Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Using an electric car :

a situated, instrumented and emotional activity

Béatrice Cahour Claudine Nguyen Jean-François Forzy Christian Licoppe


C.N.R.S. LTCI Renault/Tél.ParisTech Renault SA/ DELT’A Télécom ParisTech
Télécom ParisTech Technocentre Renault Technocentre Renault SES department
46 rue Barrault 1, av du Golf 1, av du Golf 46 rue Barrault
75013 Paris FR 78288 Guyancourt 78 288 Guyancourt 75013 Paris FR
beatrice.cahour@teleco claudine.nguyen@renaul jean.forzy@renault.com Christian.licoppe@telec
m-paristech.fr t.com om-paristech.fr
1 INTRODUCTION1
ABSTRACT Using an electric car is an activity which is different from
using a classical car, especially because the electric
Motivation – The objective of the study was to specify how “autonomy”2 is more limited, and drivers may then be afraid
drivers deal with the autonomy of their car, be it traditional or of running out of energy. We will show here how this activity
electric, how they plan the recharge and how they check it of driving an electric car and dealing with its autonomy is a
while driving. cognitive but also emotional experience, highly dependent on
Research approach – For the first part of the study on the situational context and with a decisive importance of the
traditional cars use, we conducted 14 in-depth interviews and instruments available during the activity.
344 questionnaires were filled. For the second part of the We consider the driving activity as a user experience where
study on electric cars use, we lend an electric car to 9 subjects actions, cognitive activities, sensorial perceptions and
during two weeks; they filled a diary and videotaped the emotions are intimately mixed. “Experience can be seen as
interesting events during the driving activity. Then they had 3 the irreducible totality of people acting, sensing, thinking,
self-confrontation interviews to document their lived feeling and making sense in a setting, including their
experience, based on the video and on the diaries. perception and sensation of their own actions.” (McCarthy &
Findings/Design – People may have various emotional Wright, 2004, p.54). The cognition is then clearly embodied,
relations to the risk of breakdown, and it influences their emotional feelings are constitutive of all activities, and we
activity and how they deal with the risky situations, especially will see how emotions can have a role of action orientation
in terms of anticipative conduct, reflexive instruments which is still often ignored. Emotional states have a bodily
interpretation and avoidance. and a cognitive side and most authors studying the psychology
of emotions (Scherer & al, 1986) consider that they are
Research limitations – Qualitative study of VE use with 9
generally composed of (1) subjective feelings which can be
subjects
expressed verbally by the subjects, (2) expressive behaviours
Originality/Value – The research highlights the link between such as mimics, postures, prosody, (3) physiological reactions
cognitive and affective aspects of situated activities. such as variations of blood pressure or cardiac rhythm. Lots of
Take away message – the management of a risk depends on studies indicate nowadays that emotions orient cognition and
the affective relation to this risk, on the degree of uncertainty social relations, and have an impact on memory processes,
and on the instruments available to cope with it. judgments, creativity and cooperation (Cahour 2012). We will
show how the affective relation to a risk may impact the
Keywords activity, and how cognition and action are intimately tied to
Electric vehicle, activity, autonomy, recharge, uncertainty, emotions. The autonomy of a vehicle, especially when it is
worry, emotions, instruments, reflexivity, contextual situation. electric (because of the possibility of gaining energy), is a
source of uncertainty since we cannot generally know the
exact distance that we can drive without running out of
energy. It depends on the precision of the petrol gauge and
other instruments. Different studies indicate that uncertainty
Permission to make digital or hard copies of part or all of this work for may be an intense source of worry for those who are intolerant
personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are to it (Birrel & al, 2011, Burh & Dugas, 2006).
not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that
copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights We will also show how the management of the risk of
for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be breakdown with an electric vehicle is intimately linked to the
honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, to situational context (Suchman 1987) and on the way drivers
republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior interpret and react to the reflexive instruments which are
specific permission and/or a fee. available.
ECCE '12, August 28 - 31 2012, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
Copyright 2012 ACM 978-1-4503-1786-3/12/08…$15.00.
1
This study has been financed by Renault Department of
Research (PhD of C.Nguyen).
2
We use the term « autonomy » in the sense of the amount of
power available (petrol or battery range).

