Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Invited Article

Harnessing brain and cognitive reserve for the prevention of dementia


Michael Valenzuela, Perminder S. Sachdev
School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales, Sydney; Neuropsychiatric Institute, Prince of Wales Hospital,
Randwick, Australia
Downloaded from http://journals.lww.com/indianjpsychiatry by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4
XMi0hCywCX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC1y0abggQZXdgGj2MwlZLeI= on 03/25/2024

ABSTRACT

The concepts of brain and cognitive reserve capture several elements of common wisdom – that we all differ in the
neural resources we are endowed at birth, that experience and especially complex mental activities then modify how
these neural resources are organized and cultivated, and that after any form of brain injury there is significant individual
variation in the degree to which clinical deficits may manifest. Transforming these insights into a formal and refutable
working definition, however, has been more challenging. Depending on the scale of analysis, brain and cognitive reserve
have been defined from neurocentric, neuropsychological, computational, and behavioral perspectives. In our research,
we have focused on the behavioral definition, whereby an individual’s lifetime exposure to complex mental activities
is used for prediction of longitudinal cognitive and neurological change. This approach also benefits from a wealth of
epidemiological studies linking heightened complex mental activity with reduced dementia risk. Research in the field
of cognitive training is also beginning to indicate that incident cognitive decline can be attenuated, with recent clinical
trials addressing the major challenges of transfer of gain and durability of effect. High quality randomized clinical trials
are therefore the most urgent priority in this area so that the promise of brain and cognitive reserve can be harnessed for
the purpose of the primary prevention of dementia.

Key words: Brain reserve, cognitive exercise, cognitive reserve, cognitive training, dementia, prevention

WHAT IS RESERVE? throughout their neocortex in comparison to those who


expressed clinical symptoms, suggesting that the lack of
While most neuroscientists and clinicians would agree that symptoms in the former was because of a ‘greater reserve’.
the brain has reserve capacity, the concept remains difficult The neurocentric version of brain reserve therefore implies
to define. What exactly is it that researchers mean when an advantage based on increased neuronal numbers. This
invoking ‘reserve’? Part of the difficulty results from the fact interpretation was also consistent with action of a linear
that this question can be approached from many angles. threshold mechanism,[2] whereby either a larger static
These are briefly outlined below. lesion, or the more protracted course of a progressive
lesion, was required before an individual may cross below a
Neurocentric perspective hypothetical functional threshold.

Katzman et al.[1] first noted that cognitively normal Since neuronal counts are not possible in life, clinical
individuals with elevated Alzheimer’s disease (AD) pathology studies have often relied on brain volume as a marker of
had almost double the number of large pyramidal neurons such reserve capacity. Use of a gross measure such as whole
brain volume, however, risks confusing a predictor variable
(one’s estimate of reserve) with a known outcome variable
Address for correspondence: Dr. Michael Valenzuela, (age-related cerebral atrophy). An alternative that has been
Neuropsychiatric Institute, Prince of Wales Hospital, Randwick,
Sydney NSW 2031, Australia. E-mail: michaelv@unsw.edu.au
explored in the context of dementia is intracranial volume
(ICV) as a proxy for maximal brain volume, and even head
circumference[3] and head width[4] as more basic measures.
How to cite this article: Valenzuela M, Sachdev PS. Harnessing
In general, these studies have failed to show a general
brain and cognitive reserve for the prevention of dementia.
Indian J Psychiatry 2009;51:S16-21.
association with dementia incidence across the full range of
ICV, but rather an increased risk only in the low to very low

S16 Indian J Psychiatry 51: Supplement, January 2009


Valenzuela and Sachdev: Cognitive reserve and prevention of dementia

ranges[5] or when in the presence of an additional risk factor this definition is extraordinarily difficult to measure. Simply
such as ApoE4.[3] asking subjects about their use of deliberate strategies whilst
performing memory tasks can, for example, produce more
An obvious problem for the neurocentric interpretation questions than answers.[11] On the other hand, if we assume
relates to the implication that brain reserve is a non- that one’s capacity for deliberate cognitive flexibility is akin
modifiable entity. Maximum ICV and head circumference are to greater problem solving or ability to set-shift, we again
generally achieved by puberty[6] and reflect genetic variance risk confounding a predictor with an outcome variable (i.e.,
in neuronal quantum as well as developmental, nutritional, age-related decline in executive functions).
Downloaded from http://journals.lww.com/indianjpsychiatry by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4

