Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

573439

research-article2015
HPQ0010.1177/1359105315573439Journal of Health PsychologyCrockett et al.

Article

Journal of Health Psychology

Boredom proneness and emotion


2015, Vol. 20(5) 670­–680
© The Author(s) 2015
Reprints and permissions:
regulation predict emotional eating sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1359105315573439
hpq.sagepub.com

Amanda C Crockett, Samantha K Myhre and


Paul D Rokke

Abstract
Emotional eating is considered a risk factor for eating disorders and an important contributor to obesity
and its associated health problems. It has been suggested that boredom may be an important contributor to
overeating, but has received relatively little attention. A sample of 552 college students was surveyed. Linear
regression analyses found that proneness to boredom and difficulties in emotion regulation simultaneously
predicted inappropriate eating behavior, including eating in response to boredom, other negative emotions,
and external cues. The unique contributions of these variables to emotional eating were discussed. These
findings help to further identify which individuals could be at risk for emotional eating and potentially for
unhealthy weight gain.

Keywords
bored eating, boredom proneness, emotional eating, emotion regulation

The number of people in the world, and in the and Epel (2007) suggest that interactions between
United States, who are overweight is of concern the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis
because of the significant impact obesity has on and the reward circuitry of the brain may mediate
health and the associated medical costs (Ogden the relationship between stress and eating by
et al., 2012; World Health Organization (WHO), simultaneously lessening the stress response and
2013). While a variety of cultural and environ- motivating the intake of high caloric foods.
mental issues clearly contribute to this problem, Heatherton and Baumeister (1991) proposed an
psychological factors are also important. In par- escape model in which the need to avoid negative
ticular, the role of negative emotions has been self-awareness results in behavioral disinhibition
gaining interest (Spoor et al., 2007). Emotional and overeating. Likewise, difficulties in managing
eating, or distress induced eating, has been con- negative affect could lead to alternative and
ceptualized as overeating in response to nega- inappropriate ways of coping, like overeating
tive emotions (Arnow et al., 1995; Van Strien (Schachter et al., 1968; Spoor et al., 2007).
and Ouwens, 2007). It has been suggested that
negative emotions prompt binge eating epi- North Dakota State University, USA
sodes (Stice, 2001; Stice et al., 2002) as well as
encourage overeating in vulnerable individuals Corresponding author:
Paul D Rokke, Department of Psychology, North Dakota
(Van Strien et al., 2005). State University, Dept. 2765, PO Box 6050, Fargo, ND
There may be several reasons why negative 58108-6050, USA.
emotions contribute to inappropriate eating. Adam Email: Paul.Rokke@ndsu.edu
Crockett et al. 671

The negative emotions most commonly Steptoe et al., 1995). In addition, alexithymia,
investigated in the eating literature include the inability to recognize and identify emotional
depression, anxiety, and anger (Nguyen- states, has been associated with disordered eat-
Rodriguez et al., 2009; Schneider et al., 2010). ing (Quinton and Wagner, 2005) and emotional
Another negative emotion that has not been eating (Larsen et al., 2006) and was shown to
often examined, but may also be an important moderate the relationship between distress and
contributor to emotional eating, is boredom. As overeating (Van Strien and Ouwens, 2007).
early as 1977, it was experimentally demon- Using maladaptive strategies to cope with nega-
strated that boredom led to increased consump- tive emotions has also been determined to
tion of food for both healthy and obese increase food intake (Evers et al., 2010).
participants (Abramson and Stinson, 1977). Thus, we surveyed undergraduate college
However, it has only been recently that research- students to assess their self-reported eating hab-
ers have taken a renewed interest in the role of its, their likelihood to experience negative emo-
boredom. Walfish and Brown (2009) found that tion in the form of boredom, and difficulties in
bariatric patients endorsed eating more fre- emotion regulation. The survey included scales
quently when bored than when experiencing of eating in response to boredom, emotional
other negative emotions and suggested that eating, eating in response to external cues, and
interventions aimed at overcoming boredom restrained eating. It was predicted that a ten-
may be critical to post-surgical success. Others dency to experience boredom coupled with dif-
have since conducted psychometric studies on ficulties in regulating negative emotions would
questionnaires to which items related to eating lead to more emotional eating. We hypothesized
when bored were added (Koball et al., 2012; that if boredom is unique in its experience and
Perpiñá et al., 2011). In both cases, eating in effects, then proneness to boredom would be
response to boredom was endorsed with enough predictive of eating when bored, but may not be
frequency to warrant further investigation. as strongly associated with eating in response to
In the present study, we would like to address other negative emotions. It was hypothesized
two primary issues that have been raised by this that difficulties in emotion regulation would
literature. First, very few studies have looked moderate the relationship between proneness to
specifically at boredom or boredom in relation boredom and eating in response to boredom.
to other negative emotions (Macht, 2008). It has Others have wondered whether eating in
been suggested that boredom is an emotional response to boredom simply reflects an over-
state that is distinct from other negative emo- sensitivity to external cues (Koball et al., 2012;
tions (Goldberg et al., 2011). Thus, it is impor- Wardle et al., 2001). Since boredom is inher-
tant to determine whether eating in response to ently about a state without engaged attention
negative emotions is a generalized phenomenon and in need of something to do, we also thought
or whether boredom has unique influences on that proneness to boredom would predict eating
eating behavior. in response to external cues, although we did
Second, since negative emotions often result not expect boredom to be associated with
in a loss of appetite (Adam and Epel, 2007) and restrained eating. The measurement of the like-
there is variability in the likelihood of eating in lihood to experience boredom, along with
response to negative emotions, researchers have measures of emotion regulation and different
also begun to investigate individual differences eating problems, provides us with an opportu-
that may moderate the relationship between nity to investigate whether proneness to bore-
emotions and overeating (Canetti et al., 2002). dom is a unique precipitant to bored eating and
Among the first personality measures to be to begin to unravel the relationship between
examined, neuroticism has been associated boredom, emotion regulation, and the various
with eating problems (Heaven et al., 2001; problems of eating behavior.
672 Journal of Health Psychology 20(5)

