Efectos de La Consideracion Selectiva de Alternativas

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

JOURNAL OF CONSUMER PSYCHOLOGY, 12(3), 203–213

Copyright © 2002, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. E FFE CT SOF


P OSAVAC,
SE LE CT IVE
SANBONMATS
CONSID ERATI
U, HO
ON

The Effects of the Selective Consideration of Alternatives on


Consumer Choice and Attitude–Decision Consistency
Steven S. Posavac
Simon Graduate School of Business
University of Rochester

David M. Sanbonmatsu and Edward A. Ho


Department of Psychology
University of Utah

There are many instances of consumer decision making in which more consideration is given to
1 brand than to others in the choice set. This research explored how selective consideration of a
brand affects attitudes toward the brand, relative standing of the focal brand within the choice
category, and decision making. Experiment 1 demonstrated that when participants were
prompted to consider a randomly determined focal alternative, that alternative was more likely
to be chosen than nonfocal alternatives. Moreover, willingness to pay for an alternative was
higher if it was the focus of consideration. Attitudinal data suggest that the selective consider-
ation effect occurred because attitudes toward the focal alternative became more positive com-
pared to those toward other alternatives in the choice set. Experiment 2 elucidated this attitudi-
nal effect by demonstrating that selective consideration could cause the extremity of
consumers’ attitudes toward a focal brand to become more positive. Experiment 3 explored the
potential of the selective consideration of a focal alternative to influence the consistency be-
tween consumers’ attitudes and decisions and established that the initial attitude toward a focal
alternative moderated the selective consideration effect.

Making the best possible consumer choice requires careful cal alternative more extreme, which in turn may lead to more
consideration of all the attributes of all the possible alterna- favorable judgments and increased purchase likelihood of the
tives that might satisfy a need. Indeed, Payne, Bettman, and focal alternative. We also explored the implications of the se-
Johnson (1993) forwarded the weighted additive decision lective consideration of alternatives for the consistency be-
rule, in which the expected utility of each alternative is calcu- tween consumers’ attitudes and decisions.
lated based on attribute levels and importance weights, as the
normative prescription for rational choice behavior. How-
ever, there are clearly many instances in which consumers SELECTIVE VERSUS COMPARATIVE
give more consideration to some alternatives than to others. PROCESSING
For example, seeing an ad, hearing about a brand from a friend,
or being drawn to a particularly salient brand in a store may all The results of recent research in marketing and psychology
facilitate the selective consideration of a particular brand. suggests that in many cases, in contrast to normative prescrip-
In this research, we examined the possibility that simply tions, individuals do not give consideration to a large set of
selectively focusing on one brand from a choice set may have competing options prior to making a judgment or choice (e.g.,
implications for product judgments and subsequent choice Hsee, Loewenstein, Blount, & Bazerman, 1999; Kardes,
from the set. Specifically, we investigated how selective fo- Kalyanaram, Chandrashekaren, & Dornoff, 1993; Lehman &
cus on a choice alternative may make attitudes toward the fo- Pan, 1994; Lynch, Marmorstein, & Weigold, 1988;
Nedungadi, 1990; Shapiro, Macinnis, & Heckler, 1997). In-
Requests for reprints should be sent to Steven S. Posavac, Simon Gradu- stead, individuals often engage in selective hypothesis testing
ate School of Business, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14627. of the merit of alternatives (Sanbonmatsu, Posavac, Kardes,
E-mail: posavac@simon.rochester.ed u & Mantel, 1998; Trope & Liberman, 1996). A hypothesis was
204 POSAVAC, SANBONMATSU, HO

defined broadly by Sanbonmatsu et al. to include possible es- mental alternatives is considered. In much the same way,
timations, interpretations, evaluations, explanations, rules, or choices may be best when all information about all the choice
solutions when judgments are made under uncertainty. Selec- alternatives is considered. We suggest here that the selective
tive hypothesis testing refers to the tendency of individuals to consideration of a focal choice alternative may have effects
evaluate competing hypotheses one at a time. Rather than similar to the selective consideration of a judgmental alterna-
search for evidence for multiple hypotheses and compare the tive. If consumers focus on one choice alternative from a set,
evidence for the various alternatives, individuals tend to fo- the selective consideration of the attributes of the focal alter-
cus on the evidence for one hypothesis (i.e., the focal hypoth- native may lead consumers to develop an attitude toward the
esis; Dhar, Nowlis, & Sherman, 1999; Dhar & Simonson, alternative that is both more positive than the attitude was
1992). If evidence for the focal hypothesis is readily obtained, prior to consideration and more positive in relation to atti-
individuals often conclude that it is true or is the best hypothe- tudes toward other alternatives in the set.
sis. If evidence supporting the focal hypothesis is difficult to Work on attitude polarization supports the notion that se-
come by, typically the hypothesis is rejected, and the individ- lectively focusing on an object may have implications for
ual considers another hypothesis. Of course, a confident as- subsequent attitudes toward the object (e.g., Leone, 1984;
sessment of which hypothesis is best requires the consider- Tesser, 1976; Tesser & Cowan, 1975; Tesser & Leone, 1977;
ation of the evidence for all of the competing hypotheses. for a review, see Tybout & Artz, 1994). Attitude polarization
This bias in evidence gathering and biases in the subsequent refers to the phenomenon whereby merely thinking about an
interpretation and assimilation of evidence often result in the object results in attitudes toward the object becoming more
focal hypothesis appearing particularly attractive. That is, extreme, or polarized, in the initial direction of the attitude
had other hypotheses been considered, the individual may (i.e., positive or negative; see also Judd & Brauer, 1995). For
have concluded that a different hypothesis was correct. example, Sadler and Tesser (1973) found that asking individ-
Consumer researchers have delineated a variety of exam- uals to think about a liked other caused subsequent evalua-
ples of judgmental errors that result from the selective consid- tions of the other to become more positive.
eration of a focal option. For example, Houghton and Kardes Initial conceptions of, attitudes toward, or hypotheses
(1998) reported that consumers asked to estimate the market about an object may bias consumers’ thinking about it. Rather
share enjoyed by a particular company tended to (a) focus on than thinking about how the object compares with alterna-
attributes of the focal company, (b) neglect consideration of tives, consumers may tend to focus on the evidence for the ob-
attributes of competing companies, and accordingly, (c) dra- ject that is the focus of consideration. Much of this evidence, of
matically overestimate the market share held by the focal course, is consistent with the initial attitude. Moreover, the
company. Other consequences of selective hypothesis testing processing of evidence tends to be biased, as consumers may
include overestimations of the utility of a focal solution to a interpret ambiguous evidence in a manner that is consistent
managerial problem (Kardes & Cronley, 2000) and the with the initial attitude. In addition, inferences about missing
positivity of traits of a focally considered individual (Klar & or unknown attributes tend to be consistent with initial con-
Giladi, 1997). Moreover, Sanbonmatsu et al. (1998) sug- ceptions, and evidence that is inconsistent with the initial atti-
gested that diverse judgmental errors, such as biases in causal tude may be likely to be discounted. All of these processes and
attribution, the hindsight bias, probability overestimation, tendencies may lead to attitudes that are more extreme than
overconfidence, illusory correlation, and the fundamental at- was the case prior to focal consideration of the alternative. It
tribution error, may result from individuals’ tendency to en- should be noted that selective consideration of an alternative
gage in selective hypothesis testing. In each of these cases, a does not necessarily lead to attitudes that are more positive. In-
focal alternative is judged to be superior to competitors be- stead, the attitude after selective consideration will be more
cause evidence supporting the focal alternative receives more extreme than the initial evaluation of the alternative but will
consideration than evidence for other possibilities. likely be in the same direction as the initial attitude.