and environmental factors in early life.[7] More importantly,


XMi0hCywCX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC1y0abggQZXdgGj2MwlZLeI= on 03/25/2024

since these measures do not generally change after the Computational perspective
onset of adulthood, does this mean that underlying brain
reserve can also not change? As will be reviewed below, the A more sophisticated approach to reserve revolves around
weight of evidence from the epidemiological, clinical, and the notions of computational redundancy and flexibility. High
experimental literature suggests the opposite. Dementia cognitive reserve individuals may not only have a wider
risk appears to be highly modifiable by experience well into repertoire of conscious and preconscious strategies, but
late life. For these reasons, a neurocentric version of brain also a greater number of potential neural pathways for
reserve is not tenable, although it is important to note that execution of these cognitive processes, thus allowing
this reflects current technological limitations rather than maintenance of function despite neurological insult. A person
any underlying conceptual flaw. Brain reserve, of whatever with high reserve and high disease burden may therefore
flavor, must at some level have a neurobiological correlate. remain asymptomatic due to compensatory and resistant
adaptations to functional brain networks; alternatively, a low
Cognitive perspective reserve individual may express symptoms after only a trivial
brain insult due to a lack of redundant neural pathways and
Rather than focus on the maximal quantity or volume of an inflexible or intolerant functional network.[12,13]
neurons, the cognitive reserve perspective is more interested
in ‘how well we use what has been left behind’.[8,9] Two non- Whilst this approach is attractive as it unifies brain and
trivial distinctions have been made. First, we could replace cognitive reserve with a specific mechanism, problems with
“neuronal numbers” with “neuropsychological competence” operationalization again arise. Do we attempt to estimate
or “intelligence”, and so apply a similar threshold mechanism neurocomputational factors such as network efficiency
whereby high cognitive reserve individuals simply perform and redundancy via some type of functional neuroimaging
better on cognitive tests to start with and therefore ‘stress test’? Whilst of high interest, a working definition of
require a larger fall before reaching diagnostic threshold. brain reserve along these lines is currently not practical.
Importantly, this account suggests no interaction with the
underlying and progressive neurodegenerative process, and Behavioral perspective
so predicts no differential rate in cognitive decline, only
different starting points for the decline. It has therefore The strategy adopted in our research has been a behavioral
been termed a passive account[8] and been identified as one and simply asks how mentally active and engaged a
a potential source of systematic error in longitudinal person is in comparison with the average? It therefore seeks
studies.[10] Clearly, in the context of ageing and dementia, a reliable definition at the level of day-to-day observable and
any reserve definition must be able to independently verifiable facts related to complex mental activity. The type of
predict differential rates of neuropsychological change, not information relevant to this version of reserve includes level
just differences in incidence of impairment. and duration of formal education, the nature and complexity
of occupational history, and the diversity, frequency and
Alternatively, a more dynamic form of cognitive reserve cognitive challenge of past and present leisure activities.
suggests that individuals who have developed a range This has been combined into a validated assessment tool,
of deliberate cognitive strategies for solving complex the Lifetime of Experience Questionnaire (LEQ),[14] which takes
problems, such as navigating around the neighborhood, 15 min to complete and is now available online for research
and perform well on neuropsychological tests, are more use (http://train.headstrongcognitive.com/leq.aspx). Higher
likely to remain within normal limits for longer despite LEQ scores independently predict not only attenuated
the parallel progression of underlying disease. This active cognitive decline over time, but also a reduced rate of
account therefore suggests that two individuals may hippocampal atrophy.[15] The main advantage from such a
begin at the same neuropsychological starting point and straightforward formulation is therefore the provision of an
suffer the same longitudinal burden of disease, but due to operational definition which is clinically relevant.
increased strategic mechanisms, one may perform better at
follow-up testing, or suffer from less day-to-day symptoms. Importantly, this behavioral approach to measuring brain
Whilst capturing part of the ecological nature of reserve, or cognitive reserve does not identify itself with a specific

