Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

714587

research-article2017
AUT0010.1177/1362361317714587AutismGeorge and Stokes

Original Article

Autism

Gender identity and sexual orientation 1­–13


© The Author(s) 2017
Reprints and permissions:
in autism spectrum disorder sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1362361317714587
https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361317714587
journals.sagepub.com/home/aut

Rita George and Mark A Stokes

Abstract
Clinical impressions indicate that there is an overrepresentation of gender-dysphoria within the autism spectrum
disorder. However, little is presently known about the demographics of gender-identity issues in autism spectrum
disorder. Based upon what little is known, we hypothesized that there would be an increased prevalence of gender-
dysphoria among those with autism spectrum disorder compared to a typically developing population. We surveyed
gender-dysphoria with the Gender-Identity/Gender-Dysphoria Questionnaire among 90 males and 219 females with
autism spectrum disorder and compared these rates to those of 103 males and 158 females without autism spectrum
disorder. When compared to typically developing individuals, autistic individuals reported a higher number of gender-
dysphoric traits. Rates of gender-dysphoria in the group with autism spectrum disorder were significantly higher than
reported in the wider population. Mediation analysis found that the relationship between autistic traits and sexual
orientation was mediated by gender-dysphoric traits. Results suggest that autism spectrum disorder presents a unique
experience to the formation and consolidation of gender identity, and for some autistic individuals, their sexual
orientation relates to their gender experience. It is important that clinicians working with autism spectrum disorder
are aware of the gender-diversity in this population so that the necessary support for healthy socio-sexual functioning
and mental well-being is provided.

Keywords
autism spectrum disorder, gender-dysphoria, gender identity, heterosexuality, homosexuality, lesbian, gay, bisexual,
and transgender, sexual orientation

Introduction
Gender is not inherently defined by one’s physical anat- discontentment over the incongruence between their self-
omy. It is a psychological identification of oneself as a perceived and assigned gender, leading to significant dis-
man, a woman, both, neither or otherwise (Prince, 2005). tress to the person, challenges in social or occupational
Children are generally cognizant of their gender between functioning, and a desire to live a cross-gender life
the ages 18 months and 3 years (Martin and Ruble, 2010), (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). It is estimated
and by the beginning of school years, most children will that between 0.005% and 0.014% of natal males and
have achieved a sense of their gender identity and a certain 0.002%–0.003% of natal females would be diagnosed
degree of gender constancy, at which time children begin with GD, based on current diagnostic criteria (Zucker
to realize that gender is a permanent state that cannot be and Lawrence, 2009). Expressions of GD include a pref-
altered by a change of clothing or activity (Paikoff and erence for cross-dressing (dressing up in clothes typically
Brooks-Gunn, 1991). By the age of 4 years, children typi- worn by the opposite sex as defined by the person’s cul-
cally outline preferences for same-sex companions, gen- tural norms), cross-gender roles, and additionally in
dered-activities, and sex-typed play (Rosenfield and
Wasserman, 1993).
While generally an individual’s gender identity corre- Deakin University, Australia
lates with the gender roles that a given society considers
Corresponding author:
appropriate for males and females, this is not always the Mark A Stokes, School of Psychology, Deakin University, 225 Burwood
case. Gender-dysphoria (GD) is a clinical condition Highway, Burwood, VIC 3125, Australia.
where the individual experiences a persistent sense of Email: mark.stokes@deakin.edu.au
2 Autism 00(0)

children, make-believe play and a strong inclination for Stokes, 2016; Gilmour et al., 2012), where neural mascu-
play-mates of the opposite sex. Importantly, a handful of linization in some females might contribute toward attrac-
case studies (Galluci and Schmidt, 2005; Kraemer et al., tion to other females. It is speculated that the common
2005; Landén and Rasmussen, 1997; Mukaddes, 2002; denominator between female ASD and female GD might
Tateno et al., 2008) and empirical reports suggest an be an excess of fT (Hines et al., 2004).
association between autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and Another study that assessed for AQ traits in a clinical
GD (De Vries et al., 2010; Jones et al., 2012; Pasterski population diagnosed with GD also found evidence of an
et al., 2014). association between ASD and GD; 7.1% of the females
Recently, it has been apparent that there may be some with GD (N = 28) and 4.8% of males with GD (N = 63) met
overlap between ASD and GD. For instance, researchers screening diagnostic cut off based on their AQ scores
from the Netherlands (DeVries et al., 2010) found a higher (Pasterski et al., 2014). This finding was different from
incidence of ASD (7.8%) in a sample of 204 children Jones et al.’s (2011) where only females with GD reported
referred to a gender-identity clinic for management of their higher AQ scores than TD individuals. Pasterski et al.
GD. This rate is higher than the prevalence rate of ASD in noted that individuals with GD who met the screening cut-
the general population which is estimated to be between off for ASD reported an onset of GD after puberty. On the
0.6% and 2% (Blumberg et al., 2013; Fombonne, 2005). contrary, Pasterski et al. (2011) report that gender-dys-
DeVries et al. recognized the substantial variance in GD phoric ideation is often expressed in early-to-middle child-
symptoms displayed by those with ASD, where a diagno- hood when gender-segregation or the general tendency for
sis of Gender-Dysphoria–Not Otherwise Specified children to prefer interactions with their same-sex is at its
(GD-NOS) was frequently issued. GD-NOS is given when peak. The literature also points to the role of social naivéte
the cross-gender behavior and interests were atypical in unsuccessful relationships among individuals with ASD
(interest in clothing items of the opposite sex as a prefer- (Howlin et al., 2000). Pasterski et al. (2014) speculated
ence for specific sensory input typical of ASD; beliefs that that this might falsely lead some individuals to attribute
any existing social difficulties stem from GD, despite an their social challenges to their gender and believe that they
absence of any cross-gendered behavior; and transvestic would better fit with the opposite gender. Other authors
fetishism or sexual arousal when handling or wearing similarly suggest that certain behavioral and psychological
female clothing). characteristics of autism might mimic GD-like symptoms
Another group (Jones et al., 2011) examined the co- or even lead to the development of a clinical GD (De Vries
occurrence of GD and ASD in a group of adults with GD, et al., 2010; Jones et al., 2011).
by comparing scores on a measure of autistic traits, the While some research has assessed for ASD among clin-
Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ; Baron-Cohen et al., ical populations treated for GD, newer research by Strang
2001). Jones et al. found that when compared to typically et al. (2014) measured the desire to be the opposite gender
developing (TD) females, females with GD reported a in a population of 147 children with ASD. On analyzing
higher mean AQ, but males with GD did not differ from parental responses to the item “Wishes to be the opposite
TD males. Jones et al. suggested the increased number of sex” on the Child Behavior Check List (CBCL), it was
autistic traits among their female-to-male transsexual sub- noted that the desire to be the opposite gender was 7.6
jects was due to the Extreme Male Brain (EMB; Baron- times more common in the ASD group than among non-
Cohen, 2002) theory of autism. EMB theory posits that ASD participants. However, the study assessed children
autistic females are hyper-masculinized in specific aspects only. The desire to be opposite gender in children has been
of their cognition and behavior, related to elevated levels known to often decline when they reach puberty, thus lim-
of fetal testosterone (fT; Auyeung et al., 2009). At the same iting generalizability to adult populations. Prospective
time, a positive correlation has been demonstrated in neu- studies on GD indicate a likely resolution of GD symp-
robiological literature between fT and autistic traits. It is toms with increasing age through the adoption of a sexual
suggested that some autistic females may more readily orientation that is congruent with their experienced gender
identify with the male gender and may face challenges in identity (Bailey and Zucker, 1995; Rieger et al., 2008).
assimilating into a typical female peer group, which in a Higher rates of minority sexual orientations are reported
minority of cases may lead to the development of a GD. among individuals with GD (Drummond et al., 2008;
Additionally, there are a striking number of similarities Wallien et al., 2008).
between some autistic girls and girls with GD (Green and Bejerot and Eriksson (2014) similarly found a gender-
Young, 2001; Knickmeyer et al., 2008) such as cross-sex atypical pattern in their sample of 50 adults with ASD
typical play, reduced empathy and social skills, and hand- when compared to 53 TD individuals in a Swedish popula-
edness patterns. Furthermore, higher prevalence rates of tion. Males were less masculine and females were less
homosexuality and bisexuality have been reported among feminine compared to TD participants, when tested on the
females with GD (Drummond et al., 2008) as well as Bem Sex Role Inventory (Bem, 1974). It was also found
among autistic females (Bejerot et al., 2014; George and that participants with GD scored higher on the AQ
George and Stokes 3

