SDQ 3

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 49:12 (2008), pp 1304–1312 doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01942.

Construct validity of the five-factor


Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)
in pre-, early, and late adolescence
Betty Van Roy,1,2* Marijke Veenstra,3 and Jocelyne Clench-Aas4
1
Institute of Psychiatry, University in Oslo, Norway; 2Akershus University Hospital, Division of Mental Health,
Norway; 3NOVA-Norwegian Social Research, Oslo, Norway; 4Norwegian Institute of Public Health, Oslo, Norway

Background: The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) is designed to measure psychological
adjustment in children and adolescents. Psychometric evaluations of the instrument have shown
satisfactory convergent and discriminant validity, while factor analysis studies have shown mixed
results across countries. In the present study, the construct validity of the five-factor SDQ is evaluated
in a large community sample of Norwegian pre-, early, and late adolescents. Methods: The sample
consisted of 26,269 children and adolescents (10–19 years) with valid answers on all 25 items of the
SDQ self-report. Complete parent/proxy data of respective pre-adolescent children was available for
6,645 cases. A Lisrel approach to Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was used to evaluate the five-
factor model and the presence of a positive construal factor. In the sample of pre-adolescents and their
parents/proxies, convergent and discriminant validity was evaluated by a CFA approach to multitrait-
multimethods (MTMM). Results: Fit statistics for the hypothesized five-factor model were satisfactory,
but introducing correlated error terms for some of the items led to significant model improvement in all
age groups. All factor loadings were higher than .30, except for item 11 (good friend). The loadings
differed across age groups and differed markedly between the parent/proxy and self-report measures.
The MTMM showed that the source of ratings made a difference on the validity of all subscale ratings,
with self-reports discriminating more on ratings of emotional and peer problems, and parents/proxies
discriminating more on hyperactivity symptoms. A positive construal factor was identified but had a
modest effect compared with the original five traits. Results suggested an unclear construct and
meaning of the Prosocial behaviour subscale. Conclusion: The results of the present study indicated
support for the proposed five-factor structure of the SDQ (Goodman, 2001) across a wide age range (10–19
years), including older adolescents and different informants. However, some improvements should be
considered to improve internal reliability and conceptual clarity. Keywords: SDQ, construct validity,
CFA, adolescents, psychometrics, questionnaires. Abbreviations: SDQ: Strengths and Difficulties
Questionnaire.

The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) and behavioural difficulties (Goodman, Meltzer, &
is designed to measure psychological adjustment in Bailey, 2003; Marzocchi et al., 2004; Mathai,
children and adolescents (Goodman, 1997), and has Anderson, & Bourne, 2004; Obel et al., 2004; Wo-
been established as a widely used instrument in erner et al., 2004). In Norway, the SDQ has been
child mental health research (Vostanis, 2006). It is a used in several community screening programs to
short questionnaire (25 items), easy to complete and study the overall well-being of large groups of young
user friendly because of its briefness and positive people (Heiervang et al., 2007; Heyerdahl, 2003;
attributes items, and is sensitive to change. The five Ronning, Handegaard, Sourander, & Morch, 2004;
subscales of the SDQ cover children’s emotional and Van Roy, Groholt, Heyerdahl, & Clench-Aas, 2006).
behavioural functioning (i.e., emotional symptoms, One of these studies (Van Roy et al., 2006) suggested
conduct problems, hyperactivity–inattention, and that the SDQ could also be a valuable screening
peer problems) as well as personal strengths (i.e., instrument for older adolescents (16–19 years).
prosocial behaviour). The inclusion of both strength The usefulness of any measurement instrument
and difficulty items in the SDQ supposedly makes depends on its validity and reliability. Internal reli-
the questionnaire especially suitable for studies of ability and test–retest stability of the SDQ have been
general populations where the majority of children termed satisfactory, despite modest levels of internal
are healthy. The SDQ is available as both a self- reliability for several subscales (Muris, Meesters, &
report and a proxy report questionnaire. van den Berg, 2003; Palmieri & Smith, 2007). It has
The instrument has been used in epidemiological, been argued that low internal reliability may be due
developmental and clinical research in many coun- to the positively worded reverse-scored items on the
tries and different cultures, increasing its utility for conduct, peer problem and hyperactivity subscales
cross-country comparison of children’s emotional (Muris, Meesters, Eijkelenboom, & Vincken, 2004).
One of the most important properties of a meas-
Conflict of interest statement: No conflicts declared. urement tool is construct validity, which here refers
Ó 2008 The Authors
Journal compilation Ó 2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Published by Blackwell Publishing, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA
Construct validity of the SDQ 1305