22
The objective of the study was to specify how drivers deal
with the autonomy of their car. It is not a central part of the
driving activity most of the time, but it is a prerequisite and
preoccupation which is more or less in the background, and
may become a central focus at some moments. It becomes a
central issue for the use of electric vehicles which have a more
limited capacity of ‘autonomy’. No studies being available on
this issue, in a first step we began studying this activity of
managing the energy with traditional cars and in a second
step we studied the drivers experience with electric cars.

2 METHODOLOGY
Our methodological approach is mostly based on “re-
situating” interviews, that is interviews limiting the
rationalisations and reconstructions by getting the subjects to
remember precisely the situation (Cahour & Salembier, 2012).
We helped the subject remembering as vividly as possible
specific situations (and not describe general situations), (1)
with specific techniques of questioning developed by
Vermersch (1994), such as recalling the context, avoiding
inductive questioning, keeping near the subject’s description,
avoiding “why” questions (2) and also with the video support
for the second part of the study.
For the first part of the study on traditional cars use, we
conducted 14 in-depth interviews (7 men, 7 women, 23 to 63 Figure 1 : In-car system to videotape and video-based
years old). After a general description of how they tend to interview.
manage the autonomy (“could you describe how you manage
the petrol refilling? do you wait until a certain point to go to
the station or not? Does it depend on the circumstances? Have 3 RESULTS3
you ever run out of petrol? ... all this sort of situations”), we In this section we present firstly the results concerning how
asked them to remember and describe the lived experiences of drivers deal with their petrol autonomy using a traditional car,
specific moments of this type. and secondly how they deal with the limited autonomy of an
electric vehicle.
We then analysed the interviews, and from the analysis we
constructed a questionnaire of 7 items (described later in the 3.1 Managing the autonomy of traditional
text) to get quantitative data on specific points, filled by 344 cars
drivers of 21 to 65 years old.
The analysis of the 14 in-depth interviews indicates that this
For the second part of the study on electric vehicles (EV) experience of managing the autonomy of a car is variable and
use, we lend an electric car to 9 subjects during two weeks (7 we could identify different styles of reactions: some drivers
men, 2 women, 24 to 55 years old, of the four styles); the lend are more apprehensive than others towards the risk of running
cars are of three different types and trends, their official out of petrol and tend to anticipate more the refill in a station,
electric range is from 150 km to 160 km and the time needed whereas some others will wait until the last moment to go and
to fully charge the battery is 7-8 hours. refill the vehicle.
(1) we gave them a diary to describe: 3.1.1 Contextual and personal determinants:
- The journeys : destination, planification, unexpected events, From the analysis of the 14 interviews we could identify the
renunciations and motivations; elements which determine such or such type of reaction
concerning the refilling of the car are the following. They are
- The recharge : mode, place, impressions and other remarks
a mix of personal tendencies (such as the affective relation to
- Other impressions or specific events with the VE the risk of breakdown or to the stations) and of contextual
(2) they had an in-car system to videotape the driving scene, elements such as the reliability of the instruments, the
the dashboard and the face of the subject, when they thought temporal priorities, the familiarity with such or such journey
that something noticeable happened about their driving. At the and the persons who may share the car.
end of days 1, 8 and 14 we had self-confrontation interviews, - The affective relation to the risk of breakdown: here the
based on the video of what the subject was seeing during the risk is to run out of energy, to be stopped in the middle of
driving, and based also on the diaries. The mode of nowhere, maybe in a dangerous place, to loose time, look
questioning is largely based on the Explicitation Interview stupid, and/or have to wait. For some persons it is a risk that
developed by Vermersch (1994) to produce “re-situating they absolutely want to avoid because it frightens them, it is a
interviews” where the subjects remember vividly the situation source of worry; for others it is not such a terrible situation,
and describe their phenomenological experience of it, with or not enough to avoid it absolutely. We have here an example of
without the trace of the activity (Cahour & Salembier 2012, a driver who clearly anticipates the refilling not to feel
Light 2006). The objective was to help them describe the way anxious:
they dealt with the question of the limited autonomy when
Ana : “ I’ve never broken down. And I’m only very, very
using the electric car.
occasionally low on fuel (...) I don’t want any trouble. I