Indian J Psychiatry 51: Supplement, January 2009 S17


Valenzuela and Sachdev: Cognitive reserve and prevention of dementia

neurological quantum, computational property or cognitive maintaining, or restoring mental function through the
process. It seems self evident that participation in complex repeated and structured practice of tasks which pose
mental activities will lead to changes in a number of interacting an inherent problem or mental challenge. Importantly,
mechanisms at different temporal and spatial scales,[12] which this definition does not include training in strategies to
together may alter an individual’s risk for dementia and compensate for deficits, traditionally a rehabilitative or
cognitive dysfunction. In our opinion, there is no one brain remedial approach.[25]
or cognitive reserve, but a number of reserves.[16] Our working
definition is hence at the behavioral scale for essentially Randomized controlled trials in late
Downloaded from http://journals.lww.com/indianjpsychiatry by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4

pragmatic reasons because it is arguably more amenable to life


XMi0hCywCX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC1y0abggQZXdgGj2MwlZLeI= on 03/25/2024

precise characterization and hence general utility.


Repetitive cognitive training undoubtedly improves
Links between mental exercise and performance on the trained task – there is indeed more than
dementia risk 20 years of cognitive psychology research on this topic.[26]
To determine whether cognitive training could potentially
Several large-scale epidemiological studies have linked help reduce or delay the incidence of dementia, two major
participation in complex mental activity to reduced dementia issues need consideration:
risk. A systematic review of 22 longitudinal cohort studies found 1) Generalization or transfer of effect
that individuals with higher levels of education, occupation or Does the cognitive training intervention only lead
engagement in complex cognitive activities were at 46% lower to improvement in the trained task, or does it also
risk for incident dementia than those with low levels.[17] transfer to non-trained tasks? We propose a hierarchy
of generalization of increasing clinical relevance:
When restricting analysis to those cohort studies of older a. Transfer to non-trained tasks in same cognitive
persons that have focused on cognitive complexity in their domain
current lifestyle, protective effects remain significant even b. Transfer to non-trained tasks in other cognitive
after controlling for earlier life exposures such as education, domains
occupation, baseline cognition, and cardiovascular risk c. Transfer to global measures of general cognitive
factors.[18-22] Moreover, evidence for a dose dependent ability (e.g., Alzheimer’s disease assessment scale -
relationship is evident from studies that show cognitive, tests for general intellectual ability, etc.)
corresponding decrements in dementia risk as a function d. Transfer to measures of general function (e.g.,
of increasing participation in cognitive lifestyle activities.[23] instrumental activities of daily living, quality of life,
For example, Verghese et al. found that compared to those mood, etc.)
older individuals with the lowest level of participation in
cognitive lifestyle activities, those with moderate activity 2) Persistence or durability of effect
levels had 50% lower risk for dementia over 5 years follow- Does the effect of cognitive training intervention
up, and those with the highest activity levels had 67% lower last beyond the immediate post-training period, or
risk for incident dementia.[24] is continual cognitive training required? Longitudinal
follow-up of cognitive training efficacy is required to
There is now highly consistent epidemiological support answer this question.
for an association between complex mental activity and
dementia risk, but the nature of the relationship remains We have recently published a systematic review of
contentious for two main reasons. First, the biological randomized controlled trials (RCT) of cognitive training
mechanisms which may mediate a nexus between mental in healthy older individuals in which longitudinal follow-
activity and dementia are not clear. Some potential up was a critical design feature.[27] The results of seven
mediating mechanisms are briefly reviewed below. Second, identified trials suggest that a discrete program of cognitive
and more importantly, there are questions about the direction exercise in the order of 2-3 months may have long-lasting
of causality: does preceding mental activity reduce or delay and persistent protective effects on cognition over a number
expression of future dementia or is preclinical dementia of years. The overall weighted mean difference was strong
causing a reduction in participation in activities prior to in magnitude, estimated at 1.07 (CI: 0.32-1.83); the overall
formal diagnosis? To disentangle this complex ‘chicken or non-weighted average Cohen’s d effect size was 0.5.
egg’ problem, data from clinical trials using cognitive training
interventions are required. The ACTIVE study is the largest trial in the area[28] and
examined the effects of 10 sessions of cognitive training
Clinical trials of cognitive training on 2832 healthy older individuals. Participants completed
three different intervention groups: memory training,
Definition of cognitive training reasoning training, and processing speed training. Two
Cognitive training is any intervention aimed at improving, years later, each intervention improved cognitive ability