to the degree the questionnaire items are valid as proposed by Goodman (2001) in a large com-
measures of the five constructs of the SDQ. Con- munity sample of Norwegian pre-, early, and late
vergent and discriminant validity are main aspects of adolescents. This involves the following steps: i)
construct validity. Because we cannot prove con- analysis of the latent structure of the SDQ in
struct validity, it is important to collect increasing pre-adolescents, early, and late adolescents, and
amounts of support that the measurements appear parent/proxies; ii) evaluation of convergent and
to be sensible, that the postulated constructs behave discriminant validity in the sample of pre- adoles-
as anticipated, and that there are no grounds for cents and their parent/proxies; and iii) assessment
rejecting them (Fayers & Machin, 2007, p. 83). of the presence of a ‘positive construal factor’ in
Convergent validity of the SDQ has been evaluated addition to the five hypothesized factors of the SDQ.
by showing substantial correlations with other
instruments of psychological adjustment, such as
the Rutter questionnaire, upon which it was Methods
originally based, the Child Behaviour Check List and Sample
the Youth Self-Report (Achenbach, 1991; Becker,
Woerner, Hasselhorn, Banaschewski, & Rothenber- Pupils from the 5th grade in primary school to the last
year of high school (29,631 pupils, mean age: 14.7,
ger, 2004; Elander & Rutter, 1996; Goodman, 2001;
range: 10–19 years; boys: 50.9%, girls: 49.1%,
Goodman et al., 2003; Goodman & Scott, 1999;
response rate: 84.3%) were invited to participate in
Smedje, Broman, Hetta, & von Knorring, 1999). a health profile study, including SDQ, undertaken
Further, the SDQ discriminates well between in Akershus County in 2002 by the Norwegian Health
children with and without psychopathological Services Research Centre (Van Roy et al., 2006). In
symptoms (Goodman, 2001). grades 5–7, parents and children completed a health
Factor analysis studies have shown somewhat profile questionnaire (response rate = 78%). All 22
mixed results across countries. In general, five-fac- municipalities in the county, which surrounds the
tor solutions that correspond with the hypothesised Norwegian capital, participated. Classes at each school
domains of psychopathology and personal strengths level were selected at random to obtain a sample rep-
are indicated (Becker et al., 2006; Goodman, 2001; resentative of the county as a whole. Participation in the
study was voluntary. The parents were informed by the
Muris et al., 2003; Smedje et al., 1999). However,
local school and asked to give their consent. The chil-
results from a study from the United States (Dickey
dren completed the questionnaire at school during
& Blumberg, 2004) suggested a three-factor solution regular classes, while the parents received the ques-
corresponding to internalization, externalization and tionnaire at home via the child and returned materials
prosocial problems. This was also found in Finnish in a sealed envelope. The questionnaires of each parent
youth self-report data (Koskelainen, Sourander, & and their child had the same registration number, so
Vauras, 2001). The presence of both positively and that they could be matched without violating the an-
negatively worded items can confound the factor onymity of the participants.
structure. Positively worded items tend to cluster The SDQ scoring algorithms for subscale scores and
together in what can be termed a ‘positive construal total difficulties scores allow scale scores to be prorated
method factor’, irrespective of the trait they sup- if at least three of five scale items are complete. For
analytical purposes, in the present article, only
posedly measure. Several studies have pointed to the
respondents with valid answers on all 25 items of the
presence of such a ‘positive construal method’ factor
SDQ (N = 26,269) were included in the analyses. Miss-
in the SDQ, with substantial loadings for positively ing item responses varied between .7% and 2.3% per
worded items that belong to different sub- item, with the highest percentage of missing responses
scales (Dickey & Blumberg, 2004; Goodman, 2001; among late adolescents. The final number of respond-
Koskelainen et al., 2001; Palmieri & Smith, 2007; ents in pre-adolescence (primary school, age range:
Ronning et al., 2004). 10–13 years), early adolescence (secondary school, age
The self-report version of the SDQ was originally range: 13–16 years) and late adolescence (high school,
developed for children and adolescents between 11 age range: 16–19 years) was 8,320; 8,582 and 9,367,
and 16 years old. A previous study found support for respectively. Complete parent/proxy data of respective
the validity and reliability of the self-report version of pre-adolescent children was available for 6,645 cases.
These 6,645 cases were used for further analyses of the
the SDQ in 8–13-year-olds (Muris et al., 2004). A
parent/proxy sample and for the multitrait-multi-
Europe-wide study of children with ADHD, including
methods (MTMM) analyses.
older adolescents (6–18 years), confirmed the valid-
ity and reliability of the parent-reported SDQ scale
structure for the sample as a whole (Becker et al., Measures
2004). Separate psychometric properties of the SDQ
The Norwegian version of the SDQ self-report, obtained
self-report version in older adolescents (16–19 yrs) by translation and back-translation, was used in the
have, to our knowledge, not been assessed study.
previously. The SDQ (25 items) was designed by Goodman on the
The overall objective of the present study is to basis of nosological concepts of the ICD-10 (WHO,
evaluate the construct validity of the five-factor SDQ 1994) and the DSM IV (APA, 1994). The questionnaire
Ó 2008 The Authors
Journal compilation Ó 2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
1306 Betty Van Roy, Marijke Veenstra, and Jocelyne Clench-Aas

