Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Received: 25 May 2023 | Revised: 26 March 2024 | Accepted: 3 April 2024

DOI: 10.1002/jgc4.1903

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Understanding the psychological impact of identifying carrier


status on young adults: A qualitative study exploring peer
reactions

Edie Bowen | John Langston | Harriet Fletcher | Julia Domek | Fiona Ulph

Manchester Centre for Health Psychology,


Division of Psychology & Mental Health, Abstract
School of Health Sciences, Faculty of
The benefits and harms of identifying carriers in childhood have long been debated
Biology, Medicine and Health, University
of Manchester, Manchester Academic with European Guidelines advising against this practice. Yet over a thousand carri-
Health Science Centre, Manchester, UK
ers are identified via newborn bloodspot screening per year in the United Kingdom
Correspondence alone. One of the concerns about identification is the impact it has on an individual's
Fiona Ulph, Manchester Centre for Health
identity. This, in part, will be determined by how parents and peers view carriers,
Psychology, Division of Psychology &
Mental Health, School of Health Sciences, particularly during young adulthood. To address the paucity of research looking at
Faculty of Biology, Medicine and Health,
how carriers are perceived by peers, this study sought to explore the views of young
University of Manchester, Manchester
Academic Health Science Centre, Oxford adults, who themselves are not carriers, toward carriers. As the narratives around
Road, Manchester M13 9PL, UK.
COVID-­19 increased, the salience of the term “carrier”, the impact of such narra-
Email: fiona.ulph@manchester.ac.uk
tives on perceptions, was also explored. Twenty-­five 18–25 year olds participated in a
diary-­interview study in the United Kingdom during 2021 to explore their perceptions
of carriers via hypothetical scenarios. Data were analyzed using thematic analysis.
Interviewees believed carriers would experience stigma—including societal and self-­
stigma. This was because people used existing illness beliefs to make sense of carrier
status about which they had low levels of understanding. Interviewees believed car-
riers would experience challenges in familial and romantic relationships due to others'
judgments. They also believed parents of carriers would experience a burden around
making reproductive decisions, with clear views on what society would view as ac-
ceptable choices. Importantly interviewees felt knowledge of ones' own carrier sta-
tus conferred complex communication challenges within relationships. These findings
suggest an urgent need for more research and support for young adults entering a key
stage in life for identity formation who have knowledge of their carrier status. The
results suggest that support targeted toward the carrier regarding navigating complex
communication and targeted more broadly to avoid stigma based on misunderstand-
ing should be researched and developed.

KEYWORDS
carrier testing, communication, COVID-­19, newborn screening, qualitative methods, stigma

This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly cited.
© 2024 The Authors. Journal of Genetic Counseling published by Wiley Periodicals LLC on behalf of National Society of Genetic Counselors.

J Genet Couns. 2024;00:1–13.  wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jgc4 | 1


|

15733599, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/jgc4.1903 by Test, Wiley Online Library on [26/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
2 BOWEN et al.

1 | I NTRO D U C TI O N
What is known about this topic
Newborn bloodspot screening (NBS) aims to identify babies with
rare, serious, and treatable conditions. NBS also, in some countries, Research has previously shown that carrier information
identifies carriers of autosomal recessive conditions such as cystic fi- can lead to stigmatization of adults. European guidelines
brosis and sickle cell. As the health implications of being a carrier are raise concerns about the impact of carrier identification in
primarily reproductive (Hussein et al., 2018), such identification in childhood but acknowledge there is a lack of research in
childhood has been viewed as problematic (Borry et al., 2006; Royal this area.
College of Physicians et al., 2022). Debates exist around the benefits
and harms of identifying carriers (Armstrong et al., 2020) and telling What this paper adds to this topic
a child their carrier status (Miller et al., 2009). The piloting of whole-­ This is the first study to explore how young adults iden-
genome sequence-­based NBS in the United Kingdom, which enables tified as autosomal carriers could be perceived by their
many more conditions (an expansion from 9 to 200+ conditions) to be peer group and the impact on their romantic relationships.
identified and, to our understanding, will result in some families being Findings suggest that young adults expect carriers to dis-
told their child is a carrier following subsequent testing. This has inten- close their status early in the formation of relationships,
sified calls to better understand the benefits and harms of NBS results while simultaneously being concerned about the impact of
including carriers (Ulph & Bennett, 2022). When considering the ben- disclosure on relationships.
efits and harms of identifying carriers via NBS from a psychological
perspective, we must consider this from the perspective of the parents
in the immediate term and the child in the longer term.
2020). Proposed benefits of identifying carriers via NBS include par-
ents and children becoming aware of the future reproductive risks
1.1 | Carrier identification—Impact on parents (Miller et al., 2009), although whether this information truly does
influence reproductive decisions has been hard to discern (Hayeems
The process of determining carrier status often involves recalling et al., 2008). Although there is support for knowing this information
parents to hospital for further tests. As such parents can undergo a (Hayeems et al., 2008), there are still many misunderstandings about
period of uncertainty during which they may worry their child has the difference between being a carrier and having the condition, and
a serious life-­limiting condition, this has been linked to depression, adults have been shown to believe carriers have more health issues
anxiety, exhaustion, and isolation (e.g., Chudleigh et al., 2016; Lang (Hayeems et al., 2008; Mayo-­Gamble et al., 2018). It is important to
et al., 2011; Moran et al., 2007; Salm et al., 2012; Ulph et al., 2015). establish whether young adult peers will also do this.
Ulph et al. (2015) conducted semi-­structured interviews with 67 fam-
ily members of 51 infants identified as carriers via NBS. Many valued
knowing about their child's carrier results, but found the disclosure ex- 1.2 | Impact on young adults
tremely distressing. A common finding of such research is that parents
are often able to recall their experiences a number of years later with Emerging adulthood (a pivotal life stage 18–25 years old; Arnett, 2000)
great lucidity. Known as “flashbulb” memories (Brown & Kulik, 1977), is a critical time for identity formation as individuals explore love, work,
these memories are often formed about events that are unexpected and world views. Within this, peer relationships and the ability to form
and severely emotionally arousing. Ulph et al. (2015) attributed this romantic relationships are crucial parts of identity formation (Joshi &
distress to two processes. Firstly, parents being unaware their children Deuskar, 2016). Parents plan to disclose carrier status to their children
may be identified as carriers via NBS, and therefore assuming their in adolescence or young adulthood when the individual starts becom-
child had the condition when contacted about results. Secondly, the ing sexually active (Ulph et al., 2014), so young adults identified as
way in which health professionals communicated the screening results carriers via NBS should be aware of their status. Although guidelines
amplified concerns that their child had the condition. That parents feel about genetic testing in childhood emphasize the need to balance ben-
unprepared for screening results is a common finding internationally efits and harms of carrier identification (Royal College of Physicians
(Chudleigh et al., 2016; Moran et al., 2007; Salm et al., 2012; Schwan et al., 2022), there is minimal research into the experiences of knowing
et al., 2019; White et al., 2021) and across result types. Research sug- you are a carrier at this age. Research into school-­based carrier test-
gests that both the carrier result itself and the way this information is ing suggests that children do not have adverse reactions to learning
disclosed to parents have both immediate (Moran et al., 2007; Ulph such information (Barlow-­Stewart et al., 2003; Mitchell et al., 1993,
et al., 2015) and long-­lasting (Lewis et al., 2006) consequences. How 1996). However, what is not always clear is whether the children fully
prescreening communication practices could be changed to better understood their results, or if this adaptation can only be achieved in
engage and prepare parents for such screening has been researched students studying relevant subjects when all peers understand what it
(Ulph et al., 2017) with repeated calls being made to improve such means to be a carrier. Research with genetic counselors suggests that
communication (Kai et al., 2009; Tluczek et al., 2022; Ulph et al., 2017, ensuring children understand their carrier status can be challenging,
|