compared to TD participants. Another study by the same Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the study sample.
group of researchers found a negative correlation between
ASD (N = 310) TD (N = 261)
AQ traits and body gender coherence. Body gender coher-
ence was estimated by assessing digital photographs and Race/ethnicity (%)
voice samples of the participants for gender-typicality Caucasian 81.6 71.4
(Bejerot et al., 2012). However, the use of a non-validated African-American 0.6 1.1
instrument to measure gender identity in a purely Swedish Hispanic 1.0 2.2
sample limits the generalizability of the study findings. Latino 0.3 0.5
The comorbidity of ASD and GD is worth noting as the Middle-Eastern 0.0 3.8
prevalence of both conditions is reasonably low. Aside Asian 1.9 8.1
from a few case reports that have indicated an association Pacific Islander 0.0 0.0
 Indigenous or Australian 0.3 0.0
between ASD and GD, and empirical studies that pointed
Aboriginal
to an increased number of autistic traits among clinical GD
Multiracial 8.1 7.0
populations, the scant evidence base that specifically Other/rather not say 6.2 6.0
assessed for gender-dysphoric traits (GDT) in ASD has Education level (%)
yielded findings limited by sample restrictions and meth- Primary school 2.7 1.3
odology. Thus, there was a need for a larger study with a High school 17.7 24.2
broader sampling methodology to explore the association Trade/vocational school 7.4 5.8
between ASD and cross-gender ideation, operationalized Some university 29.7 25.5
by measuring GDT through the use of validated psycho- Bachelor’s degree 23.2 25.8
metric measures. Previously cited literature indicates that Master’s degree 10.3 11.9
individuals with GD have an elevated number of autistic Doctoral degree 3.5 2.9
traits (Jones et al., 2012; Pasterski et al., 2014). This study Other/not sure 5.5 2.6
investigated this in the reverse direction and consequently
ASD: autism spectrum disorder; TD: typically developing.
predicted that autistic individuals would demonstrate
higher levels of GDT compared to TD individuals. Based
on observations of a large number of GD-NOS diagnoses sexual orientation and autistic traits. It was accordingly
among individuals with ASD (De Vries et al., 2010), a hypothesized that GDT will mediate between an individu-
diagnosis rendered when GD symptoms are sub-clinical, al’s autistic traits and their sexual orientation.
this study investigated for cross-gender ideation by meas-
uring GDT rather than GD diagnoses, as a continuum
might provide more statistical power than a categorical Method
approach. Moreover, measuring GDT would capture any Participants
substantial variations across the sample, without any limi-
tations imposed by a categorical approach. One study This study compared 261 TD individuals (Mage = 30.20,
found sex-specific differences in the association between standard deviation (SD) = 11.92; 103 males and 158
ASD and GD (Jones et al., 2011), while another did not females) with 310 individuals with a diagnosis of ASD
(Pasterski et al., 2014). This study chose to further investi- (Mage = 31.01, SD = 11.37; 90 males, 219 females, and 1
gate this by analyzing rates of GDT separately between individual who was intersex). The individual with an inter-
males and females by diagnosis. Based on the EMB the- sex identity was excluded from relevant analyses that
ory, it was hypothesized that autistic females would report examined sexuality by birth-sex group. Ethnic background
higher levels of GDT than males with ASD. Furthermore, is reported in Table 1. Additionally, the sample was largely
the literature suggests a high prevalence rate of non-heter- highly educated (see Table 1). There were no significant
osexual orientations among those with GD (Drummond differences between the individuals with and without an
et al., 2008; Wallien and Cohen-Kattenis, 2008) and with ASD on ethnicity or their levels of education.
ASD (George and Stokes, 2016; Gilmour et al., 2012), as
well as an elevated number of autistic traits among those Materials
with GD. While the literature informs us that autistic traits
are elevated among individuals with GD, we do not know AQ. The AQ (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001) is a screening tool
the relationship between autistic traits and GDT at this which provides a continuous, quantitative measure of
stage. This study affords the opportunity to study the rela- autistic traits. The questionnaire comprises 50 items, each
tionship of all three variables: GDT, autistic traits, and of which is in a forced-choice format. Each question allows
sexual orientation together, to better understand any poten- the respondent to indicate “definitely agree,” “slightly
tial associations between the constructs. Thus, this study agree,” “slightly disagree,” and “definitely disagree.”
explored whether GDT had a mediation effect between Approximately half the questions are worded to prompt an
4 Autism 00(0)