consists of five subscales of five items covering emo- unconstrained factor models for the different grades
tional problems, conduct problems, hyperactivity was evaluated with a chi-square difference test, using a
problems, peer problems and prosocial behaviour. Each critical value (a) of 5%.
item uses a three-point ordinal Likert format and can be Model 2 indicated the CFA approach to MTMM
answered with: ‘not true’, ‘somewhat true’ or ‘certainly (Campbell & Fiske, 1959; Loehlin, 1998; Marsh &
true’. Responses can be rated 0–2 for negatively worded Hocevar, 1983). This approach was used to attempt to
items and rated inversely 2–0 for positively worded separate out the true variance on the five SDQ constructs
items. In this way, for all items, higher scores indicate from variance resulting from measurement methods
more problematic attributes. Subscores were generated (self-report versus parent/proxy). The logic of MTMM
for each subscale (range: 0–10). All subscores except designs is that proxy and self-report measures of the
the prosocial score were added up to a total difficulties same construct should be highly correlated, but that
score (range: 0–40). The prosocial subscale measures measures of different constructs have low correlations.
the adolescent’s ability to act prosocially, independent In the hypothesized model, each subscale was measured
of the difficulties measured by the other subscales. by both a method factor (parent/proxy or self-report) and
An extended version of the SDQ includes a brief a trait-factor. By calculating squared factor loadings,
‘impact supplement’ that assesses chronicity, distress, explained variance in subscales resulting from the
social impairment and burden to others. In this study, underlying trait and the reporting method was estim-
the impact supplement was not included in the psycho- ated. The part left unexplained is termed uniqueness.
metric analyses. Trait factor correlations and method factor correlations
were estimated but the correlation between traits and
methods was constrained to zero.
Statistical analysis Model 3 represented the positive construal factor
model. This model indicated that the positive construal
We used a structural equation approach (SEM) to
factor would contribute to an increase in explained
Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA; Bollen, 1989) to
variance in the positively worded items that did not
evaluate the latent structure of the hypothesized five-
belong to the Prosocial construct. Because all the items in
factor model of the SDQ. Unlike traditional Exploratory
the Prosocial construct are positively phrased, this
Factor Analysis (EFA), a SEM approach to CFA takes
construct already meets at least some of the criteria of a
measurement error into account and allows for explicit
positive construal factor. We let this sixth construct
modelling of the five hypothesized constructs. As such,
correlate with the other five constructs of the SDQ. This
it proposes a specific test of whether the items of the
model posits that positively worded items might reflect
SDQ load on to the five hypothesised latent constructs
method variance rather than conceptually distinct
(factors) exactly as predicted. The analyses comprised
dimensions.
of several steps: i) Multigroup CFA to evaluate the five-
Cross-scale correlations were derived from the CFA
factor model and to test the equality of factor loadings
approach in LISREL and, as such, are error free. SPSS
across samples of pre-adolescents, early adolescents,
14 was used for descriptive purposes and to assess the
late adolescents and parents/proxies, ii) CFA approach
internal reliability (Cronbach’s alpha) of the five sub-
to MTMM analysis to evaluate convergent and
scales.
discriminant validity; and iii) CFA to evaluate the
presence of a positive construal factor over and above
the hypothesized five trait factors.
To conduct CFA, we used LISREL 8.70 software,
which is suited for modelling ordinal, non-normally
Results
distributed data. Based on the large sample size and the Table 1 presents the results from the CFA in all three
ordinal nature of the data, we applied diagonally age groups and for the parent/proxy sample (model 1).
weighted least squares (DWLS) estimation methods with Fit statistics for the hypothesized five-factor model
polychoric correlation and asymptotic covariance
were satisfactory. However, the MI suggested that
matrices (Jöreskog, 2005). We used the following criteria
introducing correlated error terms for items 2
to evaluate the model fit (Bollen, 1989; Loehlin, 1998):
average residual correlations as measured by the root (Restless) and 10 (Fidgety) within the Hyperactivity
mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), where an Problems construct, as well as for items 18 (Lies) and
RMSEA below .08 indicates an acceptable model fit, and 19 (Bullied), would lead to significant model
an RMSEA below .05 indicates good model fit; a Good- improvement in all three age groups. This resulted in
ness-of-Fit Index (GFI) and Comparative Fit index (CFI) RMSEA indices that indicated good fit for all age
above .90 and the root-mean-square-residual (SRMR) groups. For the parent/proxy sample, only the cor-
below .10. The five-factor model (model 1), the MTMM related error terms for items 2 and 10 were sub-
model (model 2) and the Positive Construal Factor model stantial and the final model fit was good. Error-free
(model 3) are presented in Figure 1. intercorrelations between the five constructs were
Model 1 indicated the five-factor model consisting of
strongest between Conduct Problems and Hyperac-
five correlated constructs with each construct reflecting
tivity in all samples (.66–.84), followed by Peer
five items. Modification indexes (MI) were used to find
potential sources of significant model improvement. Problems and Emotional Problems (.62–.72). The
Owing to the large sample size, a relatively small MI will size of these correlations suggests considerable
suggest a significant improvement of model fit. There- conceptual overlap between these traits. Lowest
fore, we only used the largest MI as indicators of model correlations were found between the constructs of
improvement. The equivalence of constrained and Prosocial and Emotional Problems (|.01–.16|).
Ó 2008 The Authors
Journal compilation Ó 2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Construct validity of the SDQ 1307

