Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

NeuroImage 49 (2010) 865–874

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

NeuroImage
j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s e v i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / y n i m g

A common functional brain network for autobiographical, episodic, and semantic


memory retrieval
Hana Burianova a,b,⁎, Anthony R. McIntosh a,b, Cheryl L. Grady a,b,c
a
Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
b
Rotman Research Institute at Baycrest, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
c
Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The objective of this study was to delineate a common functional network that underlies autobiographical,
Received 22 April 2009 episodic, and semantic memory retrieval. We conducted an event-related fMRI study in which we utilized
Revised 9 August 2009 the same pictorial stimuli, but manipulated retrieval demands to extract autobiographical, episodic, or
Accepted 31 August 2009
semantic memories. To assess this common network, we first examined the functional connectivity of
Available online 8 September 2009
regions identified by a previous analysis of task-related activity that were active across all three tasks. Three
of these regions (left hippocampus, left lingual gyrus, and right caudate nucleus) appeared to share a
common pattern of connectivity. This was confirmed in a subsequent functional connectivity analysis using
these three regions as seeds. The results of this analysis showed that there was a pattern of functional
connectivity that characterized all three seeds and that was common across the three retrieval conditions.
Activity in inferior frontal and middle temporal cortex bilaterally, left temporoparietal junction, and anterior
and posterior cingulate gyri was positively correlated with the seeds, whereas activity in posterior occipito-
temporo-parietal regions was negatively correlated. These findings support the idea that a common neural
network underlies the retrieval of declarative memories regardless of memory content. This proposed
network consists of increased activity in regions that represent internal processes of memory retrieval and
decreased activity in regions that mediate attention to external stimuli.
© 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Introduction multiple memory systems view of the organization of declarative


memory (Tulving, 1987). This view posits that there are separate
Despite extensive research in the past several decades, a consensus memory systems, which specialize in the processing of distinct types
has yet to be reached as to the neural organization of declarative of information and recruit functionally independent neural networks,
memory retrieval. Conceptually, declarative memory retrieval has each mediating a specific memory function (Cabeza and Nyberg,
been traditionally dissociated into three types: (1) semantic retrieval, 2000; Gabrieli, 1998; Nyberg, McIntosh and Tulving, 1998; Nyberg et
characterized by the conscious recollection of factual information and al., 2002; Tulving and Schacter, 1990; Tulving, 1987). Empirical
general knowledge (Tulving, 1972); (2) episodic retrieval, character- evidence supporting this view comes primarily from studies showing
ized by the conscious recollection of experienced events, which functional dissociations between episodic, autobiographical, and
originally included personally relevant events (Tulving, 1972), but semantic memory. Neuropsychological studies show that patients
today typically pertains to memory for stimuli encoded in the with medial temporal lobe lesions are usually impaired on tasks
laboratory (i.e., ”laboratory memory”; see Cabeza and St. Jacques, involving autobiographical memory, but not on tasks involving
2007); and (3) autobiographical retrieval, characterized by the semantic memory (Gadian et al., 2000; Hirano and Noguchi, 1998;
conscious recollection of personally relevant events (Conway and Vargha-Khadem et al., 1997), suggesting that the medial temporal
Pleydell-Pearce, 2000). Much of the data from neuropsychological and lobes (the hippocampus, in particular) engage autobiographical
neuroimaging experiments (e.g., Cipolotti and Maguire, 2003; Gadian memory exclusively (Tulving et al., 1991; Tulving and Markowitsch,
et al., 2000; Hirano and Noguchi, 1998; Manns et al., 2003; Nyberg, 1998; Vargha-Khadem et al., 1997). Conversely, patients with
McIntosh and Tulving, 1998; Vargha-Khadem et al., 1997) support the semantic dementia whose neural damage often involves frontotem-
poral lobar degeneration (Hodges and Miller, 2001; Neary et al., 1998)
are characterized by severe semantic memory loss, whereas their
⁎ Corresponding author. Psychology Department, University of Toronto, 100 St.
autobiographical memory is relatively spared (Graham et al., 2003;
George Street, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5S 3G3. McKinnon et al., 2006; Snowden, Griffiths and Neary, 1994;
E-mail address: hburian@rotman-baycrest.on.ca (H. Burianova). Westmacott and Moscovitch, 2003). Further neural dissociations

1053-8119/$ – see front matter © 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.066
866 H. Burianova et al. / NeuroImage 49 (2010) 865–874

have been found in neuroimaging studies that show regional activity [e.g., Goldberg et al., 2007]). In healthy individuals, activity in the right
in the left inferior prefrontal cortex and left posterior temporal areas prefrontal cortex was shown to underlie retrieval of novel and
related to semantic retrieval (Graham et al., 2003; Vandenberghe et creative semantic relations (Dobbins and Wagner, 2005; Seger et al.,
al., 1996; Wiggs, Weisberg and Martin, 1999), left-lateralized activity 2000), whereas activity in the left prefrontal cortex was found to
in the medial temporal and ventromedial frontal regions, tempor- subserve some aspects of episodic remembering (Nolde et al., 1998).
opolar areas, retrosplenial cingulate cortex, and cerebellum related to This support for the unitary view of declarative retrieval even
autobiographical retrieval (Conway et al., 2002; Gilboa, 2004; Graham extends to the hippocampus, traditionally thought to subserve
et al., 2003; Maguire, 2001), and activity in the right dorsolateral episodic and autobiographical memory only (Tulving et al., 1991;
prefrontal areas subserving “laboratory” episodic retrieval (Duzel et Tulving and Markowitsch, 1998; Vargha-Khadem et al., 1997). For
al., 2004; Gilboa, 2004). It is critical to point out, however, that the example, there is evidence that hippocampal amnesics, when
focus of most of these neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies compared to healthy controls, exhibit impairments in semantic
was largely on differences and functional dissociations of autobio- retrieval in addition to profound deficits in episodic and autobio-
graphical, episodic, and semantic memory retrieval. In other words, graphical retrieval (Kopelman and Kapur, 2001; Manns et al., 2003;
the experimental paradigms (e.g., using different tasks to test episodic Squire and Zola, 1998). Similarly, recent imaging studies of healthy
and semantic memory) and statistical methods (e.g., subtracting individuals have reported hippocampal activation in both episodic
activity during one memory condition from another, instead of using a and semantic retrieval (e.g., Burianova and Grady, 2007; Ryan et al.,
neutral baseline) were designed to dissociate neural correlates among 2008). These data suggest that the hippocampus and the medial
the presumably different types of declarative memory. temporal cortices are involved in all declarative retrieval.
An alternative perspective to the multiple systems view is the In a previous study, we identified regional activations common to
unitary system view of the organization of declarative memory, which autobiographical, “laboratory” episodic, and semantic retrieval (Bur-
proposes the idea of a single declarative memory system (Baddley, ianova and Grady, 2007). While these results reflect the important
1984; Friston, 2002; Kihlstrom, 1984; McIntosh, 1999; Rajah and neural substrates that are involved in declarative retrieval, they do not
McIntosh, 2005; Roediger, 1984). Proponents of this view conjecture provide direct evidence about the functional connectivity of these
that a unitary memory system gives rise to all declarative memory neural regions. It seems reasonable to assume that memory retrieval,
retrieval, although memories can vary along a contextual continuum a highly complex cognitive process, would not be localized to discrete
of several dimensions, such as time, space, emotion, or strength of brain region(s), but rather would be mediated by the interaction
recollection (e.g., Baddley, 1984). This view rests on several among a number of functionally related neural areas. This idea is not
theoretical assumptions. Firstly, encoding of to-be-remembered new, as many researchers have argued that it is the activity of
material is almost always contextual (i.e., embedded in already distributed neural networks and the interactions among anatomically
attained knowledge; Baddley, 1984); at retrieval, memories may or connected brain regions that directly yield cognitive functions, such as
may not become decontextualized along one or more of these memory (e.g., Finger, 1994; Friston, 1997; McIntosh, 1998, 2000;
dimensions (Baddley, 1984; Rajah and McIntosh, 2005; Westmacott Mesulam, 1990). In other words, cognitive functions are the emergent
and Moscovitch, 2003). Autobiographical and semantic types of properties of the neural interactions (i.e., influences that neural
memory may thus be conceptualized as the opposite ends of the constituents have on one another) among numerous brain areas that
contextual continuum (Baddley, 1984; Kihlstrom, 1984; Roediger, comprise a neural network (McIntosh, 1999). An essential aspect of
1984). Secondly, even if autobiographical memory is at the most the network approach to the neural organization of cognitive function
detailed end of the contextual continuum, it is not free of factual, is the examination of the neural context associated with a specific
semantic information (Gilboa, 2004; Levine et al., 2002); hence, there behaviour (McIntosh, 1998, 1999). Neural context is conceptualized
is considerable overlap among these ‘types’ of memory, and the as the activity in a selected brain region that arises as a consequence of
boundaries between them are often unclear. Finally, in a similar vein, modulatory influences from other brain regions (McIntosh, 1998,
semantic memory is rarely entirely context-free, but rather contains 1999). Thus, what is important in determining the neural underpin-
some contextual and episodic components, although these may be ning of a cognitive function is the relation of activity in a brain area
degraded and lack rich detail (Gilboa, 2004; Westmacott and with activity of those brain areas with which it is connected. In
Moscovitch, 2003; Westmacott et al., 2004). parallel with this argument is the notion that by measuring only how
Evidence supporting the unitary memory system view consists of mean neural activity changes with task in a specific brain area or
both neuroimaging and neuropsychological data that show functional areas, one might fail to observe relevant interregional functional
overlap and interdependence of different memory functions (Duncan interactions that occur despite no significant change in the mean
and Owen, 2000; Kopelman and Kapur, 2001; Manns et al., 2003; activity (e.g., Grady et al., 1998; McIntosh et al., 1994).
Rajah and McIntosh, 2005; Squire and Zola, 1998). These include One approach to quantifying neural interactions is to assess the
studies comparing working, episodic, and semantic memory (Nyberg degree of functional connectivity among brain regions, i.e., the degree
et al., 2002, 2003), working and episodic memory (Braver et al., 2001; to which activity in a specific region correlates or covaries with
Cabeza et al., 2002; Duzel et al., 1999), and semantic and episodic activity in other areas across the whole brain, thus functioning
memory (Rajah and McIntosh, 2005; Ryan et al., 2008), all of which together as a network (Friston et al., 1993; Friston, 1994; Horwitz et
reported commonalities in neural activations across the memory al., 1984). A network is thus defined as a pattern of spatially remote
tasks. Rajah and McIntosh (2005) provided evidence for the unitary brain regions whose activity levels are correlated, or functionally
view by modeling separate functional networks for episodic and connected across participants, in order to support a particular
semantic retrieval tasks and assessing interregional correlations behaviour, regardless of whether the average level of activity in any
across the two tasks. The results showed no significant differences single region of the network is different between the experimental
in the interregional correlations, despite differences in regional conditions (Friston, 1994; Habib et al., 2003). To statistically study
activations in the two memory tasks, suggesting the involvement of complex neural interactions between different brain structures, the
a single memory system or network. Other data (e.g., Moscovitch, analytical methods must provide a means to quantifying the relation
1992; Seger et al., 2000; Shimamura, 1995) contradict the prefrontal between brain regions, rather than focusing on mean activity
hemispheric differentiation of episodic and semantic retrieval (i.e., differences. Multivariate approaches, such as the partial least squares
activity in the right prefrontal cortex is associated solely with episodic (PLS) approach to image analysis, enable investigation of functional
retrieval [Buckner et al., 1998; Cabeza and Nyberg, 2000] and activity connectivity of neural regions by calculating the covariance between
in the left prefrontal cortex is solely associated with semantic retrieval the activity within selected seed voxels and all other brain voxels
H. Burianova et al. / NeuroImage 49 (2010) 865–874 867