“agree” response from TD individuals, and half to prompt and importance of partner-gender in a romantic or sexual
a “disagree” response. The items cover five different relationship. This was followed by administration of the
domains associated with ASD: social skills, communica- different psychometric measures, presented below in the
tion skills, imagination, attention to detail, and cognitive- order they appeared in the survey.
inflexibility. Evidence for convergent validity is
demonstrated through the strong positive correlation of the
Statistical analyses
AQ with the Social Responsiveness Scale (r = 0.64; Con-
stantino and Gruber, 2005), which is another continuous Chi-square tests were used to examine the relationship
quantitative measure of ASD symptoms. Test–retest relia- between different variables, including diagnosis and gender
bility of the AQ has been reported as being substantial identity, diagnosis and use of hormone replacement therapy
(r = 0.95; Broadbent et al., 2013). At a cut-off score of 32, (HRT) in management of gender-identity issues, diagnosis
the sensitivity of the AQ is 0.76 and the specificity is 0.74 and cohabitation, and diagnosis and importance of gender
(Woodbury-Smith et al., 2005). A cut-off score of 32 identity of romantic/sexual partner as a factor in decision-
(Baron-Cohen et al., 2001) has been used in this study to making. Effect sizes for these tests are reported as phi (ϕ),
screen individuals with a diagnosis of ASD. which estimates the proportion of explained variance
(Howell, 2013). A number of comparisons were made
Gender-Identity/Gender-Dysphoria Questionnaire for adoles- between the TD and ASD groups, and across birth-sex.
cents and adults. The Gender-Identity/Gender-Dysphoria These used independent samples t-tests and included tests to
Questionnaire for Adolescents and Adults (GIDYQ; establish a difference in autistic traits between diagnostic
Deogracias et al., 2007) is a standardized 27-item instru- groups and to evaluate whether there were differences
ment, which assesses cross-gender behavior and different between diagnostic groups for GDT. Furthermore, the test
aspects of GD among adolescents and adults, with the past was also used to evaluate the hypothesis that autistic females
12 months as the time frame. Questions have parallel male with ASD would report higher GDT than males with ASD.
and female versions, and participants were administered An independent sample t-test was also used to examine
the version according to their reported birth-sex. The ques- whether individuals with a non-heterosexual orientation
tionnaire includes 13 items that assess subjective aspects, would report higher GDT than individuals with a hetero-
such as “Are you satisfied with being a man/woman?”; 9 sexual orientation. Sexual orientation was coded as a dichot-
items that investigate social aspects, such as “Have you omous variable with two categories; heterosexual and
ever presented yourself as a man/woman at social gather- non-heterosexual orientations. The non-heterosexual orien-
ings?”; 3 items that measure somatic aspects, such as tation category included all non-heterosexual response
“Have you taken hormone treatments to change your gen- options (lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, questioning,
dered-appearance?”; and 2 items for socio-legal aspects, asexual, pansexual, intersexual, and other). Effect sizes for
such as “Have you bothered with having to identify as these tests are reported as Cohen’s d, which estimates the
male/female on official forms such as your driver’s license proportion of an SD that is explained by the independent
or your passport?” The response options are coded along a variables (IVs) (Howell, 2013). A one-way analysis of vari-
5-point scale as 1 (always), 2 (often), 3 (sometimes), 4 ance (ANOVA) was undertaken to investigate how levels of
(rarely), or 5 (never). A cut-off mean score of 3.00 is rec- GDT differed between different sexual orientations. A new
ommended by the authors to discriminate between indi- variable for sexual orientation was computed, as some of the
viduals with and without GD, which has a sensitivity of categories had very few participants. The new variable had
90.4% for clients with GD and a specificity of 99.7% for five categories: homosexuality (gay and lesbian), bisexual-
clients without GD (Deogracias et al., 2007). ity, heterosexuality, asexuality, and other (transgender, ques-
tioning, pansexual, intersexual, other). To test the hypothesis
that GDT would mediate between an individual’s autistic
Procedure traits and their sexual orientation, a mediation analysis
Upon receiving ethical approval for the research from (PROCESS, 2013 Andrew F. Hayes, http://www.afhayes.
Deakin University Human Research Ethics Committee com/) was undertaken to assess the relation between autistic
(DUHREC 2014-008), national and international autism traits and sexual orientation, where GDT was assessed as a
organizations were contacted. TD participants were mediator. Effect sizes are reported as b-weights, which indi-
recruited by word of mouth and through social media such cates the change required in the IV to achieve a single unit
as Facebook. Participants completed the data collection of change in the dependent variable (DV) (Howell, 2013).
online and first completed a series of demographic ques-
tions related to their age, country of residence, race, level
Demographic trends
of education, birth-sex, diagnostic status, comorbid medi-
cal/psychiatric conditions, cohabitation status, gender Participants in the ASD group reported significantly
identity, hormone replacement related to gender identity, more traits characteristic of autism assessed by the AQ
George and Stokes 5

Table 2. Gender identities reported by individuals in the ASD and TD groups by birth-sex.

Gender identity ASD TD

Birth-sex male Birth-sex female Birth-sex male Birth-sex female

% (N = 90) % (N = 219) % (N = 103) % (N = 158)


Man 77.8 2.3 93.1 1.3
Transgender 7.8 2.3 3.9 0.6
Woman 7.8 67.1 0.0 87.3
Bigendered 2.2 3.3 1.0 3.8
Cross-dresser 1.1 0.0 0.0 0.0
Genderqueer 1.1 12.0 0.0 5.1
Other 3.3 13.4 2.0 1.9

ASD: autism spectrum disorder; TD: typically developing.

factor to their decision-making (χ2(1) = 57.83, p < 0.001,


ϕ = 0.32). Two females reported having a diagnosis of con-
genital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH), one with and one with-
out ASD. The autistic female reported an asexual
orientation, while the TD female reported being pansexual.
Both described their gender identity as a man.

Results
Data screening
Data were screened for missing values (of which there
were none), outliers, and normality. Independent samples
Figure 1. Gender identity by relative proportion reported by t-tests were conducted to analyze GD scores and AQ scores
respondents by diagnosis and birth-sex. between groups with and without an ASD. Assessments of
normality revealed significant skew and kurtosis on the
AQ and GD scales within groups. Examination of histo-
(t(460.57) = 21.01, p < 0.001, d = 1.96; M = 34.98, SD = 7.29) grams revealed that violations were due to a high level of
than participants in the TD group (M = 19.11, SD = 10.20). autistic traits and GD symptoms among transgender peo-
Cronbach’s alpha for the current sample on the AQ scale ple in each group. Based on reviewed literature, this non-
was 0.93. normality was not unexpected (Jones et al., 2011; Pasterski
Comparisons were undertaken between diagnostic et al., 2014). Sample sizes within each group were greater
groups on various demographic variables. Results indi- than 250 cases. Tabachnik and Fidell (2001) advise that
cated that gender identity was contingent on diagnosis non-normality is less of an issue under such conditions.
(χ2(6) = 34.06, p < 0.001, ϕ = 0.25). Autistic individuals Data were thus retained in the analysis untransformed.
reported a more diverse range of gender identities. Table Where violations of homogeneity of variance were
2 and Figure 1 show the percentages of the different gen- detected, as indicated by Levene’s homogeneity test,
der identities reported by males and females in the study. degrees of freedom were accordingly adjusted. ANOVAs
Gender diversity was overall greater among females than were conducted to analyze socializing patterns between
males. groups based on GDT status.
The results of other comparisons on demographic vari-
ables between individuals in the ASD and TD groups are
Hypothesis testing
presented in Table 3 and shows number of individuals
within each category for the two birth-sexes separately. Comparisons were made on total scores on the GIDYQ
Autistic individuals were more likely to utilize HRT related and scores on the different subscales of the GIDYQ.
to their gender identity than TD participants (χ2(1) = 6.99, Levene’s test for equality of variances indicated unequal
p = 0.006, ϕ = 0.11). Furthermore, when compared to TD group variances for each of the variables tested. The
participants, autistic individuals were less likely to report adjusted degrees of freedom test were used to address this
cohabitation (χ2(1) = 49.81, p < 0.001, ϕ = 0.29) and the violated assumption. The two groups significantly differed
gender of their romantic/sexual partner as an important from each other on all scales (see Table 4). Results
6 Autism 00(0)