Figure 1 Three hypothesized CFA models of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; five-factor model (model 1);
CFA approach to MTMM (model 2); A positive construal model (model 3). Error terms are not shown. Model 1. Five-
factor model SDQ. Item numbers as shown in table 2. Model 2. CFA approach to multitrait-multimethod analysis of
the five subscales (P = Parent/Proxy; S = Self-report). Model 3. Positive Construal Factor (PCF) model (Correlations
between PCF and SDQ traits not shown)

Ó 2008 The Authors


Journal compilation Ó 2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
1308 Betty Van Roy, Marijke Veenstra, and Jocelyne Clench-Aas

Table 1 Confirmatory factor analysis of the SDQ, model fit statistics in pre-adolescents (PA), early adolescents (EA), late adolescents
(LA) and parent/proxy (PP)

PA EA LA PP

N 8,320 8,582 9,367 6,645


Model 1: Five-factor model unadjusted
Satorra Bentler scaled v2(DF) 4,226 (265) 7,132 (265) 7,933 (265) 3,470 (265)
RMSEA .042 .055 .056 .043
GFI .96 .95 .95 .97
CFI .96 .96 .96 .98
SRMR .07 .08 .08 .07
Model 2: Five-factor model modified#
Satorra Bentler scaled v2(DF) 3,406 (263) 5,868 (263) 5,997 (263) 2,395 (264)
RMSEA .038 .049 .048 .035
GFI .97 .96 .96 .98
CFI .97 .97 .97 .99
SRMR .06 .07 .08 .07
Model 3: Positive construal factor model
Satorra Bentler scaled v2(DF) 1,957 (253) 3,790 (253) 4,581 (253) 1,872 (254)
RMSEA .029 .040 .043 .031
GFI .99 .98 .97 .99
CFI .98 .98 .98 .99
SRMR .05 .06 .07 .05
#
Correlated errors items 2 and 10; and 18 and 19. Correlated error items 2 and 10.

The standardized factor loadings corresponding to construct of Prosocial behaviour was characterised
these modified models are presented in Table 2. All by a relatively large uniqueness that underlined
loadings were higher than .30, except for item 11 the problems with interpreting the meaning of this
(Good friend) in the parent/proxy sample, which had scale. We found modest correlations between the
a loading of .29. The results in Table 2 suggested method factors (.25).
that the factor loadings for the self-report groups Finally, we evaluated the presence of a positive
were relatively similar. However, chi-squared differ- construal factor (model 3). The fit statistics of this
ence tests comparing the self-reports simultaneously six-factor model are presented for the self-report
(v2dif = 1,101.46, df = 54, p < .0001), and pairwise groups as well as the parent/proxy sample in the
comparisons, indicated that factor loadings were not lower part of Table 1. Introduction of a positive
equal across groups. construal factor contributed to significantly
Cronbach’s alpha for the total difficulties score improved model fit in all samples, as indicated by
was .74 in the pre-adolescent group, .78 in early the decrease in chi-square and the improved
adolescence, .80 in late adolescence and .78 in the RMSEA statistics. This factor was highly correlated
parent/proxy sample. In all groups, Cronbach’s with the Prosocial construct (.63–.70). However, a
alpha was lowest for the construct of Conduct closer inspection of the variance components
Problems, ranging from .44 to .59 (Table 2). attributable to the postulated traits indicated a
Hyperactivity had high internal consistency in the relatively modest role for the positive construal
parent/proxy sample (.76) but was, at best, modest factor (Table 4). For the self-report groups, factor
in the self-report samples. loadings for all five positively phrased items were
Our next step was to evaluate the convergent higher than .30. Yet, only item 21 (Reflective) and
and divergent validity of the SDQ in the sample of item 7 (Obedient) were, to a larger extent, deter-
parent/proxy and preadolescents (model 2). The mined by the Positive Construal Factor rather than
CFA approach to MTMM with two method factors their original trait (Hyperactivity/Conduct). The
and five trait factors had an acceptable model fit: relatively large amount of uniqueness related to
v2 = 288, df = 14; RMSEA = .056; CFI = .99; these two items indicated that their meaning may
AFI = .99 and SRMR = .02. Table 3 presents a be unclear in self-report groups. In particular, item
summary of the variance components attributed by 7 (‘Generally obedient, usually does what adults
trait, method and uniqueness. The source of rat- request’) seemed to be less appropriate for late
ings made a difference in the validity of all sub- adolescents as a measure of Conduct Problems.
scale ratings. The relatively high trait variance For parent/proxies, item 11 (Good friend) was not
components suggested that self-report tend to be related to the Positive Construal Factor. The
more discriminating on ratings of Emotional exceptionally large uniqueness related to this item
Symptoms and Peer Problems. Similarly, parent/ (.91) emphasized the lack of clarity in its inter-
proxies seemed to be particularly discriminating on pretation. On the whole, ratings of parent/proxies
ratings for Hyperactivity, where they were on the positively phrased items were little affected
practically not subject to any method effects. The by the Positive Construal Factor.
Ó 2008 The Authors
Journal compilation Ó 2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Construct validity of the SDQ 1309

