Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 4

Why do we perceive

logarithmically?

Why do small children place 3 halfway between 1 and 10? Why do two light bulbs not seem twice as bright as one?
Why do we perceive so many things logarithmically? Lav R. Varshney and John Z. Sun can explain it, through
evolution, the statistics of nature – and reducing error.

How we perceive the world perceiving numbers logarithmically rather than based on information theory for why sensory
linearly could give an evolutionary advantage: it perception is the way it is.
Whether it is hearing a predator approaching or could be more important to know whether it is The principle that animals are well adapted
a family member talking, whether it is perceiving five lions facing you or three than to know if the to their environments, whether in structure or
the size of a cartload of chimpanzees or a pride deer herd you are chasing contains 100 animals in behaviour, has been a powerful method to
of lions, whether it is seeing a ripe fruit or an ex- or just 98. But in fact perceptual systems of all answer the “why” questions of biology in formal
plosion, the ability to sense and respond to the kinds display a nonlinear relationship between mathematical ways. For example, the princi-
natural world is critical to our survival. Indeed, external stimulus and internal representation. If ple precisely predicts the facet size of insect
the world is a random place where all intensities we double the force on your hand, it will feel compound eyes by balancing resolution and
of sensory stimuli can arise, from smallest to like less than double the pressure. If we double diffraction for natural light; it predicts exactly
largest. We must register them all, from the buzz the salinity of water, the taste will not be twice when mammals shift their gait from walking to
of an insect to the boom of an avalanche or an as salty. Nonlinear scalings that give greater trotting to galloping so as to minimise energy
erupting volcano: our sensory perception must cost of movement. We can apply it to the struc-
allow us to efficiently represent the statistical ture of the brain as well: for example, one of us
distributions of the natural world. has previously argued that the physical micro-
If kindergarten children are asked to point to architecture of synapses in the brain is optimal
the correct location for a spoken number word Counting logarithmically could for memory storage capacity per unit volume3.
on a line segment labelled with endpoints 0 and give an evolutionary advantage The same principle applies to the way that
100, they place smaller numbers closer to 0 and the brain operates – the way that we receive,
larger numbers closer to 100, but they do not
– and could optimise error-free process and perceive information from the out-
distribute numbers evenly. They give more space estimation of how many lions side world. If we do it efficiently, we are at an
on the line to small numbers. The larger ones are you are facing obvious evolutionary advantage. Which invites
crammed into a narrow space at the ‘100’ end. the question: what architecture of information-
If they are given the numbers 1 to 10, they put processing within our brains would qualify as
3 at about halfway. In other words, they place “most efficient”?
numbers in a compressed logarithmic mapping. One answer might be “one that reduces er-
They perceive numbers logarithmically. perceptual resolution to less intense stimuli ror to a minimum”. Which invites the further
Fourth graders, however, map linearly rather are ubiquitous across animal species and across question: what sort of error needs to be thus
than logarithmically. But if one asks an adult sensory modalities: heaviness, pain, warmth, reduced?
member of the Mundurucu – an Amazonian in- taste, loudness, pitch, brightness, distance, time Here we can turn to the nineteenth-century
digenous group with a reduced numerical lexicon delay, and colour saturation, among others, are physicist and biologist Ernst Weber. He used to
and little formal education – to complete the all perceived this way. Moreover, these mappings ask how the structure of the nervous systems
same task, the logarithmic mapping occurs between observable stimulus and our internal leads to a functional representation of external
again. It seems that placing numbers evenly perception-space – these psychophysical scales space. This question led him to the first general
along a line is something that is learned, not and laws – are approximately logarithmic. results in experimental psychophysics: that the
innate. Without the intervention of critical With multifarious organisms adapted to a change in stimulus intensity, ΔS, that will be
educational experiences, the way we perceive variety of niches, biology is incredibly variable; just noticeable, ΔP, is a constant ratio K of the
numbers is logarithmic1. so why is the same psychophysical law present original stimulus intensity S:
Number is important to human survival – you in so many animals and so many sensory modes?
would want to know whether one lion is facing In our recent work with Grace Wang and Vivek ΔS
ΔP = K
you or several. Indeed, it could be argued that Goyal2 at MIT, we have proposed an explanation S