Table 3. Contingency of diagnosis on various demographic variables showing number of cases in each category.

Variable ASD TD

Male (N = 90) Female (N = 219) Total (N = 309) Male (N = 103) Females (N = 158) Total (N = 261)
HRT 11 10 21 3 3 6
Cohabitation 63 153 216 39 67 106
Partner-gender unimportant 32 127 159 13 41 54

HRT: hormone replacement therapy; ASD: autism spectrum disorder; TD: typically developing.
The individual who reported birth sex as intersex was excluded from this analysis.

Table 4. Independent group t-test between diagnosis and gender-dysphoria (GD) scores.

TD (N = 261) ASD (N = 310) t-test (df = 558.39)

M SD M SD
GD total 10.00 16.48 24.11 22.20 8.54***
GD subjective 6.02 10.08 12.65 12.32 6.24***
GD somatic 0.77 2.20 2.00 3.24 5.19***
GD social 3.57 5.03 8.25 6.64 9.19***
GD socio-legal 0.34 0.98 1.05 1.73 6.08***

ASD: autism spectrum disorder; TD: typically developing; SD: standard deviation; df: degrees of freedom.
***p < 0.001.

Table 5. Independent group t-test results between males and females with and without an ASD on gender-dysphoria (GD) scores.

ASD TD

Male (N = 90) Female (N = 219) t-test Male (N = 103) Female (N = 158) t-test

M SD M SD M SD M SD
GD total 21.68 25.71 25.77 21.64 1.42 7.62 15.24 11.24 17.58 1.72
GD subjective 10.34 13.15 13.87 12.03 2.28* 3.27 8.67 5.19 9.34 1.67
GD somatic 2.24 4.17 2.02 3.03 0.52 0.56 2.08 0.93 2.35 1.28
GD social 7.38 7.68 8.65 6.60 1.47 2.66 4.31 3.81 5.50 1.79
GD socio-legal 0.97 1.87 1.07 1.66 0.47 0.22 0.79 0.43 1.09 1.65

ASD: autism spectrum disorder; TD: typically developing; SD: standard deviation.
*p < 0.05.

indicated that autistic individuals reported higher GDT modest positive correlation between the two variables
than TD individuals. Cronbach’s alpha for the study sam- (r = 0.37, p < 0.001). Increases in AQ scores were signifi-
ple on the GIDYQ was 0.94. cantly correlated with increases in GDT.
Males and females within the TD group did not differ on Correlations between different subscales of the AQ
total GDT and on individual subscales of the GIDYQ (see with GDT were also undertaken to assess for any differ-
Table 5). Within the ASD group, females did not differ from ences in relationships between autistic traits and GDT
males on their total GDT. On individual GD subscales, rela- across the different domains of the AQ. There were posi-
tive to their male peers, autistic females only demonstrated tive correlations between GDT and the different subscales
significantly higher scores on the GD-subjective scale (see of the AQ: communication (r = 0.36, p < 0.001), social
Table 5). (r = 0.35, p < 0.001), cognitive-inflexibility (r = 0.34,
A mediation analysis assessed the relation between p < 0.001), imagination (r = 0.17, p < 0.001), and detail
autistic traits and sexual orientation, with GDT as a media- (r = 0.20, p < 0.001). The correlation between GDT with
tor. First, a Pearson correlation of the relationship between AQ communication subscale was stronger than the corre-
autistic traits (as measured on the AQ) and GDT was lation with AQ imagination (Fisher’s Z = 3.50, p < 0.001)
obtained, by measuring these rates across the entire sam- and AQ detail (Z = 2.97, p < 0.01). Additionally, the corre-
ple, by collapsing across diagnostic groups. There was a lation between GDT with AQ social subscale was stronger
George and Stokes 7

Figure 2. Relationship between autistic traits and gender-dysphoria. AQ scores are split at AQ = 32, with separate lines of best fit
shown for each and R2 values.

Figure 3. Levels of GDT by sexual orientation.

than the correlation with AQ imagination (Z = 3.23, revealed that compared to heterosexual individuals
p < 0.001) and AQ detail (Z = 2.70, p < 0.01). The relation- (n = 179, M = 7.43, SD = 11.61), levels of GDT were sig-
ship between AQ and GDT was also assessed separately nificantly higher among all other groups (homosexual:
by birth-sex. Results indicated that the relationship n = 45, M = 24.36, SD = 23.72; bisexual: n = 60, M = 21.38,
between AQ and GDT was significant among males SD = 21.25; asexual: n = 208, M = 22.55, SD = 21.20; and
(r = 0.27, N = 193, p < 0.001) and females (r = 0.40, N = 377, other: n = 78, M = 34.06, SD = 25.69; see Figure 3).
p < 0.001) and that the relationship was stronger among Thereafter, the mediation effect of GDT was explored
females than males (Z = 1.65, p < 0.05) (Figure 2). between AQ traits and sexual orientation. There was a sig-
Following this, the association between sexual orienta- nificant direct effect between AQ traits and sexual orienta-
tion and GDT was examined in an independent sample tion (b = 0.079, SE = 0.009, 95% confidence interval (CI):
t-test. Levene’s test of homogeneity of variance was vio- 0.062−0.096). AQ traits were significantly related to the
lated and degrees of freedom were adjusted accordingly. mediator, GDT (b = 0.66, SE = 0.071, 95% CI:
Results indicated a significantly higher mean of GDT in 0.524−0.803), while the mediator was significantly related
the non-heterosexual group (M = 27.13, standard error to the dependent variable, sexual orientation (b = 0.05,
(SE) = 1.39) than in the heterosexual group (M = 7.76, SE = 0.007, 95% CI: 0.036−0.063). The indirect effect of
SE = 0.75; t(446.81) = 12.21, p < 0.001, d = 1.02). the mediator was significant (b = 0.03, SE = 0.006, 95% CI:
A one-way ANOVA revealed that heterosexual and 0.024−0.047; Goodman’s Z = 5.68, p < 0.001). GDT par-
non-heterosexual groups differed significantly on GDT tially mediated the relationship of AQ traits to sexual ori-
levels (F(4, 566) = 47.54, p < 0.001). A Tukey post hoc test entation (see Figure 4).
8 Autism 00(0)