Table 2 Cronbach’s alpha and standardized factor loadings Table 4 Trait and method variance components in the Positive
for SDQ items (CFA) in pre-adolescents (PA), early adolescents Construal Factor CFA model in pre-adolescents (PA), early
(EA), late adolescents (LA) and parent/proxy (PP) adolescents (EA), late adolescents (LA) and parent/proxy (PP)

PA EA LA PP Item number

N 8,320 8,582 9,367 6,645 7 11 14 21 25


Emotional symptoms
Cronbach’s a .61 .71 .70 0,67 Trait PA .18 .54 .39 .17 .43
Somatic (3) .47 .60 .60 .51 EA .11 .65 .30 .13 .42
Worries (8) .57 .63 .64 .77 LA .06 .58 .31 .12 .44
Unhappy (13) .74 .84 .81 .81 PP .22 .08 .67 .35 .62
Clingy (16) .57 .57 .55 .69 Positive Construal Factor PA .19 .12 .23 .21 .11
Fears (24) .58 .74 .76 .67 EA .19 .22 .16 .25 .09
Conduct problems LA .17 .14 .11 .2 .09
Cronbach’s a .44 .59 .54 .5 PP .13 .01 .15 .16 .02
Tempers (5) .50 .52 .55 .68 Unique PA .63 .34 .38 .62 .46
Obedient (7) .44 .46 .37 .52 EA .70 .13 .54 .62 .49
Fights (12) .54 .71 .69 .73 LA .77 .28 .58 .68 .47
Lies (18) .58 .69 .70 .72 PP .65 .91 .18 .49 .36
Steals (22) .41 .66 .64 .54
Hyperactivity Items: 7 = obedient; 11 = good friend; 14 = popular;
Cronbach’s a .57 .65 .66 .76 21 = reflective; 25 = persistent.
Restless (2) .36 .46 .43 .62
Fidgety (10) .34 .41 .43 .59
Distractible (15) .70 .75 .80 .86
predicted five-factor structure in late adolescence
Thinks before .43 .49 .46 .64 (16–19 years). It also holds in a sample of parent/
acting (21) proxy raters.
Persistent (25) .64 .70 .73 .80 Although all models were acceptable without any
Peer problems modifications, several of our findings call for closer
Cronbach’s a .54 .64 .60 .56
inspection.
Solitary (6) .57 .55 .53 .68
Good friend (11) .67 .84 .79 .29 First, large and significant correlated error terms
Popular (14) .54 .59 .59 .80 for two pairs of items were found for all age groups.
Bullied (19) .69 .81 .86 .74 The correlated error terms between items 2 (Restless)
Adults (23) .40 .49 .44 .56 and 10 (Fidgety) correspond to a previous Norwegian
Prosocial behaviour
study. Ronning et al. (2004) argued that there is a
Cronbach’s a .64 .65 .64 .66
Considerate (1) .69 .78 .78 .87 close semantic similarity between these two items in
Shares (4) .47 .53 .56 .58 the Norwegian language. These items can possibly be
Caring (9) .71 .64 .67 .66 reformulated in order to obtain more distinct indi-
Kind to kids (17) .61 .65 .59 .68 cators; another possibility is to take out one of the
Often volunteers .58 .43 .42 .58
to help (20)
two items.
Second, we could not conclude that the factor
SDQ item numbers in parentheses. loadings were equal across age groups. This may be
partly the result of the very large sample, where quite
small differences from ideal measurement can lead
Table 3 CFA approach to multitrait-multimethod (MTM); Trait
and Method variance components (N = 6,645) to large chi-squares. Examination of the factor
loadings indicated that these were relatively similar
Measured variables Trait Method Uniqueness for early and late adolescents and generally higher
Self-report Emotional symptoms .41 .14 .45 than in the pre-adolescent group. One explanation
Conduct problems .19 .38 .42 may be that younger children experience problems in
Hyperactivity .10 .38 .51 a less differentiated way than older adolescents,
Peer problems .62 .07 .31 according to their cognitive capability. Factor load-
Prosocial behaviour .18 .08 .73
ings from the parent/proxy measures were markedly
Parent/ Emotional symptoms .19 .41 .40
proxy Conduct problems .25 .34 .41 different from self-reports, indicating that, for these
Hyperactivity .83 .16 .01 raters, items were weighing differently on their
Peer problems .18 .22 .60 underlying construct. The positively phrased item 11
Prosocial behaviour .32 .10 .57 (Good friend) had a surprisingly low factor loading in
the parent/proxy sample, and was not related to a
Positive Construal Factor. This calls for an inspec-
Discussion
tion of the Norwegian translation of this particular
The results of the present study indicate support for item in the proxy questionnaire.
the proposed five-factor structure of the SDQ Third, there was considerable conceptual overlap
(Goodman, 2001) in three age groups in a Norwegian between some subscales, which suggests a lack of
community sample, inclusive late adolescents. This discriminatory validity. High correlations between
is the first study showing support for Goodman’s Hyperactivity and Conduct Problems indicated that
Ó 2008 The Authors
Journal compilation Ó 2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
1310 Betty Van Roy, Marijke Veenstra, and Jocelyne Clench-Aas

