclusions about potential causality. While it would be valuable to re-conduct this as an
experimental study to assess the causality of these findings, given the unique context of the study situated in the
pandemic which included city- and state-wide lockdowns it may be extremely difficult to replicate experimentally.
Secondly, single items measures were used to index the constructs of mask-wearing and physical distancing here.
Thirdly, some of public health questions may not have been worded appropriately for the context. For example,
the mask-wearing item from P ­ fattheicher45 asked how much participants “will” wear a mask, which was originally
measured when mask-wearing was voluntary. Thus, our responses may have reflected mask-related mandates
across jurisdictions. Although we made a composite public health measure to ensure that no individual item
was given too much weight, future studies should update public health questions to be contextually relevant.
Second, we did not measure some potentially relevant individual difference characteristics. For example, the
effects of trait empathy may depend on cognitive load ­too15,66, and other individual- and group-level differences
including but not limited to political belief, community identification, employment status and conspiracist belief
may have accounted for changes in state empathy, support for public health measures or ­vaccination39,48,61,76,78.
Indeed, a cross-sectional psychometric analysis of the Pandemic Fatigue Scale found that pandemic fatigue was
highest among those who believed they were unlikely to be infected, believed the disease was mild and had low

Scientific Reports | (2023) 13:824 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-28098-x 6


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol:.(1234567890)
www.nature.com/scientificreports/

concern about the pandemic ­overall79. Thus, it is possible that our constructs are affected by the third variable of
scepticism to the disease and the pandemic, which would include belief in COVID misinformation.
Finally, we only assumed here cognitive load in the context of the pandemic. Although we found that pan-
demic fatigue reflected cognitive load, cognitive load itself was not directly induced or measured. We deliberately
tried to capture naturally occurring cognitive load, and prior laboratory research supported our predictions that
pandemic fatigue would introduce substantial and chronic cognitive load in the specific cohort being assessed.
However, direct manipulation of cognitive load in future studies would provide stronger evidence for our model.
One way in which future studies might directly manipulate cognitive load would be by presenting participants
with pandemic information they should memorise or asking people how much they think about the pandemic
in daily life.
It is also important to acknowledge that our findings were from a single Western country during a global
pandemic. Assessments of psychological literature have highlighted the disproportionate use of American and
other White-Western populations in psychological research despite their statistical infrequency relative to the
global population, and data taken from Western populations may not generalise to the human population at
­large80–83. Therefore, it would be critical for behavioural science to incorporate representative and cross-cultural
samples from non-Western countries to determine if the model of cognitive load, empathy, and prosocial behav-
iour proposed here can be cross-culturally validated.

Conclusion
In conclusion, we used a paradigm of naturalistic cognitive load to assess the impact of empathy on the rela-
tionship between cognitive load and prosocial behaviour in the COVID-19 pandemic. While cognitive load in
the form of pandemic fatigue decreased prosocial behaviour by inducing mental fatigue, empathy moderated
the effect of cognitive load on prosocial behaviour and specific prosocial action by reinvigorating motivation to
protect others that, in turn increased prosocial behaviour. However, we did not find a direct effect of empathy
manipulation on prosocial behaviour intention. These findings highlight those real-world circumstances can
influence the association between empathy and prosocial behaviour, and secondarily in understanding factors
that contribute to or detract from support for health measures that may be used to ensure robust public health
responses.

Methods
Participants. Australian participants were recruited via Prolific in September 2021. They completed a short
survey regarding their “attitudes about the coronavirus (COVID-19) pandemic”. Participants were 18 or over
and resided in Australia before March 20, 2020, when the international border was closed. A power analysis
determined that at least 202 participants would provide 95% power to detect an effect of r > 0.25 at p < 0.05 using
a two-tailed test, and 302 participants would provide 95% power to detect an effect of Cohen’s f > 0.25 at p < 0.05
for a multiple linear r­ egression84. Data were collected from 604 participants, with four excluded from data analy-
sis for not meeting the residency criterion, resulting in a sample of 600 participants (356 female, 232 male, 9
non-binary, 3 N/A; M = 29.6 years, SD = 12.5; 467 vaccinated, 133 unvaccinated; additional demographic factors
in Supplementary Materials). All participants passed the attention check.
All participants provided informed consent to participate in the experiment, which received approval from
the Human Research Ethics Committee at the University of Queensland. The study was conducted in accord-
ance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants were financially compensated for study participation.