males. The results of this study completely supported the


first hypothesis and partially support the second. Autistic
individuals reported a higher number of gender-dysphoric
symptoms than TD individuals on the all subscales of the
GIDYQ. However, comparison by birth-sex within the
ASD group demonstrated that females differed from males
only on one GD subscale—the subjective scale. Finally,
the hypothesis that GDT will mediate between an individ-
ual’s autistic traits and their sexual orientation was also
supported, where GDT partially mediated the relationship
of AQ traits to sexual orientation.
Figure 4. Model of AQ traits as a predictor of sexual The finding that autistic individuals scored significantly
orientation (heterosexual or non-heterosexual) mediated
by GDT. The confidence interval for the indirect effect is a
higher on GDT is consistent with the literature pointing to
bootstrapped CI based on 1000 samples. increased gender-variance in the autistic population
(Bejerot et al., 2014; George and Stokes, 2016; Strang
et al., 2014) and supports the relationship between ASD
GD and transgenderism rates
and GD. Reasons for the ASD-GD comorbidity has been a
Deogracias et al. (2007) found that a cut-off mean score of topic of emerging interest and several speculations attempt
3.00 on the GIDYQ could distinguish between individuals to account for this association. Previous literature has
with and without a diagnosis of GD. Mean scores of par- relied on the neurobiological basis of ASD to explain
ticipants indicated that there were seven individuals with increased GDT in ASD. Baron-Cohen (2002) proposes
a mean score of 3.00 and above. Of these, six individuals that ASD is an overdevelopment of certain male-typical
had a diagnosis of ASD, comprising five females and one traits such as logical thinking, low emotionality, and high
male. The TD individual was a female. All seven individ- level of perseverance. fT is the biological mechanism
uals had AQ scores well over 32 and reported non-con- implicated in this hyper-masculinization (Knickmeyer
forming gender identities. Accordingly, the rate of GD et al., 2006) and consistently, elevated levels of fT posi-
that would otherwise have led to a clinical diagnosis in the tively correlate with autistic traits and with masculinized
ASD sample was 1.9%. This is approximately 135 times neural development (Auyeung et al., 2009).
more than that found in the wider population given exist- Human and non-human primate research shows that fT
ing rates of GD estimated at 0.005%–0.014% among levels influence human sexual behavior (Hines et al.,
males and 0.002%–0.003% among females (Zucker and 2004). Rare congenital conditions such as found among
Lawrence, 2009). However, these conclusions are a result women with CAH afford opportunities to understand the
of data-analyses and not clinical evaluation and thus effects of altered levels of fT on sexual development. Girls
should be interpreted with caution, especially that the with CAH have an enzyme-deficiency driven overproduc-
GIDYQ has not been validated in an autistic sample tion of fT and show an increased number of autistic traits
before. as measured on the AQ (Knickmeyer et al., 2006),
Autistic individuals were more likely to report non- increased male-typical play behaviors and masculinized
conforming gender identities than their TD counterparts. gender identities (Hines et al., 2004; Pasterski et al., 2005),
Of these, 12 individuals with ASD self-reported transgen- and an increased prevalence of homosexual and bisexual
derism (7 genetic males and 5 genetic females) as well as orientations (Meyer-Bahlburg et al., 2008). Male-typical
five TD individuals (4 genetic males and 1 genetic female). thinking and behavior may lead some autistic females to
The rate of experienced transgenderism in the ASD sample interpret themselves as masculine relative to non-ASD
was 3.9% which was approximately 20–40 times higher females, and this could in some cases pave the way to the
than existing prevalence estimates (1:1000–1:2000; development of GD.
Olyslager and Conway, 2007). However, the same reasoning does not straightfor-
wardly explain the increased prevalence of GD among
males with ASD. Higher levels of fT would support the
Discussion
expression of a pronounced male gender identity (Dessens
The aim of this study was to explore the association et al., 2005). Amniotic measurements of testosterone con-
between autistic traits and GD traits in a sample of indi- firm its role in the development of male-typical behavior
viduals with ASD and compare this to TD individuals. It (Auyeung et al., 2009). Given the association between
was hypothesized that autistic individuals would demon- elevated levels of fT in ASD, why then would autistic
strate a higher number of gender-dysphoric symptoms than males demonstrate higher GDT, when the converse would
TD individuals, and that autistic females would report be expected, or at the least, similar rates GDT between
greater rates of gender-dysphoric symptoms than autistic ASD and TD males? Conclusions related to prenatal
George and Stokes 9

