Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

14678624, 2005, 4, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00883.x by Universidad De Cadiz, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2022].

See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Child Development, July/August 2005, Volume 76, Number 4, Pages 869 – 882

How Do Parents Learn About Adolescents’ Experiences?


Implications for Parental Knowledge and Adolescent Risky Behavior
Ann C. Crouter Matthew F. Bumpus
The Pennsylvania State University Washington State University

Kelly D. Davis and Susan M. McHale


The Pennsylvania State University

Cluster analysis was used to identify groups defined by the patterning of fathers’ and mothers’ sources of
knowledge about adolescents’ experiences in a sample of 179 families with adolescents (M 5 16.5 years). Three
clusters emerged for fathers (relational, relies on spouse, relies on others) and mothers (relational, questioners,
relies on others). Cluster membership was associated with socioeconomic status, work hours, personal char-
acteristics, and parent – child relationship quality. Longitudinal path analyses revealed that knowledge sources
predicted levels of knowledge, which in turn predicted risky behavior 1 year later, indirect paths that were more
consistent for fathers than for mothers. Although direct associations between sources of knowledge and sub-
sequent risky behavior were scant, when fathers relied on spouses, youth engaged in less risky behavior.

There is widespread consensus in the fields of de- Recently, researchers have questioned whether
velopmental and clinical psychology, family studies, parental knowledge is driven by parents’ own be-
and public health that parental knowledge of chil- haviors or whether it is the product of a more com-
dren’s and adolescents’ daily experiences is an im- plex array of contextual, relational, and individual
portant correlate of problem behavior: Children forces. Crouter, MacDermid, McHale, and Perry-
whose parents are not well informed about their day- Jenkins (1990) conceptualized parental monitoring as
to-day activities, whereabouts, and companions are a dyadic phenomenon, requiring interest and atten-
more likely to engage in delinquency and other tiveness on the part of the parent and a willingness to
forms of problem behavior (see review by Crouter share information and experiences on the part of the
& Head, 2002). Traditionally, parents’ day-to-day child. Stattin and Kerr (2000; Kerr & Stattin, 2000,
knowledge of their children’s experiences has been 2003) argued that parental knowledge develops in
referred to by researchers as parental monitoring, the context of a trusting parent – child relationship
defined by Dishion and McMahon (1998) as ‘‘a set of and has more to do with the child’s willingness to
correlated parenting behaviors involving attention to confide than with the parent’s disposition or ability
and tracking of the child’s whereabouts, activities, to track and monitor the child. Laird, Pettit, Bates,
and adaptations’’ (p. 61). Studies in this genre have and Dodge (2003), using data from four annual as-
tended to equate parental monitoring or parental sessments of parental knowledge and adolescent
knowledge with parents’ tracking and surveillance. delinquent behavior, found evidence for cross-lag-
ged associations in both directions: More knowledge
predicted subsequent lower levels of delinquency,
but lower levels of delinquency predicted higher
This research was supported by a grant from the National In-
levels of knowledge.
stitute for Child Health and Human Development (R01-HD32336), Unraveling the meaning of parental knowledge
Susan McHale and Ann Crouter, co-principal investigators. The requires focusing on how parents learn about their
authors are grateful to their collaborators: Christina Chhin, Aryn children’s daily lives, a topic that has only recently
Dotterer, Melissa Fortner, Heather Helms, Kristen Johnston, Marni received attention (Kerr & Stattin, 2000, 2003; Stattin
Kan, Ji-Yeon Kim, Mary Maguire Klute, Jaime Marks, Ashleigh
& Kerr, 2000; Waizenhofer, Buchanan, & Jackson-
May, Carolyn Ransford, Lilly Shanahan, Cindy Shearer, Corinna
Jenkins Tucker, Kim Updegraff, Shawn Whiteman, and Megan Newsom, 2004). In this study, we investigated how
Winchell and to the dedicated families who participated in the parents acquire information about their children’s
research. experiences, using a pattern-analytic approach.
Correspondence concerning this articles should be addressed to
Ann Crouter, Center for Work and Family Research, 105 White
Building, The Pennysylvania State University, University Park, r 2005 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
PA, 16802. Electronic mail may be sent to ac1@psu.edu. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2005/7604-0008
14678624, 2005, 4, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00883.x by Universidad De Cadiz, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
870 Crouter, Bumpus, Davis, and McHale

Specifically, we performed cluster analysis to identify and actively participating in activities with the ado-
distinct groups of fathers and mothers who varied in lescent, (b) the passive-child method involved re-
the patterning of their sources of knowledge. We then ceiving unsolicited information from the adolescent
explored the correlates of the groups and examined (akin to Stattin & Kerr’s, 2000, notion of ‘‘child self-
whether, over time, parental knowledge is a potential disclosure’’), (c) the passive-spouse method involved
mechanism that links parents’ strategies for acquiring receiving unsolicited information about the adoles-
information to their adolescents’ subsequent levels of cent’s day from the spouse, and (d) the passive-other
participation in risky behavior. method involved extrapolating from general rou-
tines to the specific (e.g., today is Tuesday so he must
have had soccer practice) and receiving unsolicited
Sources of Parental Knowledge About Offspring’s Daily
information from other people such as teachers and
Experiences
parents of friends. Note that the active method and
The literature points to several ways mothers and the passive-other method combined different sourc-
fathers may acquire knowledge about their off- es into one category, yet each source may have dif-
spring’s experiences. In a large survey of Swedish ferent implications for parental knowledge. Mothers
14-year-olds, Stattin and Kerr (2000) focused on three relied more on active and passive-child methods of
sources: child self-disclosure, parental solicitation, acquiring knowledge than did fathers, whereas fa-
and parental control (i.e., parents have rules in place thers relied more on the passive-spouse method than
that structure children’s behavior, making it easy for did mothers. Parents knew more about daughters
parents to keep abreast of what is going on). Of these than sons, and parents of daughters relied more on
three, child self-disclosure was the strongest corre- the passive-child method than did parents of sons.
late of parental knowledge and of adolescents’ de- For mothers in dual-earner families, relying on the
linquent behavior. When youth self-disclosed more, active method and the passive-other method pre-
parents were better informed and offspring engaged dicted higher levels of knowledge. For fathers, rely-
in fewer delinquent behaviors. In a set of related ing on the active method and the passive-other
analyses, Kerr and Stattin (2000) found that child method predicted higher levels of knowledge, re-
self-disclosure fully or partially mediated the asso- gardless of the family’s earner status. Sources of
ciation between parents’ knowledge and a wide knowledge, however, were not associated with ado-
range of indexes of adolescent adjustment. Parental lescent deviance.
solicitation and parental control, in contrast, did not
explain the association between parental knowledge
Taking a Pattern-Analytic Approach
and adolescent outcomes.
One limitation of Stattin and Kerr’s (2000; Kerr & The slim body of research that has examined the
Stattin, 2000) research is that they did not differentiate sources of parent knowledge has taken a variable-
between mothers and fathers but asked questions oriented approach. That is, the focus has been
about parents generically. Blurring the identity of the on ascertaining the relative importance of various
focal parent is problematic because mothers and fa- sources of knowledge as predictors of parental
thers often have distinctly different roles and ap- knowledge or adolescent outcomes. Results from
proaches to parenting (Parke, 2002). Differences such variable-oriented approaches are informative,
between maternal and paternal roles can be seen in a but they may obscure interesting subgroups of par-
study of dual-earner families raising two school-age ents for whom patterns look different. In this study,
children (Crouter, Helms-Erikson, Updegraff, & we adopted a pattern-analytic approach (Bergman,
McHale, 1999). Overall, mothers were better informed Cairns, & Kagan, 1998) to identify distinct groups of
about their school-age children’s daily experiences fathers and mothers defined by their sources of pa-
than were fathers. Moreover, in families with sons and rental knowledge. In an effort to examine a broad
daughters, fathers were relatively more knowledgea- range of potential sources of parents’ knowledge, we
ble about sons than daughters, and mothers were examined sources identified by Stattin and Kerr
more informed about daughters than sons. (2000; Kerr & Stattin, 2000) and by Waizenhofer et al.
Waizenhofer et al. (2004) compared mothers’ and (2004), and we added some of our own. Specifically,
fathers’ sources of knowledge, using a sample of 95 we examined six potential sources: adolescent self-
families with adolescent offspring ages 10 to 17. They disclosure, parent solicitation, a subtler form of pa-
identified four methods of acquiring knowledge: (a) rental surveillance that we call ‘‘parental listening
the active method combined asking others (includ- and observing,’’ relying on spouse, relying on sib-
ing the adolescent) about the adolescent’s activities lings, and relying on others outside the family (e.g.,
14678624, 2005, 4, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00883.x by Universidad De Cadiz, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Sources of Fathers’ and Mothers’ Knowledge 871