Study materials and procedure. Participants rated their empathy for people most vulnerable to COVID-
19 taken from ­Pfattheicher45. This measure consisted of three items adapted from the Empathic Concern sub-
scale of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index, such as “I feel compassion for those most vulnerable to COVID-19”
(see Supplementary Materials)8. These three scores were averaged to generate a final empathy score, with Cron-
bach’s alpha = 0.876.
They also reported the extent they supported public health measures taken to control COVID-1945,46. These
included one item about physical distancing, one item about mask-wearing, and four items about lockdowns
(e.g., “Lockdowns are very effective in stopping the spread of COVID-19”; Cronbach’s α = 0.846). For all the
items mentioned above, participants used a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree). The final
score of support for public health measures was created through a mean of all the items (Cronbach’s α = 0.806).
Participants indicated whether they had received at least one dose of a COVID-19 vaccine. Those who selected
“yes” were asked how soon they received a vaccine when made available to them on a 4-point scale ranging from
1 (“3+ months after it was made available”) to 4 (“As soon as it was made available”). Those who selected “no”
reported whether they intend to get vaccinated on a 5-point scale used by P ­ fattheicher47, ranging from 1 (“I will
definitely not get vaccinated”) to 5 (“I will definitely get vaccinated”).
Participants then completed the Pandemic Fatigue Scale, asking them to rate six items about pandemic fatigue
on a 7-point scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree85. Pandemic fatigue in this scale was divided
into the two factors of information fatigue (e.g. “I am sick of hearing about COVID-19”) and behavioural fatigue
(e.g., “I am losing my spirit to fight against COVID-19”). The six items were averaged to create a final score for
pandemic fatigue (Cronbach’s α = 0.866), with the items for information fatigue and behavioural fatigue aver-
aged to create separate scores for information fatigue (Cronbach’s α = 0.871) and behavioural fatigue (Cronbach’s
α = 0.781). Finally, they completed demographic questions relating to their gender, age, nationality, the country
they lived in the longest, current location, and a screening question about Australian residency. Finally, partici-
pants completed a brief attention check item. Data files and study materials are available at https://​osf.​io/​c7hra/?​
view_​only=​90740​96e5f​73458​eba1a​a3683​5e624​53.

Scientific Reports | (2023) 13:824 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-28098-x 7


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol.:(0123456789)
www.nature.com/scientificreports/

Statistical analysis and pre‑registration. Statistical analyses were performed on jamovi version
2.2.5.086. The structural validity of each scale was tested using Reliability Analysis (Cronbach’s alpha). The alpha
level set to determine significance for this study was 0.05, with the FDR for multiple comparisons controlled to
αFDR = 0.05 using the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure. 95% confidence intervals are provided for r values in the
Pearson correlation analysis, standardised beta coefficients in the multiple linear regression analysis, and odds
ratios in the binomial logistic regression analysis. Model fit for individual models in the multiple regression
analyses were measured using the Akaike information criterion (AIC). The normality distribution of the vari-
ables was assessed using a series of Shapiro–Wilk tests. The tests for state empathy, pandemic fatigue, support for
public health measures and vaccination willingness were significant (p’s < 0.001), indicating that the distribution
for all the variables violated the assumption of normality. Accordingly, non-parametric tests were conducted for
all statistical analyses.
In pre-registration, this project was designed as two separate studies involving correlational analysis as Study
1 (https://​osf.​io/​vxyhr/?​view_​only=​43343​2c34f​5441e​49451​d3abe​0265e​cf) and an experimental study including
an empathy manipulation as Study 2 (https://​osf.​io/​3buzr/?​view_​only=​e7450​f62ce​524fb​db05d​6eb29​4c8d3​90).
Pre-registration for Study 1 was completed after data collection had taken place, but Study 2 was pre-registered
before data collection. However, statistical analyses were not conducted on any of the data until after pre-regis-
tration. Thus, not all this study’s data collection was pre-registered but the study predictions and analyses were
pre-registered. The studies used independent participants and the same questionnaire measures. However, the
empathy manipulation in Study 2 was not significant for state empathy, support for public health measures or
vaccination intention among those who hadn’t received a COVID-19 vaccine on three two-tailed Mann–Whit-
ney U test with the Benjamini–Hochberg procedure applied to control the FDR (p’s > 0.042), we deviated from
the pre-registered designs to combine the datasets for statistical analyses. We chose to use the more proximal
measure of state empathy as a predictor variable in subsequent analyses. Datasets for Study 1 and 2 were analysed
separately prior to dataset combination with results in Supplementary Materials (Tables S1–S3), which found
similar results to the analyses conducted using the combined dataset.

Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are openly available in the Open Science Framework at https://​
osf.​io/​c7hra/?​view_​only=​9e42d​d9781​3b472​5be9a​839a2​2ea9c​71.

Received: 12 July 2022; Accepted: 12 January 2023

References
1. Bernhardt, B. C. & Singer, T. The neural basis of empathy. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 1–23 (2012).
2. Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. The neural bases for empathy. Neuroscientist 17, 18–24 (2011).
3. Walter, H. Social cognitive neuroscience of empathy: Concepts, circuits, and genes. Emot. Rev. 4, 9–17 (2012).
4. Batson, C. D., Lishner, D. A. & Stocks, E. L. The empathy–altruism hypothesis. In The Oxford Handbook of Prosocial Behavior
259–281 (eds Schroeder, D. A. & Graziano, W. G.) (Oxford University Press, 2015). https://​doi.​org/​10.​1093/​oxfor​dhb/​97801​95399​
813.​013.​023.
5. Lamm, C., Rutgen, M. & Wagner, I. C. Imaging empathy and prosocial emotions. Neurosci. Lett. 693, 49–53 (2019).
6. Roberts, W., Strayer, J. & Denham, S. Empathy, anger, guilt: Emotions and prosocial behaviour. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 46, 465–474
(2014).
7. Danziger, N., Faillenot, I. & Peyron, R. Can we share a pain we never felt? Neural correlates of empathy in patients with congenital
insensitivity to pain. Neuron 61, 203–212 (2009).
8. Davis, M. H. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Cat. Sel. Doc. Psychol. 10, 85 (1980).
9. Zaki, J. Empathy: A motivated account. Psychol. Bull. 140, 1608–1647 (2014).
10. Schiano Lomoriello, A., Meconi, F., Rinaldi, I. & Sessa, P. Out of sight out of mind: Perceived physical distance between the observer
and someone in pain shapes observer’s neural empathic reactions. Front. Psychol. 9, 1824 (2018).
11. Han, S. Neurocognitive basis of racial ingroup bias in empathy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 400–421 (2018).
12. Vanman, E. J. The role of empathy in intergroup relations. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 11, 59–63 (2016).
13. Kalyuga, S. Cognitive load theory: How many types of load does it really need?. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 23, 1–19 (2011).
14. Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W. & Krull, D. S. On cognitive busyness: When person perceivers meet persons perceived. J. Pers. Soc.
Psychol. 54, 733 (1988).
15. Bajouk, O. & Hansenne, M. Dispositional perspective-taking and empathic concern modulate the impact of cognitive load on
empathy for facial emotions. Psychol. Rep. 122, 2201–2219 (2019).
16. Gu, X. & Han, S. Attention and reality constraints on the neural processes of empathy for pain. Neuroimage 36, 256–267 (2007).
17. Morelli, S. A. & Lieberman, M. D. The role of automaticity and attention in neural processes underlying empathy for happiness,
sadness, and anxiety. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 160 (2013).
18. Rameson, L. T., Morelli, S. A. & Lieberman, M. D. The neural correlates of empathy: Experience, automaticity, and prosocial
behavior. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 235–245 (2012).
19. Fan, Y. & Han, S. Temporal dynamic of neural mechanisms involved in empathy for pain: An event-related brain potential study.
Neuropsychologia 46, 160–173 (2008).
20. Hiraoka, D. & Nomura, M. The influence of cognitive load on empathy and intention in response to infant crying. Sci. Rep. 6,
28247 (2016).
21. Samson, K. & Kostyszyn, P. Effects of cognitive load on trusting behaviour—An experiment using the trust game. PLoS ONE 10,
e0127680 (2015).
22. Lockwood, P. L., Seara-Cardoso, A. & Viding, E. Emotion regulation moderates the association between empathy and prosocial
behavior. PLoS ONE 9, e96555 (2014).
23. Mizuno, K. et al. Mental fatigue caused by prolonged cognitive load associated with sympathetic hyperactivity. Behav. Brain Funct.
7, 17 (2011).
24. World Health Organisation [WHO]. WHO Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19) Dashboard. WHO Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19)
Dashboard https://​covid​19.​who.​int/​table (2021).