S18 Indian J Psychiatry 51: Supplement, January 2009


Valenzuela and Sachdev: Cognitive reserve and prevention of dementia

in the targeted area. Importantly, at this stage there was to primary prevention of dementia as part of a holistic risk
no evidence of transfer of gain to other domains, nor any reduction strategy.
effect on instrumental activities of daily living. Follow
up at five-years, however, found that reasoning training How could mental exercise prevent
specifically protected against functional decline compared dementia?
to any of the other interventions or the control wait-and-
see condition.[29] This is therefore the first large clinical trial Complex mental activity clearly alters the structure and
to demonstrate transfer of effect since cognitive training function of the brain on a number of levels. We have
Downloaded from http://journals.lww.com/indianjpsychiatry by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4

produced enduring benefits on a general functional outcome previously reviewed how the spatial and temporal nature
XMi0hCywCX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC1y0abggQZXdgGj2MwlZLeI= on 03/25/2024

that is highly relevant to dementia onset. of these changes may contribute to defense against
dementia,[12] with information gained mainly from decades
There is also a high degree of community and commercial of work studying the effects of enrichment on the rodent
interest in computer-based cognitive training. One group brain[34] and from human brain imaging. Here, we present a
has conducted a RCT with one such product in a report highly condensed summary.
that included co-authors from the parent company.[30] The
attraction of computerized cognitive training is that training Molecular mechanisms
can be standardized and allows a gradient in task difficulty Short periods of increased activity alter long-term
to be incorporated as individuals’ skill levels progress. potentiation and depression,[35] possibly by upregulation
Neuropsychological tests immediately after the end of of AMPA receptors.[36] Neurotrophic hormones are also
the training period found verbal memory performance increased, particularly BDNF[37,38] and NGF.[39] Strikingly,
improved by up to 25% of a standard deviation, and testing 3 transgenic Alzheimer model mice show dramatic decrements
months later showed that short-term memory performance in pathology[40-42] and improvements in behavior[43]
remained enhanced. after several weeks of enrichment compared to control
littermates. The time frame for detection of biological
The combination of both cognitive and physical exercise changes secondary to enrichment is minimal, given that
is also of great interest. This has yet to be tested in a microarray analysis has shown dozens of gene expression
rigorous RCT. The Sim-A study investigated the effects changes following as little as three hours of exposure.[44]
of cognitive, physical, and combined training in healthy
older individuals over a five year period.[31] Thirty Cellular mechanisms
paper-and-pencil cognitive training sessions produced Synaptogenesis is arguably the most robust cellular change,
a significant effect over both the 12 month and 5-year in the order of 150-300%,[45] and potentially the most
follow-up periods. Moreover, this effect seemed to clinically relevant, as decline in synaptic density is strongly
transfer to a measure of general cognition. The group correlated with cognitive status in dementia.[46] Experience-
that did both cognitive and physical training experienced dependent neurogenesis[47] and angiogenesis[48] also occur,
a larger effect size than those who completed just one which in combination may explain why enrichment seems to
type of training, suggesting a potential for synergy lead to increased gross brain volume,[7] as well as increased
between these modalities. Other smaller studies with regional volumetric changes in humans.[49,50]
samples of less than 100 individuals have found positive
trends but have lacked power.[32,33] Cortical network mechanisms
Repeated cognitive exercise leads to increased metabolic
The overall effect size and consistency across longitudinal efficiency across whole brain networks,[51] whilst selectively
trials of cognitive training is therefore promising, yet and temporarily increasing haemodynamic responsivity in
many questions remain. There has been a wide variety of brain areas engaged by the cognitive tasks.[52,53] There is also
primary outcome measures, for example, across the trials, evidence for an enhanced ability to adapt and compensate
and details of the applied cognitive tasks also varied. against progressive disease in the medial temporal lobe,
Quality of trial design and reporting has in general been particularly through functional reorganization in the
low. prefrontal cortex.[54-57]

It is however encouraging that those studies with longer Recommendations and challenges for the future
term follow-up showed no evidence of less potent effects. A confluence of research streams has emerged in the last
A durable long-term effect from cognitive training may 10 years as the basis for intense medical, community and
therefore be realistic. Finally, two of the more recent commercial interest in trying to harness the power of brain
clinical studies have shown that their training protocols and cognitive reserve for the prevention of age-related
generalize to domains beyond the narrow focus of the cognitive dysfunction. Perhaps the greatest influence has
trained tasks.[29,31] Well designed cognitive training been a wider sociological trend away from pharmacological
interventions may thereby have the potential to contribute agents and towards behavioral and lifestyle modification and