15733599, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/jgc4.1903 by Test, Wiley Online Library on [26/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
BOWEN et al. 3

with few resources available (Ulph et al., 2010). This is a critical gap as “carrier” were frequently used during the pandemic to refer to indi-
during this age span peer acceptance is an important driver of psycho- viduals without COVID-­19 symptoms who could transmit the virus
logical well-­being. (WHO, 2020). Such people were a health threat and to be avoided.
This contrasts with carriers of genetic conditions who do not pose any
health threat (Kai et al., 2009). There is concern that the use of words
1.3 | Psychological impacts of identification “asymptomatic” and “carrier” has been associated with being contagious
and harmful to others. As such, this information may be used to form
Research has shown the detrimental effects of misinterpretation of opinions of genetic carriers when people have little knowledge. Noke
carrier status (Mischler et al., 1998) leading to stigmatization and dis- and Ulph (2014) have shown that, even when unable to confidently name
crimination (Lewis et al., 2010; Ulph et al., 2011) even within com- features of genetic conditions, people use their pre-­existing knowledge
munities with high levels of awareness (Kai et al., 2009). Specifically, to form assumptions about condition features. Furthermore, these mis-
there is evidence that genetic carriers experience both self and social conceptions about genetic conditions were often stated in front of peers
stigmatization (Lewis et al., 2010). Social stigma involves the labe- with confidence, which could lead others to assimilate them as fact.
ling of differences as undesirable, resulting in discrimination (Bathje Although there have been a number of studies that have looked at the in-
& Marston, 2014), and self-­stigma is when a person agrees with and fluence of risk perceptions on beliefs about COVID-­19 and engagement
internalizes the stereotypes of the group they perceive themselves to in various behaviors (Cipolletta et al., 2022), fewer have looked at how
belong in (Corrigan et al., 2010). Stamatoyannopoulos (1974) found living through the pandemic altered risk perceptions of other conditions.
that when notified of their status, carriers of sickle cell would feel This study used qualitative methodology to explore young
embarrassed, inferior, or anxious, suggesting a harmful internalization adults' views of carriers of a genetic condition and the possible
of this label, potentially leading to adverse effects on identity. Evers-­ impact that COVID-­19 has had on these. These young adults were
Kiebooms et al. (1994) evaluated the emotional reaction to obtain- not themselves carriers, so as to better understand peer reactions,
ing a carrier result for both carriers and non-­carriers. A comparison which could influence carrier identity formation. In addition, we ex-
between the two groups' responses on the Health Orientation Scale plored the views toward carriers generally, rather than focusing on
(HOS) revealed that carriers had substantially lower positive feelings one particular condition, so as to ensure that the perceptions about
regarding themselves, in contrast with non-­carriers, describing them- the condition itself were not driving the views.
selves as weaker, less happy, and worse. Both groups ascribed more
negative feelings toward genetic carriers than non-­carriers, suggesting
the presence of societal stigma in relation to carrier identification. This 2 | M ATE R I A L S A N D M E TH O DS
was later replicated by Gordon et al. (2003), who reported identical
findings in a larger sample (353 respondents, 47 of whom had been 2.1 | Participants
identified as cystic fibrosis carriers) and had previously been shown
in sickle cell carriers (Wooldridge & Murray Jr., 1988). These authors A convenience sample of 25 young adults (23 = female, 2 = male),
concluded this was due to a stigmatizing effect of carrier identifica- aged 18–25 years, was recruited via an advert on a study recruitment
tion. Of concern, all these papers included adults who received genetic site for psychology students at a University in the North of England.
counseling, making it even more necessary to explore the understand- The advert explained the study and that those who knew they
ing of young adult carriers identified via NBS and their peers, neither were carriers of a genetic condition were not invited to participate.
of whom are likely to receive information from specialist health pro- We excluded carriers to ensure that the sample represented more
fessionals. One study in the field investigated how 10-­to 17-­year-­old closely reactions of peers toward carriers and because we could not
non-­carriers would respond to a peer who discloses their carrier status be sure that views were not distressing to carriers. Most participants
(Noke, 2015). Findings showed that children's views were stigmatizing, described a lack of previous genetic knowledge, with few partici-
and carriers were seen as “different,” with beliefs that they would have pants confident in their understanding and knowledge of genetics.
lowered self-­concept and would not fit into their peer group. Thus,
although carrier status results are seen as medically benign, they may
not be psychosocially benign (Ulph & Bennett, 2022). 2.2 | Instrumentation and procedure

This two-­phase study used diaries and interviews to explore partici-


1.4 | Impact of COVID-­19 pants' views.

The impact that large-­scale public health emergencies can have on


people's knowledge, attitudes, and perceptions has been recognized 2.2.1 | Phase 1—Diary study
(Adefisoye, 2021) and it is accepted that people's health knowledge
will have been impacted by the magnitude of the COVID-­19 pandemic Diaries were used as they allow creativity and flexibility in par-
(WHO, 2020). Relevant to this work, the terms “asymptomatic” and ticipants' responses (Alaszewski, 2006), enable short-­term daily
|

15733599, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/jgc4.1903 by Test, Wiley Online Library on [26/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
4 BOWEN et al.