Scientific Reports | (2023) 13:824 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-28098-x 8


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol:.(1234567890)
www.nature.com/scientificreports/

25. Chu, D. K. et al. Physical distancing, face masks, and eye protection to prevent person-to-person transmission of SARS-CoV-2
and COVID-19: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet 395, 1973–1987 (2020).
26. Cheng, Y. et al. Face masks effectively limit the probability of SARS-CoV-2 transmission. Science 372, 1439–1443 (2021).
27. Mégarbane, B., Bourasset, F. & Scherrmann, J.-M. Is lockdown effective in limiting SARS-CoV-2 epidemic progression?—A cross-
country comparative evaluation using epidemiokinetic tools. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 36, 746–752 (2021).
28. World Health Organisation [WHO]. Pandemic Fatigue: Reinvigorating the Public to Prevent COVID-19—Policy Framework for
Supporting Pandemic Prevention and Management. https://​apps.​who.​int/​iris/​bitst​ream/​handle/​10665/​335820/​WHO-​EURO-​2020-​
1160-​40906-​55390-​eng.​pdf (2020).
29. Witteveen, D. & Velthorst, E. Economic hardship and mental health complaints during COVID-19. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.
117, 27277–27284 (2020).
30. Petherick, A. et al. A worldwide assessment of changes in adherence to COVID-19 protective behaviours and hypothesized pan-
demic fatigue. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5, 1145–1160 (2021).
31. Paas, F., Tuovinen, J. E., Tabbers, H. & Van Gerven, P. W. M. Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load
theory. Educ. Psychol. 38, 63–71 (2003).
32. Bohnen, N., Houx, P., Nicolson, N. & Jolles, J. Cortisol reactivity and cognitive performance in a continuous mental task paradigm.
Biol. Psychol. 31, 107–116 (1990).
33. Braunstein-Bercovitz, H. Does stress enhance or impair selective attention? The effects of stress and perceptual load on negative
priming. Anxiety Stress Coping 16, 345–357 (2003).
34. Allen, P. M., Edwards, J. A., Snyder, F. J., Makinson, K. A. & Hamby, D. M. The effect of cognitive load on decision making with
graphically displayed uncertainty information. Risk Anal. 34, 1495–1505 (2014).
35. Zaman, U. et al. The bridge at the end of the world: Linking expat’s pandemic fatigue, travel FOMO, destination crisis marketing,
and vaxication for “greatest of all trips”. Sustainability 14, 2312 (2022).
36. Link, E. Information avoidance during health crises: Predictors of avoiding information about the COVID-19 pandemic among
German news consumers. Inf. Process. Manag. 58, 102714 (2021).
37. Song, S., Yao, X. & Wen, N. What motivates Chinese consumers to avoid information about the COVID-19 pandemic?: The per-
spective of the stimulus-organism-response model. Inf. Process. Manag. 58, 102407 (2021).
38. Betsch, C. How behavioural science data helps mitigate the COVID-19 crisis. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 438–438 (2020).
39. Böhm, R., Betsch, C., Korn, L. & Holtmann, C. Exploring and promoting prosocial vaccination: A cross-cultural experiment on
vaccination of health care personnel. Biomed. Res. Int. 2016, e6870984 (2016).
40. Cheng, K. K., Lam, T. H. & Leung, C. C. Wearing face masks in the community during the COVID-19 pandemic: Altruism and
solidarity. Lancet 399, E39–E40 (2020).
41. Coroiu, A., Moran, C., Campbell, T. & Geller, A. C. Barriers and facilitators of adherence to social distancing recommendations
during COVID-19 among a large international sample of adults. PLoS ONE 15, e0239795 (2020).
42. MacNeil, S. et al. Group-based trajectories and predictors of adherence to physical distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic.
Psychol. Health 1–19 (2021).
43. Heffner, J., Vives, M.-L. & FeldmanHall, O. Emotional responses to prosocial messages increase willingness to self-isolate during
the COVID-19 pandemic. Personal. Individ. Differ. 170, 110420 (2021).
44. Jordan, J. J., Yoeli, E. & Rand, D. G. Don’t get it or don’t spread it: Comparing self-interested versus prosocial motivations for