teachers, parents of friends). We examined fathers idence that depression may interfere with parents’
and mothers separately, reasoning that the resulting abilities to interact positively and constructively with
typologies and correlates of the typologies may differ their children (Downey & Coyne, 1990).
for mothers and fathers, and that we could juxtapose A fourth domain is marital quality. Bumpus,
the father and mother groups to examine possible Crouter, and McHale (1999) found that parents in
interparental congruence. families characterized by high paternal work de-
mands knew less about their children’s daily expe-
riences when the marital relationship was less
What Contextual and Personal Conditions Underlie
happy. In contrast, when happily married fathers
Parents’ Source of Knowledge?
held demanding jobs, parents’ knowledge did not
After identifying groups of parents defined by the suffer. Parents in happier marriages may rely more
patterning of their sources of parental knowledge, on one another for knowledge than parents in less
we investigated the correlates of group membership happy unions, and children may feel more comfort-
to understand better the groups. The literature on able self-disclosing to parents who present a happy,
parental knowledge and parental monitoring sug- united front.
gests five domains of possible correlates. The first Kerr and Stattin’s (2000) research suggests that an
pertains to socioeconomic and background charac- important fifth domain is the quality of the parent –
teristics of the family. Parents who are less educated, child relationship. Children may be more willing to
less affluent, or who have more children may find it self-disclose to parents when they enjoy a close
more challenging to stay informed about their chil- relationship with the parent. We examined two di-
dren’s daily experiences or may have different mensions of the parent – child relationship: accept-
strategies for attaining that knowledge than do par- ance and conflict.
ents with more resources (Crouter & Head, 2002).
We included mothers’ and fathers’ work hours as
How Are Parents’ Sources of Knowledge Related to Levels
a second domain because Crouter et al. (1999) re-
of Knowledge and Adolescent Problem Behavior?
ported that, although mothers’ own levels of
knowledge did not vary as a function of their work One criterion for the utility of the profiles gener-
hours, fathers exhibited higher levels of knowledge ated here is whether they are related to how much
about their children’s daily experiences when their parents know about their children’s daily lives and
spouses worked longer hours. Crouter et al. found to adolescent problem behavior. As mentioned ear-
no associations between fathers’ work hours and lier, Stattin and Kerr (2000) found that youth self-
parents’ knowledge, but it is possible that fathers disclosure was the strongest predictor of parental
who work long hours favor certain knowledge- knowledge and problem behavior. Waizenhofer et al.
acquisition strategies, such as relying on their spouse. (2004) found links between sources of knowledge
A third domain pertains to the personal qualities and parents’ levels of knowledge, although they did
adolescents and parents bring to the parent – child not find links between sources of knowledge and
relationship (Auhagen & Hinde, 1997). For youth, adolescent problem behavior. We tested one possible
these include gender (Crouter et al., 1999; Wai- longitudinal mechanism for how these constructs
zenhofer et al., 2004); expressivity or stereotypically may be interrelated: Parents who have different ap-
feminine qualities, such as friendliness and sensi- proaches to obtaining knowledge about their off-
tivity (Crouter et al., 1999); and children’s openness spring’s experiences acquire different levels of
to supervision (Kerns, 2001). Adolescents who are knowledge, and knowledge in turn mediates the link
more expressive and open to supervision may keep between knowledge-acquisition strategies and ado-
their parents better informed or simply be easier to lescent risky behavior. Some would argue, however,
track than other children. Fathers’ and mothers’ that risky behavior is as much an antecedent of
personal qualities may matter as well (Belsky, 1984). parents’ knowledge acquisition methods as an out-
Adults’ expressive characteristics are positively come because youth hold back information when
linked to closer relationships (Auhagen & Hinde, they engage in risky behavior (Darling, 2001; Kerr &
1997) and thus may be associated with how mothers Stattin, 2003; Laird et al., 2003; Stattin & Kerr, 2000).
and fathers learn about their children’s experiences. To address this concern, we used path analysis to
For example, expressive parents may inspire trust in examine the links among risky behavior at age 13,
children that results in more self-disclosure (Kerr, sources of knowledge at age 16, parents’ levels of
Stattin, & Trost, 1999). Another potentially important knowledge at age 16, and offspring risky behavior at
parental characteristic is depressive affect given ev- age 17.
14678624, 2005, 4, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00883.x by Universidad De Cadiz, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
872 Crouter, Bumpus, Davis, and McHale