Method the amount of time spent engaged in activities of


low, moderate, or vigorous levels of intensity
Participants across leisure, domestic, work, and transport-
The participants were 552 undergraduate stu- related domains. Each type of activity is weighted
dents (334 women) at a Midwestern state univer- by its energy requirements. A metabolic equiva-
sity who were enrolled in a psychology course. lent (MET) rate score was calculated by multi-
They participated in exchange for course credit. plying the energy equivalent by the minutes of
Participants ranged in age from 18 to 46 years, activity on an average day and then again by the
with an average age of 19.25 years (standard number of days on which the activity was per-
deviation (SD) = 2.15 years). The majority of the formed. The sum of the three activity levels
sample was White (82.6%), with 2.7 percent resulted in a total MET score. Craig and col-
reporting to be Black, 2.7 percent Asian/Pacific leagues found the short form to have a test–retest
Islander, 2.5 percent Hispanic, and 9.4 percent reliability of .76 and concurrent validity of .67.
reporting as other. Four percent of the sample
consisted of international students and 5.6 per- Boredom Proneness Scale. The Boredom Prone-
cent reported that English was a second lan- ness Scale (BPS) was used to measure individual
guage. The average height of participants was differences in the experience of boredom. The
67.79 in (SD = 5.57 in). They weighed an average scale includes 28 true and false questions (Farmer
of 156.51 pounds (SD = 31.48 pounds), for an and Sundberg, 1986). Examples include “Time
average body mass index (BMI) of 23.67 always seems to be passing slowly,” “I often find
(SD = 3.80, range = 17–46). myself at ‘loose ends’, not knowing what else to
do,” and “Much of the time I just sit around
doing nothing.” Scores range from 0 to 28, with
Measures higher scores reflecting a greater likelihood of
Demographics. Participants reported their age, experiencing boredom. The scale has satisfac-
sex, weight, height, and ethnicity. They were tory levels of internal consistency, α = .79, and
also asked whether they were an international test–retest reliability, R = .83 (Farmer and Sund-
student and whether English was their first lan- berg, 1986).
guage. BMI was found using height in inches
and weight in pounds. BMI was calculated Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. The Dif-
using the Centers for Disease Control and Pre- ficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS;
vention (CDC; 2011) approved formula of Gratz and Roemer, 2004) measures how well
weight divided by height squared and then mul- people recognize and regulate their emotions.
tiplied by 703. Responses to the 41-item questionnaire are on a
5-point scale from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost
International Physical Activity Questionnaire. The always). Items representing six subscales cover
International Physical Activity Questionnaire the content of nonacceptance of emotional
(IPAQ) short form was administered in a self- responses (“When I’m upset, I become angry
report format and measured the intensity and with myself for feeling that way”), difficulties
amount of physical activity over the previous engaging in goal-directed behavior (“When I’m
7 days (Craig et al., 2003). We included this upset, I have difficulty getting work done”),
measure because of the potential for activity lev- impulse control difficulties (“When I’m upset, I
els to be associated with BMI and with boredom lose control over my behaviors”), lack of emo-
proneness. However, we did not have strong pre- tional awareness (“I’m attentive to my feelings,”
dictions about the relationships among these reverse scored), limited access to emotion regu-
variables and viewed the analyses which lation strategies (“When I’m upset, I believe that
included them as exploratory in nature. The wallowing in it is all I can do”), and lack of emo-
IPAQ consists of 7 items. Respondents indicate tional clarity (“I have difficulty making sense
Crockett et al. 673