COVID-19 prevention behaviors. Sci. Rep. 11, 20222 (2021).
45. Pfattheicher, S., Nockur, L., Böhm, R., Sassenrath, C. & Petersen, M. B. The emotional path to action: Empathy promotes physical
distancing and wearing of face masks during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychol. Sci. 31, 1363–1373 (2020).
46. Petrocchi, S. et al. Affective empathy predicts self-isolation behaviour acceptance during coronavirus risk exposure. Sci. Rep. 11,
10153 (2021).
47. Pfattheicher, S., Petersen, M. B. & Böhm, R. Information about herd immunity through vaccination and empathy promote COVID-
19 vaccination intentions. Health Psychol. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​hea00​01096 (2021).
48. Enea, V. et al. Intentions to be vaccinated against COVID-19: The role of prosociality and conspiracy beliefs across. Health Com-
mun. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1080/​10410​236.​2021.​20181​79 (2022).
49. Mathieu, E. et al. A global database of COVID-19 vaccinations. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5, 947–953 (2021).
50. Mao, F. Covid in Australia: Melbourne to exit 112-day lockdown. BBC News (2020).
51. Mao, F. Covid Australia: Sydney celebrates end of 107-day lockdown. BBC News (2021).
52. Barrett, J. A weary Australia plans reopening as COVID-19 death toll hits 1000. Reuters (2021).
53. Díaz, R. & Cova, F. Reactance, morality, and disgust: The relationship between affective dispositions and compliance with official
health recommendations during the COVID-19 pandemic. Cogn. Emot. 36, 120–136 (2022).
54. Pew Research Center. Global Attitudes & Trends Spring 2021. https://​www.​pewre​search.​org/​global/​datas​et/​spring-​2021-​survey-​
data/ (2021).
55. Smith, D. T., Attwell, K. & Evers, U. Support for a COVID-19 vaccine mandate in the face of safety concerns and political affilia-
tions: An Australian study. Politics https://​doi.​org/​10.​1177/​02633​95721​10090​66 (2021).
56. Tranter, B. K. Your money or your life? Public support for health initiatives during the COVID-19 pandemic. Aust. J. Soc. Issues
1–18 (2022).
57. MacIntyre, C. R. et al. Mask use, risk-mitigation behaviours and pandemic fatigue during the COVID-19 pandemic in five cities
in Australia, the UK and USA: A cross-sectional survey. Int. J. Infect. Dis. 106, 199–207 (2021).
58. National Mental Health Commission. “A real need to replenish” in the face of pandemic fatigue. National Mental Health Commis-
sion https://​www.​menta​lheal​thcom​missi​on.​gov.​au/​news/​2021/​july/a-​real-​need-​to-​reple​nish-​in-​the-​face-​of-​pande​mic (2021).
59. Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat.
Soc. Ser. B (Methodol.) 57, 289–300 (1995).
60. Yuan, H., Long, Q., Huang, G., Huang, L. & Luo, S. Different roles of interpersonal trust and institutional trust in COVID-19
pandemic control. Soc. Sci. Med. 293, 114677 (2022).
61. Van Oost, P. et al. The relation between conspiracism, government trust, and COVID-19 vaccination intentions: The key role of
motivation. Soc. Sci. Med. 301, 114926 (2022).
62. Morbée, S. et al. Adherence to COVID-19 measures: The critical role of autonomous motivation on a short- and long-term basis.
Motiv. Sci. 7, 487–496 (2021).
63. Becher, M., Stegmueller, D., Brouard, S. & Kerrouche, E. Ideology and compliance with health guidelines during the COVID-19
pandemic: A comparative perspective. Soc. Sci. Q. 102, 2106–2123 (2021).
64. Choma, B. L., Hodson, G., Sumantry, D., Hanoch, Y. & Gummerum, M. Ideological and psychological predictors of COVID-
19-related collective action, opinions, and health compliance across three nations. J. Soc. Polit. Psychol. 9, 123–143 (2021).
65. Batson, C. D. Altruism in Humans (Oxford University Press, 2011).
66. Hiraoka, D. & Nomura, M. Would situational stress be harmful for anyone? The influence of situational factors and trait empathy
on women’s response to infant crying. Infant Behav. Dev. 48, 147–156 (2017).
67. Meiring, L., Subramoney, S., Thomas, K. G., Decety, J. & Fourie, M. M. Empathy and helping: Effects of racial group membership
and cognitive load. S. Afr. J. Psychol. 44, 426–438 (2014).