Indian J Psychiatry 51: Supplement, January 2009 S19


Valenzuela and Sachdev: Cognitive reserve and prevention of dementia

positivistic health attitudes. Yet this enthusiasm should not Alzheimer’s type. Chichester: Wiley And Sons; 1988. p. 39-52.
10. Tuokko H, Garrett D, Mcdowell I, Silverberg N, Kristjansson B. Cognitive
obscure our demand for rigorous scientific evidence when decline in high-functioning older adults: Reserve or ascertainment bias?
attempting to translate preclinical findings to individual and Aging Mental Health 2003;7:259-70.
11. Naveh-Benjamin M, Brav T, Levi O. The associative memory deficits of older
community-wide interventions. The greatest challenges for adults: The role of strategy utilization. Psychol Aging 2007;22:202-8.
the area are therefore in the domain of clinical trials. No trial 12. Valenzuela MJ, Breakspear M, Sachdev P. Complex mental activity and
the ageing brain: Molecular, cellular and cortical network mechanisms.
has, for example, definitively shown that cognitive training Brain Res Rev 2007;56:198-213.
reduces the incidence of dementia, as opposed to the rate 13. Rubinov M, Mcintosh A, Valenzuela M, Breakspear M. Simulation of
of cognitive decline. Higher caliber RCTs are required, with neuronal death and network recovery in a computational model of
Downloaded from http://journals.lww.com/indianjpsychiatry by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4

distributed cortical activity. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008;In Press.


close attention to design of control groups, longitudinal
XMi0hCywCX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC1y0abggQZXdgGj2MwlZLeI= on 03/25/2024