Research Questions The 179 families who are our primary focus re-
sided in small cities, towns, suburban areas, and
To summarize, toward the end of understanding
rural communities and were working and middle
how parental knowledge works as a family process
class. All families were European American except
that facilitates positive youth development, we ad-
one in which there were two adopted Asian Ameri-
dressed the following research questions:
can children. In Year 6, fathers were on average 44.52
1. Are there distinct groups of fathers and moth- years old (SD 5 5.05) and had received 14.80 years of
ers defined by the patterning of their sources of education (SD 5 2.44). Their spouses were on aver-
knowledge about their offspring’s experiences? age 42.36 years old (SD 5 3.97) and had received
Is there within-family concordance in fathers’ 14.84 years of education (SD 5 2.18). The sample in-
and mothers’ group membership? cluded 87 firstborn boys and 92 firstborn girls, and
2. How is group membership associated with family size, including children and parents, aver-
socioeconomic status and family background aged 4.63 (SD 5 .86).
characteristics, parents’ work hours, adoles- Our sampling procedure did not allow us to com-
cents’ and parents’ personal characteristics, pute a response rate because we could not determine
and the quality of marital and parent – child how many eligible families did not respond to the
relationships? invitation to participate. Comparisons of our original
3. Does parental knowledge mediate the cross- sample of 203 families with 1990 U.S. Census data on
time association between parents’ group families from the same counties, however, revealed
membership and subsequent risky behavior? that the parents in our sample were slightly older and
considerably better educated than their dual-earner
Census counterparts. We also compared the 24 fami-
Method lies who either had dropped out of the study or were
omitted from the analyses with the 179 participating
Participants
families on Year 1 data on parents’ education, income,
Most of the data for these analyses were drawn and family size. The nonparticipants differed signifi-
from Year 6 of an ongoing longitudinal investigation cantly in only one way: Fathers in nonparticipating
of development in middle childhood and adoles- families were less educated (M 5 13.63, SD 5 2.30)
cence. We focused on Year 6 because it was the only than fathers in participating families (M 5 14.80,
year in which we asked about sources of knowledge. SD 5 2.44), F(1, 201) 5 5.10, po.05.
Firstborns (the focus of this study) were, on average,
16.5 years old. The path analyses examined associa-
Procedures
tions across a 4-year period, specifically, the links
among risky behavior at age 13.5, sources of In Years 1, 2, 3, 6, and 7, a team of interviewers
knowledge (when offspring were age 16.5), levels of made home visits and conducted separate interviews
parental knowledge (when offspring were age 16.5), with the mother, father, and firstborn and second-
and risky behavior at age 17.5. born siblings. These interviews took approximately 2
To recruit the original sample of 203 families, let- hr. Families received a $100 honorarium in Years 1, 2,
ters were sent home from school that described the and 3, and a $200 honorarium in Years 6 and 7. In
study and asked families to return a self-addressed Years 4 and 5, a period when the project was between
postcard if they were interested and met the criteria. funding cycles, families returned mailed surveys.
We sought nondivorced, two-parent families in Because data were less complete in those 2 years, we
which the firstborn was in the fourth or fifth grade do not rely on them here. With the exception of three
and there was at least one other sibling 1 to 4 years measuresFparents’ expressivity (asked only at Year
younger. Between Years 1 and 6, 10 families dropped 1), parents’ perceptions of their children’s openness
out of the study. Of the 193 families who participated to supervision (asked only at Year 3), and adolescent
in Year 6, we omitted 13 families because we lacked risky behavior (data from Years 3 and 7 were used in
data from one of the parents because of: father de- the longitudinal analyses)Fwe relied on Year 6 data.
clined to participate (4), father deceased (5), and di-
vorce (4). In addition, one family was dropped
Measures
because of missing data on a clustering variable.
Thus, the sample on which most of the analyses were Sources of knowledge. Mothers and fathers com-
based consisted of 179 families. The longitudinal pleted six scales about how they acquired informa-
path analyses were based on a sample size of 171. tion about their child’s activities, whereabouts, and
14678624, 2005, 4, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00883.x by Universidad De Cadiz, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Sources of Fathers’ and Mothers’ Knowledge 873

companions. We borrowed two measures from Stat- own expressivity and parents’ reports of children’s
tin and Kerr (2000): a six-item measure of child self- openness to supervision (asked at Year 3). Express-
disclosure (e.g., ‘‘Does your child spontaneously tell ivity was measured using the 6-item expressivity
you about what he/she has been doing with scale from the Antill Trait Questionnaire (Antill,
friends?’’) and a five-item measure of parental soli- Russell, Goodnow, & Cotton, 1993), which consists of
citation (e.g., ‘‘Do you start conversations with your six female-typed characteristics on which youth rate
child about his/her free time activities?’’). Both themselves. A sample item is ‘‘Gentle: This is the sort
measures used 5-point scales ranging from 1 (almost of person who is careful not to hurt other people. Are
never) to 5 (almost always). The other four source you like that?’’ Adolescents used a 5-point scale
measures were developed for this study, borrowing ranging from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always);
from the approaches used by Stattin and Kerr (2000) Cronbach’s alpha was .78. In Year 3, parents com-
and Waizenhofer et al. (2004). Specifically, each of the pleted the Children’s Openness to Supervision
other four sources was tapped with three items. Measure, a 12-item scale adapted from the Child
Parents were asked three general questions: ‘‘How Check-in Scale (2001; K. A. Kerns, personal com-
do you usually learn about when your child has munication, October 1996) that taps how amenable
misbehaved in some way, e.g., disobeyed, broken a children are to parental efforts to keep track of their
rule?’’ ‘‘How do you usually learn about how your activities. Using a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (not at
child has been spending his/her free time?’’ ‘‘How all) to 5 (very often), parents indicated how often, for
do you usually learn about what homework assign- example, their child ‘‘volunteers information about
ments your child has?’’ For each question, using a 5- plans’’ and ‘‘can be relied upon to return at the
point scale ranging from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost agreed upon time.’’ Cronbach’s alphas were .92 and
always), parents indicated how often they used each .90 for fathers and mothers, respectively.
of four different approaches: (a) ‘‘I can usually tell In Year 1, parents completed the Bem Sex Role
about this by observing and listening to my child’’; Inventory (Bem, 1974) which includes 60 adjectives,
(b) ‘‘My spouse usually keeps me informed about 20 of which are considered feminine or expressive
this’’; (c) ‘‘My child’s sister(s) and brother(s) usually (e.g., warm, friendly, likes children), using a 7-point
keeps me informed about this’’; and (d) ‘‘People scale ranging from 1 (never or almost never true) to 7
outside the family (e.g., teachers, neighbors, child’s (always or almost always true). Cronbach’s alphas were
friends) usually keep me informed about this.’’ .69 for mothers and .76 for fathers. In Year 6, mothers
Cronbach’s alphas ranged from .79 to .85 for fathers and fathers completed the 10-item Center for Epi-
and from .66 to .84 for mothers. (The .66 alpha was demiological Studies Depression Scale (Devins &
for listen and observe.) We did not ask adolescents to Orme, 1985) that measures depressive symptoms
report on how much their parents listened and ob- experienced during the past week (e.g., ‘‘I felt that
served, relied on spouse, relied on sibling, or relied everything I did was an effort’’) on a scale from 1
on others because we did not think youth would be (rarely or none of the time) to 4 (most of the time).
able to report accurately on these processes. Youth Cronbach’s alphas were .82 for fathers and .84 for
did complete the measures of child self-disclosure mothers.
and parent solicitation, however, and parents’ and Quality of the marital relationship. Using a 9-point
adolescents’ reports were significantly correlated scale, mothers and fathers completed the nine-item
(r 5.45, po.001; r 5.25, po.001; r 5 .53, po.001; marital love (e.g., ‘‘How committed do you feel to-
r 5.37, po.001 for self-disclosure to mother, mother ward your partner?’’) and five-item conflict (e.g.,
solicitation, self-disclosure to father, and father soli- ‘‘How often do you and your partner argue with one
citation, respectively). another?’’) scales from the Relationships Question-
Family background. Mothers and fathers reported naire (Braiker & Kelley, 1979). Cronbach’s alphas
their ages, educational attainment, occupations were .93 and .94 for the love scale, and .71 and .72 for
(coded for prestige using the NORC system; Nakao & the conflict scale, for fathers and mothers, respec-
Treas, 1994), annual wages from jobs, and family size. tively.
Parents’ work hours. Mothers and fathers reported Parent – adolescent relationship. Mothers and fa-
the number of hours they worked per week and the thers completed a 24-item subscale of the parent
number of hours they spent on work-related activi- version of the Child’s Report of Parental Behavior
ties at home, which were summed to create an index Inventory (CRPBI; Schwarz, Barton-Henry, &
of total weekly work hours. Pruzinsky, 1985; Schaefer, 1965) to tap feelings of
Personal characteristics. In addition to gender and warmth and acceptance in the parent – child rela-
age, we had access to adolescents’ reports of their tionship. Using a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (not
14678624, 2005, 4, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00883.x by Universidad De Cadiz, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
874 Crouter, Bumpus, Davis, and McHale