across the experimental conditions (McIntosh et al., 1996; McIntosh Control stimuli
and Gonzalez-Lima, 1994). For instance, Addis et al. (2004a) identified Five photographs were selected from the set of 50 described above
a number of autobiographical regions that differentiated general and and scrambled using a Matlab script. This ensured that the visual
specific autobiographical memories from a control baseline using stimulus was rendered meaningless.
univariate contrasts, but only a subsequent multivariate PLS analysis
(Addis et al., 2004b) revealed a shared functional network with a Procedure
critical connection to the hippocampus. Furthermore, Addis et al.
(2007) expanded on these findings by mapping an effective The study consisted of one control and three memory retrieval
autobiographical network in healthy individuals and epileptic conditions during fMRI scanning. Four 14-min runs of 50 trials each
patients. In addition to Addis' work, other researchers have assessed were presented to the participants in a counterbalanced order. Trials
the autobiographical network (Levine et al., 2004; Maguire et al. were randomized within each run. In each trial, an experimental or
2001), pinpointing the interplay of frontal (medial, middle, inferior, control stimulus was shown for 4 s. Each experimental stimulus was
and superior frontal gyri) and temporal areas (temporal pole, shown three times during the experiment, but never in sequence or in
hippocampus, and parahippocampal gyrus), temporoparietal junc- the same scanning run. Participants were asked to pay attention to the
tion, retrosplenial cortex, and posterior cingulate gyrus. Episodic photograph, so that they could successfully answer a subsequently
retrieval of stimuli learned in the laboratory has been linked to a presented question that pertained to the stimulus. After the 4-s
functional network that overlaps to some degree with the autobio- presentation of each picture, a question appeared on the screen with
graphical network, but that also recruits unique brain regions, for three possible answers.
instance, the insula, occipito-temporal cortex, posterior parietal areas, Participants had 10 s to respond by pressing 1, 2, or 3 on a number
and precuneus (e.g., Nyberg et al., 2002; for a review see Gilboa et al., pad. Note that accuracy of memory retrieval was emphasized over
2004). Semantic memory processes have been generally reflected in speed, and the participants were instructed not to guess. The response
the inter-correlation of activity in a number of temporal regions period was chosen to provide sufficient time for autobiographical
(inferior, middle, and superior temporal gyrus; Martin and Chao, memory retrieval. According to recent electrophysiological evidence,
2001; Vandenberghe et al., 1996), as well as in an overlap with the the range of retrieval times for autobiographical memory is between 3
autobiographical network, particularly in the lateral temporal gyrus, and 9 s, with an average time of 5 s (Conway et al., 2003). After the 10
temporoparietal junction, anterior cingulate gyrus, and ventrolateral second response period there was a 1-s inter-trial interval, followed
prefrontal cortex (Lee et al., 2002; Mummery et al., 1996). However, by the next trial. The three memory conditions were as follows:
despite these explorations of the functional and effective connectivity
1. Autobiographical condition, in which the stimulus was followed by
of the neural correlates that underlie declarative memory, no study to
a cue designed to elicit a personal memory (e.g., “Think of the last
date has attempted to identify a single network common to
time you went camping”). Participants were asked to relive the
autobiographical, episodic, and semantic retrieval in one experiment
memory as vividly as possible and subsequently rate the memory
and/or analysis.
according to its vividness (1 = “very vivid,” 2 = “somewhat vivid,”
The purpose of this study was to delineate a functional network of
3 = “not vivid at all”).
spatially distributed neural regions whose activity covaries across the
2. Episodic condition, in which the stimulus was followed by a
three memory conditions, i.e., to map a common functional
question about the photograph itself (e.g., “In the picture, which
declarative retrieval network. To do so, the data were analyzed
you have just seen, what is the colour of the tent?”). Participants
with seed voxel PLS analysis (Della-Maggiore et al., 2000; McIntosh,
chose from three answers presented to them (1 or 2 being correct,
1999; McIntosh et al., 1997; Schreurs et al., 1997) to determine
3 = I don't know). To ensure that this condition did not engage only
whole-brain patterns of activity correlated with seed regions that
working or short-term memory, we varied the degree of difficulty
showed similar increases of activity across the memory conditions
of retrieved contextual information; hence, the participants were
(Burianova and Grady, 2007). We hypothesized that autobiograph-
unaware of which piece of information about the stimulus they
ical, “laboratory” episodic, and semantic retrieval would recruit a
would be asked to retrieve. In addition, the presentation of the
large-scale functional network that underlies the general processes of
three experimental conditions was randomized and the temporal
memory retrieval, such as top-down attentional control, response
lag between subsequent presentations of the same visual stimulus
monitoring, integration of contextual information, working memory
was at least 14 min; hence, our intent was that participants would
manipulation of to-be-retrieved information, and processing of
need to engage episodic memory retrieval from long-term memory
semantic representations.
storage about the perceptual details of the stimuli, and not solely
working memory processes.
Methods
3. Semantic condition, in which the stimulus was followed by a
factual type of question (e.g., “Are there more than 100 camping
Participants
grounds in Algonquin Park?”). Responses were made in the same
fashion as in the episodic condition.
Twelve right-handed, healthy young participants (mean age = 27
years; range = 21–37 years; 3 males), with at least 16 years of In the control condition, the presentation of a scrambled
education, took part in the study. All participants signed an informed photograph was followed by an arbitrary instruction that was
consent that was approved by ethics boards at Baycrest and unrelated to the stimulus itself (e.g., “Press a key that corresponds
Sunnybrook Health Science Centre. to the letter ‘C’”). As in the experimental conditions, responses were
made by pressing 1, 2, or 3 on a keypad, and the correct key was either
Stimuli 1 or 2. Reaction times for each button press were recorded across all
conditions. These behavioural results have been reported elsewhere
Experimental stimuli (Burianova and Grady, 2007).
Fifty colour and black and white photographs depicting general, A post-scan interview was administered immediately after the
everyday events (e.g., driving or camping), as well as one-time but scan session. Participants viewed the 50 photographs again and were
highly publicized occurrences (e.g., the 9/11 attack on the World asked to describe the autobiographical memory that had been
Trade Center) were used as visual cues for the experimental retrieval retrieved during the scan in as much detail as possible. Temporal
conditions. and spatial information as well as the content of the event and
868 H. Burianova et al. / NeuroImage 49 (2010) 865–874