14. Valenzuela MJ, Sachdev P. Assessment of complex mental activity across


evaluation, choice of cognitive training protocol and the lifespan: Development of the lifetime of experiences questionnaire.
Psychol Med 2007;37:1015-25.
outcome measures, and recruitment of relevant samples. 15. Valenzuela M, Sachdev P, Wen W, Chen X, Brodaty H. Lifespan mental
activity predicts diminished rate of hippocampal atrophy. Plos One
2008;3:e2598.
This information will then need to inform wider community 16. Valenzuela MJ. Brain reserve and the prevention of dementia. Curr Opin
programs, and many more questions arise. For example, Psychiatry 2008;21:296-302.
17. Valenzuela MJ, Sachdev P. Brain reserve and dementia: A systematic
is starting a new cognitively demanding hobby as good as review. Psychol Med 2006;36:441-54.
many hours of computer-based cognitive training? If so, 18. Wang HX, Karp A, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. Late-life engagement in social
are all activities equally effective or only some? How often and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia:
A longitudinal study from the kungsholmen project. Am J Epidemiol
and what intensity of engagement is required? Is group 2002;155:1081-7.
participation better than individual practice at home? 19. Fratiglioni L, Wang HX, Ericsson K, Maytan M, Winblad B. Influence of
social network on occurrence of dementia: A community-based longitudinal
Generic issues will also arise such as scalability, accessibility, study. Lancet 2000;355:1315-9.
economy, and accountability. 20. Scarmeas N, Levy G, Tang M, Manly J, Stern Y. Influence of leisure activity
on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 2001;57:2236-42.
21. Wilson RS, Mendes De Leon CF, Barnes LL, Schneider JA, Bienias JL,
In the meantime, the general public needs to be well Evans DA, et al. Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk
of incident Alzheimer disease. JAMA 2002;287:742-8.
informed about the links between complex mental activity 22. Fabrigoule C, Letenneur L, Dartigues JF, Zarrouk M, Commenges D,
and reduced dementia risk. Given the negligible potential Barberger-Gateau P. Social and leisure activities and risk of dementia: A
for harm, it is sensible to encourage all individuals to prospective longitudinal study. J Am Geriatr Soc 1995;43:485-90.
23. Valenzuela MJ, Sachdev P, Rundeck T, Bennett D. Cognitive leisure
increase their levels of complex, enjoyable, and engaging activities, but not watching TV, for future brain benefits. Neurology
cognitive activity for optimal brain health, particularly after 2006;67:729.
24. Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, Hall CB, Derby CA, Kuslansky G, et al.
retirement. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N Engl J Med
2003;348:2508-16.
25. Sitzer DI, Twamley EW, Jeste DV. Cognitive training in Alzheimer’s disease:
ACKNOWLEDGMENT A meta-analysis of the literature. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2006;114:75-90.
26. Rebok G, Carlson M, Langbaum J. Training and maintaining memory
We thank, National Health and Medical Research Council of abilities in healthy older adults: Traditional and novel approaches. J
Gerontol Series B 2007;62:53-61.
Australia program for the grant #350833. MV is a University of 27. Valenzuela M, Sachdev P. Can cognitive exercise prevent the onset of
New South Wales Vice Chancellor’s research fellow. dementia? A systematic review of randomized clinical trials with longitudinal
follow up. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008; in press.
28. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, Marsiske M, et al.
REFERENCES Effect of cognitive training interventions with older adults: A randomised
control trial. JAMA 2002;288:2271-81.
29. Willis SL, Tennstedt SL, Marsiske M, Ball K, Elias J, Koepke KM, et al.
1. Katzman R, Terry R, DeTeresa R, Brown T, Davies P, Fuld P, et al. Clinical, Long term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in
pathological and neurochemical changes in dementia: A subgroup with older adults. JAMA 2006;296:2805-14.
preserved mental status and numerous neocortical plaques. Ann Neurol 30. Mahncke HW, Connor BB, Appelman J, Ahsanuddin ON, Hardy JL, Wood
1988;23:138-44. RA, et al. Memory enhancement in healthy older adults using a brain
2. Satz P. Brain reserve capacity on symptom onset after brain injury: A plasticity-based training program: A randomised, controlled study. Proc
formulation and review of evidence for threshold theory. Neuropsychology Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006;103:12523-8.
1993;7:273-95. 31. Oswald W, Gunzelmann T, Rupprecht R, Hagen B. Differential effects of
3. Borenstein GA, Mortimer JA, Bowen JD, McCormick WC, McCurry SM, single versus combined cognitive and physical training with older adults:
Schellenberg GD, et al. Head circumference and incident Alzheimer’s The sima study in a 5-year perspective. Eur J Ageing 2006;3:179-92.
disease: Modification By apolipoprotein E. Neurology 2001;57:1453-60. 32. Derwinger A, Stigsdotter Neely A, Backman L. Design your own memory
4. Jorm AE, Creasey H, Broe A, Sulway M, Kos S, Dent O. The advantage strategies! self-generated strategy training versus mnemonic training in
of being broad-minded: Brain diameter and neuropsychological test old age: An 8-month follow-up. Neuropsychol Rehabil 2005;15:37-54.
performance in elderly war veterans. Personality Individual Differences 33. Scogin F, Bienias J. A three-year follow-up of older adult participants in a
1997;23:371-7. memory-skills training program. Psychol Aging 1988;3:334-7.
5. Schofield PW, Logroscino G, Andrews HF, Albert S, Stern Y. An association 34. Nithianantharajah J, Hannan AJ. Enriched environments, experience-
between head cicumference and Alzheimer’s disease in a population- dependent plasticity and disorders of the nervous system. Nat Rev
based study of aging and dementia. Neurology 1997;49:30-7. Neurosci 2006;7:697-709.
6. Mortimer JA. Brain reserve and the clinical expression of Alzheimer’s 35. Artola A, von Frijtag JC, Fermont PC, Gispen WH, Schrama LH, Kamal
disease. Geriatrics 1997;52:S50-3. A, et al. Long-lasting modulation of the induction of LTD and LTP in rat
7. Altman J, Wallace R, Anderson W, Das G. Behaviourally induced changes hippocampal CA1 by behavioural stress and environmental enrichment.
in length of cerebrum in rat. Dev Psychobiol 1968;1:112-7. Eur J Neurosci 2006;23:261-72.
8. Stern Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the 36. Naka F, Narita N, Okado N, Narita M. Modification of AMPA receptor
reserve concept. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2002;8:448-60. properties following environmental enrichment. Brain Dev 2005;27:275-8.
9. Mortimer J. Do psychosocial risk factors contribute to Alzheimer’s 37. Mohammed AH, Zhu S, Darmopil S, et al. Environmental enrichment and
disease? In: Henderson A, Henderson J, editors. Etiology of dementia of the brain. In: Hofman M, Boer G, Hotmaat A, et al, editors. Progress in