28 february2013 © 2013 The Royal Statistical Society


Two Mundurucu children – and how many butterflies? Credit: Stock Connection Blue/Alamy

In other words, we do not notice absolute for explaining – indeed for predicting – the loga- representation of continuous-valued signals
changes in stimuli; we notice relative changes. rithmic laws which seem to govern so much of must incur some noise or distortion. Perception
Which leads to an answer to the question above: our perception of the world. By applying our must obey this rule as well. Thus the ear may re-
the error that needs to be reduced in the brains optimisation approach to reducing the relative ceive sound over a range of frequencies, and will
of organisms such as ourselves is not absolute error in information-processing, we can produce transmit that information through communica-
error, but relative error. a model which shows why the Weber–Fechner tion channels to the brain, where it is processed
By solving Weber’s differential equation and law should apply, and why we perceive so many to form a “sensation”. But somewhere along the
broadly putting forth a philosophy that internal things logarithmically. way distortion – error – will have crept in. The
representations are tied to properties of the sensation that is the eventual output from the
external world, Gustav Fechner developed what brain must be as robust to – unaffected by – that
is now called the Weber–Fechner law4. It states Towards an optimality theory distortion as possible. In perception, as we have
that perceived intensity P is logarithmic to the seen, it is the relative error that is the critical
stimulus intensity S (above a minimal threshold If you are trying to minimise relative error, how one. We want to consider information-processing
of perception S0) – see box. It has become a should you go about doing it? In other words, methodologies that biological information-pro-
centrepiece of psychophysics. But until now it how should we go about formulating an optimal- cessing systems might use to optimise expected
has been an empirical law only. Theory to ex- ity theory for sensory perception? Information relative error in the presence of the noise or
plain why it should be has been lacking. theory, the statistical theory of communication, distortion that necessarily arises.
Invoking our optimisation approach to biolo- was first formulated by Claude Shannon in the In formulating a Bayesian model for under-
gy, we can argue that organisms are well adapted 1940s and provides a principled framework standing psychophysical scales we depend on a
to the statistics of natural external stimuli – the and mathematical language to study optimal branch of information theory called quantisation
distributions of intensities that assail our sen- informational systems, whether technological or theory. All humans quantise on a daily basis;
sory organs – and are also internally efficient biological. we often round to the nearest dollar or pound
for processing the information that results. This One of the central insights from the pre- at the supermarket or interpret approximate
simple principle provides a formal methodology history of information theory was that any statistics in news articles. Quantisation is also