3
This study has been financed by Renault Department of
Research (PhD of C.Nguyen).

23
don’t want to feel anxious because of it. So really, I plan also depends on the situational context. For instance most of
things ahead to avoid finding myself in a stressful them anticipate more the re-supplying before a long trip or
situation (…). So I mean, well, I reassure myself from the when they go in unfamiliar sites. The EV use will probably be
start. I don’t have to deal with that constraint. I’ve sorted still more sensitive to the length and familiarity of the
it out, so I can concentrate on the road, and on enjoying journeys.
the drive. When I don’t have that constraint anymore, then - Collective influence: the tendency of a driver to anticipate
I can drive along peacefully”. or not may be also transformed by the fact that he/she shares
It is probable that this relation to the risk of breakdown will the vehicle with someone else, the spouse or the parents for
not change much with EV. instance, but also the colleagues in the case of shared
- Disgust of the stations : Drivers may also be reluctant to professional vehicle. The others then influence the way the
stop in a filling station because they dislike the smell, the fact driver will manage the vehicle autonomy.
to have petrol on the hands, or even the possibilities of This variation between the drivers’ ways of managing the
explosion with the mobile phones. They consequently delay vehicle’s autonomy can be schematised through profiles; these
the re-supplying. The agreeability of the place where the styles are based on the tendency of anticipation of the re-
person has to stop impacts the management of the autonomy. filling activity before or after the signal alerting that the
Lise : “It’s always a little grotty, so, um I mean it often is. reserve has been reached and on the perceived risk.
Every time there were some plastic gloves available, I In order to get a quantitative idea of how this variation of the
would grab a pair. But sometimes there aren’t any, so … relation to the breakdown risk and refilling activity was
Your hands are filthy, so if you want to go shopping or distributed among a population larger than our 14 interviewed
anything... Even after using it, you put your hands back on drivers, we built a short questionnaire which has been filled
the wheel, and it’s, it’s not very nice. Or sometimes you by 344 drivers. It provides quantitative information about the
walk right into the fuel. That side of it, it’s not … I didn’t styles of reactions.
like … well, it wasn’t the nicest thing».
Noé : « It stinks. It sucks».
3.1.2 Different styles of activity and feelings
For EV situations, it puts into light the importance of regarding the non-autonomy risk:
designing pleasant places for the electricity charge, especially
With the questionnaire we specify the quantitative repartition
if the persons need to wait.
of the styles found in the qualitative interviews, and look
- The reliability of the reflexive instruments to evaluate the more precisely at the link between the anticipation tendency
remaining autonomy: the available instruments may be the and the worry concerning the limit of the vehicle autonomy.
gauge, the indicator of remaining kilometres, and the
• To the question of the questionaire “In general, in your
ecometer when one wants to drive in an economic way, more
daily life, you go and take some gasoline...”, the
slowly and smoothly. After entering in the reserve, the
“anticipators” (17,4% of our population) responds “far
knowledge of the remaining kilometres is generally vague,
before the signal of energy stock”. Clearly they are pro-
and some instruments may be unreliable or ambiguous in their
active to avoid the stress of the failure risk since, to the
meaning.
question “when the stock signal comes on, do you feel:
Chloé : « It’s quite random, because, um … My fuel level very serene, rather serene, nor serene nor worry, rather
lowers a lot faster in the second half of the tank than it does in worried, very worried”, 48,3% of them say “rather
the first. Say, I might do 400 km on the first half of the tank, worried” or very worried”.
and 300 on the second half. Hence the fact that I have run out
• The “moderate anticipators” (36,6% of the population)
on occasion, thinking I might finish the journey with a single
full tank. » tend to re-supply the vehicle “just before the stock
signal”. The autonomy management is more serene since
When the instruments are reliable and precise, then the drivers 22,2% of them are “rather worried” or “very worried”
feel safer for avoiding the risk of a failure. Some users who when the stock signal lights on.
prefer to anticipate also use internet services before the
journey to know how long they will drive, how much petrol • The drivers who are “reactive to the signal” (29,9%)
they will consume, and where are the station sites ; this generally wait “right after the stock signal (1 to 15 km)”
anticipation is also a way for being less worried. We will see for re-supplying. Only 6,9 % declare to be “rather
how this point is crucial for EV design. worried” or “very worried” when it lights on.