out of my feelings”). Responses to all items found to be reliable with a Cronbach’s alpha of
were summed to form a total score. Higher .79 (Larsen et al., 2006).
scores indicate greater difficulty in regulating
emotion. The DERS has high internal consist-
Procedure
ency of α = .93 (Gratz and Roemer, 2004).
Participants volunteered and responded using
Revised Emotional Eating Scale. The original an online survey program. They were first
Emotional Eating Scale (EES) was a self-report presented with an informed consent form. The
measure composed of 25 items that measured the questionnaires were completed in the follow-
individual’s desire to eat when feeling certain ing order: demographic questions, IPAQ,
emotions, for example, when resentful, shaky, DEBQ, Revised EES, BPS, and the DERS.
blue, uneasy, and irritated (Arnow et al., 1995). (Between the demographic questions and the
Participants rate their desire to eat using a 5-point IPAQ, participants were also presented with
scale—1 (indicating no desire to eat) and 5 (an eight short sentences describing an emotional
overwhelming urge to eat). The original EES did situation. They were asked to describe their
not use boredom as an emotion contributing to most likely responses to those situations. As
eating. In the revised scale, six boredom items these open-ended questions did not provide
were added (Koball et al., 2012). Examples any useful data, further details were not
include, “Blah, nothing to do, unstimulated, included here.) The study did not have a time
unexcited, restless, and disinterested.” The inter- limit, but most participants concluded within
nal consistency for the new EES total scale is 25 minutes.
α = .93. Koball and colleagues reported three fac-
tors, consisting of depression, boredom, and
Results
anxiety/anger. Thus, a total score and three sub-
scales can be scored. Because we were interested The distribution of scores on all primary meas-
in examining bored eating separately from emo- ures was examined to ensure that the recorded
tional eating more generally, for the purposes of data fell within the range and normal parame-
this study, two scores were derived from the 31 ters of each instrument. There were no responses
items. They represented the average strength of that were excluded on this basis. A correlational
desire to eat when experiencing boredom (EES- analysis was conducted as an initial evaluation
bored) and the sum of the subscales for depres- of the relationships among the primary varia-
sion and anxiety/anger (EESother). bles. Table 1 shows the Pearson correlation
coefficients, means, and SDs of these variables.
Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire. The Dutch There are a few important details of this table
Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ; Van that should be pointed out. Previous research
Strien et al., 1986) measures restrained eating shows that the rates of eating disorders, for
(e.g. “Do you deliberately eat less in order not example, bulimia and binge eating disorder, are
to become heavier?”), eating in response to higher among women than men (Hudson et al.,
external cues (e.g. “If you walk past the baker 2007), and that women report more emotional
do you have the desire to buy something deli- eating than men (Larsen et al., 2006). Likewise,
cious?”), and emotional eating (e.g. “Do you our data show that women were more likely to
have a desire to eat when you are depressed or report emotional eating than men. Boredom
discouraged?”). Each of the 33 items are rated proneness and emotional regulation were both
on a 5-point scale from 1 (never) to 5 (very strongly correlated with the emotional eating
often). The average ratings of the items on each variables. Also, activity levels were negatively
scale were calculated. Higher scores indicate a correlated with emotional eating. In planning
higher frequency of restrained, external, or this study, and in the absence of a known litera-
emotional eating. The complete scale has been ture or strong expectations, we speculated that
674 Journal of Health Psychology 20(5)

Table 1. Correlations among primary variables.