like you) to 4 (a lot like you), respondents rated what po.001, and Source  Parent  Gender, F(5, 885) 5
they were like as parents (e.g., ‘‘I am a parent who 4.83, po.001, interactions. We followed up in two
seems to see my child’s good points more than his/ ways. First, we conducted a series of 6 (source)  2
her faults’’). Cronbach’s alphas were .92 and .93 for (gender) ANOVAs separately for each source of
mothers and fathers, respectively. The measure of knowledge. As can be seen in Table 1, mothers’ mean
conflict between parents and adolescent offspring, scores on child self-disclosure, listen and observe,
adapted from Smetana (1988), assessed the frequen- and solicitation were significantly higher than fa-
cy of conflict in 11 domains: chores, appearance, thers’, but fathers relied on mothers significantly
homework and schoolwork, social life, dating and more than mothers relied on them for self-disclosure,
romantic relationships, bedtime and curfew, health, F(1, 177) 5 63.18, po.001; listen and observe, F(1,
choosing activities, money, behavior and personality, 177) 5 31.07, po.001; solicitation, F(1, 177) 5 68.85,
and relationships with brothers and sisters. Mothers po.001; and relies on spouse, F(1, 177) 5 159.40,
and fathers answered each item using a 6-point po.001. Mothers and fathers did not differentially
Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 6 rely on siblings or others. Gender differences were
(several times a day) to represent how often they had apparent only for self-disclosure, F(1, 177) 5 6.17,
conflicts in these areas during the previous year. po.05, and relying on others, F(1, 177) 5 5.22, po.05.
Cronbach’s alphas were .84 and .85 for mothers and Girls (M 5 32.72, SD 5 5.34) self-disclosed more than
fathers, respectively. did boys (M 5 30.67, SD 5 4.65), and parents of boys
Parental knowledge. Parents provided estimates of (M 5 8.95, SD 5 2.92) relied more on others for in-
how much they knew about their adolescents’ daily formation than did parents of girls (M 5 7.84,
experiences using a measure developed by Stattin SD 5 2.99). The only significant Parent  Gender
and Kerr (2000). This nine-item measure (e.g.,’’How interaction emerged for relies on spouse, F(1,
often do you know what (your child) does in his/her 177) 5 10.03, po.02. Fathers relied more on mothers
free time?’’) asked how much the parent knew about than mothers relied on fathers overall, but parents
the adolescent’s daily activities, whereabouts, and each relied more on spouses for knowledge about
companions on a scale from 1 (almost never) to 5 (al- offspring of the other sex. Thus, fathers relied more
most always). Cronbach’s alphas were .85 and .83 for on mothers about daughters (M 5 11.46, SD 5 2.39)
fathers’ and mothers’ reports, respectively. Parents’ than sons (M 5 10.44, SD 5 2.51), whereas mothers
reports of knowledge were positively correlated with relied more on fathers about sons (M 5 7.49,
adolescents’ estimates of their parents’ knowledge SD 5 3.06) than daughters (M 5 6.54, SD 5 3.16).
(r 5.37, po.001; r 5.32, po.001, for mothers and fa- We also computed t tests for dependent samples
thers, respectively). to compare each parent’s use of particular sources of
Adolescents’ risky behavior. Youth completed an 18- knowledge. Fathers were most likely to report rely-
item measure adapted from Eccles and Barber’s ing on their spouse, followed by solicitation (see
(1990) Risky Behavior Scale assessing involvement in Table 1). Mothers were most likely to favor solicita-
behaviors such as drinking, using drugs and tobacco, tion, followed by child self-disclosure. For both
and skipping school. On a 4-point scale (1 5 never, parents, relying on others outside the family was the
2 5 once, 3 5 2 – 10 times, 4 5 more than 10 times), least common source of knowledge.
adolescents indicated how often they had engaged in
the activities over the past year. Cronbach’s alphas Table 1
were .82 (Year 3), .89 (Year 6), and .88 (Year 7). Means (and Standard Deviations) for Fathers’ and Mothers’ Sources of
Knowledge

Results Variable Father (n 5 179) Mother (n 5 179)

Parents’ Sources of Knowledge: Descriptive Analyses Child self-disclosure 3.23 (0.61)a,y 3.64 (0.70)a,z
Listen/observe 3.22 (0.74)a,y 3.61 (0.68)a,z
We performed a 2 (parent)  6 (source)  2 (ado- Solicitation 3.61 (0.56)b,y 4.10 (0.57)b,z
lescent gender) mixed model analysis of variance Spouse 3.66 (0.83)b,y 2.34 (1.05)c,z
(ANOVA) to examine mean differences in sources Siblings 2.37 (0.89)c 2.40 (0.87)c
of knowledge. Parent and source were treated as Other 1.76 (0.74)d 1.91 (0.88)d
within-group factors, and gender was treated as
a between-group factor. This analysis revealed a Note. Subscripts a, b, c, and d indicate significant (po.05) within-
parent source differences (i.e., source means that differ within
main effect for source, F(5, 885) 5 364.12, po.001, columns); subscripts y and z indicate significant (po.05) mother –
and significant Source  Parent, F(5, 885) 5 94.64, father differences (i.e., source means that differ within rows).
14678624, 2005, 4, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00883.x by Universidad De Cadiz, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Sources of Fathers’ and Mothers’ Knowledge 875

Table 2
Correlations Between Sources of Knowledge for Mothers and Fathers (N 5 179)

1 2 3 4 5 6

1. Disclosure .44 .29 .03 .12 .36


2. Listen/observe .41 .36 .29 .20 .01
3. Solicitation .40 .30 .07 .06 .06
4. Spouse .23 .20 .05 .47 .36
5. Sibling .03 .13 .02 .27 .43
6. Other .13 .01 .03 .04 .27

Note. Mothers’ data are above, and fathers’ data are below the diagonal.
po.05. po.01. po.001.

We next computed bivariate correlations among 176) 5 8.22, po.001; solicitation, F(2, 176) 5 5.41,
the six sources of knowledge separately for mothers po.01; spouse, F(2, 176) 5 47.30, po.001; and others,
and fathers (see Table 2). For both parents, child self- F(2, 176) 5 6.14, po.01. A nonsignificant trend was
disclosure, listening and observing, and solicitation found for siblings, F(2, 176) 5 2.52, po.10. Tukey
were positively correlated, and relying on spouse follow-up tests revealed that, compared with other
was positively related to listening and observing and fathers, Cluster 1 (n 5 62) fathers reported high levels
relying on siblings. For fathers, but not mothers, re- of child self-disclosure, parental solicitation, and
lying on spouse was also positively related to ado- listen and observe, and relatively low scores on re-
lescent self-disclosure. For fathers, the only lying on spouse, siblings, and others. Because Clus-
significant correlate of relying on others outside the ter 1 fathers learned about their children’s daily
family was relying on siblings. For mothers, relying experiences by relating directly to their children, we
on others was significantly and positively associated called this cluster relational. Fathers in Cluster 2
with relying on spouse and on siblings, and nega- (n 5 56) had mid-range scores on all sources except
tively associated with self-disclosure. spouse. Because these fathers relied heavily on their
spouses as sources of information as compared with
the other groups, we called this cluster relies on
Identifying Father and Mother Clusters
spouse. Fathers in Cluster 3 (n 5 61) had the lowest
Each cluster analysis was conducted on stand- scores on self-disclosure, listen and observe, solici-
ardized variables reflecting the target parent’s six tation, and spouse, and the highest scores on siblings
sources of knowledge. We used an agglomerative and others. Because the mean differences between
hierarchical approach (Blashfield, 1976; Blashfield & clusters were more striking for others than for sib-
Aldenderfer, 1978, 1988); the similarity index was lings, we called Cluster 3 relies on others.
cosine (Cronbach & Gleser, 1953), and average was
specified as the cluster linkage method (Sneath &
Sokal, 1973). For both fathers and mothers, the three-
1
cluster solution produced interesting groups with
reasonable cell sizes. The cluster solutions were
replicated by splitting the sample randomly in half, 0.5
conducting a cluster analysis on each half, and ex-
amining the q correlations between the newly 0
formed clusters. For mothers, q correlations between
the similar clusters ranged from .85 to .92; for fathers,
q correlations ranged from .89 to .94 (all pso.05). −0.5