reflections (McDonnell et al., 2017) and so the collection of partici- Each interview lasted approximately 1 h. After the interviews, par-
pants' thoughts and feelings as and when they occur, driving depth of ticipants were emailed a debrief sheet. Interviews were transcribed
consideration of the research topic. However, this method of research verbatim removing identifiable information to ensure anonymity.
is not without its own issues, such as compliance and willingness to
complete the diaries in detail (Stone et al., 2002), which in turn can af-
fect the quality of the data (Elliott, 1997). Therefore, diaries are often 2.3 | Reflexivity
used in addition to other methods, such as interviews (McDonnell
et al., 2017). Interested participants contacted the researchers who All authors had previously undertaken qualitative research and used
sent them the participant information sheet. Twenty-­four hours later, thematic analysis to identify patterns and themes across data (Braun
participants were emailed the first diary entry. Returning this signified & Clarke, 2006). EB had undertaken a placement in perinatal mental
consent to take part in the diary study, so upon return, a consent form health. Her pre-­existing knowledge of the perinatal period informed
to participate in the interview study was also sent. Participants were the development of interview questions. JD, JL, and HF could not
emailed a diary sheet daily over the course of 7 days which was de- rely on tacit perinatal knowledge so asked more follow-­up questions
signed for this study by the research team including young adults and to clarify participants' meanings.
an experienced qualitative researcher with expertise in this topic. The
initial diary entry explored participants' pre-­conceptions of carriers.
Each day, participants completed a number of tasks, considering their 2.4 | Data analysis
initial thoughts around carriers, and considering the hypothetical situ-
ation of how they would feel if they were informed that either they A pragmatic decision was made not to present the diary data due
themselves or their newborn baby was a carrier of a genetic condition to journal word limits that could have created a superficial sum-
and whom they think they would tell if they received this informa- mary of both data sets. As such, data analysis focused on the in-
tion. Participants were also asked to describe the characteristics of terview data as the diary responses were captured and reflected
two stick people, one labeled as “carrier” and the other “non-­carrier,” within these data. Analysis was conducted by EB, JD, JL, and HF
further encouraging them to consider their thoughts on carriers prior with supervision from FU—reviewing and advising on each of the
to the interview. Finally, participants were asked whether their re- six stages.
sponses had been influenced by anything they had been exposed Reflexive thematic analysis (TA) is a widely used qualitative ana-
to about COVID-­19 that day. On completion of each daily response, lytic method that involves searching for themes or patterns across an
they password protected the document and emailed it back to the entire data set (Braun & Clarke, 2021). It is commonly used to provide
researchers. Researchers reviewed the responses before conducting accessible accounts of complex data. We took a critical realist stance,
the interview, taking detailed notes, and interweaving them into the which holds that while there is a knowable reality out there, people's
interview schedule. In this way, the diary entries shaped the inter- experiences and accounts of this are shaped by the social world includ-
views to an extent and provided prompts and areas to explore further. ing their language (Braun & Clarke, 2021). Themes were developed
inductively using both the semantic and latent levels, centered around
both explicit and implicit meaning (Fereday & Muir-­Cochrane, 2006).
2.2.2 | Phase 2—Interview Data analysis followed the six stages outlined by Braun and Clarke
(2021). We have highlighted these stages in bold with the exception
On-­line semi-­structured interviews enable the collection of rich of the sixth which is writing up. Firstly, familiarization of the data oc-
data on participants' thoughts, feelings, and beliefs without being curred by “repeated reading,” actively searching for meaning across
restricted by the researchers' pre-­formulated ideas (Berg, 2009) participants' accounts, and noting any initial thoughts in the margins
increasing the opportunity for unanticipated insights. Online inter- of the interviews at the semantic (or explicit) level. After familiar-
views are convenient, flexible, and cost-­effective (Hewson, 2008; ization with the breadth and depth of the data, researchers worked
Horrell et al., 2015) aspects that participants appreciate (Deakin & on the transcripts of the interviews they conducted and initial ideas
Wakefield, 2013) and a necessity as the data were collected dur- for coding were noted and returned in order to facilitate the formal
ing the COVID-­19 lockdown in 2021. The interview schedule, de- coding process and organization of data into codes or “meaningful
veloped by the team, covered similar topics to the diary: views of groups” (Tuckett, 2005). A schedule was set so that the team met after
carriers, a vignette that their baby was diagnosed as a carrier, and the coding of each interview to discuss their codes and resolve que-
reflection on whether COVID-­19 coverage affected their views. ries regarding the coding process. Once the data were coded, codes
While the schedule was used across interviews, the sequence and and quotes were collated into a Microsoft Excel document creating
phrasing of questions were altered to explore in more detail partici- a compilation of the coding. At this stage, each analyst (of the four)
pants' responses, both in the interview and their diaries, and adapt selected areas of the data to focus on and generated initial themes
questions to the individual and probe (Kvale, 1996). The interviewer for their areas (such as romantic relationships, disclosure, and im-
made use of the screen share function on Zoom to show participants pact on self). Following this, themes were developed and reviewed.
their diary entries in order to enquire further about their responses. Through this process, some themes were collapsed and others were
|

15733599, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/jgc4.1903 by Test, Wiley Online Library on [26/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
BOWEN et al. 5

re-­named as subthemes. Finally, the coded data were then actively partner can't have children or if my parents wish for
re-­read alongside the consequent themes to refine, define, and name grandchildren.
final themes to ensure they formed a coherent pattern, all the while (P16)
considering the validity of the themes in relation to the entire data-
set. The analysts created a clear and internally coherent account with Other, more general stigma was also seen in some participants:
an associated narrative both within each individual theme and across
each theme in relation to the research question. For this paper, the I was thinking about how if the baby has inherited
separate analyses were reviewed and combined to enable an account disease how is this linked to reincarnation […] Is it a
of the entire dataset and broader research question. punishment for people who did bad things during pre-
In exchange for taking part, participants received course credits. vious lives?
The study was approved by the University of Manchester Research (P19)
Ethics Committee.
This participant indicates that he/she see being a carrier as a neg-
ative trait for the reason that the baby could be punished by having
3 | R E S U LT S the condition they carry, suggesting an element of blame as well as
stigma. Probing revealed that this participant felt that if there were
Three themes were created. “Stigma” included both societal and self-­ any “good genetic carriers,” which they explained to mean the abil-
stigma. These beliefs appeared to be driven by people using exist- ity to pass on beneficial traits rather than conditions, then it would
ing illness belief models to make sense of carrier status about which not be a bad thing to be a carrier. However, oftentimes stigma was
they had low levels of understanding. This resulted in people believ- much more implicit, with participants believing carriers would not
ing carriers were ill or suffered in some way. Those who had these lead completely normal lives:
beliefs, followed through in their explanations in the second theme
“Impact on relationships” where some interviewees believed carriers whereas a non-­carrier doesn't really have that worry
would experience challenges in familial and romantic relationships and sees themselves as completely normal and healthy
due to others' judgments. They also believed parents of carriers (P16)
would experience a burden around making reproductive decisions,
with clear views on what society would view as acceptable choices. This idea that carriers were less healthy than non-­carriers was a
However, data in this theme showed there was some counterbalance common justification for stigma.
where people could appreciate that carriers did not have the condi-
tion. Most, however, felt that knowledge of ones' own carrier status
conferred complex communication challenges within relationships 3.2 | Stigma: Self-­stigma
that are explored in the final theme “Communication Challenges.”
When considering what life as a carrier would be like, many partic-
ipants felt it would be very different from their life currently and
3.1 | Stigma: Societal stigma showed similar stigma as expressed above. It was common for being
a carrier to be viewed as some kind of “deficit” (participants' words,
Almost all participants believed that carriers would be stigmatized not ours). For example, when asked what life as a carrier would be
at a societal level. like, one participant said:

they tend to like label them as ohhh that girl with that I guess it could hard for me to find someone willing
genetic condition. So, I feel like that's what you be- to raise children with me if I have a really bad genetic
come known for, so errr I guess people would label deficit.
you and put you into that kind of… that stigma I guess. (P10)
(P3)
Similarly, other participants hinted at the fact that they would
Despite being aware of the potential for discrimination, some assume potential partners would discriminate against them. This
participants showed stigmatizing views themselves. Commonly, par- participant suggests they might inherently value themselves less
ticipants believed that carriers would be most different in romantic and some participants even stated explicitly that they viewed their
relationships: hypothetical carrier status as a sign of weakness:

I don't want it to become a strain on my personal re- I know some people won't think like that but person-
lationships. If being a carrier means me and my future ally like I would like that's on me. I've given her my
|