HOW THE SELECTIVE CONSIDERATION HOW SELECTIVE CONSIDERATION


OF A FOCAL BRAND MAY INFLUENCE MAY AFFECT CHOICE
ATTITUDES
The tendency to consider objects selectively may have impli-
What implications might the tendency to selectively consider cations for consumers’ choices if attitudes toward the focal
alternatives have for consumers’ attitudes toward choice op- brand are influenced by selective consideration. If a con-
tions? In the same way that judgment requires the integration sumer focuses on one brand in a category to the exclusion of
of evidence relevant to the truth or likelihood of hypotheses, other brands, the attitude toward the focal brand may come to
consumer choice requires the gathering and integration of in- be more favorable than it was prior to consideration. More-
formation about the various choice alternatives. Judgments over, if nonfocal brands are not given consideration, the focal
are typically most accurate when information about all judg- option may gain in relative standing within the choice cate-
EFFECTS OF SELECTIVE CONSIDERATION 205

gory. An interesting implication of this reasoning is that any particular choice category. Experiment 2 clarified how selec-
number of different alternatives within a particular choice tive consideration influenced attitudes. Specifically, the
category may benefit from selective consideration effects and purpose of this experiment was to demonstrate that selective
become relatively more preferred compared to other alterna- consideration changed attitudes themselves versus merely in-
tives that are not the focus of consideration. creasing the accessibility of the attitude toward the focal al-
If these hypothesized effects of selective consideration on ternative. Experiment 3 explored the potential of selective
attitudes toward objects within a choice category are correct, consideration to decrease attitude–decision consistency and
consumers’ choices are likely to be influenced by selective demonstrated that the initial attitude toward a focally consid-
consideration. Specifically, if attitudes toward a focally con- ered choice option was a moderator of the selective consider-
sidered choice option become more positive relative to other ation effect.
choice options in the category, choice from the category may
be biased in favor of the focally considered alternative.
Research on the mere measurement effect supports the no- EXPERIMENT 1
tion that focusing on particular products or consumer catego-
ries can influence consumers’ buying behavior (Feldman & Experiment 1 explored the effects of selectively considering
Lynch, 1988; Fitzsimons & Morwitz, 1996; Morwitz, John- one option from a choice set on decision making from the set.
son, & Schmittlein, 1993). For example, Fitzsimons and We predicted that the brand that was the focus of consider-
Morwitz reported that asking automobile owners which ation would be more likely to be chosen than nonfocal brands
brand they would purchase if in the market for a new car in- and that participants would report a higher willingness to pay
creased the likelihood of a future purchase of the brand cur- for the focal brand. The experiment also explored the process
rently owned by the respondent. Ostensibly, measurement by which selective consideration may influence choice. Spe-
strengthened the existing positive attitudes owners held to- cifically, we expected that selective consideration would
ward their current brand, and these attitudes became more have implications for relative attitudes toward category alter-
likely to guide subsequent choice (see also Fazio, 1995). Con- natives such that the focal option would be evaluated more fa-
sistent with this notion, Fitzsimons and Morwitz also found vorably than nonfocal brands. The hypotheses (H1, H2, and
that measuring intent increased the consistency between H3) tested in this experiment, then, were the following:
stated preferences and purchase behavior.
Interestingly, our logic suggests that the consistency be- H1: Participants will be more likely to choose a focally
tween attitudes and decisions may be either increased or di- considered alternative than nonfocal alternatives.
minished by selective consideration of an alternative. If an H2: Participants will be willing to pay more for a focally
option that an individual likes best at the time of initial atti- considered alternative than nonfocal alternatives.
tude measurement is selectively considered, attitude–deci- H3: Attitudes toward a focally considered alternative will
sion consistency may increase because the considered option be more positive than attitudes toward nonfocal alter-
may be particularly likely to be chosen. In contrast, consider natives.
an option that is liked but not the most liked option in a choice
category. If such an option receives selective consideration,
attitude–decision consistency may decrease because the like- Method
lihood of the focal option being chosen may increase, and ac-
cordingly, the likelihood of choice of the previously most Participants. Twenty-five undergraduates participated
preferred option would decrease. in exchange for extra course credit. Students participated in-
dividually and were randomly assigned to an experimental
condition.
OVERVIEW OF THE EXPERIMENTS