S20 Indian J Psychiatry 51: Supplement, January 2009


Valenzuela and Sachdev: Cognitive reserve and prevention of dementia

brain research. Elsevier Science; 2002. p. 109-33. in ceerenellar cortex of adult rats. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 1990;87:
38. Adlard PA, Perreau V, Engesser-Cesar C, Cotman CW. The time course 5568-72.
of induction of brain-derived neurotrophic factor mrna and protein in the 49. Draganski B, Gaser C, Busch V, Schuierer G, Bogdahn U, May A.
rat hippocampus following voluntary exercise. Neurosci Lett 2004;363: Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature
43-48. 2004;427:311-2.
39. Pham TM, Ickes B, Albeck D, Söderström S, Granholm AC, Mohammed 50. Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Scalf PE, Kim JS, Prakash R, McAuley E,
AH. Changes in brain nerve growth factor levels and nerve growth factor et al. Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J
receptors in rats exposed to environmental enrichment for one year. Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2006;61:1166-70.
Neuroscience 1999;94:279-86. 51. Haier R, Siegel B, Nuechterlein K. Cortical glucose metabolic rate
40. Lazarov O, Robinson J, Tang YP, Hairston IS, Korade-Mirnics Z, Lee VM, correlates of reasoning and attention studied with positron emission
et al. Environmental Enrichment reduces Aß levels and amyloid deposition tomography. Intelligence 1988;12:199-217.
Downloaded from http://journals.lww.com/indianjpsychiatry by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4

in transgenic mice. Cell 2005;120:701-13. 52. Olesen PJ, Westerberg H, Klingberg T. Increased prefrontal and parietal
XMi0hCywCX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC1y0abggQZXdgGj2MwlZLeI= on 03/25/2024

41. Adlard PA, Perreau VM, Pop V, Cotman CW. Voluntary exercise decreases activity after training of working memory. Nat Neurosci 2004;7:75-9.
amyloid load in a transgenic model of Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci 53. Moore CD, Cohen MX, Ranganath C. Neural mechanisms of expert skills
2005;25:4217-21. in visual working memory. J Neurosci 2006;26:11187-96.
42. Arendash GW, Garcia MF, Costa DA, Cracchiolo JR, Wefes IM, Potter 54. Cabeza R, Grady CL, Nyberg L, McIntosh AR, Tulving E, Kapur S, et al.
H. Environmental enrichment improves cognition in aged Alzheimer’s Age-related differences in neural activity during memory encoding and
transgenic mice despite stable B-amyloid deposition. Neuroreport retrieval: A positron emission tomography study. J Neurosci 1997;17:
2004;15:1751-4. 391-400.
43. Jankowsky JL, Melnikova T, Fadale DJ, Xu GM, Slunt HH, Gonzales V, 55. Grady CL, McIntosh AR, Beig S, Keightley ML, Burian H, Black SE.
et al. Environmental enrichment mitigates cognitive deficits in a mouse Evidence from functional neuroimaging of a compensatory prefrontal
model of Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci 2005;25:5217-24. network in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci 2003;23:986-93.
44. Rampon C, Jiang CH, Dong H, Tang YP, Lockhart DJ, Schultz PG, et al. 56. Rosen AC, Prull MW, O’Hara R, Race EA, Desmond JE, Glover GH,
Effects of environmental enrichment on gene expression in the brain. Proc et al. Variable effects of aging on frontal lobe contributions to memory.
Nat Acad Sci USA 2000;97:12880-4. Neuroreport 2002;13:2425-8.
45. Levi O, Jongen-Relo AL, Feldon J, Roses AD, Michaelson DM. Apoe4 57. Scarmeas N, Zarahn E, Anderson KE, Habeck CG, Hilton J, Flynn J, et al.
Impairs hippocampal plasticity isoform-specifically and blocks the Association of life activities with cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer disease.
environmental stimulation of synaptogensis and memory. Neurobiol Dis Arch Neurol 2003;60:359-65.
2003;13:273-82.
46. Scheff SW, Price DA. Synaptic pathology in Alzheimer’s disease: A review
of ultrastructural studies. Neurobiol Aging 2003;24:1029-46.
47. Kempermann G. Adult neurogenesis. New York: Oxford University Press; Source of Support: National Health and Medical Research
2006. Council of Australia program grant #350833.
48. Black JE, Isaacs KR, Anderson BJ, Alcantara AA, Greenough WT. Learning
causes synaptogenesis, whereas motor activity causes angiogenesis,
Conflict of Interest: None declared

Indian J Psychiatry 51: Supplement, January 2009 S21

You might also like