these scales at least partly share the same underly- also the case for the SDQ. The results indicated
ing trait. The same phenomenon was found for the that method factors had little influence on the
Peer Problems and Emotional subscales. These report of Peer Problems and Emotional Symptoms
findings are consistent with the three-factor solu- in self-reports, and on the report of Hyperactivity
tions found by Dickey and Blumberg (2004) and problems in parent/proxies. Goodman (2001)
Koskelainen et al. (2001). Although we did not find demonstrated that the full combination of the
clear evidence for a three-factor solution, external- three-informant SDQ (parent, teacher, and self)
izing and internalizing dimensions are likely to be questionnaires yielded the highest sensitivity in
responsible for overlap between these particular predicting psychiatric disorders. Our findings
scales. On the other hand, our results supported the support the use of multi-informant SDQ in the
findings of Goodman (2001), who concluded that assessment of children’s mental health.
scales from the internalizing dimension are not Finally, internal reliability, examined with Cron-
substantially correlated with scales from the exter- bach’s alpha, was satisfactory for the summary
nalizing dimension. score of the SDQ consisting of 20 items but modest
Fourth, relatively low correlations between Proso- for some of the subscales. The low Cronbach’s
cial Behaviour and the other four constructs of the alpha for the subscale measuring Conduct Problems
SDQ seem to support Goodman’s claim (2003, is problematic and has been reported in previous
p. 174) that ‘the absence of prosocial behaviour is studies as well (Bourdon, Goodman, Rae, Simpson,
conceptually different from the presence of psycho- & Koretz, 2005; Muris et al., 2003). Results
logical difficulties’. However, the Prosocial scale also measured in this subscale should be interpreted
suffered from low agreement between parent/proxy with caution. Low Cronbach’s alpha suggests that
and self-rating, the presence of a method effect, and the items in this subscale are not only tapping
was highly correlated with a positive construal fac- Conduct Problems, which is also reflected by the
tor. Our findings suggested difficulties with inter- correlated error terms between item 18 (Lies) with
preting the meaning of the Prosocial Behaviour item 19 (Bullied) from Peer Problems. Cronbach’s
subscale and indicated, consistent with the findings alpha is relatively modest for the Hyperactivity and
of Palmieri and Smith (2007) that this subscale Peer problem subscales in preadolescence (<0.60).
might reflect a methodological artefact. This emphasises that, in this age group in particular,
Fifth, similar to the recent findings of Palmieri the items in the different subscales not only reflect
and Smith (2007), the results of the present study their respective domains, but are related to several
indicated the presence of a positive construal factor, domains. Higher cross-scale correlations in younger
in particular for self-reports. Ratings from parent/ children are not surprising, because possible
proxies were least affected by this factor. Positively emotional and behavioural difficulties are less
worded items in the SDQ tend to cluster together distinct early in life, and many children have
irrespective of the subscale they belong to. On the problems in different areas at the same time. The
one hand, this is a well-known detrimental method- limited number of response categories (three) might
ological phenomenon. Detrimental, as it reduces the be another reason for low internal reliability (Fayers
clarity of the factor structure and may have con- & Machin, 2007, p. 126).
tributed to decreased internal reliability in some of
the subscales. On the other hand, the inclusion of
Study limitations
positively worded items is theoretically as well as
clinically motivated and refers to the strengths part The five-factor SDQ has been widely adopted in epi-
of the SDQ. As Rothenberger and Woerner (2004) demiological research in Norway. In the present
underline, it is necessary to include positive behav- study we therefore choose a CFA approach to eval-
iours, protective factors, and compensatory uate this particular latent structure. Although the
resources to gain broad and clinically meaningful results indicated satisfactory model fit for the pro-
psychopathological profiles – otherwise, description posed structure, there may be alternative models
of a child’s psychiatric status will remain incomplete that fit equally well or perhaps even better.
and intervention planning may run into difficulties. Another potential limitation is that no proxy data
Also, as pointed out by previous studies (Dickey & were available for early and late adolescents. The
Blumberg, 2004; Ronning et al., 2004), removal correspondence between self-report and proxies for
of the positively construed items could result in older age groups is likely to differ from findings in
decreased acceptability, and is therefore not preadolescence. Of that reason, the findings on
recommended. proxy data in our study cannot be generalized to all
Sixth, as indicated by different factor loadings, age groups.
parents and their children only modestly agree in Finally, in a validation process, two questions
ratings on children’s strengths and difficulties need to be addressed explicitly (Messick, 1975): Is
(Goodman, 2001). Method factors tend to make the SDQ any good as a measure of psychological
large contributions to psychological measures, and adjustment in children and adolescents and, should
the results of the present study confirmed this is the SDQ be used for the proposed purpose?
Ó 2008 The Authors
Journal compilation Ó 2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Construct validity of the SDQ 1311