- Temporal priorities : some drivers say that the re-supplying • The “late refillers” (16,3%) who get some energy “far
in petrol is often not their priority, and even if they know that after the signal (more than 15 km)” are also only 7,1%
they should stop in a station, they can sometimes forget it, feeling “rather worried” or “very worried” when the
because they are late to go to a meeting for instance ; that is signal lights on. They run out of gasoline sometimes.
the case of Mireille who often runs out of petrol. Two results are surprising:
Mireille : “I was doing long days. In the evening, I was dead (1) The important diversity of reactions, some with a lot of
tired: Oh, I’ll do it (fill the tank) tomorrow, when I leave. In anticipation and some without much anticipation, with 16%
the morning, when you want to fill the tank and arrive on and 17% at the more extreme profiles. This means that drivers
time, I probably had an appointment on that particular really have different relations to this type of risk.
morning. So I take the car. I hear that little sound, you know, Consequently we cannot generalise too quickly by saying that
out of fuel. And I say to myself: Rats, I’m going to run out of drivers have globally such or such conduct regarding this
time. I’ll try and go to work. Then I’ll do it after the issue of their vehicle autonomy. It is necessary to refine the
meeting. ». description according to contextual elements but also to
This should work more or less the same for EV drivers. personal affective reactions. These individual tendencies to
avoid any risk of breakdown or to be tolerant to this
- Familiarity with journeys and station sites: even if the possibility is of course relevant for the use of an electric
drivers can generally say if they tend to anticipate or not, it vehicle.