To describe the clusters, we conducted a series of


one-way ANOVAs separately for each parent, with −1
the focal parent’s clusters as the independent varia- Child Disclose Listen Solicit Spouse Sibling Others

ble and the sources of knowledge as the dependent Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3
variables. For fathers (see Figure 1), significant Figure 1. Fathers’ three-cluster solution defined by six standard-
cluster differences were found for self-disclosure, ized measures of sources of knowledge about adolescent offspring
F(2, 176) 5 20.56, po.001; listen and observe, F(2, experiences.
14678624, 2005, 4, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00883.x by Universidad De Cadiz, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
876 Crouter, Bumpus, Davis, and McHale

1 mothers’ Cluster 3 relies on others to signal its simi-


larity to fathers’ Cluster 3. A 3 (father clusters)  3
0.5 (mother clusters) chi-square analysis revealed sig-
nificant parental concordance, w2(4, n 5 179) 5 15.35,
0 po.01 (see Table 3).

−0.5
Correlates of Father and Mother Clusters
−1
Child Disclose Listen Solicit Spouse Sibling Others Demographic correlates. We next explored the cor-
Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 3
relates of the clusters. Our first step was to examine
associations among cluster membership, adolescent
Figure 2. Mothers’ three-cluster solution defined by six standard-
gender, and family background characteristics. First
ized measures of sources of knowledge about adolescent offspring
experiences. we conducted two 3 (cluster)  2 (adolescent gender)
chi-square analyses. A significant association was
found for fathers, w2(2, n 5 179) 5 8.42, po.02. We
For mothers (see Figure 2), there were significant performed follow-up chi-square analyses separately
cluster effects for self-disclosure, F(2, 176) 5 31.56, for each cluster to pinpoint significant gender dif-
po.001; listen and observe, F(2, 176) 5 10.63, po.001; ferences. Relational fathers were equally likely to
solicitation, F(2, 176) 5 3.26, po.05; spouse, F(2, have daughters (n 5 31; 50%) and sons (n 5 31; 50%),
176) 5 36.14, po.001; siblings, F(2, 176) 5 20.55, w2(1, n 5 62) 5 .00, ns. Fathers who relied on spouses
po.001; and others, F(2, 176) 5 30.88, po.001. Cluster were significantly more likely to have daughters
1 mothers (n 5 78) stood out in terms of high levels of (n 5 37; 66%) than sons (n 5 19; 34%), w2(1,
child self-disclosure and listen and observe, leading n 5 62) 5 5.79, po.02. There was a trend for fathers
us to call them relational because they resembled fa- who relied on others to be more likely to have sons
thers’ Cluster 1. Cluster 2 (n 5 48) had no counter- (n 5 37; 61%) than daughters (n 5 24; 39%), w2(1,
part among the father clusters: These mothers n 5 61) 5 2.77, po.10. The overall chi-square analysis
reported relatively low levels of all sources of for mothers revealed that the association between
knowledge except parental solicitation. Because this cluster membership and adolescent gender was at
was the only cluster with above average scores on the level of a trend, w2(2, n 5 179) 5 4.74, po.10, but
solicitation, we called this group questioners (but note follow-ups performed separately for each cluster
that the follow-up test did not reveal significant revealed no significant gender differences, leading
mean differences on maternal solicitation). Cluster 3 us to conclude that gender and cluster were not as-
mothers (n 5 53) relied heavily on other people, but sociated for mothers.
unlike the relies-on-others father cluster, these We next conducted a series of one-way ANOVAs
mothers relied on their spouses, other children in the separately for fathers and mothers, focused on the
family, and others outside the family. In other words, measures reflecting socioeconomic status (i.e., edu-
for mothers, unlike fathers, spouses did not stand cation, occupational prestige, income) and back-
out as a special source of knowledge. We called ground characteristics (i.e., parents’ and adolescent’s

Table 3
Within-Family Concordance in Fathers’ and Mothers’ Cluster Membership (N 5 179)

Father clusters

1 2 3
Mother clusters Relational Relies on spouse Relies on others Total

1 30 29 19 78
Relational 48% 52% 31%
2 17 18 13 48
Questioners 27% 32% 21%
3 15 9 29 53
Relies on others 24% 16% 48%
Total 62 56 61 179

Note. w2(4, n 5 179) 5 15.35, po.01; percentages refer to columns.


14678624, 2005, 4, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00883.x by Universidad De Cadiz, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Sources of Fathers’ and Mothers’ Knowledge 877