hormones and their effect on gender identity are often scores on the GD-subjective subscale. Compared to men,
drawn from sexual orientation studies that typically sug- women are generally believed to be more socially preco-
gest that male homosexuality is related to gender non-con- cious and empathetic (Lutchmaya and Baron-Cohen,
formity (Bailey and Zucker, 1995; Lippa, 2003; Rieger 2002) and possess stronger verbal and communicative
et al., 2008) and do not clearly explain the association prowess. These are domains that are saliently affected in
between androgen-exposure and gender-conforming male ASD, and this might explain why autistic females may
homosexual people. Gender non-conformity among homo- experience GD symptoms to a greater extent than their
sexual individuals has been hypothesized to be an outcome male peers.
of atypical neural development as a consequence of altered Another feature that may play a role in the ASD-GD
prenatal hormone environments (Blanchard et al., 2006; association is the frequent presence of intense obsessive–
Lalumière et al., 2000; Rahman and Wilson, 2003). compulsive behaviors in ASD. Pharmacological studies
Manning (2002) noted that some research argued that have created a compelling argument for the association
increased prenatal testosterone could lead to homosexual- between ASD and obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD)
ity (Geschwind and Galaburda, 1985), while others argued (Hollander and Pallanti, 2002; McCauley et al., 2004).
the opposite (Ellis and Ames, 1987). Put together, neuro- Cognitive inflexibility is characteristic of OCD (Meiran
hormonal pathways leading to higher prevalence rates of et al., 2011). Findings from this study showed a significant
male GD in ASD is not as clear, though potentially this is correlation between the cognitive-inflexibility subscale on
a pathway for female GD in ASD, yet this needs to be the AQ with the GIDYQ. It is likely that GD might develop
established. Nonetheless, this may be instructive to our as a sequela to ASD, where a person’s preoccupation with
understanding for a subset of autistic males who may cross-gender activities and objects may not be related to
develop GD as a result of an altered interaction between confusion over one’s gender identity, but may just be part
the developing brain and sex hormones. of the pervasive obsessions and distress with cross-gender
To limit a construct as complex as gender identity to roles. Accordingly, several researchers view inflexible
biological factors would be overly reductionistic. An indi- thinking and restricted interests seen in clients with GD as
vidual’s gender identity is most likely an interaction of possible manifestations of the cognitive inflexibility inher-
their biological and psychological make-up (Bradley and ent to ASD (Galluci et al., 2005; Landén and Rasmussen,
Zucker, 1997) and certain ASD-specific psychological fea- 1997; Perera and Gadambanathan, 2003; VanderLaan
tures may increase the risk of the development of gender- et al., 2015).
dysphoric expressions. Consistently, this study found as This study investigated the relationship between autis-
autistic traits increase, there is a moderate relationship to tic traits, GDT, and sexual orientation and found that the
GD traits, a relationship that was found in previous litera- relationship between autistic traits and sexual orientation
ture as well (De Vries et al., 2010; Jones et al., 2011; was mediated by GDT. It is speculated that for some autis-
Pasterski et al., 2014; Vanderlaan et al., 2015). tic individuals, their sexual orientation relates to their gen-
This study found significant and clinically important der experience, which, in turn, may be associated with the
correlations between various subscales of the AQ and the strength of their autistic symptomology. Bem (2000)
GIDYQ. For instance, the GIDYQ was significantly cor- relates GDT and sexual orientation by way of the “exotic
related to AQ communication skills and to AQ social is erotic” theory. Gender-dysphoric children often feel dif-
skills, and these correlations were significantly larger than ferent from their same-sex peers and identify more with
other domains measured by the AQ, suggesting that per- their opposite-sex peers (Wallien et al., 2010). Bem (2000)
haps for some autistic individuals, challenges with com- proposes that gender-dysphoric children may experience
munication and with social skills may contribute in some heightened, nonspecific physiological arousal in the pres-
way toward the development of gender-dysphoric feelings. ence of same-sex peers from whom they feel different
This is not surprising, given that gender is possibly one of (exotic) and that this physiological arousal might in later
the most intensively socialized constructs with many years translate to sexual arousal toward their same-sex
“rules” that are not explicit. While gendered norms are peers (erotic). Perhaps for some individuals in the sample,
typically intuitively understood, and followed by most GDT in childhood might be one factor that contributes
individuals, the challenges associated with negotiating toward the development of same-sex attraction and other
these norms involving fine judgments, understanding non-heterosexual orientations. However, it must be
abstractions, and balancing multiple considerations related stressed that the relationship between non-heterosexuality
to gender and sexual innuendo might be exaggerated in the and GDT is only one possible pathway, and we suggest
presence of ASD-related socio-communicative features cautiously interpreting any relationship between GDT and
(Abelson, 1981). Furthermore, the correlation between sexual orientation. The cross-sectional data in this study
scores on the AQ and the GIDYQ were higher among are limited in its capacity to address the nature of different
autistic females than among autistic males and relative to pathways toward the development of sexual orientation,
their male peers, females with an ASD reported higher and as such is beyond the scope of this article.
10 Autism 00(0)

An interesting difference between male and female sexuality and gender were wide-ranging, conflicting and
groups with ASD was the higher rates of transgenderism, “fluid,” and gave participants the opportunity to respond to
cross-dressing, and HRT among males than females. questions in an unlimited manner (thesis, unpublished).
Despite higher rates of GDT among females, it was the Providing fixed options or norm-setting may limit indi-
males that acted on these feelings in a remedial sense. viduals with an ASD from freely describing what some
These trends may be reflective of the gender-polarizing may consider atypical in this domain. A free-format ques-
cultural climate of the times, where it is more acceptable tionnaire to learn about sexuality and gender in this group
and easier for women to take on masculine traits, clothing, might thus be more instructive and respectful of the indi-
and activities and live as a male would than vice versa. vidual’s unique journey.
Perhaps there are acceptable gender stereotypes for women
with GDT (“butch” lesbians) than gender stereotypes for
Conclusion
men with GDT. Some women with GDT may thus choose
to remain as they are, with the trappings of masculinity Aside from the limitations, the study yielded some impor-
rather than to resort to HRT or surgery. tant findings. It supports assertions that autistic individu-
als are more diverse in their gender identities than the
wider population. Moreover, autistic individuals experi-
Limitations ence more GD traits than the wider population and this is
Results of this study must be interpreted in light of both more pronounced among autistic females. These differ-
its strengths and limitations. Participants who volunteer in ences might be an outcome of sociocultural gendered
a study examining gender and sexuality may have had a norms, which highlight any deficits in social functioning
pre-existing interest in such matters. Given that conveni- in ASD more conspicuously among females than males.
ence sampling was used, and the study sample may not be Paradoxically, the same sociocultural system that penal-
population-representative, results need to be interpreted izes social deficits among autistic females more harshly
with this in mind, as this could have important implica- than it does in males may provide some respite to gender
tions for the prevalence estimates cited. Second, partici- non-conforming females more than it does to gender non-
pants self-reported that they had a clinical diagnosis of conforming males, due to society’s increased tolerance
ASD. However, mean AQ scores of individuals with a toward female but not male gender-deviations (Kite and
diagnosis of ASD fell above the recommended cut-off on Whitley, 1996).
the AQ, and as such it is likely that individuals with a Biological explanations for the increased prevalence of
diagnosis of ASD fell on the spectrum. Furthermore, par- GDT in ASD present a stronger case for females than it
ticipant recruitment took place through a network of does for males. However, social reasons provide an expla-
reputed autism organizations worldwide and online autism nation for GDT in both sexes adequately and might be the
communities, making it likely that this study sample was better explanation at this stage. Tensions between an “ASD
more diverse in composition than clinically referred sam- personality” with a “neurotypical personality” could be
ples. The sample had a large representation of females, misguidedly attributed to gender identity as being the rea-
which is not common in studies of ASD. This might be son for discord across different contexts. It is however
due to the online format of the study, where females are equally tenable that in some cases, feelings of dissatisfac-
more likely than males to rely on the Internet for interac- tion with one’s gender identity could progress to a clinical
tion and self-help (Addis and Mahalik, 2003; Santor et al., GD. The levels of complexity in terms of clinical manage-
2007). As such, the study provided an opportunity to ment of clients with ASD presenting with gender-related
study females with an ASD. issues would be higher, as the convergence of ASD-related
Finally, based on participant-feedback, a qualitative social difficulties and gender-related issues would afford
approach to a study on sexuality and gender-related mat- unique challenges for disentangling the relative contribu-
ters might be better suited for autistic individuals. tions of each factor and for developing effective interven-
Participants from the autistic group sometimes reported tions. This study also found that autistic individuals
not finding response options that suit them. Other times demonstrated gender-fluid attitudes in endorsing a large
there were requests to the researcher to rephrase a question number of non-conforming gender identities and noncha-
or include a new option, as some participants felt margin- lance toward the gender of their romantic or sexual part-
alized, when having to choose an option such as “other” to ner. It is known that autistic individuals overall are less
questions asking about sexual or gender identity, because concerned with social rules and may not always adhere to
none of the existing options suited them. As such, the social etiquette (Attwood, 1998). Perhaps examining gen-
GIDYQ has not been previously validated in an autistic der from the perspective of someone with ASD licenses a
population and participant-feedback proved informative in denaturalization of gendered norms and expectations. At
designing a qualitative questionnaire to measure sexuality this stage, there is sufficient evidence to consider gender-
and gender identity. Qualitative responses related to dysphoric expressions in ASD as one of the presenting
George and Stokes 11