24
(2) The fact that the emotional relation to the risk of running personal determinants explaining an anticipative or late
out of gasoline appears to be clearly linked to the decision of refilling, we analysed the real use of electric
proactive/reactive behaviour: the more drivers are anxious vehicles and the link with the styles.
regarding the risk of breakdown, the more they anticipate the The issue was to know how drivers manage the limited
action of refilling. The moment when one runs out of energy autonomy of EV (around 150 km) in their daily use. The style
is not totally controlled, there is always some uncertainty of affective relation to the risk of breakdown (from strong
regarding this risk. Several studies show that people who are worry to more indifference) should also play a role in the
not tolerant to uncertainty are more worried during uncertain activity and especially in the process of limiting or not the
situations (Birrel & al, 2011, Burh & Dugas, 2006). Though type of journeys that they plan to do, and the risk they take
most of the models of risk existing in ergonomics do not take concerning their autonomy. This issue of the autonomy is
into account this dimension (e.g. Wilde, 2002 ; Fuller, 1988): especially tricky for the EV use because it is more limited,
dealing with a risk is supposed to depend on a compromise and some drivers can be worried about that. It is still more
between costs/capabilities and performance but the emotional difficult to have precise estimations of the remaining
relations that each person can have towards such or such risk autonomy with an EV than it is already for traditional cars,
is not taken into account. We show here how the subjective because the consumption depends more on the topography
worry or indifference towards this risk, the personal construct (going up and down hill), and because the user may regain
that each driver has elaborated according to his fears, energy back, when they decelerate for instance.
priorities, disgusts etc, is an important part of his experience
which influences the way he takes decision and acts. Electric cars have been lent to nine subjects (of the four styles
with traditional cars). They used this car freely, as they
wished, filled a diary every day and videotaped the driving
3.1.3 Using various instruments to avoid the (see above); then we had self-confrontation interviews with
risk or to adapt to it them at day 1, 8 and 14 in order to see the evolution of the
appropriation of the new type of vehicle concerning the
Some drivers who anticipate and are afraid of the risk of
autonomy.
breakdown use instruments before the journey like web sites
(such as Mappy or Michelin) to have a clearer idea of the
journey, the number of kilometres and the places of the filling
stations on the way. They compare it with the remaining
3.2.1 Limiting the risky journeys
We observe that the subjects, especially the “anticipators”,
kilometres on their gauge and evaluate if they need to refill
generally use the EV for well-known journeys, mainly from
before the departure. They calculate as much as they can to
avoid any risk. home to the workplace, for visiting friends or for usual
shopping.
Yann : “a quick look on Mappy and I look at how many
Few of them took the risk to improvise, or only for small
kilometres I will have to travel, and then, depending on my
detours of some kilometres, for dropping a friend or for
petrol tank and its capacity, well I look if I need to refill or not
shopping. Those who tried unknown journeys prepared and
to go to the place”.
planned them carefully, for instance one “anticipator” who
Anticipation can also be based on GPS visual information had checked the distance to cover and the possibility to
during the drive. recharge when arriving, and one “late refiller” who had
Yann : “It’s true that with the GPS I knew that there was a checked the itinerary in advance.
station later, so I could stop at the next one (...) I knew that One of the subjects, “reactive to the signal”, preferred to
with the next one it was cheaper, it was at 25 km so I didn’t abandon the plan to go and visit different real estate agencies
hesitate since I had the autonomy”. because he did not know the precise distance he would have to
The drivers who do not anticipate much, can use “reflexive” cover, and preferred car-pooling.
instruments, which give them a dynamic feedback on their Only two subjects with the profile “late refillers” have taken
situation state, when they have few petrol left: the risk of driving the EV with very limited autonomy. This
Maelle : “when I am in a situation where I begin to be in the indicates that the styles of reactions with traditional car are
red zone, yes I switch on the instantaneous consumption to try similar with EV.
to, actually, consume a bit less but um, it’s more with the 3.2.2. The description of Omar’s journey: driving the
objective to force me to drive a bit slower”. Electric Vehicle with the risk of breakdown
Some “late refillers”, such as Marie for whom the breakdown We develop here a specific journey of Omar, one of those two
is considered as a repetitive fate, use instruments not to limit subjects who are “late refillers” and who took the risk to drive
the risk of breakdown but to cope with the breakdown with limited autonomy . This example indicates how the
situation itself : a can in the trunk and a pipe. driving experience is situated and anchored in the context, and
Marie : “So, since we break down all the time, now we have a how it is emotionally embodied. We will show how the
can in the trunk and well we thought : well so we’re going to activity and feelings of the driver depend on:
find some petrol you know, we have a can of five or six liters, - The reflexive instruments available and their
so we thought: we’re going to get petrol with the car of my interpretation (gauge, econometer, other signals) ; the
friend’s parents.” construction of their meaning depends on the style of
The situation is then de-dramatized; the risk becomes anticipation and worry,
predictable. - The geographic context (hills, proximity of the arrival);
the driver constructs a representation of it to anticipate
the evolution of his autonomy,
3.2 Managing the autonomy of Electric - The other participants’ reactions.
Vehicles Unsurprisingly Omar has a profile of “late refiller”, he
Once we had a clearer idea of the way drivers manage their
generally waits until the last moment to go and refill his
autonomy with traditional vehicles and of the contextual and