participant's emotion at the time of its occurrence were recorded by Table 1


the experimenter, to ensure that each autobiographical memory was Seed voxel regions.

accompanied by a vivid recollection of a particular episode. Region Hem BA Talairach coordinates Ratio

x y z
fMRI data acquisition
Caudate nucleus R n/a 12 15 −4 6.2
Lingual gyrus L 18 −4 −70 3 4.9
Anatomical and functional images were collected using a 3T GE Hippocampus L n/a −28 −20 −16 6.9
scanner with a standard head coil. A standard, high resolution, T1-
Abbreviations: Hem = hemisphere; BA = Brodmann's area; R = right; L = left;
weighted volumetric anatomical MRI (124 axial slices, 1.4 mm thick, Ratio = salience/SE ratio from the bootstrap analysis; x coordinate = right/left; y
FOV = 22 cm, acquisition matrix = 256 × 256 × 124, TR = 35 ms, coordinate = anterior/posterior; z coordinate = superior/inferior.
TE = 6 ms, flip angle = 35°) was acquired for each participant. Brain
activation was assessed using the blood oxygenation level-dependent
(BOLD) effect (Ogawa et al., 1990) with optimal contrast. For functional This analytical procedure for seed PLS was threefold: firstly, the
imaging, 26 axial slices of 5 mm thickness were obtained, utilizing a BOLD values from the selected seed(s) were extracted (i.e., from the
T2⁎-weighted pulse sequence with spiral in-out readout (TR = peak voxels identified in our previous study), across 8 timepoints after
2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, FOV = 20, acquisition matrix = 64 × 64 × 26, flip each presentation of the question cue to capture activity during the
angle = 80°). retrieval phase of the trial. The activity for each seed was averaged
Visual stimuli were presented using fMRI-compatible goggles across the peak and adjacent timepoints, and then this average
(Avotec, Inc.) mounted on the head coil. Responses were collected measure of seed activity was correlated with activity in all other brain
with the Rowland USB Response Box (RURB). voxels, across all participants, within each condition. Secondly, these
correlations were combined into a matrix and decomposed with
fMRI data preprocessing singular value decomposition (SVD), resulting in a set of latent
variables (LVs; mutually orthogonal variables). Each LV consists of a
Images were reconstructed and motion-corrected utilizing the singular image (i.e., “brain LV,” or the pattern of brain regions that
Analysis of Functional Neuroimages (AFNI; Cox, 1996). The images covary in activity with the seed voxel), a singular profile (i.e., “seed LV,”
were spatially co-registered to correct for head motion of the or the pattern of covariance of the seed voxel and the rest of the brain
participants by using a 3D Fourier transform interpolation. The peak across the experimental conditions), and a singular value (i.e., the
range of head motion did not exceed 1.2 mm across all participants. To amount of covariance accounted for by each LV). Finally, the
enable group comparisons, each brain scan was spatially normalized, significance for each LV is determined by using a permutation test
i.e., scaled and warped to match a standard template (the Montreal (McIntosh et al., 1996), which involves a random reordering of the
Neurological Institute [MNI] spiral template) utilizing Statistical data matrix and calculation of a new set of LVs for each reordering. The
Parametric Mapping (SPM99) software. The warping of the brain singular value of each newly permuted LV is compared to the singular
surface was achieved via a linear transformation with sinc interpo- value of the original LV, yielding a probability of the number of
lation (i.e., a signal resampling method designed to minimize aliasing occurrences that the permuted values exceed the original value. Five
in the signal). Lastly, the images were smoothed with a 6 mm hundred permutations were conducted. Because this study used an
Gaussian filter (in SPM), which, acting as a low pass filter, makes the event-related design, the PLS analysis provided a set of correlated
data less noisy by reducing the images' high-frequency components. regions (i.e., a map of areas correlated with the seeds) for each of the
The voxel size, after preprocessing, was 4 × 4 × 4 mm. eight TRs in the analysis. For each TR, a “brain score” was calculated for
each participant that is an index of how strongly that participant
Seed voxel PLS shows the particular pattern of brain activity identified for that TR. The
brain scores can be used to examine differences in brain activity across
In the seed PLS analysis, we included those trials for the semantic conditions, because greater activity in brain areas with positive (or
and episodic conditions for which participants made a correct negative) weights on a latent variable will yield positive (or negative)
response, and all “very vivid” and “somewhat vivid” trials for the mean scores for a given condition. We calculated the correlation
autobiographical condition. Seed PLS is a multivariate statistical between the brain scores from each significant LV and the seed BOLD
method utilized in the investigation of the relation of activity in a values to assess the relation between the whole-brain pattern and
selected brain region or regions (i.e., a seed voxel) and activity in the activity in the three reference regions. In addition to the permutation
rest of the brain across the task conditions (Della-Maggiore et al., test, a second and independent step is to determine the reliability of
2000; Schreurs et al., 1997; McIntosh, 1999; McIntosh et al., 1997). In the saliences (or weights) for the brain voxels characterizing each
other words, seed PLS analysis examines task-related functional pattern identified by the LVs. To do this, all saliences for each TR were
connectivity. The selection of the seed voxel(s) can be either data- submitted to a bootstrap estimation of the standard errors (Efron and
driven (i.e., determined by previous analyses of the data) or Tibshirani, 1985), randomly resampling participants, with replace-
hypothesis-driven (i.e., determined by theoretical assumptions), or ment, and computing the standard error of the saliences after 100
both. In our study, the selection of the seed voxels used in the seed PLS bootstrap samples. Peak voxels with a salience/SE ratio N3.0 were
analysis was data-driven. In a previous study (Burianova and Grady, considered to be reliable, as this approximates p b 0.005 (Sampson et
2007), we identified eight brain regions whose activity was al., 1989). Because PLS uses images in the format developed by the
significantly increased across all three memory conditions, thus Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI), all coordinates resulting from
showing activity common to the conditions. To determine whether the PLS analyses were converted from MNI space to Talairach
these regions are also a part of the same functional network, we coordinates using the algorithm developed by Brett and colleagues
entered all eight regions into a seed PLS analysis. The analysis revealed (www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/Imaging/Common/mnispance.shtml).
a connectivity pattern across the three memory conditions, but not all
regions were reliably correlated with this pattern. Therefore, a Correlation analysis
subsequent analysis was carried out, using the three seed regions
that covaried most strongly with the rest of the brain across the three The seed PLS, using the three seeds, resulted in a set of 18 regions
memory conditions—the left hippocampus, right caudate nucleus, and whose activity was reliably and strongly correlated with all three, and
left lingual gyrus (see Table 1 for the coordinates). this set of regions constituted the putative common network. To
H. Burianova et al. / NeuroImage 49 (2010) 865–874 869