Three experiments were conducted to explore how selective Procedure. On arrival at the laboratory, participants
consideration of a brand affects attitudes toward the brand, were told that the purpose of the study was to investigate con-
relative standing of the focal brand within the choice cate- sumers’ attitudes toward charitable organizations and their
gory, and decision making. Moreover, in this research, we at- decisions when they decide to give to a charity. Participants
tempted to elucidate the attitudinal processes that may under- were told that their first task would be to rate one charity that
lie the effects of selective consideration on choice, when would be randomly selected. The experimenter relayed that
selective consideration affects choice, and how selective con- one purpose of the study was to obtain specific ratings of sev-
sideration affects the likelihood that choice will be consistent eral charities but that each participant would rate only one
with attitudes. Experiment 1 explored the consequences of charity in the interest of time. After receiving these initial in-
the selective consideration of a focal alternative on choice, structions, participants were shown a spinner board that con-
willingness to pay, and attitudes toward brands belonging to a tained the names of four charities that each were previously
206 POSAVAC, SANBONMATSU, HO

determined in a pilot study to be regarded favorably. Partici- crease the relative positivity of attitudes toward the focal op-
pants then spun the spinner to select a charity to rate. Partici- tion, we compared the average attitude toward the focal
pants’ use of the spinner to randomly select their focal charity option with the expected attitude if the focus manipulation
precluded any possibility that participants would perceive was ineffectual. Because there were four charities on the list,
that there was anything unique from the experimenters’ per- the expected ranked attitude toward a randomly selected
spective about the focal charity compared to the nonfocal charity would be 2.5 if the manipulation did not influence atti-
charities. Accordingly, the use of a spinner reduced the likeli- tudes. The average attitude toward the focal option was 1.76,
hood of demand characteristics in participants’ responses. which was significantly more favorable than 2.5, t(24) = 4.00,
The experimenter then asked participants to provide several p < .001.
ratings of the focal charity (e.g., “As a whole, how important Taken together, the results of this experiment clearly dem-
are the functions served by the [focal charity]?” “What is your onstrate that simply thinking about a focal brand may bias
attitude towards the [focal charity]?”). choice in its favor. Moreover, willingness to pay for a focal al-
After the selective consideration manipulation, partici- ternative may be higher than willingness to pay for nonfocal
pants’ relative attitudes toward each of the charities were alternatives. The data suggest that these choice effects occur
measured. Participants were given a list of the four charities because the structure of preference within the category may
and asked to rank order their preferences after reading change if one brand receives selective consideration. Spe-
through the entire list. Participants were asked to read through cifically, attitudes toward the focal brand may become more
the list so that a consideration-sets explanation of the results positive relative to other choice options that do not receive
would be untenable (i.e., because all four options would have such consideration.
been present in the consideration set). Next, the experimenter A limitation of this experiment regards the rank-order atti-
asked the participants to suppose that they were going to tude measure. Although the focal option was ranked higher
make a donation to one of the four charities they previously than nonfocal options, there are two competing explanations
rated and to indicate their choice. To increase the realism of for this effect. The mechanism we would like to suggest is
the choice task, participants were instructed that the charity that thinking about a focal option causes consumers to orga-
chosen most by participants would receive a donation (we did nize information in memory regarding that option, thereby
make a charitable donation of $150 after the completion of leading to a polarization of attitudes. In turn, the resulting atti-
data collection). Finally, participants were asked to imagine tude becomes more positive than attitudes toward nonfocal
that they were going to give a donation to charity and to indi- options, which influences choices in the favor of the focal op-
cate how much they personally would be willing to pay to tion. The competing explanation is that thinking about a focal
each of the four charities on the list. object simply increases the accessibility of the attitude to-
After the dependent variables were collected, participants ward the focal option but that the attitude itself does not
were probed for suspicion, debriefed, given extra credit, and change (i.e., the extremity of the attitude stays the same). If
excused. the accessibility of the attitude toward the focal option is in-
creased, the attitude may be more likely to direct decision
making, which is consistent with the results we obtained (cf.
Results and Discussion Fazio, Powell, & Williams, 1989). We conducted a second
experiment to explore this question.
The primary analysis of this experiment explored whether the
focal option was more likely to be chosen than nonfocal op-
tions. A goodness-of-fi t test demonstrated that more partici- EXPERIMENT 2
pants chose the focal option (12 chose the focal option, 13
chose a nonfocal option) than would be expected by chance The purpose of Experiment 2 was to more fully understand
(i.e., 6.25 choices of the focal option, 18.75 choices of the the process by which selective consideration influences atti-
nonfocal option), c2(1, N = 25) = 7.05, p = .008. Thus, H1 was tudes. More specifically, Experiment 2 was conducted to
supported. Next, we calculated the percentage of partici- demonstrate that the attitude toward a focal brand actually be-
pants’ intended contributions allocated to the focal charity comes more favorable, versus simply becoming more acces-
compared to their total contribution intention. H2 was sup- sible, as a result of selective consideration. Some have argued
ported, as the percentage of the total contribution that was al- that thought is equivalent to rehearsal of an attitude (Judd &
located to the focal charity (39.44%) was greater than ex- Brauer, 1995), rehearsal has been shown to increase the ac-
pected if the manipulation was ineffectual (i.e., 100%/4, or cessibility of an attitude (Fazio, 1995), and highly accessible
25%), t(24) = 2.65, p = .014. attitudes are more likely to guide behavior than less accessi-
We expected that selective focus on a charity would bias ble attitudes (Fazio, 1995; Fazio et al., 1989). Although selec-
choice in its favor because attitudes toward the focal charity tive consideration of an alternative may indeed make atti-
would be more favorable than attitudes toward nonfocal op- tudes toward it more accessible, we expected that selective
tions. To explore the potential of selective consideration to in- consideration would lead the attitude toward a focal alterna-
EFFECTS OF SELECTIVE CONSIDERATION 207