25
traditional vehicle. The interview took place at day 8 of the that he remains in the zone of limited consumption ; he also
lending, two days after the journey. We summarize the cuts off the instruments which consume electric energy: the
journey with some extracts of the interview. radio and the air-conditioning.
On a Sunday, Omar decided to go in a shopping area which He then imagines the possibility of a breakdown and the
was about 15 km from his home place, with his wife. He tells possible actions he could produce: “I would call a breakdown
us during the interview: “I had estimated that there was 15 km truck and they come to catch us on the side of the road (...) I
but that, potentially, I could do the round trip (...) I was rather call my assistance”.
confident”. But then “at about half of the journey, a little bit He arrives in the commercial zone and thinks about finding an
before, I saw that the last bars of the gauge were electric plug but abandons the idea. He also remembers that a
disappearing in a rather fast way (...) I realize that finally member of his family is leaving in a city nearby: “I wondered
there is a risk relatively important that I cannot do the return also if I wouldn’t stop at their place”. But finally he decides to
journey without breaking down”. try to come back home with the energy left. He has 15 km to
We can note that the activity and feeling of confidence is drive, with two bars left in the gauge but he is still optimistic
largely based on the reflexive instruments which reflect the and wants to believe that he can do it.
state of the vehicle: here it is first the bars of the gauge which “When I parked, there were still two bars (on the gauge) (...) I
give an indication of the autonomy left, and of course Omar is thought: well ok let’s go, with the foot a bit off (“pied léger”),
reactive to their evolution. it should be possible to come back home (...) I played a little
bit, to know the limits of the vehicle (...) but it is very risky
nevertheless, it was not very comfortable”
Bars Recharge symbol blinking
Omar only makes the manoeuvre to go out of the parking
place and he sees that he has lost one of the two bars: “I was
far less confident, it was far less comfortable”. But still he
decides to try to come back home, slowly : “very clearly then,
my speed was inferior to the traffic speed, I was the one who
plugged the traffic on the road (...) and I try also to imagine
progressively, um, to remember a bit the topology of the
journey, to know more particularly if I had to go up or down
hills, where I could gain a bit of energy, and I even try to see
globally the altitude between this point here and my place”.
Only one of the thirteen bars is remaining and Omar feels far
less confident and comfortable; he then develops a mental
activity for estimating if he could gain energy when going
down hills and tries to remember the topology of the future
journey. This is a mental activity to anticipate the following
Place where the tortoise appears states since the topography is important for the EV driving.
Figure 2 : Reflexive instruments on the instrument panel His wife then proposes to take a forbidden shortcut instead of
of Omar’s electric vehicle. On the left side, the gauge of taking a roundabout but he refuses. Then he says “on the seat
electric autonomy with the number of black bars which behind there is the PC with all the instruments so, pof! , I cut
decrease or increase (if energy gaining). The charge symbol it off; I preferred coming back than recording!”. Here again,
is blinking when a limited charge threshold is passed; a the wife looks more anxious to breakdown, and she is ready to
turtle appears when the remaining charge is very limited. On take other types of risks for avoiding the breakdown risk.
the right side, the number of remaining kilometres (130 Even the material of the researchers (to record the journeys) is
here). On the centre, the econometer which indicates the finally cut off.
consumption : the more the red needle is in the white zone Later on: “the little symbol (of the electric plug), there is a
on the right, the more the immediate consumption is high ; problem, okay, it is blinking but I don’t know, well, I don’t
in the green zone, it is correct; in the blue zone, the driver is know what I can do, that is... I understand that I am now in
regaining energy. the reserve but well, the reserve it is how many kilometres, I
don’t know at all, it’s a bit adventurous (...) it is stressful, not
even the obligation to pay attention to the driving, but mainly
When they see that the bars disappear rapidly, his wife the fact to be uncertain of being able to come back home”.
proposes him to go back home, he thinks about it but decides
to go on. Here we see that Omar’s rather serene style about Omar’s stress is growing when the symbol is blinking; he
the risk of breakdown impacts the action decision; someone does not know the exact meaning of this signal but, as he says,
more worried about this risk, such as his wife, would have it is the growing uncertainty to reach his place which is
avoided the risk. stressful. Omar could have interpreted it in a more dramatic
way and thought that it was the really end of the journey.
He goes on: “there was 2 or 3 km to arrive at the commercial Though he ignores the meaning of this visual signal in terms
zone (...) clearly I adopt a mode of driving which is more of kilometres.
fluent and much more eco (...) I take my foot a bit off the
accelerator (“pied léger” in French), consequently my speed He then has to go up hill and “I really wondered before this
is reduced (...) it was totally constraining (...) I tried, with the going uphill if I was not going to stop in the middle of it (...)
econometer, to look at what happened and not to pass the phew! First release (after it) and behind it I had to go
vertical position of the needle, you see, to keep in the eco zone downhill and also phew! Because progressively I get nearer
(...) then I am less and less sure to be able to come back home my house (...) when I am a few kilometres before my place I
(...) here I had cut off everything (radio, air-conditioning)” can accept more easily to let the vehicle on the side of the
road (...) then I go downhill for the second time (...) then here
Here Omar, who feels less confident, finds solutions to limit in a way I am a bit released because I am not far from home
the risk : eco-driving and gazes at the econometer to check