Evaluation of psychometric properties refers to the References


first question, which has been the topic of the pres-
Achenbach, T.M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior
ent paper. However, the present study lacked infor-
Checklist/ 4–18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington: Uni-
mation on important psychometric properties such versity of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.
as sensitivity, responsiveness and test–retest American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic
repeatability. To be of best practical value for and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV)
screening, clinical purposes or in randomised trials, (4th edn). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
it is important also to address these properties. Association.
Answering the second question requires an evalua- Becker, A., Steinhausen, H.C., Baldurson, G., Dalsg-
tion of the potential consequences of the use of SDQ aard, S., Lorenzo, M.J., Ralston, S.J., Döpfner, M., &
in epidemiological and clinical studies. It is neces- Rothenberger, A. (2006). Psychopathological screen-
sary to take into account ethical/social consider- ing of children with ADHD: Strengths and Difficulties
Questionnaire in a pan-European study. European
ations, balancing between a wish to investigate the
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 15(Suppl. 1), 56–62.
mental health of children and the availability of
Becker, A., Woerner, W., Hasselhorn, M., Banaschew-
effective treatment services, when knowledge on ski, T., & Rothenberger, A. (2004). Validation of the
possible psychopathology and burden is established. parent and teacher SDQ in a clinical sample. Euro-
How to manage this challenge deserves closer pean Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 13(Suppl. 2),
attention in future research. II11–II16.
Bollen, K.A. (1989). Structural equations with latent
variables. New York: John Wiley.
Conclusions Bourdan, K.H., Goodman, R., Rae, D.S., Simpson, G., &
Koretz, D.S. (2005). The Strengths and Difficulties
The results of the present study indicated support for Questionnaire: U.S. normative data and psycho-
the proposed five-factor structure of the SDQ (Good- metric properties. Journal of the American Academy
man, 2001) across a wide age range (10–19 years), of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 557–564.
including older adolescents and different informants. Campbell, D.T., & Fiske, D.W. (1959). Convergent and
However, some improvements should be considered discriminant validation by the multitrait-multi-
to improve internal reliability and conceptual clarity. method matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 2, 81–105.
Our results indicated support for the suggestions Dickey, W.C., & Blumberg, S.J. (2004). Revisiting the
factor structure of the Strengths and Difficulties
made by Ronning et al. (2004) that the Norwegian
questionnaire: United States, 2001. Journal of the
version of the SDQ would probably benefit from
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychi-
‘avoiding semantic similarities in item wording, posi- atry, 43, 1159–1167.
tive and negative wording within the same construct, Elander, J., & Rutter, M. (1996). Use and development
and two sentences within the same item’. of the Rutter Parents’ and Teachers’ Scales. Inter-
However, as stated by Goodman, Iervolino, Colli- national Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research,
shaw, Pickles, and Maughan (2007), potential 6, 63–78.
changes in item wording should only be adapted Fayers, P.M., & Machin, D. (2007). Quality of life: The
after careful consideration and pilot testing as assessment, analysis and interpretation of patient
seemingly minor changes to a questionnaire can reported outcome. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
make a big difference to mean scores and create Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties
Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child
problems for meaningful comparisons between the
Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581–586.
original and the modified measure and for combining
Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the
results in meta-analyses. Strengths and Difficulties questionnaire. Journal of
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psy-
chiatry, 40, 1337–1345.
Acknowledgements Goodman, R., Iervolino, A.C., Collishaw, S., Pickles, A.,
& Maughan, B. (2007). Seemingly minor changes to a
This research was completed with assistance from
questionnaire can make a big difference to the mean
EXTRA funds from the Norwegian Foundation for scores: A cautionary tale. Social Psychiatry and
Health and Rehabilitation, and performed in coop- Psychiatric Epidemiology, 42, 322–327.
eration with the Norwegian Health Services Research Goodman, R., Meltzer, H., & Bailey, V. (2003). The
Centre, incorporated in the Norwegian Knowledge Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A pilot
Centre for Health Services. study on the validity of the self-report version. Inter-
national Review of Psychiatry, 15, 173–177.
Goodman, R., & Scott, S. (1999). Comparing the
Correspondence to Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire and the
Child Behavior Checklist: Is small beautiful? Journal
Betty Van Roy, University in Oslo, Institute of Psy- of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 17–24.
chiatry, PB 1078 Blindern, 0316 Oslo, Norway; Tel: Heiervang, E., Stormark, K.M., Lundervold, A.J., Hei-
+47 23 49 21 20; Fax: +47 23 49 23 02; Email: mann, M., Goodman, R., Posserud, M.B., et al.
r.e.van@medisin.uio.no (2007). Psychiatric disorders in Norwegian 8- to