tive to actually become more positive. As discussed earlier, ment 1. Following rating attributes of the focal charity and
consumers’ initial conceptions of an object may bias subse- restaurant, participants completed an attitude measure similar
quent thinking about it because evidence consistent with the to the one they completed in the first experimental session.
initial attitude is likely to be generated, ambiguous informa- After responding to the attitude measure, participants were
tion is likely to be perceived to be consistent with the initial debriefed and excused.
attitude, and information inconsistent with the initial attitude
may be discounted. Accordingly, we hypothesized that the
extremity of the attitude toward a focally considered brand Results and Discussion
would increase following a manipulation of selective focus:
Analyses focused on exploration of the hypothesis that selec-
H4: Attitudes toward a focally considered alternative will tive focus on an alternative would increase the positivity of
become more extreme as a result of focal consider- participants’ attitudes toward the focal alternative. The mean
ation. attitudes toward the focal alternative and nonfocal alterna-
tives considered together are presented in Table 1.
A secondary goal of the study was to replicate the selective For both of the product categories, we conducted 2 × 2
consideration effect on attitudes within a different product completely within-subjects analyses of variance (ANOVAs)
category. of average attitudes. Focus (focal alternative vs. nonfocal al-
ternatives) and time (premanipulation vs. postmanipulation)
served as the independent variables. The ANOVA of the res-
Method taurant data revealed a significant effect of time such that atti-
tudes were more positive after the selective focus
Participants. Eighty-nine students in an introductory manipulation, F(1, 86) = 12.63, p = .001. There was no main
marketing class participated in exchange for extra credit. Stu- effect of focus, F(1, 86) < 1. Importantly and consistent with
dents participated in large group administrations. predictions, the interaction between focus and time was sig-
nificant, F(1, 86) = 6.27, p = .014. To explore the nature of the
interaction, a contrast was calculated that directly assessed
Procedure. This experiment took place in two sessions whether attitudes toward the focal alternative became more
that were separated by a week. The purpose of the first session positive as a result of the selective focus manipulation. This
was to measure participants’ attitudes toward four fast food contrast was a within-subjects t test that compared the change
restaurants and four charities. The alternatives included in in attitudes between Time 1 and Time 2 as a function of focus.
this experiment were generally popular, well-known, and This analysis showed that attitudes toward the focal option
liked brands. Participants were given lists of objects belong- became more positive over time than did attitudes toward
ing to a variety of categories, including the two categories of nonfocal options, t(86) = 2.50, p = .014.
interest, and were asked to indicate their attitudes on 9-point Results for the charities closely mirrored the restaurant re-
Likert-type scales ranging from –4 (dislike very much) to 4 sults. The ANOVA of the charity data revealed a significant
(like very much). After indicating their attitudes toward the effect of time such that attitudes were more positive after the
items in each product category, participants were excused. selective focus manipulation, F(1, 85) = 55.65, p < .001. There
In the second session, participants were told that the exper- was no main effect of focus, F(1, 85) < 1. As expected, there
imenter was interested in more specific evaluations of some was a significant interaction between focus and time, F(1, 85)
of the items they rated in the first session. Participants were = 8.17, p = .005. A contrast demonstrated that attitudes toward
further told that in the interests of saving time (the experiment the focal option became more positive over time than did atti-
was conducted in a class), they would each be asked to evalu- tudes toward nonfocal options, t(85) = 2.86, p = .005.
ate only one restaurant and one charity that would be ran- Taken together, the results for charities and restaurants
domly determined. The experimenter relayed that the clearly demonstrate that the attitude toward an alternative can
questionnaires about to be passed out to students contained
different items to be rated and that although only one exem- TABLE 1
plar from each category would be rated by each student, all Mean Attitudes Toward Focal and Nonfocal Alternatives
Premanipulation and Postmanipulation: Experiment 2
the exemplars would be rated by some students. These in-
structions minimized the possibility that participants would Premanipulation Postmanipulation
respond in accordance with experimental demand. The
Restaurants
packet given to participants contained the name of one restau-
Focal 0.57 1.23
rant and one charity. Participants were asked to think about Nonfocal 0.68 0.91
the service, the food items, and the overall experience of eat- Charities
ing at the focal restaurant. Participants were asked to think Focal 1.71 2.69
about the focal charity in the same way they did in Experi- Nonfocal 1.90 2.48
208 POSAVAC, SANBONMATSU, HO

change as a result of selective consideration. Although selec- H7: The selective consideration effect will not obtain
tive focus may indeed increase the accessibility of an attitude, when the initial attitude toward the focal option is
the extremity of the attitude (i.e., the attitude itself) also negative.
changes when consumers selectively consider a focal option.
These findings also clarify where the “action” is in the se- A final purpose of Experiment 3 was to show effects of se-
lective consideration effect. The selective consideration ef- lective consideration on choice in a different product category
fect occurs because attitudes toward the focal brand become than charities (i.e., vs. attitudes alone as in Experiment 2).
more positive, not because attitudes toward nonfocal alterna-
tives become more moderate.
Method