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1. Sex –
2. Activity .24** –
3. BMI .14** −.08 –
4. BPS .07 −.14** .10* –
5. DERS .00 −.08 .06 .52** –
6. EESb −.19** −.12** −.01 .27** .39** –
7. EESo −.15** −.11* .04 .27** .42** .77** –
8. DBQem −.31** −.15** .05 .26** .36** .66** .77** –
9. DBQex −.02 −.03 −.02 .22** .25** .48** .42** .45** –
10. DBQre −.34** −.12** .18** .13** .23** .17** .17** .27** .03 –
M 3159.1 23.7 8.9 82.2 14.2 49.9 32.0 31.1 25.4
SD 2435.6 3.8 4.8 21.2 4.8 17.5 11.2 6.1 9.0

BMI: body mass index; BPS: Boredom Proneness Scale; DERS: Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; EESb: Revised
Emotional Eating Scale, bored subscale; EESo: Revised Emotional Eating Scale, other emotions combined; DEBQem:
Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire, emotional eating scale; DEBQex: Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire, external
eating scale; DEBQre: Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire, restraint eating scale; SD: standard deviation; MET: meta-
bolic equivalent.
N = 552 for all pairs, except those involving BMI for which N = 544 due to missing data. Sex was coded as 0 = women,
1 = men. Activity levels are in MET minutes.
*p < .05; **p < .01.

activity levels might be associated with the (EESother), and the DEBQ emotional eating
experience of boredom and bored eating and, scale (DEBQem). In order to test the specificity
therefore, eventually with body mass. However, of these predictive models for emotional eating,
these assumptions were not confirmed. As in we also used them to predict DEBQ external
other studies of unselected young adults (e.g. eating scale (DEBQex) scores and the DEBQ
Ellickson-Larew et al., 2013), it can be seen that restraint eating scale (DEBQre) scores. All
emotional eating was not at all associated with scores were standardized prior to entering into
BMI in this sample. In our sample, men reported the analyses.
higher levels of activity than women. Our initial
regression analyses included activity levels in
Bored eating
the prediction of emotional eating, but activity
and its interactions with sex of the participant Sex, BPS, DERS, and their interactions were
were never significant predictors, once the vari- entered into a regression equation to predict
ance due to sex was accounted for. Therefore, EESbored scores. The overall model was sig-
activity levels and BMI were not included in nificant, F(7, 544) = 16.05, p < .001, R = .414,
any further analyses. R2 = .171. Table 2 lists the regression table for
Five separate regression analyses were run this analysis. When significance values can be
to determine possible predictors of emotional stated more specifically than listed in the table,
eating. In each analysis, sex, BPS scores, DERS they will be incorporated into the text. It can be
scores, and their interactions were entered seen that women were more likely than men to
simultaneously to predict eating outcomes. eat while bored. The proclivity to be bored
Three analyses were concerned with emotional (p = .001) and difficulties in emotion regulation
eating and used the following scores as the were both predictive of bored eating, although
dependent variable: the EES bored subscale none of these variables interacted with each
(EESbored), the EES sum of all other emotions other (all ps > .14).
Crockett et al. 675

Table 2. Predictors of emotional eating.

Variable EESbored EESother DEBQem

b SE b SE b SE
Constant .031 .044 .044 .118 .044 .042
Sex −.206*** .044 −.437*** .118 −.321*** .042
BPS .150** .046 .254* .126 .124** .044
DERS .276*** .047 1.054*** .127 .299*** .045
Sex × BPS −.040 .046 −.227 .125 −.068 .044
Sex × DERS .042 .048 .195 .129 .032 .046
BPS × DERS −.054 .037 −.055 .101 .001 .035
Sex × BPS × DERS .000 .039 −.001 .106 .004 .037

BPS: Boredom Proneness Scale; DERS: Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; EESbored: Revised Emotional Eating
Scale, bored subscale; EESother: Revised Emotional Eating Scale, other emotions combined; DEBQem: Dutch Eating
Behavior Questionnaire, emotional eating scale; SE: standard error.
Sex was coded as 0 = women, 1 = men; b weights are unstandardized coefficients.
**p < .01; ***p < .001.