15733599, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/jgc4.1903 by Test, Wiley Online Library on [26/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
6 BOWEN et al.

genes…and like there is clearly like a weakness in the it's that I don't understand why people have this per-
line do you know what I mean? ception when there is no difference between … it's
(P18) not going to affect your life, there is no need to be
like that really.
The use of the language “line” to talk about inheritance is an ab- (P23)
breviation of “bloodline” and has historically been used in contexts,
particularly in relation to royal families—with connotations of decid- This participant had studied A-­level biology and could confi-
ing people's worth, and desirability for marriage which would today dently explain recessive inheritance. However, these examples of
be seen as discriminatory. This describes an intense feeling of inferi- correct genetic knowledge reducing stigma were rare even among
ority and inadequacy, caused by the guilt of being a carrier. a highly educated sample and may be best viewed as “the exception
that proves the rule” regarding quick schematic judgments leading
to stigma.
3.3 | Stigma: Creation—schematic knowledge

Judgments of carriers were notably quick among participants and 3.4 | Impact on relationships: Familial
many justified stigmatizing views using quick, unintentional miscon-
ceptions. Much of the stigma observed often stemmed from confu- Participants were concerned that their decision to have a child would
sion that carriers in some way had the condition. Of note, this was affect their relationship with their immediate family members, with
reported in interviews after participants had received information many fearing that family members' views on carriers would be pre-
about carriers available via the NHS. dominantly negative:

I would probably immediately think that they like had Psychologically, I would feel myself as a burden; it just
the full condition or illness or whatever. I would think feels really stressful, that I am bringing such a hassle
they would show the symptoms of it. to my family.
(P23) (P12)

This idea of carriers as sufferers was pervasive across partic- Others suggested their family would question their decision to
ipants, particularly those who expressed stigmatizing views of have a child knowing there was the potential for them to be a carrier:
carriers. However, some participants did understand that carriers
would not have medical effects of the condition. These participants I don't know if they knew that I knew before I had the
remained confused about exactly what it meant to be a carrier child, if they would then be like why did you have the
of a genetic condition: “I mean they do have the disease but they child if you knew that they were going to be a carrier.
don't have the symptoms” (P5). This particular participant went on (P14)
to refer to genetic condition sufferers as “symptomatic carriers” at
other points during the interview. Probing revealed participants For one participant, it was clear that their close family had such
were confused by the fact that carriers, who were explained in the strong negative pre-­conceptions of carriers; they were opposed to
diary entries as not having any medical symptoms, could still pass them even starting a relationship with a carrier:
on the condition without actually having it. Many expressed the
idea that if people can pass on a condition, then they must have I remember my grandad always saying to my mum like
it, even if they do not have symptoms, analogous to coronavirus. we've always had a strong family line like there's never
From this, we could see that there was a jarring between existing been anything wrong with our side so don't try and get
knowledge systems (schemas) and this novel information, which with anyone that kind of has like genetic implications.
was leading to the stigma. (P18)
Another observation present across multiple, albeit fewer, inter-
views was the idea that carrier status may mean that the effects Intertwined with their family's explicit thoughts on carriers in
of the condition were currently hidden, but may manifest later in the context of romantic relationships, there was a clear distinction
life: “I'm scared if the condition will kick in and how it will affect me between “them” and “us,” communicated through carrier status
drastically in the future” (P12). Some participants, however, could being something “wrong” with someone, and something that made
correctly explain the meaning of carrier status and did not express them weak, in contrast to this participants “strong” family line. While
stigmatizing views as frequently: it was not clear from this participant's interview whether their fam-
ily had first-­hand experience of a genetic condition, there certainly
I think it would just be annoying to know that I am no was a powerful understanding within that family that genetic con-
different and I think that's what would be the issue ditions were to be avoided. However, some participants did state
|

15733599, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/jgc4.1903 by Test, Wiley Online Library on [26/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
BOWEN et al. 7

they would be pleased about receiving this information as they were go to the doctors […] and obviously it is upsetting but
aware of the difference between being a carrier and actually having as long as I can have some sort of child with my part-
a genetic condition, and so would be grateful they were just a carrier. ner then I don't mind
(P6)

3.5 | Impact on relationships: Romantic It was clear that this participant considered being resilient as in-
tegral to overcoming the challenges associated with having a child
When participants reflected on how they would feel disclosing their with someone with known carrier status. Having a child with a car-
carrier status to a partner, it was common for participants to high- rier was viewed as “unsafe” and while medical input from a doctor
light the reproductive implications of carrier status as a barrier in would be “upsetting,” this was viewed as unavoidable and necessary
romantic relationships: to have a biological child.
Ultimately, there was a sense that participants feared judgment
like nervous and anxious that he might act… yeah from both their family members and romantic partners, and were
aversely and might yeah decide not to… want to be concerned that either may respond negatively to their own or their
with me out of not-­out of like that life risk for a child child's carrier status. For many participants, knowledge of carrier
and like not wanting that for the child which I would status presented a barrier to having future children and depending
think is fair enough on the relationship status, forced participants to consider whether a
(P13) relationship should end or was even worth starting in the first place.

The justification of ending a relationship as a young adult due to


future reproductive issues was perceived as “fair enough,” highlight- 3.6 | Impact on relationships: Parent–child
ing a belief from some participants that carriers are undesirable for relationship
romantic relationships.
Even when participants understood that the child would only in- Many participants felt anxious about the possibility of having a child
herit the condition if both parents were carriers, the perceived risk who was a carrier with a strong sense that the participants' own car-
for the child often resulted in dismissing the option of having biolog- rier status would have a significant psychological impact on their
ical children: decision to have a child:

if it was something that was quite high risk, negative, I think that being a carrier would put a lot of pres-
genetically to be passed down […] I would have to sure on someone, psychologically, the uncertainty of
come to like a compromise with them maybe if we whether to have children based on whether you think
was going to have kids, or if I really wanted kids, I'd it was worth the risk is a huge burden.
have to like go a separate way (P15)
(P24)
Concerns about their future child's carrier status also appeared
Although the possibility of compromise was discussed, this par- to be linked to a fear of judgment from others for having gone
ticular participant perceived that if the reproductive risk was too through with the pregnancy knowing the potential for their child to
great, they felt they would rather sacrifice the relationship than pur- be a carrier beforehand:
sue non-­biological ways of becoming parents. Although this at first
may seem to contradict the above narrative about dismissing bio- If my child were a carrier, I would be particularly con-
logical options for having children, it is achieving the same ends by cerned that I would be blamed.
different means—participants are still avoiding the chance of having (P9)
a child with a condition; they would rather end the relationship than
pursue assisted reproductive means. Although not all participants Others sought reassurance that their decision to have a child
said this, the implication that others perceive carriers as undesirable would not cause their child to suffer:
could be detrimental to carriers' self-­concept.
It is important to note, however, that some believed that knowing I would feel very guilty that I have caused this; I would
their carrier status was positive because it would enable them to make want reassurance that my child would be okay and
an informed decision with their partner. Such participants believed will not suffer because of this.
they could overlook their desire for a biological child, for example: (P17)