Table 2 correlated with all three seed regions (the left hippocampus, left
Correlations with seed activity. lingual gyrus, and right caudate nucleus) during the retrieval of
Region Hem BA Talairach coordinates Ratio Cluster autobiographical, episodic, and semantic memories (see Fig. 1). Positive
correlations with the three seed regions were found in temporal cortex
x y z
(bilaterally in the middle temporal gyrus and superior temporal gyrus),
Positive correlations
frontal cortex (bilateral inferior frontal gyrus, medial frontal gyrus, and
Inferior FG L 47 −36 38 −9 6.8 50
R 47 48 23 −11 5.6 29 left anterior cingulate gyrus), as well as in the left temporoparietal
Medial FG L 10 −4 50 −6 6.9 25 junction and posterior cingulate gyrus. Negative correlations with the
Anterior CG L 24/32 −8 35 9 6.6 30 seed regions were found in the left middle occipital gyrus, right
Superior TG L 22 −55 −53 21 5.9 35 thalamus, bilateral precuneus, bilateral fusiform gyrus, and bilateral
Middle TG L 21 −48 −20 −2 6.5 74
R 21 48 −20 −2 7.7 83
supramarginal gyrus (see Table 2 for a summary). These patterns of
Temporal pole R 22 51 8 −4 7.1 70 correlation were found across all three memory conditions.
Inferior PL (TPJ) L 40 −63 −49 32 7.0 25
Posterior CG 23/31 0 −61 21 7.3 74 Correlation analysis
Negative correlations
Middle OG L 19 −40 −81 15 −5.4 29 The seed PLS analysis identified a set of regions correlated with the
Precuneus L 7 −8 −79 45 −7.2 44 seeds across all three memory conditions. To assess further whether
R 7 12 −83 45 −6.9 95 this set of regions represents a common memory retrieval network,
Thalamus R n/a 4 −15 15 −7.5 18 we determined if the correlations among all the nodes of this putative
Inferior TG L 37 −55 −51 −8 −6.5 52
Fusiform gyrus R 20 40 −28 −19 −8.6 80
network were statistically equivalent across the tasks, as one would
Inferior PL (SMG) L 40 −36 −52 39 −9.8 100 expect if the network operates in a similar fashion regardless of the
R 40 32 −53 36 −9.3 34 task demands. Fig. 2 shows three correlation matrices representing
Abbreviations: Hem = hemisphere; BA = Brodmann's area; R = right; L = left; the 3 seeds and the 18 regions that showed correlated activity with
Ratio = salience/SE ratio from the bootstrap analysis; Cluster = size of each cluster in the seeds, both positive and negative. Each matrix shows the visual
number of voxels; FG = frontal gyrus; CG = cingulate gyrus; TG = temporal gyrus; representation of interregional correlations of activity among the 21
OG = occipital gyrus; PL = parietal lobule; TPJ = temporoparietal junction; regions, within each memory condition. No significant differences
SMG = supramarginal gyrus; x coordinate = right/left; y coordinate = anterior/
posterior; z coordinate = superior/inferior.
(p N 0.05) were found in the overall interregional correlation strengths
across the three memory conditions. The correlation matrices indicate
that during all memory conditions, most of the regions that positively
specifically examine the intercorrelations among these regions, we correlated with the seeds were also correlated positively with one
extracted the BOLD signal from them, i.e., from the peak voxel in each another (denoted in lighter shades of gray), but negatively with
cluster (see Table 2 for peak coordinates), and averaged the activity at regions that show negative correlations with the seeds (denoted in
the timepoint corresponding to the strongest correlation with the darker shades). Similarly, most of the regions with negative seed
seeds and adjacent timepoints. We then calculated interregional correlations correlated positively with each other, but negatively with
correlations within conditions (creating three 21 × 21 correlation the regions with positive seed correlations. Despite some apparent
matrices) and compared these correlation patterns across conditions. interregional differences, there are obvious clusters of strong positive
We compared the correlation patterns across the conditions by and negative correlations among the voxels that are consistent across
calculating the squared differences in correlations (for each cell of the the three memory conditions. As a whole, the strength of the overall
matrix) between conditions and summing them to obtain a single interregional correlations did not differ across the conditions,
value for the overall difference in correlations between conditions. consistent with the idea of a common functional memory network.
The statistical significance of this difference was assessed using a
permutation test on the sum of squared differences (McIntosh et al., Discussion
1996), which involved a random reordering of the data matrix and
calculation of a new set of values for each reordering. The purpose of this study was to identify a functional network that
is common to autobiographical, episodic, and semantic retrieval. The
Results results of the seed PLS analysis yielded a large-scale network of brain
regions that were functionally connected to the left hippocampus,
Behavioural performance right caudate nucleus, and left lingual gyrus in all memory retrieval
conditions. The inferior frontal gyri, medial frontal gyrus, anterior and
Behavioural performance was assessed by comparing the means of posterior cingulate gyri, left temporoparietal junction, and a number
the response times across the four conditions (correct trials only), of temporal areas (bilateral middle temporal gyrus, superior temporal
using a repeated-measures ANOVA. The effect of condition was gyrus, and right temporal pole) positively correlated with the seed
significant, F(3,33) = 73.1, p b 0.001. Pairwise t-tests with Bonferroni regions, whereas the left middle occipital gyrus, bilateral precuneus,
corrections for multiple comparisons showed that the response times right thalamus, left inferior temporal gyrus, right fusiform gyrus, and
for autobiographical retrieval (M = 6989 ms, SD = 1478) differed bilateral inferior parietal lobule negatively correlated with the seed
significantly from that for the control task (M = 1925 ms, SD = 728) regions. These results suggest that these areas, as a whole, comprise a
and episodic retrieval (M = 4640 ms, SD = 1150, both at p b 0.01). The common functional network that subserves the general processes of
difference in reaction time (RT) for autobiographical retrieval and declarative memory retrieval. We propose that these processes
semantic retrieval (M = 5858 ms, SD = 1366) approached significance include a multitude of higher-cognitive functions, for instance,
(p = .06). working memory, selective attention, error monitoring, response
verification, and comprehension of concept representations. We
Seed PLS conclude from these results that, together, these cognitive processes
and their integration are essential to what is defined as ‘declarative
The first latent variable yielded by the three-seed PLS analysis memory retrieval.’
accounted for the largest amount of covariance in the data (32%; The three seed regions selected for the functional connectivity
p b 0.002), delineating a group of brain regions whose activity analysis have been previously identified as important for memory
870 H. Burianova et al. / NeuroImage 49 (2010) 865–874

Fig. 1. Seed PLS results: common functional network. (a) A pattern of correlated activity at 6–8 s after retrieval onset is shown. (b) A pattern of correlated activity at 10–12 s after
retrieval onset is shown. (c) Correlations of activity in the three seeds with the brain scores (summary measures of whole-brain activity in each subject per condition) are plotted to
show how activity in the seeds correlates with activity in the entire network. The error bars represent confidence intervals based on the bootstrap. (d) Activity in the caudate nucleus
at 2–3 s after retrieval onset is shown. Yellow/red = positive correlations with the seeds; blue = negative correlations with the seeds.