26
(...) there was an hill to go down, a straight line, and two interviews based on existing help systems are being analysed
kilometres with the village at the end” and will be used for innovative design of appropriate systems
The release expressed by Omar when he went up the hills and of information.
arrive rather near his village, indicates the tension that he felt
during the journey.
4 DISCUSSION
“When I arrive in the village clearly I saw the symbol with the The autonomy of a vehicle cannot be calculated with a high
tortoise lit, the last straight line I did it full charge, to see a bit precision and consequently it produces some uncertainty,
how was the vehicle reacting and it was still accelerating ; it especially with the EV, but also with the traditional cars. We
was euphoria, um, euphoria so to speak”. observed that the zone of extension of the worrying
A last symbol which appears is the one of a tortoise (which uncertainty may be very different from one driver to another.
meaning is also uncertain: you should drive very slowly or the Some anticipate and/or calculate a lot to avoid the risk of
speed will be automatically reduced?) but then Omar is not breakdown, whereas others are more confident, less
afraid anymore because he is very near his place, and he preoccupied by this issue, and come very close to breakdown.
cannot avoid the pleasure to finish by an acceleration to test if Their affective relation to this risk is different and their
the car was still accelerating. activity and decisions are consequently different. Ergonomic
This example of Omar’s journey describes clearly how: models of risk should take into account this important
dimension of the affects lived during the situation.
(1) the reflexive instruments are parts of the cognitive system
We observed that most of our EV users take the vehicle for
of the drivers (Hutchins 1994) but also how the interpretation
well-known and short journeys or calculate a lot to limit the
of the signals (bars disappearing, recharge symbol blinking,
risk. The only two drivers who drove with a very limited
tortoise) depends on the worry of the driver concerning the
autonomy were of the “late-refiller” style. We also saw that
risk of breakdown. Also the tendency to stay optimistic or the
the interpretation of the alerts and instruments depends on this
willing to abandon depends on these styles of reaction to the
style: Omar and his wife do not react the same to the loss of
risky situation: we can see how Omar and his wife react
differently and would not have taken the same decisions since bars. In another situation, an “anticipator” told us that for him,
one of the alarms was appearing too late.
she would have abandoned before reaching the destination.
Consequently, the need of the different users regarding the
(2) The activity depends on the evolution of the situational
risk of a limited autonomy is variable and one of the proposed
context, and especially here the geographical context (hills,
improvements is to design flexible alarms and adaptable tools
proximity of the arrival) since there is a possibility of
for knowing the level of autonomy, so that each user have
recharging under certain conditions; the driver constructs a
some choice about when and how they appear. An indication
representation of this context to anticipate the evolution of his
of the range of possible kilometres is also most reassuring for
autonomy and it is especially important for the EV since the
driver may regain energy when decelerating. the drivers (maybe more than just one number which is not
reliable). At least the maps indicating the distances and the
(3) The activity may depend also on the social context, the places where it is possible to charge are most helpful. 4
other participant’s reactions. We do not know much about the
The methodology presented here, particularly with Omar’s
wife’s comments during the journey (Omar did not want to
case, focuses on the lived experience during the driving
say much about it) and he has not been influenced by her
situation and it shows all the developments of the situated
suggestions, probably because he is not that afraid of risking a
activity, the role of the instruments and of the topography
breakdown. Someone who does not tolerate risks such as
which sustain the feelings and the driving activity of the
Omar would have probably considered the wife’s proposal of
abandoning the project. driver. One interest of the qualitative approach based on re-
situating interviews such as the “explicitation interview” or
This Omar case is in itself very informative on the on-going self-confrontation interview (based on the video) is the
processes of driving an EV in risky situations and it shows richness of the details of the on-going activity from the point
how it a situated, instrumented and emotional activity. of view of the subject. We obtain data on the
In a complementary study, we are going to present this case to phenomenological experience including actions, thoughts,
the others participants who used an EV, and ask them at emotions and sensorial perceptions. One limit is that these in-
different points of the story how they would have reacted depth approaches are time-consuming and it may be difficult
(actions, emotions, thoughts,...) if they were in this situation. to generalise the results obtained with a limited number of
We will then see if their style (anticipative-reactive) impacts subjects.
on the way they manage a risky situation.