Emotional eating measures to predict specific aspects of eating


behavior problems, two additional regression
Two analyses were conducted in which the analyses were conducted. In these cases, the
EESother and the DEBQem scores served as same variables were used to predict the external
dependent variables. In the first, the overall and restrained eating scores of the DEBQ. When
model including sex, BPS, DERS, and their entered simultaneously with their interaction
interactions significantly predicted EESother terms, sex, boredom proneness, and difficulties
scores, F(7, 544) = 20.70, p < .001, R = .459, in emotion regulation predicted DEBQex, F(7,
R2 = .210. Table 2 presents the regression table 544) = 7.17, p < .001, R = .291, R2 = .084. The
for this analysis. When considered together, regression table is presented in Table 3. Unlike
each of the variables sex, boredom proneness the previous analyses, the sex of the participant
(p = .04), and difficulties in emotion regulation was not predictive of eating in response to exter-
significantly predicted emotional eating. None nal cues (p = .59). However, both boredom
of the interactions among these variables were proneness (p = .005) and emotional regulation
significant (all ps > .07). predicted DEBQex scores. None of the interac-
Sex, BPS, DERS, and their interactions also tions were significant (all ps > .09).
predicted DEBQem scores, the overall model The model including sex, BPS, DERS, and
resulting in F(7, 544) = 25.25, p < .001, R = .495, their interactions significantly predicted DEBQre
R2 = .245. The resulting beta weights from this scores, F(7, 544) = 16.24, p < .001, R = .416,
regression are also presented in Table 2. The R2 = .173. It can be seen in Table 3 that sex and
pattern of results is similar to the EESother DERS were significant predictors of restrained
scores with sex, bored proneness (p = .005), and eating. However, BPS did not contribute mean-
emotional regulation each making meaningful ingfully to the predictive model (p = .384), nor
contributions to the model with no interactions did any of the interaction terms (all ps > .46).
(all ps > .12).

Other eating behavior problems Discussion


Given that part of the goal of this research was Overeating in response to negative emotions
to determine the ability of individual difference serves as a risk factor for the development of
676 Journal of Health Psychology 20(5)

Table 3. Predictors of other problematic eating behaviors.

Variable DEBQex DEBQre

b Standard b Standard
error error
Constant .039 .046 −.001 .044
Sex −.024 .046 −.335*** .044
BPS .136** .049 .040 .046
DERS .195*** .049 .204*** .047
Sex × BPS −.077 .048 −.032 .046
Sex × DERS .022 .050 −.035 .048
BPS × DERS −.065 .039 .005 .037
Sex × BPS × DERS −.009 .041 −.009 .039

BPS: Boredom Proneness Scale; DERS: Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; DEBQex: Dutch Eating Behavior Ques-
tionnaire, external eating scale; DEBQre: Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire, restraint eating scale.
Sex was coded as 0 = women, 1 = men; b weights are unstandardized coefficients.
**p < .01; ***p < .001.

eating disorders as well as unhealthy weight other measures of negative affect like loneli-
gain (Spoor et al., 2007; Stice et al., 2002). ness, depression, anxiety, and hostility (Farmer
Increasingly, researchers have looked at indi- and Sundberg, 1986; Vodanovich, 2003), as
vidual difference measures in order to identify well as neuroticism (Gordon et al., 1997). The
those who are most vulnerable to emotional eat- results of the present study are thus similar to
ing as well as to learn more about the potential studies which have shown a relationship
mechanisms that promote emotional eating between neuroticism and problematic eating
(Canetti et al., 2002; Larsen et al., 2006; Van behaviors (Connolly et al., 2007; Ellickson-
Strien and Ouwens, 2007). The present study Larew et al., 2013; Heaven et al., 2001). It may
was conducted to extend this literature by spe- be that the relationships observed in this study
cifically looking at boredom proneness and dif- simply reflect a general association between
ficulties in emotional regulation. We had the likelihood of experiencing negative emo-
hypothesized that individual differences in the tion and eating in response to a variety of nega-
tendency to experience boredom as well as dif- tive emotions.
ficulties in the ability to cope with negative It is also possible that the strong and consist-
emotions would predict emotional eating, espe- ent predictive power of boredom proneness in
cially eating in response to boredom. this study is reflective of a personality trait that
Some important conclusions can be drawn could explain a unique portion of variance in
from the results of this study. First, the ten- emotional eating. One potential mechanism for
dency to experience boredom is predictive of this contribution is hinted at in the relationship
emotional eating. Although we had expected between boredom proneness and the external
this individual difference measure to be espe- scale of the DEBQ. External eating in this con-
cially predictive of eating in response to bore- text refers to the relatively greater influence of
dom, it was also predictive of eating in response external stimuli and food-related cues on eating
to a variety of negative emotions. It is possible behavior than internal states. Boredom prone-
that the Boredom Proneness Scale is tapping in ness has been shown be to be associated with
to a general tendency to experience negative distractibility and difficulties in attentional con-
affect. Previous research has shown that bore- trol (Kass et al., 2003). Even though emotional
dom proneness is modestly correlated with eating has often been assumed to be a means of
Crockett et al. 677