Participants.
EXPERIMENT 3 One hundred two students participated in the experiment in
exchange for extra course credit and the promise of a $1 res-
The first aim of Experiment 3 was to explore the potential of taurant coupon to be given after completion of the experi-
selective consideration to influence attitude–decision consis- ment. Students participated individually and were randomly
tency. If a consumer is prompted to consider an alternative assigned to condition.
that he or she likes best, the likelihood of choice of the most
favored option may increase, and accordingly, attitude–deci-
sion consistency may also be higher. Thus Procedure. The experiment took place in two experi-
mental sessions separated by a week. On arrival at the first ex-
H5: Selective focus on the alternative from a set that a par- perimental session, participants were instructed that they
ticipant likes best will increase the likelihood of that would be asked to complete several questionnaires regarding
option being chosen and increase attitude–decision demographic information and consumer preferences. The
consistency. first questionnaire contained basic demographic items and
items asking whether or not participants owned particular
If consumers focally consider an option that is liked but is products. Participants were also asked to indicate to which
initially not the most preferred option within a choice cate- media sources they were routinely exposed. The purpose of
gory, attitude–decision consistency may be disrupted if the this questionnaire was to distract participants from the true
consumer becomes more likely to choose the focal option. If nature of the experiment. After completing these items, par-
an option that is not the most preferred but is nevertheless ticipants were asked to rank order their attitudes toward ob-
liked is focally considered, consumers may come to prefer jects belonging to a variety of categories. The category of in-
that option after the cognitive organization of evaluative in- terest in this experiment was fast food restaurants, and the
formation that focal consideration engenders. In this case, a restaurants included were Arby’s, McDonald’s, Kentucky
decision made after focal consideration may be inconsistent Fried Chicken, and Burger King. Other categories included as
with a consumer’s attitudes measured before focal consider- distractors included long-distance phone companies, laundry
ation. Accordingly, we expected that the following hypothe- detergents, local TV stations, soda pop, and automobiles. Af-
sis would bear true: ter completing all the attitude measures, participants were
asked to complete a bogus consumer preferences survey. Af-
H6: Selective focus on a preferred but not most preferred ter this survey was completed, a second experimental session
option will lead to an increase in the likelihood of the was scheduled, and participants were excused.
focal option being chosen and will accordingly reduce On arrival at the second experimental session, participants
attitude–decision consistency because the previously completed additional bogus questionnaires and were then
most preferred option will be less likely to be chosen. told that the experimenters were interested in participants’
evaluations of the attributes of either fast food restaurants (the
The second purpose of the experiment was the exploration selective consideration conditions) or soft drinks (the control
of the potential of the initial attitude toward a focally consid- condition). In the selective consideration conditions, partici-
ered option to bound the selective consideration effect. If the pants were asked to think about and rate several attributes of
initial attitude toward a subsequently considered focal option one fast food restaurant. In the control condition, participants
is negative, focal consideration will result in the organization rated attributes of a soft drink. A computer program was writ-
of the negative evaluative beliefs and experiences that con- ten that ostensibly randomly determined which restaurant or
tributed to the initial overall negative attitude. Accordingly, soft drink each participant would rate. Participants pressed a
focally considering an initially disliked option should not in- key on a computer, and the phrase “Randomizing, please wait
crease the likelihood of that option being chosen. This analy- a moment” appeared on the monitor. Then, the names of all of
sis suggests the following hypothesis: the options (i.e., restaurants or soft drinks) scrolled quickly
EFFECTS OF SELECTIVE CONSIDERATION 209

across a box in the center of the monitor. After a few seconds, tain because of a strong ceiling effect in choice; participants
the computer stopped scrolling and listed the name of the res- in the control condition and those who selectively considered
taurant or soft drink the participant would be asked to rate. the most preferred restaurant demonstrated a strong tendency
This procedure was done to give participants the idea that the to choose their most preferred option. Accordingly, there was
determination of object to rate was truly random, thus reduc- not a significant difference between these conditions, c2(1, N
ing the possibility of demand effects. In actuality, the specific = 52) = .13, ns, and H5 was not supported. We would have
object rated by participants was determined prior to their ar- liked to have obtained results supportive of the notion that se-
rival. Each participant was randomly assigned to one of four lective focus on a most preferred option would increase the
conditions. In the selective consideration conditions, partici- likelihood of its being chosen. Without this result, we cannot
pants were randomly assigned to consider either the restau- make a strong statement about the potential of selective focus
rant they rated first, second, or fourth in the initial attitude to increase attitude–decision consistency.
measurement. Participants were then asked to think about at- The most important analysis in this experiment explored
tributes of the selected restaurant read by the experimenter the proposition that selectively considering the second most
and then to indicate their perception of each attribute. For ex- preferred option would sway choice in favor of that option
ample, participants were asked to think about the food options and away from the option that initially was most preferred.
offered by the focal restaurant and issues such as the quality Consistent with expectations, participants who selectively
of service and so on. Participants in the control condition considered their second most preferred option were over
were not asked to rate a restaurant but instead were asked to three times more likely to select it versus the control condi-
rate a soft drink. The selection of the soft drink was also done tion. This difference was significant, c2(1, N = 53) = 4.65, p =
by computer, and although the selection process appeared .031. Unsurprisingly, these participants were also more likely
random to the participants, in actuality the computer was pro- to choose their second most preferred option than participants
grammed to select 7-UP for each control participant. who were prompted to consider their most preferred option,
After participants considered the attributes of a focal res- c2(1, N = 53) = 4.65, p = .031. These results, then, provide
taurant or soft drink, the experimenter announced that the ex- substantial support for H6.
periment was over but offered to give participants a $1 This finding is important because it speaks to the ubiquity
coupon to the fast food restaurant of their choice as a token of of the selective consideration effect. Participants’ decision
the experimenter’s gratitude for the participant’s time. The was an easy one; there were only four alternatives, which
experimenter then handed out a sheet that contained the were all present at both the time of initial attitude measure-
names of four restaurants for which coupons were available. ment and the time of decision speaking. When no selective
This list contained the four restaurants previously rank or- consideration was prompted, participants were extremely
dered by participants in the first session of the experiment. likely to choose the alternative they previously indicated they
Each participant relayed his or her coupon choice, which was preferred most. However, when the second-rated restaurant
noted by the experimenter. Because we had difficulty obtain- was selectively considered, choice was strongly swayed in its
ing $1 coupons from each of the four restaurants, we simply favor. Accordingly, our data demonstrate a strong selective
gave participants $1 and explained that we did not have a cou- consideration effect, which is striking because the experi-
pon for their selected restaurant. mental manipulation was relatively subtle.
After indicating their choice, participants were given extra The other implication of H6 is that selective focus on a
credit, probed for suspicion, debriefed, and excused. liked option other than the most preferred option can re-
duce the likelihood of choosing the most preferred option.
Consistent with H6, participants who selectively consid-
Results and Discussion ered their second most preferred option appeared to be less
likely to choose their previously most preferred option
The first analysis explored whether attitudes were related to than participants in the control condition, although this
choice across the conditions (see Table 2). We compared the
average attitude to what the expected value of the attitude TABLE 2
would be if attitudes and choice were unrelated. If attitudes Chocie Incidence as a Function of the Attitude Toward the Focal
and choice were unrelated, the average attitude toward the Restaurant: Experiment 3
chosen restaurant would be 2.5 (i.e., the average of the ranks Attitude Rank of Focal Restaurant
of the four restaurants). Analysis revealed a strong tendency
for participants to choose a preferred option, as the average Preference Rank of Choice 1 2 4 Control
attitude toward the chosen restaurant (M = 1.3) was more fa- 1 21 17 16 22
vorable than 2.5, t(101) = 20.95, p < .001. 2 3 10 6 3
We expected that focal consideration of the previously 3 1 0 1 0
most preferred restaurant would result in an increased likeli- 4 1 0 0 1
hood that it would be chosen. However, this effect did not ob- n 26 27 23 26
210 POSAVAC, SANBONMATSU, HO