processing (e.g., Burianova and Grady, 2007). The hippocampus has involved in social functions and self-referential processes in both
been implicated as a critical component of declarative retrieval semantic and episodic memory retrieval (Addis et al., 2004a,b; Gilboa
processes (Cabeza et al., 2004; Eldridge et al., 2000; Prince et al., 2005) et al., 2004; Levine et al., 2004). The functional connectivity of these
and relational memory (e.g., memory for the relations between items frontal areas with the three seed regions suggests involvement of the
and context [e.g., Davachi and Wagner, 2002], or between objects and frontostriatal and hippocampo-frontal pathways. The frontrostriatal
features of a scene [e.g., Ryan and Cohen, 2004]). The hippocampus pathway has been implicated in abstract reasoning (Melrose et al.,
also may play a role in non-declarative processes, such as working 2007), working memory (Ashby et al., 2005; Gazzaley et al., 2004),
memory and object organization (Seeck et al., 1995) and non- and internal object representations (Kraut et al., 2002), as well as in
mnemonic processes, such as local feature extraction in form attentional control and cognitive planning and execution of a set-shift
perception (Barense et al., 2005; Beason-Held et al., 1998) and visual (Monchi et al., 2006), due to its predominant dopamine dependency
discrimination of scenes (Davies et al., 2004). The left lingual gyrus (Chudasama and Robbins, 2006) and dopamine–glutamate interac-
has been linked to working memory maintenance of visuospatial tions (Smith et al., 2004). The hippocampo-frontal pathway, involving
information (Ragland et al., 2002), imagery (Kosslyn et al., 1993; the left hippocampus, has been linked to the retrieval of contextual
Mazard et al., 2005), and crossmodal attention (Ferber et al., 2007). details (Svoboda et al., 2006), despite some researchers' arguing its
The caudate nucleus also was found to be involved in learning and sole involvement in autobiographical memory retrieval (e.g., Addis et
memory, particularly in regard to feedback processing and the ability al., 2007). The common prefrontal link of the frontostriatal and
to shift a cognitive set in response to the environment (Packard and hippocampo-frontal pathways allows for an indirect effect of the
Knowlton, 2002). caudate nucleus on the hippocampus and vice versa. Some research-
The three seed regions were found to positively covary with a ers speculate that the hippocampus and caudate nucleus functionally
number of frontal, temporal, and parietal areas. These areas of interact to jointly mediate behavioural output during performance
functional connectivity broadly overlap with the common memory- (Dagher et al., 2001; Packard and Knowlton, 2002). We propose that
related brain regions, which were found in our previous analysis the frontal nodes of the common retrieval network subserve top-
(Burianova and Grady, 2007), although the exact locations are not down attentional, inhibitory, and monitoring functions, as well as
identical. The inferior frontal gyrus has been implicated in declarative working memory manipulation and maintenance of information
and working memory retrieval (Nyberg et al., 2002), as well as in necessary for a successful declarative recollection of memories that
response inhibition and selection control (Aron et al., 2004; Brass et relate to the self and others.
al., 2005; Liddle et al., 2001), and top-down attentional control The middle and superior temporal gyri were involved in our
(Banich et al., 2000; Dove et al., 2008). The medial prefrontal areas are common network, and have been linked to the processing of personal
H. Burianova et al. / NeuroImage 49 (2010) 865–874 871

Fig. 2. Correlations among peak voxels of the common functional network. The three correlation matrices show similarities in regional interrelations across the three memory
conditions. Correlation values are indicated by shades of gray (positive correlations are denoted in lighter shades; negative correlations are denoted in darker shades). Values on the
vertical and horizontal axes correspond to the region numbers in the attached legend. The matrix is symmetrical about the main diagonal, which corresponds to perfect correlation
(+1) of each voxel with itself. STG = superior temporal gyrus; CN = caudate nucleus; IPL = inferior parietal lobule; IFG = inferior frontal gyrus; pCG = posterior cingulate gyrus;
aCG = anterior cingulate gyrus; LG = lingual gyrus; MTG = middle temporal gyrus; med FG = medial frontal gyrus; HIPP = hippocampus; MOG = middle occipital gyrus;
PCU = precuneus; TH = thalamus; ITG = inferior temporal gyrus.

and general semantic representations (e.g., Martin and Chao, 2001), critical in the encoding of personal memories (Nakamura and Kubota,
which are essential in the retrieval of all declarative memory. Martin 1996), multisensory processing of body and self (Arzy et al., 2006),
and Chao (2001) further note a temporo-frontal pathway, connecting retrieval of theory-of-mind information (i.e., when comprehending
the middle and superior temporal areas with the inferior prefrontal someone else's state of mind; Arzy et al., 2006; Moriguchi et al., 2006),
cortex, enabling the retrieval, monitoring, and manipulation of and conceptual knowledge (Graham et al., 2003). Our finding that
semantic representations. Levine et al. (1999) found impaired these temporal and parietal regions are nodes of the common
autonoetic processing (due, hypothetically, to deficits in self-regula- retrieval network suggests that these areas participate in the
tion) in patient ML, whose traumatic brain injury led to a processing of necessary semantic representations, as well as the
disconnection of the temporal and frontal lobes, in the right convergence of relevant semantic, perceptual, and others' state of
hemisphere. The functional relevance of the temporopolar area is mind information of which declarative memories are composed.
much less clear but its functional connectivity with the hippocampus, The anterior and posterior cingulate gyri are known to be
middle and superior temporal gyri, and the temporoparietal junction bidirectionally connected to one another via the cingulum bundle
suggests its involvement in multimodal perceptual analysis (Olson et (Goldman-Rakic, 1988; Vogt et al., 1979) and indirectly via the medial
al., 2007), as the temporal pole is speculated to be a convergence area, temporal lobes (Morris et al., 1999), with functional connections to
which integrates diverse streams of information into a unified whole the temporal, frontal, and parietal lobes. Thus, they are speculated to
(Markowitsch et al., 1985). Other researchers have suggested that be involved in a variety of cognitive and emotional processes (e.g.,
both the temporal pole and the left temporoparietal junction are Bush et al., 1998; Critchley et al., 2003), including self-referential
872 H. Burianova et al. / NeuroImage 49 (2010) 865–874