avoiding or coping with negative emotions There are several limitations to the present
(Heatherton and Baumeister, 1991; Macht, study. It should also be noted that the validity of
2008), the independent contributions of bore- self-reported emotional eating has been called
dom proneness and emotion regulation in all of into question (Evers et al., 2009). For this rea-
the models lead us to wonder whether the influ- son, some would recommend only conducting
ence of boredom proneness is related to some- laboratory studies in which eating in response
thing other than just negative emotion. If to negative emotions could be directly observed.
boredom proneness were simply an indicator of However, multiple methodological issues make
the likelihood of experiencing negative emo- laboratory eating studies expensive and diffi-
tion, we would expect that one’s ability to cope cult to conduct, including the adequate manipu-
with negative emotion would interact with the lation and documentation of emotion change,
experience of negative emotion. This was not taking into account prior food intake, variability
evident in any of the analyses. Further research in personal food preferences, avoiding experi-
will be needed to determine the exact nature of mental demand, as well as conducting labora-
the influence that boredom proneness plays in tory taste tests in a manner that is naturalistic
its relation to emotional eating, including the and generalizable to real-world settings. Despite
possibility of reduced attentional control. the criticisms of self-reported emotional eating,
As stated, popular theories propose that eat- some studies have demonstrated correspond-
ing in response to negative mood states is done ence between self-report and laboratory meth-
to negate the negative feelings by providing ods (e.g. Wallis and Hetherington, 2009). Given
either comfort or distraction (Heatherton and the lack of previous work on boredom and our
Baumeister, 1991; Macht, 2008; Spoor et al., desire to investigate individual differences,
2007). Since eating in response to negative which requires a large sample size, we elected
affect is not adaptive, we predicted that it is a to start with a correlational study in the hopes of
behavior which would be most likely to occur providing data which would support further
when individuals have difficulty in regulating experimental efforts.
negative emotion. In every model we tested, It should also be noted that this was a cross-
difficulties in emotion regulation predicted sectional study which obviates causal infer-
emotional eating. This is consistent with the ences. The data were collected in a young
eating disorders literature that posits a relation- healthy sample, where BMI was not correlated
ship between emotion regulation and binge eat- with emotional eating, and all data were based
ing and purging (Berg et al., 2013; Haedt-Matt on self-report. Despite these qualifications, our
and Keel, 2011; Wedig and Nock, 2010). understanding of variables that impact the like-
The lack of specificity in the predictive mod- lihood of emotional eating is important for
els might lie in the dependent variables rather determining the risks involved in weight gain
than in the predictors. That is, the correlations and health, as well as for developing interven-
among the measures of bored eating and emo- tions to prevent or remediate weight-related
tional eating were .66 and .77. Despite validation health problems.
efforts to create a scale that contributed inde- In summary, it was found that two individual
pendent variance to the EES (Koball et al., 2012), difference measures, boredom proneness and
the high correlations between these measures difficulties in emotional regulation, indepen-
indicate that there was a fair amount of overlap dently predicted the likelihood of self-reported
in the bored eating and emotional eating meas- eating in response to negative emotions. They
ures in the present sample. Thus, perhaps it is not did not interact with each other. Nor did they
surprising that boredom proneness predicted interact with the sex of the participant. Koball
responses to all of these measures. Continued et al. (2012) found that eating in response to
refinement of the measures and the concept of boredom was the most frequently endorsed item
emotional eating might be warranted. among college students, and that the factor
678 Journal of Health Psychology 20(5)