trend was only marginally significant, c2(1, N = 53) = The selective consideration effect is likely to obtain only
3.20, p = .07. when the initial attitude toward the focal alternative is suffi-
Finally, we predicted that the initial attitude toward the fo- ciently positive. Indeed, if a focal alternative is initially per-
cal option would bound the selective consideration effect. ceived to be among the worst in the choice set, selective
Consistent with H7, consideration of the worst alternative in consideration is unlikely to result in more favorable attitudes
the choice set (in this case, the restaurant rated fourth) did not toward and choice of the focal alternative (Experiment 3). We
increase the likelihood of choosing it (no participants in this suggest, however, that in most product categories selective
condition chose the focal option) and had no disruptive ef- consideration is likely to be advantageous for most brands be-
fects on the probability that the most preferred option would cause most consumer products on the market are favorably
be chosen, c2(1, N = 49) = 1.58, ns. evaluated (or they would not remain on the market long).
In sum, the results of the third experiment show that (a) se- Numerous studies in marketing and psychology have
lective consideration of a focal alternative can reduce atti- shown that attitudes are typically good predictors of con-
tude–decision consistency if the focal option is relatively sumer behavior (e.g., Bagozzi & Burnkrant, 1979; Fazio et
inferior but nevertheless liked and (b) an important boundary al., 1989; Netemeyer & Bearden, 1992). This research shows,
condition of the selective consideration effect is the initial atti- however, that in some cases, selective consideration may re-
tude toward the focal option. It should be noted that our discus- duce the consistency between attitudes and decisions (Exper-
sion of attitude–decision consistency is based on a comparison iment 3). Specifically, if a consumer selectively considers an
of choice to the attitude measures taken during the first experi- option that is liked but is not the most preferred option in the
mental session. This is not to say that participants who consid- choice category, the likelihood that it will be chosen in-
ered the option they ranked second exhibited creases, and the likelihood of the previously most preferred
attitude–decision inconsistency with respect to their attitudes option being chosen decreases. This finding is important be-
after the manipulation. Indeed, it is our contention that these cause it delineates a situation in which measured preferences
effects obtained because attitudes changed as a result of the may not be reflected in actual choice.
manipulation and that choice was commensurate with restau- Marketers promoting the best liked brand in a category
rant attitudes participants held after the manipulation. should take pause from these results. It is often assumed
Importantly, the findings of Experiment 3 rule out a de- that if consumers have relatively more favorable attitudes
mand effect explanation. If demand characteristics were toward one brand than others in the choice category, all
guiding choice, selective consideration of the least pre- other things being equal (e.g., distribution, sales promo-
ferred option would have increased the likelihood of it be- tion), this brand will be the most purchased brand. How-
ing chosen. ever, this research shows that a brand that is not the most
preferred may come to be the most preferred simply as a re-
sult of receiving selective consideration. Accordingly, the
GENERAL DISCUSSION usual attitude-to-behavior process, whereby the most pre-
ferred brand is chosen, may be interrupted if consumers
Taken together, the three experiments clearly show that give selective consideration to a competitor. It may be im-
simply focusing on one alternative from a category can portant for marketers of the most liked brand to encourage
lead to changes in subsequent choice behavior because the consideration of their brand and to discourage selective
evaluation of the focal alternative becomes more extreme. consideration of competitors. Of course, this is one benefit
When consumers selectively consider one focal alternative of advertising: It encourages the selective consideration of
from a set of alternatives belonging to some product cate- the target brand.
gory, they may be more likely to choose the focal alterna- In contrast to this note of caution to those promoting
tive (Experiments 1 and 3) and be willing to allocate more top-rated brands, this research suggests an opportunity for
money to the focal alternative (Experiment 1) compared to marketers of brands that are liked but are not the most liked
nonfocal alternatives. These choice effects occur because brand in the choice category. Marketers of such a brand may
consumers may perceive the focal alternative to be more at- typically assume that product refinement or aggressive pro-
tractive compared to other alternatives in the decision cate- motion is necessary to increase purchase likelihood. How-
gory following selective consideration (Experiment 1), in ever, this research suggests the far simpler intervention of
part because attitudes toward the focal alternative may be- encouraging selective consideration of the target brand. Con-
come more positive (Experiment 2). It should be noted that sumers may be capable of “confirming” that they like any one
these attitudinal effects do not result from the consumer of a number of brands within a choice category “best.” Sim-
learning any new information about the focal brand. In- ply being led to consider the attributes of one brand from a
stead, the act of simply considering a brand may invoke category can lead to more positive attitudes toward the focal
judgmental and inferential processes that result in an ini- brand, a more favorable ordinal position for the focal brand
tially held attitude becoming more extreme following se- with respect to preference within the choice category, and an
lective consideration. increased likelihood of choice of the focal brand.
EFFECTS OF SELECTIVE CONSIDERATION 211