(Gusnard, 2005; Gusnard et al., 2001) and visuospatial processing semantic and self-referential information. Activity in this network
(e.g., Rosenbaum et al., 2004). Although the anterior part of the during memory retrieval may be facilitated by down-regulation of an
cingulate gyrus is often linked to uniquely autobiographical processes occipito-temporo-parietal network of posterior brain areas, which
(Svoboda et al., 2006), other researchers found it active in both supports attentional modulation in response to external stimuli.
episodic and semantic processes (e.g., Mummery et al., 1996). Johnson
et al. (2006) linked both the anterior and posterior cingulate gyri to Acknowledgments
self-reflection, albeit linking the anterior cingulate cortex to instru-
mental self-reflection (i.e., directed towards the inner self) and the We thank the staff at Sunnybrook Health Science Centre for their
posterior cingulate cortex to experiential self-reflection (i.e., directed assistance in this experiment. This work was funded by the Canadian
towards the self as it relates, externally, to others). The involvement of Institutes of Health Research (grant MOP 14036). C.L. Grady is also
the anterior and posterior cingulate gyri in the common retrieval supported by the Canada Research Chairs program, the Ontario
network emphasizes the critical role of intrinsically driven and self- Research Fund, and the Canadian Foundation for Innovation.
related processes in declarative memory search and retrieval. Thus, it
should come as no surprise that these regions overlap with the Appendix A. Supplementary data
‘default-mode’ regions whose activity decreases during demanding
external tasks, but increases during self-referential processing and Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in
monitoring of the internal environment (e.g., Gusnard, 2005). The the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.066.
anterior and posterior cingulate gyri are two frequently delineated
nodes of this self-referential default-mode network (e.g., Fox et al.,
References
2005; Greicius and Menon, 2004; Gusnard et al., 2001; Toro et al.,
2008). Addis, D.R., Moscovitch, M., Crawley, A.P., McAndrews, M.P., 2004a. Recollective
Finally, the connectivity analysis yielded a number of functionally qualities modulate hippocampal activation during autobiographical memory
retrieval. Hippocampus 14, 752–762.
connected brain areas that negatively correlated with the left Addis, D.R., McIntosh, A.R., Moscovitch, M., Crawley, A.P., McAndrews, M.P., 2004b.
hippocampus, lingual gyrus, and right caudate nucleus, yet positively Characterizing spatial and temporal features of autobiographical memory retrieval
correlated with one another. These areas included the left middle networks: a partial least squares approach. NeuroImage 23, 1460–1471.
Addis, D.R., Moscovitch, M., McAndrews, M.P., 2007. Consequences of hippocampal
occipital gyrus, bilateral precuneus, right thalamus, left inferior damage across the autobiographical memory network in left temporal lobe
temporal gyrus, right fusiform gyrus, and bilateral inferior parietal epilepsy. Brain 130, 2327–2342.
lobule. Given the similarity between these regions and attentional Aron, A.R., Robbins, T.W., Poldrack, R.A., 2004. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal
cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 170–177.
networks proposed by Corbetta et al. (2000, 2008), these negatively
Arzy, S., Overney, L.S., Landis, T., Blanke, O., 2006. Neural mechanisms of embodiment:
correlating regions are likely involved in attention to external stimuli. asomatognosia due to premotor cortex damage. Arch. Neurol. 63, 1022–1025.
In a recent study, Fu et al. (2005) found a network of posterior brain Ashby, F.G., Ell, S.W., Valentin, V.V., Casale, M.B., 2005. FROST: a distributed
neurocomputational model of working memory maintenance. J. Cogn. Neurosci.
areas, including the middle occipital gyrus, precuneus, fusiform gyrus,
17, 1728–1743.
and inferior parietal lobes, that was involved in externally driven early Baddley, A.D., 1984. Neuropsychological evidence and the semantic/episodic distinc-
visuospatial attentional modulation, as well as in later perceptual tion [commentary]. Behav. Brain Sci. 7, 238–239.
feedback to the primary visual areas. In the paradigm used here, the Banich, M.T., Milham, M.P., Atchley, R.A., Cohen, N.J., Webb, A., Wszalek, T., Kramer, A.F.,
Liang, Z., Barad, V., Gullett, D., Shah, C., Brown, C., 2000. Prefrontal regions play a
processing of memory search is internally driven and our analysis predominant role in imposing an attentional ‘set’: evidence from fMRI. Brain Res.
focused only on the period of retrieval, not on the period of cue Cogn. Brain Res. 10, 1–9.
presentation. Thus, when the common network is recruited for this Barense, M.D., Bussey, T.J., Lee, A.C.H., Rogers, T.T., Davies, R.R., Saksida, L.M., Murray, E.A.,
Graham, K.S., 2005. Functional specialization in the human medial temporal lobe.
internal process (i.e., an increase in activity), activity in the externally J. Neurosci. 25, 10239–10246.
driven occipito-temporo-parietal network is reduced, or maybe even Beason-Held, L.L., Purpura, K.P., van Meter, J.W., Azari, N.P., Mangot, D.J., Optican, L.M.,
suppressed (i.e., a decrease in activity). It is likely that the externally Mentis, M.J., et al., 1998. PET reveals occipitotemporal pathway activation during
elementary form perception in humans. Vis. Neurosci. 15, 503–510.
driven network would be more active if memory were more directly Brass, M., Derrfuss, J., Forstmann, B., von Cramon, D.Y., 2005. The role of the
cued by an external stimulus, consistent with the idea of Ciaramelli et inferior frontal junction area in cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Neurosciences 9,
al. (2008) that different parts of the parietal cortex mediate attention 314–316.
Braver, T.S., Barch, D.M., Kelley, W.M., Buckner, R.L., Cohen, N.J., Miezin, F.M., Snyder,
to retrieval cues in memory search. A.Z., Ollinger, J.M., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T.E., Petersen, S.E., 2001. Direct comparison
In conclusion, the results of this study yielded a large-scale of prefrontal cortex regions engaged by working and long-term memory tasks.
functional network of frontal, temporal, and parietal areas, as well as NeuroImage 14, 48–59.
Buckner, R.L., Koutstaal, W., Schacter, D.L., Wagner, A.D., Rosen, B.R., 1998. Functional–
the anterior and posterior cingulate gyri, in line with the unitary
anatomic study of episodic retrieval using fMRI. Retrieval effort versus retrieval
systems view that suggests a single declarative memory system success. NeuroImage 7, 151–162.
(Baddley, 1984; Friston, 2002; Kihlstrom, 1984; McIntosh, 1999; Rajah Burianova, H., Grady, C.L., 2007. Common and unique activations in autobiographical,
and McIntosh, 2005; Roediger, 1984). The functional connections episodic, and semantic retrieval. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1520–1534.
Bush, G., Whalen, P.J., Rosen, B.R., Jenike, M.A., McInerney, S.C., Rauch, S.L., 1998. The
within this network did not significantly differ across the memory counting Stroop: an interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging—
conditions, indicating its importance in all types of declarative validation study with functional MRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 6, 270–282.
retrieval. This neural overlap of autobiographical, “laboratory” Cabeza, R., Nyberg, L., 2000. Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and
fMRI studies. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 1–47.
episodic, and semantic retrieval suggests that all declarative memory Cabeza, R., St. Jacques, P., 2007. Functional neuroimaging of autobiographical memory.
retrieval engages a set of functional processes, mediated by the use of Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 219–227.
this common network, and provides further support for the notion of a Cabeza, R., Dolcos, F., Graham, R., Nyberg, L., 2002. Similarities and differences in the
neural correlates of episodic memory retrieval and working memory. NeuroImage
unitary memory system. One implication of this unitary system is that 16, 317–330.
no declarative memory retrieval is ever purely “semantic” or Cabeza, R., Prince, S.E., Daselaar, S.M., Greenberg, D.L., Budde, M., Dolcos, F., LaBar, K.S.,
“autobiographical,” for example, but necessarily involves overlapping Rubin, D.C., 2004. Brain activity during episodic retrieval of autobiographical and
laboratory events: an fMRI study using a novel photo paradigm. J. Cogn. Neurosci.
processes and multiple types of content, both contextual and context- 16, 1583–1594.
free. The regions that we found to be part of the common network Chudasama, Y., Robbins, T.W., 2006. Functions of frontostriatal systems in cognition:
suggest that some of these overlapping cognitive processes that are comparative neuropsychopharmacological studies in rats, monkeys, and humans.
Biol. Psychol. 73, 19–38.
involved in declarative memory are likely to include top-down
Ciaramelli, E., Grady, C., Moscovitch, M., 2008. Top-down and bottom-up attention to
attentional, inhibitory, and monitoring processes, working memory memory: a hypothesis (AtoM) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in
manipulation, maintenance of information, and convergence of memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia 46, 1828–1851.
H. Burianova et al. / NeuroImage 49 (2010) 865–874 873