Scientific Reports | (2023) 13:824 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-28098-x 9


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol.:(0123456789)
www.nature.com/scientificreports/

68. Schurz, M. et al. Toward a hierarchical model of social cognition: A neuroimaging meta-analysis and integrative review of empathy
and theory of mind. Psychol. Bull. 147, 293–327 (2021).
69. Weisz, E. & Cikara, M. Strategic regulation of empathy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 213–227 (2021).
70. Krol, S. A. & Bartz, J. A. The self and empathy: Lacking a clear and stable sense of self undermines empathy and helping behavior.
Emotion https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​emo00​00943 (2021).
71. Main, A., Walle, E. A., Kho, C. & Halpern, J. The interpersonal functions of empathy: A relational perspective. Emot. Rev. 9, 358–366
(2017).
72. Bruneau, E. G., Cikara, M. & Saxe, R. Parochial empathy predicts reduced altruism and the endorsement of passive harm. Soc.
Psychol. Personal. Sci. 8, 934–942 (2017).
73. Decety, J., Echols, S. & Correll, J. The blame game: The effect of responsibility and social stigma on empathy for pain. J. Cogn.
Neurosci. 22, 985–997 (2010).
74. Guo, X. et al. Empathic neural responses to others’ pain depend on monetary reward. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 535–541 (2012).
75. Batson, C. D., Lishner, D. A., Cook, J. & Sawyer, S. Similarity and nurturance: Two possible sources of empathy for strangers. Basic
Appl. Soc. Psychol. 27, 15–25 (2005).
76. Hajek, A. & König, H.-H. Level and correlates of empathy and altruism during the COVID-19 pandemic. Evidence from a repre-
sentative survey in Germany. PLoS ONE 17, e0265544 (2022).
77. Murphy, B. A. & Lilienfeld, S. O. Are self-report cognitive empathy ratings valid proxies for cognitive empathy ability? Negligible
meta-analytic relations with behavioral task performance. Psychol. Assess. 31, 1062 (2019).
78. Wakefield, J. R. H., Khauser, A. Doing it for us: Community identification predicts willingness to receive a COVID-19 vaccination
via perceived sense of duty to the community. J. Community Appl. Soc. Psychol. (2021).
79. Rodriguez-Blazquez, C., Romay-Barja, M., Falcon, M., Ayala, A. & Forjaz, M. J. Psychometric properties of the COVID-19 pan-
demic fatigue scale: Cross-sectional online survey study. JMIR Public Health Surveill. 8, e34675 (2022).
80. Arnett, J. J. The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less American. Am. Psychol. 63, 602–614 (2008).
81. Cheek, N. N. Scholarly merit in a global context: The nation gap in psychological science. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 1133–1137
(2017).
82. Cheon, B. K., Melani, I. & Hong, Y. How USA-centric is psychology? An archival study of implicit assumptions of generalizability
of findings to human nature based on origins of study samples. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 11, 928–937 (2020).
83. Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. & Norenzayan, A. The weirdest people in the world?. Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 61–83 (2010).
84. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G. & Buchner, A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral,
and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191 (2007).
85. Lilleholt, L., Zettler, I., Betsch, C. & Böhm, R. Pandemic fatigue: Measurement, correlates, and consequences. Preprint at https://​
doi.​org/​10.​31234/​osf.​io/​2xvbr (2021).
86. Jamovi. The jamovi project. (2021).

Acknowledgements
R.G. is supported by a Commonwealth of Australia research training program scholarship (RTP). We also thank
L.N. for providing feedback on early drafts of the manuscript.

Author contributions
R.G. was responsible for study conception and planning, methodological design, data collection, statistical
analyses and writing of the original and revised manuscripts. J.H. and E.V. were responsible for methodological
design and manuscript revision.

Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information
Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​
10.​1038/​s41598-​023-​28098-x.
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to R.S.G.
Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional affiliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the
Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creat​iveco​mmons.​org/​licen​ses/​by/4.​0/.

© The Author(s) 2023

Scientific Reports | (2023) 13:824 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-28098-x 10


Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
Vol:.(1234567890)
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:

1. use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
2. use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
3. falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
4. use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
5. override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
6. share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at

onlineservice@springernature.com

You might also like