ages, family size). For fathers, cluster main effects Similarly, fathers in Cluster 1 (relational; M 5 50.98,
emerged for education, F(2, 176) 5 7.54, po.001; oc- SE 5 0.89) saw their offspring as more open to su-
cupational prestige, F(2, 173) 5 4.25, po.02; and age, pervision than did fathers in Cluster 3 (relies on
F(2, 175) 5 3.13, po.05. Relational fathers were better others; M 5 47.02, SE 5 0.94). The mean for fathers
educated, held jobs with higher prestige, and were who relied on spouses (M 5 48.56, SE 5 0.99) did not
slightly older than fathers in the other clusters. For differ from the other groups.
mothers, a main effect for cluster emerged for edu- Fathers’ personal characteristics (i.e., expressivity,
cation, F(2, 176) 5 7.26, po.001. Mothers in Cluster 2 depressive symptoms) were not associated with the
(questioners) were better educated than other father clusters, nor were significant correlates found
mothers. with regard to the quality of the marital relationship.
The background correlates of the father clusters With respect to the father – adolescent relationship,
were consistent, and all appeared to reflect socio- a significant main effect for cluster emerged for
economic status. Given that education emerged as an conflict, F(2, 170) 5 7.41, po.001. Fathers in Cluster 3
important cluster correlate for both fathers and (relies on others; M 5 29.49, SE 5 0.88) reported sig-
mothers, that education is frequently used as a nificantly more concurrent conflict with their ado-
marker for family socioeconomic status (Hughes & lescents than did fathers in either Cluster 1
Perry-Jenkins, 1996), and that maternal and paternal (relational; M 5 25.55, SE 5 0.83) or Cluster 2 (relies
education were correlated (r 5.47, po.001), we on spouse; M 5 24.93, SE 5 0.92); the latter groups
computed the mean of both parents’ education and were not significantly different from one another. No
treated it as a covariate in the remainder of the group differences were found in the analysis of
analyses. In addition, because adolescent gender was warmth and acceptance.
confounded with the father clusters and was likely to Correlates of mother clusters. The only domain that
be related to some of the correlates of interest, we produced significant correlates was adolescents’
included adolescent gender as a factor in subsequent personal characteristics. Significant cluster effects
analyses. were found for expressivity, F(2, 171) 5 3.63, po.05,
In a series of 3 (cluster)  2 (adolescent gender) and mothers’ prior reports of their offspring’s
analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs), treating parent openness to supervision, F(2, 171) 5 13.24, po.001.
education as a covariate, we examined correlates of Specifically, offspring of Cluster 2 mothers (ques-
cluster membership in four domains: parents’ work tioners; M 5 24.11, SE 5 0.52) described themselves
hours, adolescents’ and parents’ personal qualities, as significantly more expressive than did offspring of
marital quality, and parent – child relationship. Sig- Cluster 3 mothers (relies on others; M 5 22.16,
nificant cluster differences were followed up with SE 5 0.51). The mean for Cluster 1 (relational;
Tukey tests (all means reported are adjusted means). M 5 22.88, SE 5 0.41) was not significantly different
To streamline results, we report only main effects or from either group. All three groups differed signifi-
interactions involving cluster; we do not report main cantly on the child’s openness to supervision. Rela-
effects for adolescent gender or parent education. tional mothers (M 5 55.03, SE 5 0.65) saw their
Correlates of father clusters. A main effect for clus- children as most open to supervision, followed by
ter was found for fathers’ work hours, F(2, questioners (M 5 52.34, SE 5 0.83) and mothers who
171) 5 4.55, po.05. Fathers in Cluster 2 (relies on relied on others (M 5 49.86, SE 5 0.82).
spouse) worked significantly longer hours
(M 5 53.11 hr per week, SE 5 1.51) than fathers in
Are the Clusters Related to Parental Knowledge and to
Clusters 1 (relational; M 5 47.22, SE 5 1.36) or 3 (re-
Adolescent Risky Behavior?
lies on others; M 5 48.52, SE 5 1.44).
With respect to child characteristics, cluster effects One test of the utility of a typology defined by
emerged for adolescents’ reports of their (concur- parents’ sources of knowledge is whether group
rent) expressive qualities, F(2, 171) 5 4.27, po.05, membership is related to parents’ levels of knowl-
and fathers’ (Year 3) perceptions of children’s open- edge and adolescent risky behavior. To explore this
ness to supervision, F(2, 171) 5 4.58, po.05. Off- issue, we conducted 3 (cluster)  2 (adolescent gen-
spring of fathers in Cluster 1 (relational; M 5 23.97, der) ANCOVAs separately for fathers and mothers,
SE 5 0.45) rated themselves as more expressive than treating parental education as a covariate and pa-
did offspring of fathers in Cluster 3 (relies on others; rental knowledge and concurrent adolescent risky
M 5 22.30, SE 5 0.47); the mean for offspring of fa- behavior as the dependent variables. For fathers,
thers in Cluster 2 (relies on spouse; M 5 22.33, cluster membership was associated with knowledge,
SE 5 0.49) did not differ from the other two groups. F(2, 172) 5 14.51, po.001, and risky behavior, F(2,
14678624, 2005, 4, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00883.x by Universidad De Cadiz, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
878 Crouter, Bumpus, Davis, and McHale

177) 5 4.21, po.05. Relational fathers (M 5 36.27, .41***


SE 5 0.59) and fathers who relied on spouses
(M 5 35.43, SE 5 0.65) knew significantly more than Relational
−.30 (16 yrs) .39***
fathers who relied on others (M 5 31.79, SE 5 0.62). Risky Paternal Risky
−.22**
Means for Clusters 1 and 2 were not significantly Behavior Knowledge Behavior
(13 yrs) .06 .34*** (16 yrs) (17 yrs)
different from one another. Consistent with this
Spouse
pattern, offspring of fathers who relied on others (16 yrs)
−.19*
(M 5 29.03, SE 5 1.02) reported significantly higher
levels of risky behavior than offspring of relational −.14*
fathers (M 5 25.33, SE 5 0.96) and fathers who relied Figure 3. Path analysis depicting connections among adolescent
on spouses (M 5 25.36, SE 5 1.06); the latter groups risky behavior at age 13, fathers’ sources of knowledge, paternal
did not differ significantly from one another. knowledge, and adolescent risky behavior at age 17 (coefficients
For mothers, cluster membership was associated are standardized regression coefficients).
with knowledge, F(2, 172) 5 6.38, po.01, but not
risky behavior. Relational mothers (M 5 40.32,
was considerable stability in adolescent risky be-
SE 5 0.50) reported that they knew significantly
havior from ages 13 to 17, cluster membership was
more than questioners (M 5 38.48, SE 5 0.64) or
also important. Compared with the relied-on-others
mothers who relied on others (M 5 37.62, SE 5 0.62);
group, relational fathers and those who relied on
the latter two groups were not significantly different.
spouses knew significantly more about their ado-
lescents’ experiences, and higher levels of knowl-
Longitudinal Pathways Linking Sources of Knowledge, edge predicted lower levels of adolescent risky
Levels of Knowledge, and Adolescent Risky Behavior behavior 1 year later. A significant direct path also
emerged between the relies-on-spouse group and
In our final set of analyses we explored one pos-
risky behavior at age 17; controlling for prior risky
sible scenario for how sources of knowledge, levels
behavior, youth whose fathers relied on their
of knowledge, and risky behavior may be linked
spouses for information were less involved in risky
over time, using path analysis. In a series of regres-
behavior 1 year later. Sobel tests revealed that pa-
sion analyses, we examined the links between risky
ternal knowledge mediated the connections between
behavior at age 13, parents’ sources of knowledge
membership in the relational group and adolescent
(i.e., cluster membership) at age 16, levels of parental
behavior at age 17 (z 5 6.29, po.001), and member-
knowledge at age 16, and risky behavior at age 17. By
ship in the relies-on-spouse group and risky be-
separating in time the outcome variable from the
havior (z 5 6.24, po.001).
source clusters and parental knowledge, we reduced
As was the case for fathers, adolescent risky be-
the possibility that parents and offspring had been
havior at age 13 did not predict mothers’ sources of
jointly affected by a common event that inflated as-
knowledge (see Figure 4). Risky behavior at age 13,
sociations, such as the adolescent recently getting
however, did predict maternal knowledge at age 16,
into trouble. We anchored the path analysis with
as well as subsequent risky behavior: The more ad-
risky behavior at age 13 (Year 3), a time when risky
olescents engaged in risky behavior at age 13, the less
behavior begins to increase. Adding this step al-
their mothers knew about their experiences 3 years
lowed us to examine the connections, if any, between
prior risky behavior and parents’ sources of knowl-
edge (for this step we used multinomial regression .38***
because sources of knowledge is a categorical vari-
able), as well as the link between prior risky behavior Relational
−.04 (16 yrs) .28**
and parents’ levels of knowledge. These analyses Risky Maternal Risky
−.30***
controlled for parental education and adolescent Behavior Knowledge Behavior
(13 yrs) .14 (16 yrs) (17 yrs)
gender. .09
Questioners
Beginning with fathers, as can be seen in Figure 3, (16 yrs)
risky behavior at age 13 did not predict membership
in either the relational or the relies-on-spouse cluster −.27***
(compared with the reference group: relies on oth-
Figure 4. Path analysis depicting connections among adolescent
ers), but the more youth engaged in risky behavior at risky behavior at age 13, mothers’ sources of knowledge, maternal
age 13, the less fathers knew about their offspring’s knowledge, and adolescent risky behavior at age 17 (coefficients
experiences at age 16. In addition, although there are standardized regression coefficients).
14678624, 2005, 4, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00883.x by Universidad De Cadiz, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Sources of Fathers’ and Mothers’ Knowledge 879