I feel like I'm quite resilient so I think like if we want to Some participants felt that if their child was a carrier, this would
have children together and it's not safe to do so, we'd not only have a significant psychological impact on them but would
|

15733599, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/jgc4.1903 by Test, Wiley Online Library on [26/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
8 BOWEN et al.

also affect their child's life with their child facing the same reproduc- carrier would be “a punishment for people who did bad things during
tive decision burden they faced now: previous lives” (P19).
Other participants were worried that the distress they felt about
I guess if I have known that I had it beforehand then being a carrier would naturally transfer to their carrier child: “The dis-
I'd feel really bad cuz [sic] I'd be like, I kind of knew tress that they then might go through in processing that in probably
the risks. I still chose to do that. So, I'd feel like it's the same way that I have” (P14). Indeed the idea of transference, both
kind of I'm partially to blame for my child to have to genetically in terms of their child inheriting his or her carrier status
go through that as well. from them and in terms of the psychological impact transferred to the
(P3) child, was communicated by many participants: “I’d feel guilty that if
the child was a carrier, I’ve just passed on the burden” (P9).
For other participants, the reproductive decision burden was too However, not all participants' responses to their child's carrier
overwhelming; with one participant saying that choosing to have a status were inherently negative, and two participants described how
child would be cruel: they would feel the opposite: “I'd be quite relieved that they hav-
en't actually got it and I could help them if they had any issues with
Would you go ahead with having kids when they it” (P2) with others “So grateful that they don't have the condition”
could have like a life-­limiting disease? I think that's (P16). Importantly, participants who held previous formal education
cruel personally expressed relief and gratitude that their child was a carrier as op-
(P18) posed to suffering the condition, whereas participants who claimed
little or no prior genetic knowledge often expressed guilt and shame.
This led to some saying they would choose not to have their own
biological child at all:
3.7 | Communication challenges: Timing
I would probably think about whether to adopt or not. I
know that's a bit harsh, but I don't really want a child that When asked when carrier status should be disclosed within a ro-
I'm going to have to look after in more ways than a nor- mantic relationship, a number of participants made it clear that they
mal parent would. I would rather reduce that possibility would prefer disclosure to occur early on in a relationship and that
(P5) carriers have a certain responsibility to do so:

Participants frequently suggested that the implications of a child if they disclose that later I wouldn't blame them but…
being a carrier or having a genetic condition were both inherently yeah it would be nicer to know early on and then just
negative and, crucially, they viewed the implications of both being a have that in the back-­ well in the back of your mind
carrier and having the condition similarly negatively: sort of because it's actually quite a deep thing
(P13)
I think it's obviously not something any parent wants
for their child and I think it would be hard knowing This preference for early disclosure reflects the extra consider-
that I was a component in them developing either car- ations that may be taken when entering a relationship with a carrier.
rier status or the condition itself. Additionally, the mention that this participant would keep knowl-
(P23) edge of their partners' carrier status in the “back of [their] mind” high-
lights how some participants viewed carrier status as something that
Crucially, this participant does differentiate between being a car- would not be dismissed as trivial.
rier or having a condition, and yet still highlights a negative impact Despite commonly acknowledging the difficulty of disclosure,
on parents of passing this on. For many participants, the reproductive other participants felt that if their partner was a carrier, they them-
decision burden was underpinned by lack of knowledge about the dif- selves were entitled to know, relating this to trust and honesty
ference between carrier status and condition, leading many to feelings within the relationship:
of guilt and self-­blame, and others to choose not to have children at all.
When asked about how participants would respond to being in- Umm I think the most important thing is there shouldn't
formed of their child's carrier status, the recurrent emotion partic- be deception like yeah if my romantic partner is a car-
ipants described was guilt: “I think I might feel a bit guilty… cause I rier then umm he should tell me this. Umm I would give
was a carrier… so that's why my child is a carrier…” (P21). For some support but umm I think I have the right to know yeah
participants this sense of guilt manifested as even more intensely (P10)
negative emotions: “I would feel very responsible and shameful… I
just feel like I've done something really bad” (P17). Another partici- The use of the word “deception” is interesting. When we put this
pant even went as far as to question whether having a child who is a into the context of what else people feel they would have a right to
|

15733599, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/jgc4.1903 by Test, Wiley Online Library on [26/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
BOWEN et al. 9

know about their partner, it indicates the importance people place For context, this participant held these rejection concerns even
on this information. Although this sense of the requirement for dis- though their only concern about their partner being a carrier was re-
closure was predominant, a few participants spoke of disclosure in lated to future reproduction. This was a common pattern across the
a more casual way: interviews and this fear of judgment from others appeared routed in
concerns that others would misunderstand carrier status. These data
I think I would tell someone within like a month or also show that there could be difficulties in balancing what is import-
something maybe [INT: Yeah] yeah it's not a case of ant for others to know against your own interests of not jeopardizing
that we're having kids, I think for me it would more of the relationship early.
a ‘just so you know.’ However, although some participants believed they would feel
(P6) anxious for fear of rejection after their disclosure of their own car-
rier status, others discussed a lack of judgment if their partner dis-
Here, their preference for early disclosure reflected their per- closed their carrier status:
ception that carrier status was not a big deal so they would not
fear an adverse reaction. While only a few participants responded I wouldn't be judgmental about it. I'd actually re-
this way, across participants this response occurred where it was spect them more for telling me if they did because
believed both partners would understand that carrier status would that's quite a personal thing to share and the fact that
only pose reproductive issues if both individuals were carriers. they've trusted me with that information I'd feel quite
While some felt knowledge of their partners' carrier status was respectful.
most important in the context of reproductive decision-­making, they (P11)
also felt that disclosure should be more casual, highlighting their un-
derstanding of recessive carrier inheritance: The admiration this participant believes they would have after
disclosure highlights some participants' perceptions that disclosure
I don't know I think it should definitely be before you would increase relational closeness. This contrasted with the com-
talk about having children. But I don't think it needs to mon expectation that others would be stigmatized.
be a big announcement I don't think you need to like When asked to reflect on how they would disclose carrier status
write it in the sky or anything to a partner, one participant discussed the need for intoxication:
(P25)
I'd probably tell them when I was drunk like that's the
Ultimately, despite carrier status disclosure being perceived as only way I can see myself, like I don't think I'd be able to
an anxiety-­provoking experience, many participants believed that bring it up sober I'd just like bring it up on a night out.
disclosure should occur early on, and felt that they had a clear right (P20)
to know their partners' health information. The few who spoke more
casually about disclosure had previously discussed the insignificance This participant also reported that if their partner disclosed they
of carrier status unless both individuals were carriers, and others were a carrier, this would cause them to consider whether a relation-
who saw no need for disclosure at all had no desire for children. ship with someone else would be better.
Many participants also thought that the link of the term carrier
with COVID-­19 would cause issues:
3.8 | Communication challenges:
Projected reactions if people haven't got an awareness of what a carrier
of say, sickle cell could be, but they know that a car-
When asked to reflect on how they would feel disclosing their car- rier of Covid and then associated that with negative
rier status to a potential partner, most believed they would feel anx- because it's Covid, and you're scared and like you say
ious due to fear of judgment. Disclosure was frequently perceived it's a health hazard then people will view a carrier of
as an unnerving experience with carrier status inherently viewed as sickle cell or thalassemia as a negative health hazard
a flaw, potentially increasing the likelihood of a relationship ending: when… it's actually not.
(P6)
I would be worried if they were just going to up and
leave [INT: mhmm] just like straight away, worried Disclosure was perceived to be more difficult for carriers if oth-
that their opinion of me possibly could change for I ers generalized the negative connotations of COVID-­19 to genetic
don't know maybe they had a reason that they didn't carriers. One participant even advised that carriers keep their expe-
want to be with someone who was a carrier. riences private while the population was so exposed to COVID-­19
(P24) coverage:
|