structure of the Revised EES supported the con- lifestyle! 13 September. Available at: http://
clusion that eating in response to boredom was www.cdc.gov/healthyweight/assessing/bmi/
different from eating in response to other emo- adult_bmi/
tions. Although we were unable to confirm the Connolly AM, Rieger E and Caterson I (2007)
Binge eating tendencies and anger cop-
uniqueness of bored eating, the identification of
ing: Investigating the confound of trait neu-
two predictive individual difference variable
roticism in a non-clinical sample. European
provides an important step toward increasing Eating Disorders Review 15(6): 479–486.
our ability to identify individuals who are likely DOI:10.1002/erv.765.
to experience problems with emotional eating. Craig CL, Marshall AL, Sjostrom M, et al. (2003)
The results also suggest that interventions aimed International Physical Activity Questionnaire:
at dealing with boredom, or perhaps improving 12-country reliability and validity. Medicine &
attentional control, and alternative means for Science in Sports & Exercise 35(8): 1381–1395.
coping with negative emotions might be Ellickson-Larew S, Naragon-Gainey K and Watson
employed to reduce emotional eating and its D (2013) Pathological eating behaviors, BMI,
consequences including overweight and obesity. and facet-level traits: The roles of conscien-
tiousness, neuroticism, and impulsivity. Eating
Further research to untangle the relationship
Behaviors 14(4): 428–431. DOI:10.1016/j.eat-
between boredom and other negative emotions
beh.2013.06.015.
as they relate to emotional eating as well as to Evers C, de Ridder DTD and Adriaanse MA (2009)
determine the mechanisms by which proneness Assessing yourself as an emotional eater:
to boredom has its influence is warranted. Mission impossible? Health Psychology 28(6):
717–725. DOI:10.1037/a0016700.
Funding Evers C, Marijn Stok F and de Ridder DTD (2010)
This research received no specific grant from any Feeding your feelings: Emotion regulation
funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for- strategies and emotional eating. Personality
profit sectors. and Social Psychology Bulletin 36(6): 792–804.
DOI:10.1177/0146167210371383.
Farmer R and Sundberg ND (1986) Boredom prone-
References
ness—The development and correlates of a new
Abramson EE and Stinson SG (1977) Boredom scale. Journal of Personality Assessment 50(1):
and eating in obese and non-obese individuals. 4–17. DOI:10.1207/s15327752jpa5001_2.
Addictive Behaviors 2(4): 181–185. Goldberg YK, Eastwood JD, Laguardia J, et al.
Adam TC and Epel ES (2007) Stress, eating and the (2011) Boredom: An emotional experience
reward system. Physiology & Behavior 91(4): distinct from apathy, anhedonia, or depression.
449–458. DOI:10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.011. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 30(6):
Arnow B, Kenardy J and Agras WS (1995) The 647–666. DOI:10.1521/jscp.2011.30.6.647.
Emotional Eating Scale: The development of a Gordon A, Wilkinson R, McGown A, et al. (1997)
measure to assess coping with negative affect The psychometric properties of the Boredom
by eating. International Journal of Eating Proneness Scale: An examination of its validity.
Disorders 18(1): 79–90. Psychological Studies 42(2–3): 85–97.
Berg KC, Crosby RD, Cao L, et al. (2013) Facets Gratz KL and Roemer L (2004) Multidimensional
of negative affect prior to and following binge- assessment of emotion regulation and dys-
only, purge-only, and binge/purge events regulation: Development, factor Structure, and
in women with bulimia nervosa. Journal initial validation of the Difficulties in Emotion
of Abnormal Psychology 122(1): 111–118. Regulation Scale. Journal of Psychopathology
DOI:10.1037/a0029703. and Behavioral Assessment 26(1): 41–54.
Canetti L, Bachar E and Berry EM (2002) Food and Haedt-Matt AA and Keel PK (2011) Revisiting the
emotion. Behavioural Processes 60(2): 157–164. affect regulation model of binge eating: A meta-
DOI:10.1016/S0376-6357(02)00082-7. analysis of studies using ecological momentary
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) assessment. Psychological Bulletin 137(4):
(2011) Healthy weight—It’s not a diet, it’s a 660–681. DOI:10.1037/a0023660.
Crockett et al. 679