later, and the more involved youth were in risky supervision, and their children described themselves
behavior youth at age 17. In addition, relational as less expressive. These father – adolescent dyads
mothers knew more about offspring than mothers were also more conflictual than the other groups.
who relied on others (the reference group), and the These correlates suggest that relying on others is the
more mothers knew, the less their offspring engaged most problematic strategy for attaining information
in risky behavior at age 17. Sobel tests revealed that and may be a last resort for fathers for whom the
the indirect path from membership in the relational father – adolescent relationship is not going well.
group to risky behavior at age 17 through maternal Turning to the mother clusters, as was the case for
knowledge was significant (z 5 3.64, po.001), but the fathers, a relational group emerged. The largest of
indirect path linking membership in the questioners the three mother clusters, this group was high on
group to risky behavior was not (z 5 1.59, ns). child self-disclosure and on listening and observing
but, unlike the fathers, was not high on parental
solicitation, suggesting that asking questions may be
Discussion
part of a relational package for fathers but not for
This study identified distinctive groups of fathers mothers. Adolescents may interpret questions from
and mothers as a function of the patterning of their fathers, who tend to be less involved in parenting
sources of knowledge about their adolescent off- overall than mothers, as signs of engagement and
spring’s daily experiences. In our concluding re- interest, but interpret questioning from mothers less
marks, we compare fathers’ and mothers’ clusters positively. Mothers in the relational group had de-
and address the possible mechanisms through which scribed their children as very open to supervision 3
sources of knowledge are linked to adolescent risky years before, suggesting again that children’s char-
behavior. acteristics may play an important role in shaping
parents’ knowledge acquisition strategies.
The second group of mothers had no father
Comparing Fathers’ and Mothers’ Sources of Knowledge
counterpart. We called them questioners because
Although the father and mother clusters resem- their solicitation scores were relatively high. Off-
bled one another in some striking ways, there were spring of these mothers described themselves as
interesting differences in the clusters themselves and more expressive (stereotypically feminine) than
in their correlates. Fathers fell into three groups. youth whose mothers relied on others. Note, how-
Fathers in the relational cluster learned about their ever, that although membership in the relational
children’s experiences in direct ways: Youth self- group was related to mothers being more knowl-
disclosed, fathers asked questions, and fathers edgeable, membership in the questioners group was
observed and listened. These fathers had highly ex- not.
pressive children, whom fathers saw as open to su- The third group of mothers relied on other people
pervision, suggesting that child effects may be part (i.e., sibling, spouse, people outside the family) for
of what drives fathers’ knowledge acquisition (Kerr information. These mothers described their offspring
& Stattin, 2000, 2003; Stattin & Kerr, 2000). as less open to supervision compared with relational
A second group of fathers relied on their spouses mothers. Recall that a large group of fathers relied on
for information. These men worked longer hours their spouses for information, and this group gen-
than other fathers, a circumstance that may help erally had positive correlates. In contrast, for moth-
explain why they relied on their spouses to provide ers, relying on fathers was part of a pattern of relying
knowledge about what their adolescent offspring on others (i.e., siblings, people outside the family), a
were doing. Fathers in this group were also more constellation of sources that had negative correlates.
likely to have daughters than sons. The mother –
daughter relationship has been characterized as the
What Role Do Sources of Knowledge Play in Adolescents’
closest of the four parent – child dyads, and the fa-
Risky Behavior?
ther – daughter relationship as the most distant
(Youniss & Smollar, 1985). Having a daughter may Our path-analytic findings suggest that how par-
be one life circumstance that sets fathers on a path ents acquire their information about their adoles-
toward relying on mothers for the inside story on cents’ experiences matters (Kerr & Stattin, 2000;
daughters’ daily experiences. Stattin & Kerr, 2000) but that the linkages are gen-
The third group of fathers relied on others (people erally indirect. That is, parents’ sources of knowl-
outside the family as well as siblings) for informa- edge are related to how much they know about their
tion. These fathers saw their offspring as less open to children’s lives, and knowledge in turn predicts ad-
14678624, 2005, 4, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00883.x by Universidad De Cadiz, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
880 Crouter, Bumpus, Davis, and McHale

olescents’ involvement in risky behavior. For both Our findings suggest that parental knowledge
fathers and mothers, levels of risky behavior at age may best be thought of as a property of the family
13 did not predict cluster membership. This was system because it depends not only on the parent –
surprising given that, at least for fathers, there were adolescent relationship, as we argued in an earlier
significant associations between cluster membership study (Crouter et al., 1999), but on the ways parents
and concurrent risky behavior, and previous re- keep themselves and one another informed about
search had found that risky behavior predicted their children’s experiences. Parents acquire knowl-
subsequent parental knowledge, and vice versa edge in different ways, ways that, for mothers, have
(Laird et al., 2003). Our null finding may be due to a lot to do with what youth are like, and ways that,
the 3-year gap. Much may have happened between for fathers, reflect youth characteristics, work de-
ages 13 and 16 to make risky behavior at age 13 mands, and father – adolescent relationship quality.
unconnected to parents’ knowledge acquisition The roots of parental knowledge in adolescence
strategies at age 16. Note, however, that risky be- probably reflect the cumulative history of parents’
havior at age 13 did predict fathers’ and mothers’ relationships with their children. Those who have
levels of knowledge at age 16, suggesting that early been able to construct close relationships (in part
risky behavior is not inconsequential. Sources of because of the personal qualities their children bring
knowledge may shift with changing circumstances, to the relationship) are able to rely on adolescent self-
making concurrent or recent behavior more relevant disclosure and listening to and observing offspring.
than past behavior. Those with less positive relationships, on the other
The path analysis for fathers suggested that both hand, may have to rely on others for information
risky behavior at age 13 and sources of knowledge about their offspring, a style of knowledge acquisi-
predicted fathers’ knowledge, and that fathers’ tion that, our data suggest, is linked to lower levels of
knowledge in turn was related to adolescents’ en- knowledge and, ultimately, higher levels of adoles-
gagement in risky behavior at age 17. Specifically, cent risky behavior.
compared with fathers who relied on others for in-
formation (the reference group), relational fathers
Directions for Future Research
and fathers who relied on their spouses knew more
about their children’s experiences, and being more Our results suggest that a pattern-analytic ap-
knowledgeable in turn predicted relative declines in proach offers a useful complement to what can be
adolescents’ levels of engagement in risky behavior. learned from variable-oriented approaches to the
These indirect paths were significant. In addition, study of parental knowledge. One drawback of our
there was a direct path between membership in the data was that we had information about parents’
relies-on-spouse group and risky behavior, suggest- sources of knowledge only at one time point. In fu-
ing that offspring whose fathers relied on mothers ture research, it would be useful to assess parents’
for information became less involved in risky be- sources of knowledge at different points in their
havior over time compared with adolescents whose children’s development, including the preschool
fathers relied on other people for information. Be- years (for children in day care or preschool), the
cause this was a direct path that did not go through school-age years, and continuing into and through
paternal knowledge, other processes appeared to be adolescence. With such data, one could ask: Do the
at work. Parents’ effective teamwork may be the same groups emerge at each developmental period?
unmeasured phenomenon that explains this link, or If not, which emerge early and which emerge later in
perhaps this association can be attributed to moth- development? Is group membership stable over
ers’ effective monitoring and knowledge acquisition. time? What individual, contextual, and relational
The path analytic results for mothers, although phenomena predict changes in group membership?
sparse, were generally consistent with the father Future research would benefit from larger, more
findings. There was a significant indirect effect of heterogeneous samples. This study focused on a
membership in the relational cluster on adolescents’ working and middle-class, predominately European
risky behavior at age 17 through maternal knowl- American, two-parent sample. It remains to be seen
edge. Compared with mothers who relied on others, whether the same clusters would emerge in other
mothers in the relational group knew more about racial or ethnic groups or in single-parent families.
their offspring’s experiences, and maternal knowl- Siblings, for example, may take on special impor-
edge in turn was linked to relative declines in risky tance as a source of parental knowledge in single-
behavior, but this indirect effect was not apparent for parent families or in cultural groups in which sib-
mothers in the questioners group. lings are highly involved in caregiving.
14678624, 2005, 4, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00883.x by Universidad De Cadiz, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Sources of Fathers’ and Mothers’ Knowledge 881