february2013 29
A perceptual model that uses signal
The Weber–Fechner law states that, above a minimal threshold of perception S0, perceived compression
intensity P is logarithmic to stimulus intensity S:
S The quantisation model of perception is simple
P = K log and intuitive, and gives a beautifully concise re-
S0
lationship between the psychophysical scale and
Although not precisely true in all sensory regimes – indeed, several alternative psychophysical stimulus intensity distribution. As we have seen,
laws such as Stevens’s power law have been proposed – the logarithmic scaling of perception it provides scales of sensation that are optimal
has proven useful over the last century and a half. For example, dating to the heyday of the Bell for error. It accounts for why we feel pressures,
Telephone System and still in use today, speech representation for digital telephony has used a tastes, loudness, distance and other sensations
logarithmic scaling called the μ-law to match technology to its human end-users. An alternative on a logarithmic rather than an arithmetic scale.
logarithmic scaling, the A-law, is used in countries other than the United States and Japan. But it may not be suitable for all sensations.
The algorithms used in MP3 compression of music, and in JPEG compression of images, also use For example, numerosity is a perception that
logarithmic scaling to appropriately adjust signals for human perception. may not require sensory inputs that must pass
But why should the Weber–Fechner and related psychophysical laws exist? Previous through constrained communication channels.
explanations have invoked the chemistry of sensory receptors or the informational properties of Yet, as we have seen, numbers are still perceived
individual neurons in the brain, but did not make connections to the functional requirements of logarithmically. Is this phenomenon still a
sensory perception. Ernst Weber’s original question has until now been unanswered. consequence of optimality as derived through
quantisation theory? The answer turns out to
be yes – as long as the information is further
an important aspect of fundamental research that a range of sensory inputs gets mapped to a processed and stored in the brain for future use,
in science, where physical instruments can only single representative point within the brain. For as numbers often are.
measure a phenomenon to a small number of sig- example, even though there is a continuum of The key idea is that if the input to the quan-
nificant digits, or in engineering for applications sound intensities that raindrops make when they tiser is a continuous random variable, then the
where communicating or storing information hit the ground, our brains would only be able to output is a discrete random variable. Discrete
is costly. In most applications, quantisation is distinguish a finite number of sound levels. random variables are easy to compress, and there
thought of as a necessary evil, taking the simple We make another assumption: that the have been many successful algorithms developed
form of dropping less significant digits. However, quantisation at the perceptual level is uniform, on top of fundamental information theory, due
quantisation can be thought of as a more general meaning the spacings between elements of for example to the need to store language texts.
problem of compression: how can I represent the perception – of the perceived sound levels in Several studies have argued that the brain itself
real line with a finite number of points? our example – are the same. Figure 1 shows this can also use signal compression algorithms. Ap-
We have made the assumption that there schematically: a continuous range of sensations, plying signal compression to the output of the
is loss of information from where a stimulus S, is mapped by the quantiser function to an quantiser proposed above yields an amazing
is measured at the brain’s periphery to when evenly spaced set of discrete perceptions P. result: the optimal choice of perception space is
it is perceived at higher cognitive levels due The mathematics of our argument is not com- always logarithmic, regardless of the distribution
plex; Interested readers can find it in our Journal of the stimulus intensity distribution. If com-
of Mathematical Psychology paper3. It turns out pression is used, the optimality of logarithmic
that a quantised architecture in the brain that is
Logarithmic mapping within the Bayes optimal for maintaining immunity to rela-
tive errors in interpreting signals would map the P
brain minimises relative error in output – the perceptual space – approximately
perception logarithmically. Hence the logarithmic spacing
of the intervals along the horizontal axis in
Figure 1. One might expect the optimal map-
ping strategy would depend on the statistical
P = C(S)
to physical constraints. Our hypothesis is that distribution of the input and it does. Remark-
efficient communication through this channel ably – most remarkably – it does not depend
employs quantisation, and that the robustness much. Logarithmic perception turns out to be
of the system can be studied under a Bayesian nearly optimal no matter what the distribution
framework. Discretising a continuous signal us- of intensities of the incoming signal, as long as
ing a finite number of points leads to stability it is a power law. Remarkably also, it turns out
in information representation as well as robust- that sensations corresponding to many natural
ness to noise and mismatch. We can expect our phenomena have statistical distributions that do S
information-processing-efficient brain to display indeed obey a power law over a range of intensi- Figure 1. Quantisation model of psychophysical
those qualities. ties that are of behavioural interest. George Zipf scales. S represents sensory stimulation, such as
sound, arriving in a continuous range of intensities.
We need a model of the brain in which a few had first shown that many types of data in the
Each intensity range of stimulation is mapped to a
values are communicated well instead of a model physical and social sciences can be well modelled discrete point in our perception. Any sound within
where a wide range are communicated noisily. It using this power-law statistical distribution5. So the shaded range of intensity is perceived as the
will be a quantisation model. Our assumptions perception of sound, brightness, pressure and so single blue circle of loudness. The perception points
imply that the brain can actually only distinguish on is optimal for relative error if the perception are distributed linearly; the sensation ranges are
a discrete set of perceptual levels. This means scale is logarithmic. distributed logarithmically