5 REFERENCES
3.3 Last step: exploring new and adapted [1.] Birrell J., Meares K., Wilkinson A., Freeston M. (2011).
Toward a definition of intolerance of uncertainty: A
instruments for managing the review of factor analytical studies of the Intolerance of
autonomy Uncertainty Scale, Clinical Psychology review, 31, 1198-
To conclude this study, we asked the 9 subjects who used an 1208.
EV during two weeks to propose ideas of instrumentations for
[2.] Buhr, K., and Dugas, MJ. (2002). The intolerance of
helping them to anticipate and cope with the management of
uncertainty scale. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40.
the autonomy. As a base for reflexion, we proposed them two
931-945.
types of instruments which are developed by Renault, to
specify the remaining autonomy in kilometres, the dynamic [3.] Cahour, B. (2012). Emotions: characteristics, emergence
consumption, the points to recharge the battery, a map of the and circulation in interactional learning, in Baker, M.J.,
autonomy zone etc. These are used as supports for expressing
the new needs relative to this issue of autonomy which 4
More specific information about the design cannot be
becomes critic in the context of the EV. The results of these
developed here.

27
Andriessen, J. & Järvelä, S. Affective Learning Together: technology. Behaviour and Information Technology,
social, relational and affective dimensions of learning. 25(2), 175-187.
London: Routledge. [8.] McCarthy, J., and Wright, P. (2004). Technology as
[4.] Cahour, B. and Salembier, P. (2012). The user experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
phenomenological experience ; evoquing the lived [9.] Scherer, K. R., Wallbott, H. G., & Summerfield, A. B.
activity with re-situating interviews, CHI Conference, (1986). Experiencing emotion. A cross-cultural study.
Workshop ‘Theories behind UX research and how they Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
are used in practice’. [10.] Suchman, L. A. (1987). Plans and situated actions. The
problem of human-machine communication. Cambridge,
[5.] Fuller, R. (2005). Towards a general theory of driver England: Cambridge University Press.
behaviour. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 37, 461- [11.] Vermersch, P. (1994). L’entretien d’explicitation. Paris :
472. ESF.
[6.] Hutchins, E. (1994). Cognition in the wild, Cambridge [12.] Wilde, G.J.S. (2002). Does risk homeostasis theory have
MA: MIT Press. implications for road safety? British Medical Journal,
[7.] Light, A. (2006). Adding method to meaning. A 324, 1149-1152.
technique for exploring people’s experience with

28

You might also like