In sum, we identified distinct groups of fathers Crouter, A. C., & Head, M. R. (2002). Parental monitoring
and mothers on the basis of how parents acquired and knowledge of children. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.),
knowledge about their adolescent offspring’s expe- Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3: Being and becoming a parent
riences. Sources of knowledge, parents’ levels of (pp. 461 – 483). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
knowledge about their children’s experiences, and Crouter, A. C., Helms-Erikson, H., Updegraff, K., &
adolescent risky behavior were linked longitudinally McHale, S. M. (1999). Conditions underlying parents’
knowledge about children’s daily lives in middle
in interesting and meaningful ways, suggesting that
childhood: Between- and within- family comparisons.
parents’ sources of knowledge predict how much
Child Development, 70, 246 – 259.
they know, which in turn is linked to offspring’s
Crouter, A. C., MacDermid, S. M., McHale, S. M., & Perry-
subsequent involvement in risky behavior. A chal- Jenkins, M. (1990). Parental monitoring and perceptions
lenge for future research is to chart how parents ac- of children’s school performance and conduct in dual-
quire information about their offspring in the early and single-earner families. Developmental Psychology, 26,
years and to document continuities and discontinu- 649 – 657.
ities in those processes over time, as well as to track Darling, N. (2001, April). Adolescents’ decisions to share in-
the dynamic individual, relational, and contextual formation with their parents and the development of maternal
conditions underlying these family processes. knowledge and trust. Paper presented at the Society for
Research in Child Development biennial meeting, Min-
neapolis, MN.
Devins, G. M., & Orme, C. M. (1985). Center for Epide-
References miological Studies Depression Scale. In D. J. Keyser & R.
Antill, J. K., Russell, G., Goodnow, J. J., & Cotton, S. (1993). C. Sweetland (Eds.), Test critiques (pp. 144 – 160). Kansas
Measures of children’s sex typing in middle childhood. City, MO: Test Corporation of America.
Australian Journal of Psychology, 45, 25 – 33. Dishion, T. J., & McMahon, R. J. (1998). Parental monitoring
Auhagen, A. E., & Hinde, R. A. (1997). Individual charac- and the prevention of child and adolescent problem
teristics and personal relationships. Personal Relation- behavior: A conceptual and empirical formulation.
ships, 4, 63 – 84. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 1, 61 – 75.
Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process Downey, G., & Coyne, J. (1990). Children of depressed
model. Child Development, 55, 83 – 96. parents: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin,
Bem, S. (1974). The measurement of psychological an- 108, 50 – 76.
drogyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, Eccles, J., & Barber, B. (1990). The risky behavior scale. Un-
155 – 162. published measure, University of Michigan.
Bergman, L. R., Cairns, R. B., & Kagan, J. (1998). Methods Hughes, R., & Perry-Jenkins, M. (1996). Social class issues
and models for studying the individual: Essays in honor of in family life education. Family Relations, 45, 175 – 182.
Marian Radke-Yarrow. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Kerns, K. A. (2001). Parent – child attachment and moni-
Blashfield, R. K. (1976). Mixed model tests of cluster toring in middle childhood. Journal of Family Psychology,
analysis: Accuracy of four agglomerative hierarchical 15, 69 – 81.
methods. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 377 – 388. Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2000). What parents know, how they
Blashfield, R. K., & Aldenderfer, M. S. (1978). The literature know it, and several forms of adolescent adjustment:
on cluster analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 13, Further support for reinterpretation of monitoring. De-
271 – 295. velopmental Psychology, 36, 366 – 380.
Blashfield, R. K., & Aldenderfer, M. S. (1988). The methods Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2003). Parenting of adolescents:
and problems of cluster analysis. In J. R. Nesselroade & Action or reaction? In A. C. Crouter & A. Booth
R. B. Cattell (Eds.), Handbook of multivariate experimental (Eds.), Children’s influence on family dynamics: The ne-
psychology (pp. 447 – 473). New York: Plenum. glected side of family relationships (pp. 121 – 151). Mahwah,
Braiker, H. B., & Kelley, H. H. (1979). Conflict in the de- NJ: Erlbaum.
velopment of close relationships. In R. Burgess & T. Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Trost, K. (1999). To know you is to
Huston (Eds.), Social exchange in developing relationships trust you: Parents’ trust is rooted in child disclosure of
(pp. 135 – 168). New York: Academic Press. information. Journal of Adolescence, 22, 737 – 752.
Bumpus, M. F., Crouter, A. C., & McHale, S. M. (1999). Laird, R. D., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Dodge, K. A. (2003).
Work demands of dual-earner couples: Implications for Parents’ monitoring-relevant knowledge and adoles-
parents’ knowledge about children’s daily lives in cents’ delinquent behavior: Evidence of correlated de-
middle childhood. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, velopmental changes and reciprocal influences. Child
465 – 475. Development, 74, 752 – 768.
Cronbach, L. J., & Gleser, G. C. (1953). Assessing simi- Nakao, K., & Treas, J. (1994). Updating occupational pres-
larity between profiles. Psychological Bulletin, 50, tige and socioeconomic scores: How the new measures
456 – 473. measure up. Sociological Methodology, 24, 1 – 72.
14678624, 2005, 4, Downloaded from https://srcd.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00883.x by Universidad De Cadiz, Wiley Online Library on [05/12/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
882 Crouter, Bumpus, Davis, and McHale

Parke, R. D. (2002). Fathers and families. In M. H. Bornstein chology: Vol. 21. Development during the transition to ado-
(Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3: Being and becoming a lescence (pp. 79 – 122). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
parent (pp. 27 – 73). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Sneath, P. H., & Sokal, R. P. (1973). Numerical taxonomy. San
Schaefer, E. S. (1965). Children’s reports of parental be- Francisco: Freeman.
havior: An inventory. Child Development, 36, 417 – 424. Stattin, H., & Kerr, M. (2000). Parental monitoring: A re-
interpretation. Child Development, 71, 1072 – 1085.
Schwarz, J. C., Barton-Henry, M. L., & Pruzinsky, T. (1985). Waizenhofer, R. N., Buchanan, C. M., & Jackson-Newsom,
Assessing child-rearing behavior: A comparison of rat- J. (2004). Mothers’ and fathers’ knowledge of adoles-
ings made by mother, father, and sibling on the CRPBI. cents’ daily activities: Its sources and its links with ad-
Child Development, 55, 462 – 479. olescent adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 18,
Smetana, J. G. (1988). Concepts of self and social conven- 348 – 360.
tion: Adolescents and parents’ reasoning about hypo- Youniss, J., & Smollar, J. (1985). Adolescent relations with
thetical and actual family conflicts. In M. R. Gunnar & mothers, fathers, and friends. Chicago: University of Chi-
W. A. Collins (Eds.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Psy- cago Press.

You might also like