30 february2013
Relative Frequency of Occurence

Relative Frequency of Occurence


1 1

Predicted Psychophysical Scale

Predicted Psychophysical Scale


6 C(s) C(s)

4
4
0.5 0.5

fS(s) 2
2

fS(s)
0 0 0 0
60 45 50 55
Stimulus Intensity (dB) Stimulus Intensity (dB SPL)
Figure 2. Predicted psychophysical scale C(s) from empirical intensity distribution Figure 3. Predicted psychophysical scale C(s) from empirical intensity distribution
fS(s) of rainforest sounds fS(s) of human speech

scaling laws holds for any stimulus distribution. mammals from commercially available CDs; the say that people and animals are good processors
The statistics of the input do not factor into the human data set is of a male speaker reciting Eng- of statistical signals – or at least our sensing
psychophysical scale. lish sentences. In both data sets, we removed apparatus seems to have evolved to be so.
A model that includes signal compression silences and then extracted sound intensity over Broadly speaking, our findings support the
seems plausible for perception of numbers, but small time windows. We then estimated the dis- possibility that animals have information-
may be less meaningful when connected to tribution of sound intensities using the empiri- theoretically optimal signal acquisition and
external stimuli because properly compressing cal data. Feeding this empirical distribution of perception structures at a cognitive level. Going
signals may require long latency and such infor- sound intensities into our quantisation model, forward, it is important to make further experi-
mation can be very time-sensitive. we can predict the best psychophysical scale for mental tests of our optimality hypothesis. For
minimising expected relative error. example, there are certain specialised sensory
Our results are shown in Figures 2 and 3. They modes that are known experimentally not to
Returning to the Amazonian jungle demonstrate that the psychophysical scale – the have logarithmic psychophysical scales. Night
perceived loudness – is approximately linear vision is one. Optimising for the corresponding
We have described two models that yield psycho- when the stimulus intensity is plotted on a log stimulus distributions would provide a way to
physical scales as Bayes optimal mappings under scale, which does indeed indicate that, when the verify or falsify our optimal information-process-
constraints of communication or of compression expected relative error in a signal is minimised, ing hypothesis.
and storage. For the first model, power-law dis- there is an approximate logarithmic relationship
tributions of stimuli do indeed yield the Weber– between the stimulus and perception. References
Fechner logarithmic law, but we might wonder 1. Dehaene, Stanislas (2011) The Number
what happens when we look at actual, empirical, Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics, Oxford
stimulus data. After all, data is a true test of Conclusion University Press.
theoretical predictions. 2. Sun, John Z., Wang, Grace I., Goyal, Vivek
One well-studied sensation is the loudness of The field of psychophysics considers questions of K. and Varshney, Lav R. (2013) A framework for
sound. Loudness is one auditory cue that conveys human sensory perception, and one of its most Bayesian optimality of psychophysical laws, Journal
information related to mating rituals, predator robust findings has been the Weber–Fechner of Mathematical Psychology, in press.
warnings, and locations for food. It is likely that laws. It has helped us understand how we per- 3. Varshney, Lav R., Sjostrom, P. J. and
­Chklovskii, D. B. (2006) Optimal information storage
auditory systems have evolved to process natural ceive the world. In putting forth an optimality
in noisy synapses under resource constraints, Neuron,
sounds efficiently, as it is essential to survival theory for understanding why we perceive things 52(3), 409–423.
and reproduction. Experiments have shown that as we do, we have come to a few general ideas. 4. Fechner, Gustav Theodor (1860) Elemente
the human ear can perceive loudness at differ- Our work points out the importance of the der Psychophysik, Leipzig: Breitkopf & Härtel, 1860.
ences of about 1 dB, with small deviations at statistics of the stimulus in interpreting psycho- 5. Zipf, George K. (1949) Human Behavior
the extremal ranges; this is consistent with the physical laws. Indeed, our theory allows one to and the Principle of Least Effort, Cambridge, MA:
Weber–Fechner law. predict the logarithmic scales of our perceptions Addison-Wesley.
To test our theory of optimal psychophysical simply from the statistics of the natural world.
scales, we return to the Amazonian rainforest of Just as Gustav Fechner had suggested in the Lav R. Varshney is at the IBM T. J. Watson Research
the Mundurucu: we tested our model on data sets early days, internal representations are funda- Center. John Z. Sun is in Vivek Goyal’s Signal Transfor-
of animal vocalisation and human speech. The mentally intertwined with the external world mation and Information Representation group at the
animal data set features recordings of rainforest they describe. With an optimality result, one can MIT Research Laboratory of Electronics.

february2013 31

You might also like