It may be the case that consideration of the most preferred ent in the set. In Experiment 3, the restaurant that participants
brand within a category can increase the likelihood that it will rated first was conspicuous in the decision context, but many
be chosen and, accordingly, increase attitude–decision con- participants who selectively considered their second most
sistency. Unfortunately, there was a strong ceiling effect in preferred option chose it instead. Accordingly, in this case, at-
the Experiment 3 data such that nearly all participants in the titudes within the category must have changed in favor of the
control condition chose the restaurant they rated as most pre- option previously rated second.
ferred. Accordingly, it was impossible to determine if selec- It is also important to note that demand characteristics can-
tively considering the most preferred brand might increase not plausibly account for the choice and attitudinal effects re-
the likelihood of it being chosen. ported in this study. Although it is not clear that participants
Given that selective consideration can have significant ef- would have guessed the direction of the hypotheses even if
fects on attitudes and choice, it is important to consider when they perceived that particular options were selected by the ex-
selective consideration is likely to occur. Prior research sug- perimenters to be focal, several aspects of the methodology
gests that selective (vs. comparative) processing is common; and results are strongly indicative that demand characteristics
rather than consider all possible information and alternatives, were not operating. In Experiment, 1 participants themselves
people routinely selectively process information because randomly determined which option would be the focus of
such processing is easier and quicker compared to compara- consideration through the use of a spinner board. In Experi-
tive processing. Nevertheless, consumers clearly sometimes ment 2, participants were informed that each option from both
do engage in comparative processing. If consumers are product categories would be rated by some of the students in
highly involved in the decision and have ample opportunity to the class. Accordingly, these participants would not have per-
engage in careful decision making, they may be likely to en- ceived that there was anything special about the focal options
gage in comparative processing. Moreover, consumers with in their packets compared to nonfocal options because each
expertise within a product category may be less likely than option was focal for some students. In Experiment 3, we ex-
novice consumers to selectively consider choice options. pected that the computerized selection procedure would lead
Our data are the first to show how selective consideration participants to conclude that the option selected to be the fo-
of a brand can affect choice by changing consumers’ attitudes cus of comparison was determined randomly. The finding
toward decision options within a category. Specifically, we that the restaurant rated last in the first session was not chosen
focused on the process by which selective consideration in- in the second experimental session when it was the focal op-
fluences decision making by increasing the relative tion confirms our notions about the utility of the computer-
favorability of attitudes toward the focal brand compared to ized selection procedure and the absence of demand
other brands in the choice set. There may be other mecha- characteristics.
nisms through which the selective consideration of a brand
can affect choice. For example, in a memory-based decision
context, selective consideration of a focal brand may affect Which Brands Are Likely to Receive
choice by increasing the likelihood that the brand will be gen- Consideration and How to Encourage
erated from memory prior to decision making. Selective Consideration of a Brand
An alternate account of the choice results of Experiments 1
and 3 is that selective consideration of a focal alternative in- The optimal strategy for encouraging selective consideration
creased the likelihood that it would be included in the consid- of a target brand may vary as a function of whether the brand
eration set and that it would be subsequently chosen (vs. is typically chosen in a context that contains or does not con-
attitude change as the driver of the obtained results; cf. tain specified alternatives and whether the purchase inten-
Nedungadi, 1990; Nedungadi & Hutchinson, 1985). Several tion is set before or while the consumer is at the location in
aspects of the methodology and design of the experiments un- which purchase is made. If alternatives are not specified in
dermine the viability of this possibility. In both Experiments context or the purchase intention is set prior to the consumer
1 and 3, four options were clearly specified and salient in the visiting the store, an important determinant of whether or not
choice context (i.e., participants chose from uncluttered lists), a brand will be considered is how strongly it is linked to the
thus reducing the likelihood that consideration sets were superordinate category in the consumer’ minds (Posavac,
smaller than the set of presented options. Moreover, in Exper- Sanbonmatsu, & Fazio, 1997). To the extent that the ob-
iment 1, participants were explicitly asked to review all the ject–category association is strong, the object is likely to be
charities before making a decision. The most compelling evi- highly accessible and easily generated from memory and to
dence countering the consideration sets account is the Experi- receive consideration. Strong object–category associations
ment 3 finding that selectively considering the second most may be developed through repetition of the association be-
preferred restaurant increased the likelihood of it being cho- tween the target brand and the category, for example, with
sen. Nedungadi made the important point that the presence of branded merchandise, the utilization of memorable taglines
an inferior brand in the consideration set was not sufficient to or jingles and an increase in ad volume. Another strategy is
cause the brand to be chosen if a superior brand was also pres- the use of mystery ads, which have been shown to engender
212 POSAVAC, SANBONMATSU, HO