15733599, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/jgc4.1903 by Test, Wiley Online Library on [26/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
10 BOWEN et al.

so yeah it's just a sensitive topic that's likely to have a beliefs develop, would view this thinking as “immanent justice”, this
negative idea related to it so talking about it would be style of thinking is attributed to younger children. However, this be-
would probably be not the best idea at the moment. lief was found in our group in a scientifically literate group of young
(P21) adults. This would fit with functionalist theories of illness under-
standing, which suggests that people can have a varied profile of ill-
This account illustrates many participants' perception that car- ness knowledge based on their experiences and exposure. Although
riers' experience of disclosure would be increasingly challenging we hope these beliefs are rare, it does highlight that even scientifi-
during the COVID-­19 pandemic. cally literate adults can have harmful beliefs about topics over which
they have little knowledge. Our findings suggest such stigma, or
concerns about being stigmatized, may have been exacerbated by
4 | DISCUSSION the COVID-­19 pandemic narrative. This awareness of judgment led
many interviewees to state that there would be anxiety surround-
When trying to understand the impact of carrier identification on in- ing disclosure of their carrier status to peers, especially in romantic
dividuals via NBS, emerging adulthood, is a key age to explore. At this relationships.
stage, the reaction of peers and the formation of romantic relation- The use of a qualitative design in this study is a strength as there
ships are central elements of identity formation. This is the first study are concerns about the use of quantitative measures to explore im-
to explore the views of young adults about carrier status after the pact of genomic labels (Biesecker et al., 2013). We tried to limit the
COVID-­19 pandemic. Most participants had little knowledge about influence of disease knowledge by simply using the term “carrier”
carriers and so used their schematic (Hess & Slaughter, 1990) or pre-­ rather than linking it to a specific condition—thus making the find-
existing knowledge of conditions when considering the impact of car- ings more transferable. The use of diaries was critical in enabling our
rier status. This gave a false sense of certainty about knowledge as participants to engage with and consider the research topic in-­depth
also seen in Noke and Ulph (2014) where they are sure the information (Alaszewski, 2006) before taking part in the interview. Without this,
is correct, but can be seen using it in the wrong context. A systematic interviews run the risk of collecting responses that are more likely to
review of parents' reactions to NBS results suggests that this could be parrot socially acceptable views, without really considering what reac-
due to people's cognitive models of illness models which view results tions would be. As mentioned in the procedure, prior to every inter-
as binary—either you have a label, symptoms, treatment, etc. or you do view, the diaries were summarized, and each interview incorporated
not (Johnson et al., 2022). This could explain some of our findings and the key messages. Therefore, we are confident that this paper does
those who have previously found that people believed autosomal car- not miss any of the central messages contained in that data. Our inter-
riers would suffer health effects (Mayo-­Gamble et al., 2018) or that a views were conducted online. Research comparing face-­to-­face inter-
child was more likely to be a carrier if the mother was older (McClaren views with those conducted online using video-­conferencing software
et al., 2008). What is notable in our study, however, was that people found not only that there was no difference in interview quality
persisted in doing this even after engaging in the diary study where (Cabaroglu et al., 2010) but also that participants were more open
they were asked to engage with information about carriers produced and expressive (Deakin & Wakefield, 2013) which was our experience.
by the NHS and reflect on it. Through the screen share function, we were able to further about
Unfortunately, our data suggest that this way of understanding participants' diary responses and gave participants the space to ex-
carrier status is leading to stigma. The finding was consistent with pand on what they had written, positively contributing to the breadth
previous research showing that people stigmatize carriers (e.g., and depth of data. Although we believe the use of TA to analyze the
Evers-­Kiebooms et al., 1994) and that people are concerned about data has been beneficial given the aims of this study and intended
informing their family and their family's judgment (Ulph et al., 2011). audience, we found individuals hold conflicting views on this topic.
Although our sample could be seen as a “naïve sample” (they were To understand those conflicts and their implications an interpretative
not carriers and had no reason to be engaging with literature on phenomenological study (Smith et al., 2021), a form of in-­depth re-
genetic carriers), one interviewee could recount a family member search that can take a case study approach, enabling result narratives
warning against relationships with carriers. This fits with other re- to give a sense of the individual and how their views track across the
search that has found messages that carriers may be discriminated findings would be valuable. Caution must be taken as however well-­
against in terms of choosing a life partner (Locock & Kai, 2008; Ulph considered the responses, these are hypothetical situations. However,
et al., 2011). Within our sample, participants were even using pre- they do provide insight into the underlying beliefs and where it could
dicted views/reactions of others (as they had not in reality discussed be useful to provide information to reduce stigmatization or distress.
this) to form their own reproductive decisions and that they were Likewise, caution should be taken, as the sample is largely female, well
aware of and sensitive to the judgments of others. educated, and white. Similarly to Noke and Ulph (2014), our sample
The belief that carrier status could be punishment for an in- had low levels of genetic knowledge and they were not confident in
dividual's reactions is worth highlighting even though it only oc- their knowledge. Some mentioned knowing people with a genetic
curred once. The Piagetian theory of illness understandig (Bibace & condition and further research exploring how this impacts views and
Walsh, 1980), one of the theories that seeks to explain how illness understanding would be warranted. Yet, as with the Noke and Ulph
|

15733599, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/jgc4.1903 by Test, Wiley Online Library on [26/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
BOWEN et al. 11