issues requiring clinical management. Whether a diagnosis Bem DJ (2000) Exotic becomes erotic: interpreting the biological
of GD is warranted in clients presenting with gender-dys- correlates of sexual orientation. Archives of Sexual Behavior
phoric issues is a matter of prudent clinical discernment in 29(6): 531–548.
unravelling displaced feelings of social discontentment Bem SL (1974) The measurement of psychological androgyny.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 42: 155–
from a true clinical GD.
162.
In a society where gender has been decisively defined,
Blanchard R, Cantor JM, Bogaert AF, et al. (2006) Interaction
individuals who exist outside its norms become likely tar- of fraternal birth order and handedness in the development
gets of disapproval (Meyer, 2003). There is evidence of of male homosexuality. Hormones and Behavior 49(3):
increased psychiatric comorbidity in populations with dis- 405–414.
contentment with their gender identity (Hepp et al., 2005) Blumberg SJ, Bramlett MD, Kogan MD, et al. (2013) Changes
as well as among autistic individuals (Ghaziuddin et al., in prevalence of parent-reported autism spectrum disorder
2002). Possibly autistic individuals who also have gender- in school aged U.S. children: 2007 to 2011–2012. National
dissatisfaction bear a significant burden of distress. Health Statistics Report 65: 1–11.
Investigation into the mental health outcomes of individu- Bradley SJ and Zucker KJ (1997) Gender identity disorder:
als who belong to sexual and gender minorities within the a review of the past 10 years. Journal of the American
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 36(7): 872–880.
ASD population should be informative. Designing special-
Broadbent J, Galic I and Stokes MA (2013) Validation of autism
ized psycho-sexual programs targeting any gender-related
spectrum quotient adult version in an Australian sample.
confusion tailored to each birth-sex, to provide com- Autism Research and Treatment, 2013.
prehensive support to the autistic individual with the goal Constantino JN and Gruber CP (2005) Social Responsiveness
of reducing frustration and increasing acceptance, is Scale (SRS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
recommended. De Vries ALC, Noens ILJ, Cohen-Kettenis PT, et al. (2010)
Autism spectrum disorders in gender dysphoric children
Funding and adolescents. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders 40(8): 930–936.
The author(s) received no financial support for the research,
Deogracias JJ, Johnson LL, Meyer-Bahlburg HFL, et al. (2007)
authorship, and/or publication of this article.
The gender identity/gender dysphoria questionnaire for ado-
lescents and adults. Journal of Sex Research 44: 370–379.
References Dessens AB, Slijper FM and Drop SL (2005) Gender dysphoria
Abelson AG (1981) The development of gender identity in the and gender change in chromosomal females with congeni-
autistic child. Child: Care, Health and Development 7(6): tal adrenal hyperplasia. Archives of Sexual Behavior 34(4):
347–356. 389–397.
Addis ME and Mahalik JR (2003) Men, masculinity, and the con- Drummond KD, Bradley SJ, Peterson-Badali M, et al. (2008)
texts of help seeking. American Psychologist 58(1): 5–14. A follow-up study of girls with gender identity disorder.
American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statis- Developmental Psychology 44(1): 34–45.
tical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., text revision). Ellis L and Ames MA (1987) Neurohormonal functioning and
Washington, DC: Author. sexual orientation: a theory of homosexuality-heterosexual-
Attwood T (1998) Asperger’s Syndrome: A Guide for Parents ity. Psychological Bulletin 101(2): 233–258.
and Professionals. London, England: Jessica Kingsley. Fombonne E (2005) The changing epidemiology of autism.
Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Ashwin E, et al. (2009) Fetal tes- Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 18:
tosterone and autistic traits. British Journal of Psychology 281–294.
100(1): 1–22. Gallucci G and Schmidt CW Jr (2005) Gender identity disorder
Bailey JM and Zucker KJ (1995) Childhood sex-typed behavior in an adult male with Asperger’s syndrome. Sexuality and
and sexual orientation: a conceptual analysis and quantita- Disability 23(1): 35–40.
tive review. Developmental Psychology 31(1): 43–55. George R and Stokes M (2016) “Gender is not on my agenda!”
Baron-Cohen S (2002) The extreme male brain theory of autism. Gender dysphoria and autism spectrum disorder. In:
Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6: 248–254. Mazzone L and Vitiello B (eds) Psychiatric Symptoms
Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, et al. (2001) The and Comorbidities in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Berlin:
autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger Springer, pp.139–150.
syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, Geschwind N and Galaburda AM (1985) Cerebral lateralization:
scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and part II. Archives of Neurology 42: 521–552.
Developmental Disorders 31(1): 5–17. Ghaziuddin M, Ghaziuddin N and Greden J (2002) Depression
Bejerot S and Eriksson JM (2014) Sexuality and gender role in in persons with autism: implications for research and clini-
autism spectrum disorder: a case control study. PLoS ONE cal care. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
9(1): e87961. 32(4): 299–306.
Bejerot S, Eriksson JM, Bonde S, et al. (2012) The extreme male Gilmour L, Schalomon PM and Smith V (2012) Sexuality in
brain revisited: gender coherence in adults with autism a community based sample of adults with autism spec-
spectrum disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 201(2): trum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 6:
116–123. 313–318.
12 Autism 00(0)