strong object–category associations (Fazio, Herr, & Powell, Feldman, Jack M., & Lynch, John G., Jr. (1988). Self-generated validity and
1992). other effects of measuremen t on belief, attitude, intention, and behav-
ior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 421–435.
When the choice intention is set in the presence of many Fitzsimons, Gavan J., & Morwitz, Vicki G. (1996). The effect of measuring
alternatives (e.g., in a store or online), the salience of brands intent on brand-level purchase behavior. Journal of Consumer Re-
will help determine the set of brands that are given consider- search, 23, 1–11.
ation. Well-researched variables such as brand name and the Houghton, David C., & Kardes, Frank R. (1998). Market share overestima-
packaging of the brand can be used to attract consumers’ at- tion and the noncomplementarit y effect. Marketing Letters, 9, 313–320.
Hsee, Christopher K., Loewenstein, George F., Blount, Sally, & Bazerman,
tention. Moreover, this research suggests that marketers’ ef- Max H. (1999). Preference reversals between joint and separate evalua-
forts to secure prime shelf space for their brands may be well tions of options: A review and theoretical analysis. Psychological Bul-
rewarded if consumers are likely to quickly notice the desired letin, 125, 576–590.
brands while shopping. Judd, Charles M., & Brauer, Markus. (1995). Repetition and evaluative ex-
It bears repeating that regardless of the specifics of the tremity. In Richard E. Petty & Jon A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength:
Antecedents and consequence s (pp. 43–71). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
purchase context, the effects of selective consideration de- Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
scribed in this article will only obtain if thinking about a fo- Kardes, Frank R., & Cronley, Maria L. (2000). Managerial decision making.
cal brand leads to a more favorable brand attitude. If In S. B. Dahiya (Ed.), The current state of business disciplines (pp.
consumers do not have sufficient brand knowledge or if 2921–2934). Rohtak, India: Spellbound.
their knowledge regarding a brand is negatively valenced, Kardes, Frank R., Kalyanaram, Gurumurthy, Chandrashekaren , Murali, &
Dornoff, Ronald J. (1993). Brand retrieval, consideration set composi-
encouraging selective consideration is unlikely to convey tion, consumer choice, and the pioneering advantage . Journal of Con-
competitive advantage. If, however, consumers do favor- sumer Research, 20, 62–75.
ably regard a brand, encouraging selective consideration Klar, Yechiel, & Giladi, Eilath E. (1997). No one in my group can be below
may help raise the brand above the competitive clutter and the group’s average: A robust positivity bias in favor of anonymous
increase purchase likelihood. peers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 885–901.
Lehman, Donald R., & Pan, Yigang. (1994). Context effects, new brand en-
try, and consideration sets. Journal of Marketing Research, 31,
364–374.
Leone, Christopher. (1984). Thought-induce d changes in phobic beliefs:
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Sometimes it helps, sometimes it hurts. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
1, 272–283.
The first two authors contributed equally to this study. Re- Lynch, John G., Marmorstein, Howard, & Weigold, Michael F. (1988).
search funding provided by the Simon School of Business is Choices from sets including remembered brands: Use of recalled attrib-
utes and prior overall evaluations. Journal of Consumer Research, 15,
gratefully acknowledged. The authors wish to thank Dawn 169–184.
Iacobucci, Eric Johnson, and an anonymous reviewer for Morwitz, Vicki G., Johnson, Eric, & Schmittlein, David. (1993). Does mea-
their helpful suggestions. Thanks are also due to Eric Ander- suring intent change behavior? Journal of Consumer Research, 20,
son, Rachel Barnes, Amber Eastburn, and Ashley Hodder for 46–61.
assistance in data collection. Nedungadi , Prakash. (1990). Recall and consumer consideration sets: Influ-
encing choice without altering brand evaluations. Journal of Consumer
Research, 17, 263–276.
Nedungadi, Prakash, & Hutchinson, J. Wesley. (1985). The prototypicality
of brands: Relationships with brand awareness , preference and usage.
REFERENCES In E. Hirshunan & M. Holbrook (Eds.), Advances in consume r re-
search (Vol. 12, pp. 408–503). Provo, UT: Association for Consumer
Bagozzi, Richard P., & Burnkrant, Robert E. (1979). Attitude organization Research.
and the attitude–behavior relationship. Journal of Personality and So- Netemeyer, Richard G., & Bearden, William O. (1992). A comparative anal-
cial Psychology, 37, 913–929. ysis of two models of behavioral intention. Journal of the Academy of
Dhar, Ravi, Nowlis, Stephen M., & Sherman, Steven J. (1999). Comparison Marketing Science, 20, 49–59.
effects on preference construction. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, Payne, John W., Bettman, James R., & Johnson, Eric J. (1993). The adaptive
293–306. decision maker. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Dhar, Ravi, & Simonson, Itamar. (1992). The effect of the focus of compari- Posavac, Steven S., Sanbonmatsu, David M., & Fazio, Russell H. (1997).
son on consumer preferences. Journal of Marketing Research, 29, Considering the best choice: Effects of the salience and accessibility of
430–440. alternatives on attitude–decision consistency. Journal of Personality
Fazio, Russell H. (1995). Attitudes as object-evaluatio n associations: Deter- and Social Psychology, 72, 253–261.
minants, consequences , and correlates of attitude accessibility. In Rich- Sadler, Orin, & Tesser, Abraham. (1973). Some effects of salience and time
ard E. Petty & Jon A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents upon interpersonal hostility and attraction. Sociometry, 36, 99–112.
and consequence s (pp. 247–282). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum As- Sanbonmatsu, David M., Posavac, Steven S., Kardes, Frank R., & Mantel,
sociates, Inc. Susan P. (1998). Selective hypothesi s testing. Psychonomic Bulletin &
Fazio, Russell H., Powell, Martha C., & Williams, Carol J. (1989). The role Review, 5, 197–220.
of attitude accessibility in the attitude-to-behavior process. Journal of Shapiro, Stewart, Macinnis, Deborah J., & Heckler, Susan E. (1997). The ef-
Consumer Research, 16, 280–288. fects of incidental ad exposure on the formation of consideration sets.
Fazio, Russell H., Herr, Paul M., & Powell, Martha C. (1992). On the devel- Journal of Consumer Research, 24, 94–104.
opment and strength of category–brand associations in memory: The Tesser, Abraham. (1976). Attitude polarization as a function of thought and
case of mystery ads. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 1, 1–13. reality constraints. Journal of Research in Personality, 10, 183–194.
EFFECTS OF SELECTIVE CONSIDERATION 213

Tesser, Abraham. (1978). Self-generated attitude change. In Leonard Berkowitz (Ed.), Ad- Trope, Yaacov , & , Liberman, Akiva. (1996). Social hypothesis test-
vancesinexperimentalsocialpsychology(Vol.11,pp.290–338).NewYork:Academic. ing: Cognitive and motivational mechan isms. In Edward Tory
Tesser, Abraham, & Cowan, Claudia L. (1975). Some effects of thought and number Higgins & Arie W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology:
of cognitionson attitude change. Social Behavior and Personality, 3, 165–173. Handboo k of basic principles (pp. 239–270). New York:
Tesser, Abraham, & Leone, Christopher. (1977). Cognitive schemas and Guilford.
thought as determinants of attitude change . Journal of Experimental Tybout, Alice M., & Artz, Nancy. (1994). Consumer psychology . Annual
Social Psychology, 13, 340–356. Review of Psychology, 45, 131–169.

You might also like