Cipolotti, L., Maguire, E.A., 2003. A combined neuropsychological and neuroimaging study Graham, K.S., Lee, A.C.H., Brett, M., Patterson, K., 2003. The neural basis of
of topographical and non-verbal memory in semantic dementia. Neuropsychologia autobiographical and semantic memory: new evidence from three PET studies.
41, 1148–1159. Cognitive Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 3, 234–254.
Conway, M.A., Pleydell-Pearce, C.W., 2000. The construction of autobiographical Greicius, M.D., Menon, V., 2004. Default-mode activity during a passive sensory task:
memories in the self memory system. Psychol. Rev. 107, 261–288. uncoupled from deactivation but impacting activation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16,
Conway, M.A., Pleydell-Pearce, C.W., Whitecross, S., Sharpe, H., 2002. Brain imaging 1484–1492.
autobiographical memory. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. 41, 229–264. Gusnard, D.A., 2005. Being a self: considerations from functional imaging. Conscious-
Conway, M.A., Pleydell-Pearce, C.W., Whitecross, S.E., Sharpe, H., 2003. Neurophysio- ness and Cognition: An International Journal. Special Issue: The Brain and Its Self
logical correlates of memory for experienced and imagined events. Functional 14, 679–697.
neuroimaging of memory [Special issue]. Neuropsychologia 41, 334–340. Gusnard, D.A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G.L., Raichle, M.E., 2001. Medial prefrontal cortex
Corbetta, M., Kincade, J.M., Ollinger, J.M., McAvoy, M.P., Shulman, G.L., 2000. Voluntary and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function.
orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 98, 4259–4264.
Nat. Neurosci. 3, 292–297. Habib, R., McIntosh, A.R., Wheeler, M.A., Tulving, E., 2003. Memory encoding and
Corbetta, M., Patel, G., Shulman, G.L., 2008. The reorienting system of the human brain: hippocampally-based novelty/familiarity discrimination networks. Neuropsychologia
from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58, 306–324. 41, 271–279.
Cox, R.W., 1996. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic Hirano, M., Noguchi, K., 1998. Dissociation between specific personal episodes and
resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29, 162–173. other aspects of remote memory in a patient with hippocampal amnesia. Percept.
Critchley, H.D., Mathias, C.J., Josephs, O., O’Doherty, J., Zanini, S., Dewar, B.-K., Cipolotti, Mot. Skills 87, 99–107.
L., Shallice, T., Dolan, R.J., 2003. Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control: Hodges, J.R., Miller, B., 2001. The classification, genetics and neuropathology of
converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence. Brain: J. Neurol. 126, 2139–2152. frontotemporal dementia. Introduction to the special topic papers: part I.
Dagher, A., Owen, A.M., Boecker, H., Brooks, D.J., 2001. The role of the striatum and Neurocase 7, 31–35.
hippocampus in planning: a PET activation study in Parkinson's disease. Brain 124, Horwitz, B., Duara, R., Rapoport, S.I., 1984. Intercorrelations of glucose metabolic rates
1020–1032. between brain regions: application to healthy males in a state of reduced sensory
Davachi, L., Wagner, A.D., 2002. Hippocampal contributions to episodic encoding: input. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 4, 484–499.
insights from relational and item-based learning. J. Neurophysiol. 88, 982–990. Johnson, M.K., Raye, C.L., Mitchell, K.J., Touryan, S.R., Greene, E.J., Nolen-Hoeksema, S.,
Davies, R.R., Graham, K.S., Xuereb, J.H., Williams, G.B., Hodges, J.R., 2004. The human 2006. Dissociating medial frontal and posterior cingulate activity during self-
perirhinal cortex and semantic memory. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20, 2441–2446. reflection. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 1, 56–64.
Della-Maggiore, V., Sekuler, A.B., Grady, C.L., Bennett, P.J., Sekuler, R., McIntosh, A.R., Kihlstrom, J.F., 1984. A fact is a fact is a fact [commentary]. Behav. Brain Sci. 7, 243–244.
2000. Corticolimbic interactions associated with performance on a short-term Kopelman, M.D., Kapur, N., 2001. The loss of episodic memories in retrograde amnesia:
memory task are modified by age. J. Neurosci. 20, 8410–8416. single-case and group studies. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B biol. Sci. 356, 1409–1421.
Dobbins, I.G., Wagner, A.D., 2005. Domain-general and domain-sensitive prefrontal Kosslyn, S.M., Alpert, N.M., Thomson, W.L., Maljkovic, V., Weise, S.B., Chabris, C.F., et al.,
mechanisms for recollecting events and detecting novelty. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1993. Visual mental imagery activates topographically organized visual cortex: PET
1768–1778. investigations. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 263–287.
Dove, A., Manly, T., Epstein, R., Owen, A.M., 2008. The engagement of mid-ventrolateral Kraut, M.A., Kremen, S., Moo, L.R., Segal, J.B., Calhoun, V., Hart, J.Jr, 2002. Object
prefrontal cortex and posterior brain regions in intentional cognitive activity. Hum. activation in semantic memory from visual multimodal feature input. J. Cogn.
Brain Mapp. 29, 107–119. Neurosci. 14, 37–47.
Duncan, J., Owen, A.M., 2000. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by Lee, A.C., Graham, K.S., Simons, J.S., Hodges, J.R., Owen, A.M., Patterson, K., 2002.
diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 23, 475–483. Regional brain activations differ for semantic features but not categories.
Duzel, E., Cabeza, R., Picton, T.W., Yonelidas, A.P., Heinze, H.J., Scheich, H., Tulving, E., NeuroReport 13, 1497–1501.
1999. Task-related and item-related processes in episodic and semantic retrieval: a Levine, B., Freedman, M., Dawson, D., Black, S., Stuss, D.T., 1999. Ventral frontal
combined PET and ERP study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 96, 1794–1799. contribution to self-regulation: convergence of episodic memory and inhibition.
Duzel, E., Habib, R., Guderian, S., Heinze, H.J., 2004. Four types of novelty–familiarity Neurocase 5, 263–275.
responses in associative recognition memory of humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 1408–1416. Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J., Winocur, G., Moscovitch, M., 2002. Aging and
Efron, B., Tibshirani, R., 1985. The bootstrap method for assessing statistical accuracy. autobiographical memory: dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychol.
Behaviormetrika 17, 1–35. Aging 17, 677–689.
Eldridge, L.L., Knowlton, B.J., Furmanski, C.S., Bookheimer, S.Y., Engel, S.A., 2000. Levine, B., Turner, G.R., Tisserand, D., Hevenor, S.J., Graham, S.J., McIntosh, A.R., 2004.
Remembering episodes: a selective role for the hippocampus during retrieval. Nat. The functional neuroanatomy of episodic and semantic autobiographical remem-
Neurosci. 3, 1149–1152. bering: a prospective functional MRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1633–1646.
Ferber, S., Mraz, R., Baker, N., Graham, S.J., 2007. Shared and differential neural Liddle, P.F., Kiehl, K.A., Smith, A.M., 2001. Event-related fMRI study of response
substrates of copying versus drawing: a functional magnetic resonance imaging inhibition. Hum. Brain Mapp. 12, 100–109.
study. NeuroReport: Rapid Commun. Neuroscience Res. 18, 1089–1093. Maguire, E.A., 2001. Neuroimaging studies of autobiographical event memory. The
Finger, S., 1994. Origins of neuroscience: a history of explorations into brain function. Royal Society: Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences 356, 1441–1451.
Oxford University Press, New York. Maguire, E.A., Vargha-Khadem, F., Mishkin, M., 2001. The effect of bilateral
Fox, M.D., Snyder, A.Z., Vincent, J.L., Corbetta, M., van Essen, D.C., Raichle, M.E., 2005. hippocampal damage on fMRI regional activations and interactions during memory
The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional retrieval. Brain 124, 1156–1170.
networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 102, 9673–9678. Manns, J.R., Hopkins, R.O., Squire, L.R., 2003. Semantic memory and the human
Friston, K.J., 1994. Functional and effective connectivity: a synthesis. Hum. Brain Mapp. hippocampus. Neuron 38, 127–133.
2, 56–78. Markowitsch, H.J., Emmans, D., Irle, E., Streicher, M., Preilowski, B., 1985. Cortical and
Friston, K.J., 1997. Imaging cognitive anatomy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1, 21–27. subcortical afferent connections of the primate's temporal pole: a study of rhesus
Friston, K., 2002. Beyond phrenology: what can neuroimaging tell us about distributed monkeys, squirrel monkeys, and marmosets. J. Comp. Neurol. 242, 425–458.
circuitry? Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 25, 221–250. Martin, A., Chao, L.L., 2001. Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes.
Friston, K.J., Frith, C.D., Liddle, P.F., Frackowiak, R.S.J., 1993. Functional connectivity: the Current Opinions in Neurobiology 11, 194–201.
principal component analysis of large (PET) data sets. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. Mazard, A., Laou, L., Joliot, M., Mellet, E., 2005. Neural impact of the semantic content of
13, 5–14. visual mental images and visual percepts. Cogn. Brain Res. 24, 423–435.
Fu, S., Greenwood, P.M., Parasuraman, R., 2005. Brain mechanisms of involuntary visuospatial McIntosh, A.R., 1998. Understanding neural interactions in learning and memory using
attention: an event-related potential study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 378–390. functional neuroimaging. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 855, 556–571.
Gabrieli, J.D.E., 1998. Cognitive neuroscience of human memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 49, McIntosh, A.R., 1999. Mapping cognition to the brain through neural interactions.
87–115. Memory 7, 523–548.
Gadian, D.G., Aicardi, J., Watkins, K.E., Porter, D.A., Mishkin, M., Vargha-Khadem, F., McIntosh, A.R., 2000. Towards a network theory of cognition. Neural Netw. 13,
2000. Developmental amnesia associated with early hypoxic–ischaemic injury. 861–870.
Brain: J. Neurol. 123, 499–507. McIntosh, A.R., Gonzalez-Lima, F., 1994. Structural equation modeling and its application
Gazzaley, A., Rissman, J., D’Esposito, M., 2004. Functional connectivity during working to network analysis in functional brain imaging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 2–22.
memory maintenance. Cognitive Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 4, 580–599. McIntosh, A.R., Grady, C.L., Ungerleider, L.G., Haxby, J.V., Rapoport, S.I., Horwitz, B.,
Gilboa, A., 2004. Autobiographical and episodic memory—one and the same? Evidence 1994. Network analysis of cortical visual pathways mapped with PET. J. Neurosci.
from prefrontal activation in neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia 42, 14, 655–666.
1336–1349. McIntosh, A.R., Grady, C.L., Haxby, J.V., Ungerleider, L.G., Horwitz, B., 1996. Changes in
Gilboa, A., Winocur, G., Grady, C.L, Hevenor, S.J., Moscovitch, M., 2004. Remembering limbic and prefrontal functional interactions in a working memory task for faces.
our past: functional neuroanatomy of recollection of recent and very remote Cereb. Cortex 6, 571–584.
personal events. Cereb. Cortex 14, 1214–1225. McIntosh, A.R., Nyberg, L., Bookstein, F.L., Tulving, E., 1997. Differential functional
Goldberg, R.F., Perfetti, C.A., Fiez, J.A., Schneider, W., 2007. Selective retrieval of abstract connectivity of prefrontal and medial temporal cortices during episodic memory
semantic knowledge in left prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 27, 3790–3798. retrieval. Hum. Brain Mapp. 5, 323–327.
Goldman-Rakic, P.S., 1988. Topography of cognition: parallel distributed networks in McKinnon, M.C., Black, S.E., Miller, B., Moscovitch, M., Levine, B., 2006. Autobiographical
primate association cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 137–156. memory in semantic dementia: implications for theories of limbic-neocortical
Grady, C.L., McIntosh, A.R., Bookstein, F., Horowitz, B., Rapoport, S.I., Haxby, J.V., 1998. interaction in remote memory. Neuropsychologia 44, 2421–2429.
Age-related changes in regional cerebral blood flow during working memory for Melrose, R.J., Poulin, R.M., Stern, C.E., 2007. An fMRI investigation of the role of the basal
faces. NeuroImage 8, 409–425. ganglia in reasoning. Brain Res. 1142, 146–158.
874 H. Burianova et al. / NeuroImage 49 (2010) 865–874