Ó 2008 The Authors


Journal compilation Ó 2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
1312 Betty Van Roy, Marijke Veenstra, and Jocelyne Clench-Aas

10-year olds: An epidemiological survey of preval- Obel, C., Heiervang, E., Rodriguez, A., Heyerdahl, S.,
ence, risk factors, and service use. Journal of the Smedje, H., Sourander, A., Guethmundsson, O.O.,
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychi- Clench-Aas, J., Christensen, E., Heian, F., Mathie-
atry, 46, 438–447. sen, K.S., Magnusson, P., Njarethvik, U., Koskelai-
Heyerdahl, S. (2003). Strengths and Difficulties Ques- nen, M., Ronning, J.A., Stormark, K.M., & Olsen, J.
tionnaire: A new questionnaire for mental health (2004). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire
assessment in children and adolescents, used in in the Nordic countries. European Child and Adoles-
recent Norwegian surveys: UNGHUBRO, OPPHED cent Psychiatry, 13(Suppl. 2), II32–II39.
and TROFINN. Nordic Journal of Epidemiology, 13, Palmieri, P.A., & Smith, G.C. (2007). Examining the
127–135. structural validity of the Strengths and Difficulties
Jöreskog, K.G. (2005). Structural equation modeling Questionnaire (SDQ) in a U.S. sample of custodial
with ordinal variables using LISREL. Revised 2005. grandmothers. Psychological Assessment, 19, 189–
Available at http://www.ssicentral.com/lisrel/tech 198.
docs/ordinal.pdf. Ronning, J.A., Handegaard, B.H., Sourander, A., &
Koskelainen, M., Sourander, A., & Vauras, M. (2001). Morch, W.T. (2004). The Strengths and Difficulties
Self-reported strengths and difficulties in a commun- Self-Report Questionnaire as a screening instrument
ity sample of Finnish adolescents. European Child in Norwegian community samples. European Child
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 10, 180–185. and Adolescent Psychiatry, 13, 73–82.
Loehlin, J.C. (1998). Latent variable models. An intro- Rothenberger, A., & Woerner, W. (2004). Editorial.
duction to factor, path and structural Analysis. Mah- Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) –
wah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. evaluations and applications. European Child and
Marsh, H.W., & Hocevar, D. (1983). Confirmatory factor Adolescent Psychiatry, 13(Suppl. 2), II/1–II/2.
analysis of multitrait-multimethod matrices. Journal Smedje, H., Broman, J.E., Hetta, J., & von Knorring,
of Educational Measurement, 20, no 3, 231–248. A.L. (1999). Psychometric properties of a Swedish
Marzocchi, G., Capron, C., Di Pietro, M., Tauleria, E., version of the ‘Strengths and Difficulties Question-
Duyme, M., Frigerio, A., et al. (2004). The use of the naire’. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 8,
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) in 63–70.
Southern European countries. European Child and Van Roy, B., Groholt, B., Heyerdahl, S., & Clench-Aas,
Adolescent Psychiatry, 13(Suppl. 2), 40–46. J. (2006). Self-reported strengths and difficulties in a
Mathai, J., Anderson, P., & Bourne, A. (2004). Com- large Norwegian population 10–19 years: Age and
paring psychiatric diagnoses generated by the gender specific results of the extended SDQ-ques-
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire with dia- tionnaire. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
gnoses made by clinicians. Australian and New 15, 189–198.
Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 38, 639–643. Vostanis, P. (2006). Strengths and Difficulties Ques-
Messick, S. (1975). The standard problem. Meaning and tionnaire: Research and clinical applications. Current
values in measurement and evaluation. American Opinion in Psychiatry, 19, 367–372.
Psychologist, 75, 955–966. Woerner, W., Fleitlich-Bilyk, B., Martinussen, R., Fletch-
Muris, P., Meesters, C., Eijkelenboom, A., & Vincken, er, J., Cucchiaro, G., Dalgalarrondo, P., Lui, M., &
M. (2004). The self-report version of the Strengths Tannock, R. (2004). The Strengths and Difficulties
and Difficulties Questionnaire: Its psychometric Questionnaire overseas: Evaluations and applications
properties in 8 to 13-year-old non-clinical children. of the SDQ beyond Europe. European Child and
British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 437–448. Adolescent Psychiatry, 13(Suppl. 2), II47–II54.
Muris, P., Meesters, C., & van den Berg, F. (2003). The World Health Organization. (1994). The ICD-10 Classi-
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) – fication of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Dia-
further evidence for its reliability and validity in a gnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva: WHO.
community sample of Dutch children and adoles-
cents. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 12,
1–8. Manuscript accepted 8 April 2008

Ó 2008 The Authors


Journal compilation Ó 2008 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.

You might also like