Green R and Young R (2001) Hand preference, sexual prefer- Martin CL and Ruble DN (2010) Patterns of gender develop-
ence, and transsexualism. Archives of Sexual Behavior ment. Annual Review of Psychology 61: 353–381.
30(6): 565–574. Meiran N, Diamond GM, Toder D, et al. (2011) Cognitive
Hepp U, Kraemer B, Schnyder U, et al. (2005) Psychiatric comor- rigidity in unipolar depression and obsessive compulsive
bidity in gender identity disorder. Journal of Psychosomatic disorder: examination of task switching, Stroop, working
Research 58(3): 259–261. memory updating and post-conflict adaptation. Psychiatry
Hines M, Brook C and Conway GS (2004) Androgen and psy- Research 185(1–2): 149–156.
chosexual development: core gender identity, sexual ori- Meyer IH (2003) Prejudice, social stress and mental health in
entation, and recalled childhood gender role behavior lesbian, gay and bisexual populations: Conceptual issues
in women and men with congenital adrenal hyperplasia and research evidence. Psychological Bulletin 129(5):
(CAH). Journal of Sex Research 41(1): 75–81. 674–697.
Hollander E and Pallanti S (2002) Current and experimental Meyer-Bahlburg HF, Dolezal C, Baker SW, et al. (2008) Sexual
therapeutics of obsessive-compulsive disorder. In: Davis orientation in women with classical or non-classical con-
KL, Charney D and Coyle JT (eds) Psychopharmacology: genital adrenal hyperplasia as a function of degree of pre-
Fifth Generation of Progress. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott natal androgen excess. Archives of Sexual Behavior 37(1):
Williams & Wilkins, pp.1647–1664. 85–99.
Howell DC (2013) Statistical Methods for Psychology. 8th ed. Mukaddes NM (2002) Gender identity problems in autistic chil-
Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing. dren. Child: Care, Health and Development 28(6): 529–532.
Howlin P, Mawhood L and Rutter M (2000) Autism and devel- Olyslager F and Conway L (2007) On the calculation of the prev-
opmental receptive language disorder—a follow-up com- alence of transsexualism. In: World professional associa-
parison in early adult life. II: social, behavioural, and tion for transgender health 20th international symposium,
psychiatric outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Chicago, IL, 5–8 September.
Psychiatry 41: 561–578. Paikoff RL and Brooks-Gunn J (1991) Do parent-child rela-
Jones RM, Wheelwright S, Farrell K, et al. (2012) Brief report: tionships change during puberty? Psychological Bulletin
female-to-male transsexual people and autistic traits. Journal 110(1): 47–66.
of Autism and Developmental Disorders 42(2): 301–306. Pasterski V, Gilligan L and Curtis R (2014) Traits of autism spec-
Kite ME and Whitley BE (1996) Sex differences in attitudes trum disorders in adults with gender dysphoria. Archives of
toward homosexual persons, behaviors, and civil rights a Sexual Behavior 43: 387–393.
meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin Pasterski V, Golombok S and Hines M (2011) Sex differences in
22(4): 336–353. social behavior. In: Smith KP and Hart CH (eds) The Wiley-
Knickmeyer R, Baron-Cohen S, Fane BA, et al. (2006) Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development. 2nd
Androgens and autistic traits: a study of individuals with ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell, pp.281–298.
congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior Pasterski VL, Geffner ME, Brain C, et al. (2005) Prenatal hor-
50(1): 148–153. mones and postnatal socialization by parents as deter-
Knickmeyer RC, Wheelwright S and Baron-Cohen SB (2008) minants of male typical toy play in girls with congenital
Sex-typical play: masculinization/defeminization in girls adrenal hyperplasia. Child Development 76(1): 264–278.
with an autism spectrum condition. Journal of Autism and Perera H, Gadambanathan T and Weerasiri S (2003) Gender
Developmental Disorders 38(6): 1028–1035. identity disorder presenting in a girl with Asperger’s disor-
Kraemer B, Aba Delsignore MA, Gundelfinger R, et al. (2005) der and obsessive compulsive disorder. The Ceylon Medical
Comorbidity of Asperger syndrome and gender identity Journal 48(2): 57–58.
disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 14(5): Prince V (2005) Sex vs gender. International Journal of
292–296. Transgenderism 8(4): 29–32.
Lalumiere ML, Blanchard R and Zucker KJ (2000) Sexual orien- Rahman Q and Wilson GD (2003) Born gay? The psychobiol-
tation and handedness in men and women: a meta-analysis. ogy of human sexual orientation. Personality and Individual
Psychological Bulletin 126(4): 575–592. Differences 34(8): 1337–1382.
Landén M and Rasmussen P (1997) Gender identity disorder Rieger G, Linsenmeier JA, Gygax L, et al. (2008) Sexual orien-
in a girl with autism—a case report. European Child & tation and childhood gender nonconformity: evidence from
Adolescent Psychiatry 6(3): 170–173. home videos. Developmental Psychology 44(1): 46–58.
Lippa RA (2003) Are 2D: 4D finger-length ratios related to Rosenfield A and Wasserman S (1993) Sexual development in the
sexual orientation? Yes for men, no for women. Journal of early school-aged child. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric
Personality and Social Psychology 85(1): 179–188. Clinics of North America 2: 393–406.
Lutchmaya S and Baron-Cohen S (2002) Human sex differences Santor DA, Poulin C, LeBlanc JC, et al. (2007) Online health
in social and non-social looking preferences, at 12 months promotion, early identification of difficulties, and help seek-
of age. Infant Behavior & Development 25(3): 319–325. ing in young people. Journal of the American Academy of
McCauley JL, Olson LM, Dowd M, et al. (2004) Linkage and Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 46(1): 50–59.
association analysis at the serotonin transporter (SLC6A4) Strang JF, Kenworthy L, Dominska A, et al. (2014) Increased
locus in a rigid compulsive subset of autism. American gender variance in autism spectrum disorders and attention
Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric deficit hyperactivity disorder. Archives of Sexual Behavior
Genetics 127(1): 104–112. 43(8): 1525–1533.
Manning JT (2002) Digit Ratio. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers Tabachnick BG and Fidell LS (2001) Using Multivariate
University Press. Statistics. Northridge, CA: California State University.
George and Stokes 13

Tateno M, Tateno Y and Saito T (2008) Comorbid childhood American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 47:
gender identity disorder in a boy with Asperger syn- 1413–1423.
drome. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 62(2): Woodbury-Smith MR, Robinson J, Wheelwright S, et al. (2005)
238–238. Screening adults for Asperger syndrome using the AQ: a
VanderLaan DP, Postema L, Wood H, et al. (2015) Do children preliminary study of its diagnostic validity in clinical prac-
with gender dysphoria have intense/ obsessional interests? tice. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 35(3):
Journal of Sex Research 52: 213–219. 331–335.
Wallien MS, Veenstra R, Kreukels BP, et al. (2010) Peer group Zucker KJ and Lawrence AA (2009) Epidemiology of gender
status of gender dysphoric children: a sociometric study. identity disorder: recommendations for the standards of care
Archives of Sexual Behavior 39(2): 553–560. of The World Professional Association for Transgender
Wallien SC and Cohen-Kattenis PT (2008) Psychosexual Health. International Journal of Transgenderism 11(1):
outcome of gender-dysphoric children. Journal of the 8–18.

You might also like