Heatherton TF and Baumeister RF (1991) Binge eat- Schachter S, Goldman R and Gordon A (1968) Effects
ing as escape from self-awareness. Psychological of fear, food deprivation, and obesity on eating.
Bulletin 110(1): 86–108. DOI:10.1037/0033- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
2909.110.1.86. 10(2): 91–97. DOI:10.1037/h0026284.
Heaven PCL, Mulligan K, Merrilees R, et al. (2001) Schneider KL, Appelhans BM, Whited MC, et al.
Neuroticism and conscientiousness as predic- (2010) Trait anxiety, but not trait anger, pre-
tors of emotional, external, and restrained eat- disposes obese individuals to emotional eat-
ing behaviors. International Journal of Eating ing. Appetite 55(3): 701–706. DOI:10.1016/j.
Disorders 30(2): 161–166. DOI:10.1002/ appet.2010.10.006.
eat.1068. Spoor STP, Bekker MHJ, Van Strien T, et al. (2007)
Hudson JI, Hiripi E, Pope HG Jr, et al. (2007) The Relations between negative affect, coping, and
prevalence and correlates of eating disorders emotional eating. Appetite 48(3): 368–376.
in the national comorbidity survey replica- DOI:10.1016/j.appet.2006.10.005.
tion. Biological Psychiatry 61(3): 348–358. Steptoe A, Pollard TM and Wardle J (1995)
DOI:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.040. Development of a measure of the motives
Kass SJ, Wallace JC and Vodanovich SJ (2003) underlying the selection of food: The Food
Boredom proneness and sleep disorders as Choice Questionnaire. Appetite 25(3): 267–284.
predictors of adult attention deficit scores. DOI:10.1006/appe.1995.0061.
Journal of Attention Disorders 7(2): 83–91. Stice E (2001) A prospective test of the dual-path-
DOI:10.1177/108705470300700202. way model of bulimic pathology: Mediating
Koball AM, Meers MR, Storfer-Isser A, et al. (2012) effects of dieting and negative affect. Journal
Eating when bored: Revision of the Emotional of Abnormal Psychology 110(1): 124–135.
Eating Scale with a focus on boredom. Health DOI:10.1037/0021-843X.110.1.124.
Psychology 31(4): 521–524. DOI:10.1037/ Stice E, Presnell K and Spangler D (2002) Risk
a0025893. factors for binge eating onset in adoles-
Larsen JK, van Strien T, Eisinga R, et al. (2006) cent girls: A 2-year prospective investiga-
Gender differences in the association between tion. Health Psychology 21(2): 131–138.
alexithymia and emotional eating in obese indi- DOI:10.1037/0278-6133.21.2.131.
viduals. Journal of Psychosomatic Research Van Strien T and Ouwens MA (2007) Effects
60(3): 237–243. DOI:10.1016/j.jpsychores. of distress, alexithymia and impulsivity on
2005.07.006. eating. Eating Behaviors 8(2): 251–257.
Macht M (2008) How emotions affect eating: A five- DOI:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2006.06.004.
way model. Appetite 50(1): 1–11. Van Strien T, Engels RCME, Leeuwe JV, et al.
Nguyen-Rodriguez ST, Unger JB and Spruijt-Metz (2005) The Stice model of overeating: Tests
D (2009) Psychological determinants of emo- in clinical and non-clinical samples. Appetite
tional eating in adolescence. Eating Disorders: 45(3): 205–213. DOI:10.1016/j.appet.2005.
The Journal of Treatment & Prevention 17(3): 08.004.
211–224. Van Strien T, Frijters JER, Bergers GPA, et al. (1986)
Ogden C, Carrol M, Kit B, et al. (2012) Prevalence The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire
of Obesity in the United States, 2009–2010. (DEBQ) for Assessment of Restrained
Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Emotional, and External Eating Behavior.
Statistics. International Journal of Eating Disorders 5(2):
Perpiñá C, Cebolla A, Botella C, et al. (2011) 295–315.
Emotional Eating Scale for children and ado- Vodanovich SJ (2003) Psychometric measures
lescents: Psychometric characteristics in a of boredom: A review of the literature. The
Spanish sample. Journal of Clinical Child and Journal of Psychology 137(6): 569–595.
Adolescent Psychology 40(3): 424–433. DOI:10.1080/00223980309600636.
Quinton S and Wagner HL (2005) Alexithymia, Walfish S and Brown TA (2009) Self-assessed
ambivalence over emotional expression, and emotional factors contributing to increased
eating attitudes. Personality and Individual weight in presurgical male bariatric patients.
Differences 38(5): 1163–1173. DOI:10.1016/j. Bariatric Nursing and Surgical Patient Care
paid.2004.07.013. 4(1): 49–52.
680 Journal of Health Psychology 20(5)

Wallis DJ and Hetherington MM (2009) Emotions and Wedig MM and Nock MK (2010) The functional
eating. Self-reported and experimentally induced assessment of maladaptive behaviors: A pre-
changes in food intake under stress. Appetite 52(2): liminary evaluation of binge eating and purging
355–362. DOI:10.1016/j. appet.2008.11.007. among women. Psychiatry Research 178(3):
Wardle J, Guthrie CA, Sanderson S, et al. (2001) 518–524. DOI:10.1016/j.psychres. 2009.05.010.
Development of the Children’s Eating Behaviour World Health Organization (WHO) (2013) Obesity
Questionnaire. Journal of Child Psychology and and overweight: Fact sheet no. 311. Available
Psychiatry 42(7): 963–970. DOI:10.1111/1469- at: http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/
7610.00792. fs311/en/ (accessed 5 August 2013).
Copyright of Journal of Health Psychology is the property of Sage Publications, Ltd. and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

You might also like