paper, we again found that even in this well-­educated sample, there E T H I C S S TAT E M E N T
were indicators to suggest the merits of an improved general aware- Human studies and informed consent: Approval to conduct this
ness and understanding of what it truly means to be an autosomal human subjects research was obtained by the University of
carrier. Manchester Research Ethics Committee. All procedures followed
The findings of this study show that young adults engaging in were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible
romantic relationships expect their partner to disclose this infor- committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) and
mation. However, the findings also add that carriers could be stig- with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000. Informed
matized, particularly in romantic relationships and more so where consent was obtained from all patients to be included in the study.
there are definite reproductive implications. Although further
research should explore this in more diverse samples, the finding ORCID
warrants further careful consideration about the relative benefits Fiona Ulph https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3590-6542
and harms of identifying autosomal carriers before young adult-
hood if it impacts their romantic relationship formation. They also REFERENCES
highlight an urgent need to ensure that we do not assume that Adefisoye, J. (2021). Knowledge, attitude and perception of people to-
knowledgeable adults will not hold harmful beliefs if we provide wards the novel coronavirus disease (COVID-­19) outbreak and its
effects on their livelihoods: Perspectives from diverse populations.
them with carrier information (as was done in this study). Rather
Health Economics & Outcome Research: Open Access, 7, 169.
it suggests there is a need to work more broadly to ensure that Alaszewski, A. (2006). Using diaries for social research. Sage.
populations are helped to adjust some of the cognitive models they Armstrong, R. E., Frith, L., Ulph, F. M., & Southern, K. W. (2020).
have on how and why people get a medical label, to help people Constructing a bioethical framework to evaluate and optimise new-
born bloodspot screening for cystic fibrosis. International Journal of
understand the implications of autosomal recessive carrier status
Neonatal Screening, 6, 40.
for themselves and others. Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from
the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55, 469.
Barlow-­Stewart, K., Burnett, L., Proos, A., & al e. (2003). A genetic
screening programme for Tay-­Sachs disease and cystic fibrosis for
4.1 | Practice implications
Australian Jewish high school students. Journal of Medical Genetics,
40, e45.
Genetic counselors are well placed to shape initiatives to help people Bathje, G. J., & Marston, H. N. (2014). Self-­stigmatization. In T. Teo (Ed.),
understand the implications of being an autosomal recessive carrier Encyclopedia of critical psychology. Springer.
to reduce stigmatizing behaviors. They have direct access to families Berg, B. (2009). Qualitative research methods for the social sciences. Allyn
& Bacon.
where such discussions are involved but should also be a key voice
Bibace, R., & Walsh, M. E. (1980). Development of children's concepts of
in public health policies. The consideration of how genomic infor- illness. Pediatrics, 66, 912–917.
mation impacts social development is especially urgent as screening Biesecker, B. B., Erby, L. H., Woolford, S., Adcock, J. Y., Cohen, J. S.,
programs are trialing whole-­genome sequencing, which will increase Lamb, A., Lewis, K. V., Truitt, M., Turriff, A., & Reeve, B. B. (2013).
Development and validation of the psychological adaptation scale
the number of young adults in receipt of genomic labels.
(PAS): Use in six studies of adaptation to a health condition or risk.
Patient Education and Counseling, 93, 248–254.
AU T H O R C O N T R I B U T I O N S Borry, P., Fryns, J. P., Schotsmans, P., & Dierickx, K. (2006). Carrier test-
Fiona Ulph confirms that they had full access to all the data in the ing in minors: A systematic review of guidelines and position pa-
pers. European Journal of Human Genetics, 14, 133–138.
study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the ac-
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology.
curacy of the data analysis. All of the authors gave final approval of Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(2), 77–101.
this version to be published and agreed to be accountable for all as- Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2021). Thematic Analysis: A Practical Guide. Sage.
pects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy Brown, R., & Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition, 5, 73–99.
Cabaroglu, N., Basaran, S., & Roberts, J. (2010). A comparison between
or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated
the occurrence of pauses, repetitions and recasts under conditions
and resolved. of face-­to-­face and computer-­mediated communication: A prelim-
inary study. The Turkish Online Journal of Educational Technology, 9,
AC K N OW L E D G M E N T S 14–23.
Chudleigh, J., Buckingham, S., Dignan, J., O'Driscoll, S., Johnson, K., Rees,
We would like to thank all the participants for contributing their time
D., Wyatt, H., & Metcalfe, A. (2016). Parents' experiences of re-
to this study. ceiving the initial positive newborn screening (NBS) result for cys-
tic fibrosis and sickle cell disease. Journal of Genetic Counseling, 25,
C O N FL I C T O F I N T E R E S T S TAT E M E N T 1215–1226.
All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. Cipolletta, S., Andreghetti, G. R., & Mioni, G. (2022). Risk perception to-
wards COVID-­19: A systematic review and qualitative synthesis.
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19,
DATA AVA I L A B I L I T Y S TAT E M E N T 4649.
Data from this study are not available to share due to remit of in- Corrigan, P. W., Larson, J. E., & Kuwabara, S. A. (2010). Social psychology
formed consent given. of the stigma of mental illness: Public and self-­stigma models. In J.
|

15733599, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/jgc4.1903 by Test, Wiley Online Library on [26/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
12 BOWEN et al.

E. Maddux & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Social psychological foundations of McClaren, B. J., Delatycki, M. B., Collins, V., Metcalfe, S. A., & Aitken, M.
clinical psychology. The Guilford Press. (2008). ‘It is not in my world’: An exploration of attitudes and in-
Deakin, H., & Wakefield, K. (2013). Skype interviewing: Reflections of fluences associated with cystic fibrosis carrier screening. European
two PhD researchers. Qualitative Research, 14, 603–616. Journal of Human Genetics, 16, 435–444.
Elliott, H. (1997). The use of diaries in sociological research on health McDonnell, L., Scott, S., & Dawson, M. (2017). A multidimensional view?
experience. Sociological Research Online, 2, 38–48. Evaluating the different and combined contributions of diaries and
Evers-­Kiebooms, G., Denayer, L., Welkenhuysen, M., Cassiman, J. J., & interviews in an exploration of asexual identities and intimacies.
Van Den Berghe, H. (1994). A stigmatizing effect of the carrier sta- Qualitative Research, 17, 520–536.
tus for cystic fibrosis? Clinical Genetics, 46, 336–343. Miller, F. A., Robert, J. S., & Hayeems, R. Z. (2009). Questioning the
Fereday, J., & Muir-­Cochrane, E. (2006). Demonstrating rigor using consensus: Managing carrier status results generated by newborn
thematic analysis: A hybrid approach of inductive and deductive screening. American Journal of Public Health, 99, 210–215.
coding and theme development. International Journal of Qualitative Mischler, E. H., Wilfond, B. S., Fost, N., Laxova, A., Reiser, C., Sauer, C. M.,
Methods, 5, 1–11. & Farrell, P. M. (1998). Cystic fibrosis newborn screening: Impact
Gordon, C., Walpole, I., Zubrick, S. R., & Bower, C. (2003). Population on reproductive behavior and implications for genetic counseling.
screening for cystic fibrosis: Knowledge and emotional conse- Pediatrics, 102, 44–52.
quences 18 months later. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part Mitchell, J., Capua, A., Clow, C. L., & Scriver, C. R. (1996). Twenty-­year
A, 120, 199–208. outcome analysis of genetic screening programs for Tay-­sachs and
Hayeems, R. Z., Bytautas, J. P., & Miller, F. A. (2008). A systematic review BTHalassemia disease carriers in high schools. American Journal of
of the effects of disclosing carrier results generated through new- Human Genetics, 59, 793–798.
born screening. Journal of Genetic Counseling, 17, 538–549. Mitchell, J., Scriver, C. R., Clow, C. L., & Kaplan, F. (1993). What young
Hess, T. M., & Slaughter, S. J. (1990). Schematic knowledge influences people think and do when the option for cystic fibrosis carrier test-
on memory for scene information in young and older adults. ing is available. Journal of Medical Genetics, 30, 538–542.
Developmental Psychology, 26, 855. Moran, J., Quirk, K., Duff, A. J. A., & Brownlee, K. G. (2007). Newborn
Hewson, C. (2008). Internet-­mediated research as an emergent method screening for CF in a regional paediatric centre: The psychosocial
and its potential role in facilitating mixed methods research. In S. effects of false-­positive IRT results on parents. Journal of Cystic
N. Hesse-­Biber & P. Leavy (Eds.), Handbook of emergent methods. Fibrosis, 6, 250–254.
Guilford Press. Noke, M. (2015). The impact of carrier identification of children's wellbeing.
Horrell, B., Stephens, C., & Breheny, M. (2015). Online research with University of Manchester.
informal caregivers: Opportunities and challenges. Qualitative Noke, M., & Ulph, F. (2014). Young adults' pre-­existing knowledge of
Research in Psychology, 12, 258–271. cystic fibrosis and sickle cell diseases: Implications for newborn
Hussein, N., Weng, S. F., Kai, J., Kleijnen, J., & Qureshi, N. (2018). screening. Journal of Genetic Counseling, 23, 121–130.
Preconception risk assessment for thalassaemia, sickle cell disease, Royal College of Physicians, Royal College of Pathologists, & British
cystic fibrosis and Tay-­Sachs disease. The Cochrane Database of Society for Genetic Medicine. (2022). Genetic testing in childhood.
Systematic Reviews, 3, CD010849. Guidance for clinical practice. Report of the Joint Committee on
Johnson, F., Ulph, F., Macleod, R., & Southern, K. (2022). Receiving Genomics in Medicine.
results of uncertain clinical relevance from population genetic Salm, N., Yetter, E., & Tluczek, A. (2012). Informing parents about posi-
screening: Systematic review & meta-­synthesis of qualitative re- tive newborn screen results: Parents' recommendations. Journal of
search. European Journal of Human Genetics, 30, 520–531. Child Health Care, 16, 367–381.
Joshi, S. S., & Deuskar, M. (2016). The role of adolescent romantic re- Schwan, K., Youngblom, J., Weisiger, K., Kianmahd, J., Waggoner, R., &
lationships in identity formation and attachment: An exploratory Fanos, J. (2019). Family perspectives on newborn screening for X-­
study. Indian Journal of Health & Wellbeing, 7, 220–224. linked adrenoleukodystrophy in California. International Journal of
Kai, J., Ulph, F., Cullinan, T., & Qureshi, N. (2009). Communication of car- Neonatal Screening, 5, 42.
rier status information following universal newborn screening for Smith, J. A., Flowers, P., & Larkin, M. (2021). Interpretative phenomenolog-
sickle cell disorders and cystic fibrosis: Qualitative study of experi- ical analysis. Theory, method and research. Sage.
ence and practice. Health Technology Assessment, 13, 1–82, iii. Stamatoyannopoulos, G. (1974). Problems of screening and counseling in
Kvale, S. (1994). InterViews: An introduction to qualitative research inter- the hemoglobinopathies. Motulsky A, Lenz W, eds. In Birth defects.
viewing (Vol. xvii, p. 326). Sage Publications, Inc. Excerpta Medica.
Lang, C. W., Mccolley, S. A., Lester, L. A., & Ross, L. F. (2011). Parental Stone, A. A., Shiffman, S., Schwartz, J. E., Broderick, J. E., & Hufford, M.
understanding of newborn screening for cystic fibrosis after a neg- R. (2002). Patient noncompliance with paper diaries. BMJ (Clinical
ative sweat-­test. Pediatrics, 127, 276–283. research ed.), 324, 1193–1194.
Lewis, C., Skirton, H., & Jones, R. (2010). Can we make assumptions Tluczek, A., Ersig, A. L., & Lee, S. (2022). Psychosocial issues related to
about the psychological impact of living as a carrier, based on newborn screening: A systematic review and synthesis. International
studies assessing the effects of carrier testing. Journal of Genetic Journal of Neonatal Screening, 8, 53.
Counselling, 20, 80–97. Tuckett, A. G. (2005). Applying thematic analysis theory to practice: A
Lewis, S., Curnow, L., Ross, M., & Massie, J. (2006). Parental attitudes researcher's experience. Contemporary Nurse, 19, 75–87.
to the identification of their infants as carriers of cystic fibrosis Ulph, F., & Bennett, R. (2022). Psychological and ethical challenges of in-
by newborn screening. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 42, troducing whole genome sequencing into routine newborn screen-
533–537. ing: Lessons learned from existing newborn screening. The New
Locock, L., & Kai, J. (2008). Parents' experiences of universal screening Bioethics, 29, 1–23.
for haemoglobin disorders: Implications for practice in a new ge- Ulph, F., Cullinan, T., Qureshi, N., & Kai, J. (2011). Familial influences on
netics era. British Journal of General Practice, 58, 161–168. antenatal and newborn haemoglobinopathy screening. Ethnicity &
Mayo-­Gamble, T. L., Barnes, P. A., Cunningham Erves, J., Middlestadt, Health, 16, 361–375.
S. E., & Lin, H. C. (2018). It means everyone should know their sta- Ulph, F., Cullinan, T., Qureshi, N., & Kai, J. (2014). Informing children of
tus': Exploring lay conceptions of sickle cell trait and sickle cell trait their newborn screening carrier result for sickle cell or cystic fi-
screening among African Americans within middle reproductive brosis: Qualitative study of parents' intentions, views and support
age. Ethnicity & Health, 23, 813–829. needs. Journal of Genetic Counseling, 23, 409–420.
|

15733599, 0, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/jgc4.1903 by Test, Wiley Online Library on [26/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
BOWEN et al. 13

Ulph, F., Cullinan, T., Qureshi, N., & Kai, J. (2015). Parents' responses to
receiving sickle cell or cystic fibrosis carrier results for their child S U P P O R T I N G I N FO R M AT I O N
following newborn screening. European Journal of Human Genetics, Additional supporting information can be found online in the
23, 459–465. Supporting Information section at the end of this article.
Ulph, F., Dharni, N., Bennett, R., & Lavender, T. (2020). Consent for new-
born screening: Screening professionals' and parents' views. Public
Health, 178, 151–158.
Ulph, F., Leong, J., Glazebrook, C., & Townsend, E. (2010). A qualitative
How to cite this article: Bowen, E., Langston, J., Fletcher, H.,
study exploring genetic counsellors' experiences of counselling
children. European Journal of Human Genetics, 18, 1090–1094. Domek, J., & Ulph, F. (2024). Understanding the
Ulph, F., Wright, S., Dharni, N., Payne, K., Bennett, R., Roberts, S., psychological impact of identifying carrier status on young
Walshe, K., & Lavender, T. (2017). Provision of information about adults: A qualitative study exploring peer reactions. Journal
newborn screening antenatally: A sequential exploratory mixed-­
of Genetic Counseling, 00, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1002/
methods project. Health Technology Assessment, 21, 1–240.
White, A. L., Boardman, F., Mcniven, A., Locock, L., & Hinton, L. (2021). jgc4.1903
Absorbing it all: A meta-­ethnography of parents' unfolding experi-
ences of newborn screening. Social Science & Medicine, 287, 114367.
WHO. (2020). Clinical management of COVID-­19: Interim guidance.
Wooldridge, E. Q., & Murray, R. F., Jr. (1988). The health orientation
scale: A measure of feelings about sickle cell trait. Social Biology,
35, 123–136.

You might also like