Mesulam, M.M., 1990. Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing Ryan, L., Cox, C., Hayes, S.M., Nadel, L., 2008. Hippocampal activation during episodic
for attention, language, and memory. Ann. Neurol. 28, 597–613. and semantic memory retrieval: comparing category production and category cued
Monchi, O., Petrides, M., Strafella, A.P., Worsley, K.J., Doyon, J., 2006. Functional role of recall. Neuropsychologia 46, 2109–2121.
the basal ganglia in the planning and execution of actions. Ann. Neurol. 59, Sampson, P.D., Streissguth, A.P., Barr, H.M., Bookstein, F.L., 1989. Neurobehavioral
257–264. effects of prenatal alcohol. II. Partial least squares analysis. Neurotoxicol. Teratol.
Moriguchi, Y., Ohnishi, T., Lane, R.D., Maeda, M., Mori, T., Nemoto, K., Matsuda, H., 11, 477–491.
Komaki, G., 2006. Impaired self-awareness and theory of mind: an fMRI study of Schreurs, B., McIntosh, A.R., Bahron, M., Herscovitch, P., Sunderland, T., Molchan, S., 1997.
mentalizing in alexithymia. NeuroImage 32, 1472–1482. Lateralization and behavioural correlation of changes in regional cerebral blood flow
Morris, J.S., Scott, S.K., Dolan, R.J., 1999. Saying it with feeling: neural responses to with classical conditioning of the human eyeblink response. J. Neurophysiol. 77,
emotional vocalizations. Neuropsychologia 37, 1155–1163. 2153–2163.
Moscovitch, M., 1992. Memory and working-with-memory: a component process Seeck, M., Schomer, D., Mainwaring, N., Ives, J., Dubuisson, D., Blume, H., Cosgrove, R.,
model based on modules and central systems. J. Cogn. Neurosci. Special Issue: Ransil, B.J., Mesulam, M.-M., 1995. Selectively distributed processing of visual
Memory Systems 4, 257–267. object recognition in the temporal and frontal lobes of the human brain. Ann.
Mummery, C.J., Patterson, K., Hodges, J.R., Wise, R.J., 1996. Generating ‘tiger’ as an Neurol. 37, 538–545.
animal name or a word beginning with T: differences in brain activation. Proc. R. Seger, C.A., Desmond, J.E., Glover, G.H., Gabrieli, J.D.E., 2000. Functional magnetic
Soc. Lond., B Biol. Sci. 263, 1755–1756. resonance imaging evidence for right-hemisphere involvement in processing
Nakamura, K., Kubota, K., 1996. The primate temporal pole: its putative role in object unusual semantic relationships. Neuropsychology 14, 361–369.
recognition and memory. Behav. Brain Res. 77, 53–77. Shimamura, A., 1995. Memory and frontal lobe function. In: Gazzaniga, M. (Ed.), The
Neary, D., Snowden, J.S., Gustafson, L., Passant, U., Stuss, D., Black, S., Freedman, M., cognitive neurosciences. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Kertesz, A., et al., 1998. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical Smith, Y., Raju, D.V., Pare, J.-F., Sidibe, M., 2004. The thalamostriatal system: a highly
diagnostic criteria. Neurology 51, 1546–1554. specific network of the basal ganglia circuitry. Trends Neurosci. 27, 520–527.
Nolde, S.F., Johnson, M.K., D’Esposito, M., 1998. Left prefrontal activation during Snowden, J., Griffiths, H., Neary, D., 1994. Semantic dementia: autobiographical
episodic remembering: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroreport:An International contribution to preservation of meaning. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 11, 265–288.
Journal for the Rapid Communication of Research in Neuroscience 9, 3509–3514. Squire, L.R., Zola, S.M., 1998. Episodic memory, semantic memory, and amnesia.
Nyberg, L., McIntosh, A.R., Tulving, E., 1998. Functional brain imaging of episodic and Hippocampus 8, 205–211.
semantic memory with positron emission tomography. J. Mol. Medicine 76, 48–53. Svoboda, E., McKinnon, M.C., Levine, B., 2006. The functional neuroanatomy of
Nyberg, L., Forkstam, C., Petersson, K.M., Cabeza, R., Ingvar, M., 2002. Brain imaging of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 44, 2189–2208.
human memory systems: between-systems similarities and within-systems Toro, R., Fox, P.T. & Paus, T. (2008). Functional coactivation map of the human brain.
differences. Cogn. Brain Res. 13, 281–292. Cereb. Cortex, advance access, published Feb 21st, 2008.
Nyberg, L., Marklund, P., Persson, J., Cabeza, R., Forkstam, C., Petersson, K.M., Ingvar, M., Tulving, E., 1972. Episodic and semantic memory. In: Tulving, E., Donaldson, W. (Eds.),
2003. Common prefrontal activations during working memory, episodic memory, Organization of memory. Academic Press, Oxford, England.
and semantic memory. Neuropsychologia 41, 371–377. Tulving, E., 1987. Multiple memory systems and consciousness. Hum. Neurobiol. 6,
Ogawa, S., Lee, T.M., Kay, A.R., Tank, D.W., 1990. Brain magnetic-resonance-imaging 67–80.
with contrast dependent on blood oxygenation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 87, Tulving, E., Markowitsch, H.J., 1998. Episodic and declarative memory: role of the
9868–9872. hippocampus. Hippocampus 8, 198–204.
Olson, I.R., Plotzker, A., Ezzyat, Y., 2007. The enigmatic temporal pole: a review of Tulving, E., Schacter, D.L., 1990. Priming and human-memory systems. Science 247,
findings on social and emotional processing. Brain: J. Neurol. 130, 1718–1731. 301–306.
Packard, M.G., Knowlton, B.J., 2002. Learning and memory functions of the basal Tulving, E., Hayman, C.A., MacDonald, C.A., 1991. Long-lasting perceptual priming and
ganglia. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 25, 563–593. semantic learning in amnesia: a case experiment. J. Exper. Psychol., Learn., Mem.,
Prince, S.E., Daselaar, S.M., Cabeza, R., 2005. Neural correlates of relational memory: Cogn. 17, 595–617.
successful encoding and retrieval of semantic and perceptual associations. J. Neurosci. Vandenberghe, R., Price, C., Wise, R., Josephs, O., Frackowiak, R.S.J., 1996. Functional
25, 1203–1210. anatomy of a common semantic system for words and pictures. Nature 383, 254–256.
Ragland, J.D., Turetsky, B.I., Gur, R.C., Gunning-Dixon, F., Turner, T., Schroeder, L., Chan, Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D.G., Watkins, K.E., Connelly, A., Van Paesschen, W.,
R., Gur, R.E., 2002. Working memory for complex figures: an fMRI comparison of Mishkin, M., 1997. Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic
letter and fractal n-back tasks. Neuropsychology 16, 370–379. and semantic memory. Science 277, 376–380.
Rajah, M.N., McIntosh, A.R., 2005. Overlap in the functional neural systems involved in Vogt, B.A., Rosene, D.L., Pandya, D.N., 1979. Thalamic and cortical afferents differentiate
semantic and episodic memory retrieval. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 470–482. anterior from posterior cingulate cortex in the monkey. Science 204, 205–207.
Roediger, H.L., 1984. Does current evidence from dissociation experiments favor the Westmacott, R., Moscovitch, M., 2003. The contribution of autobiographical significance
episodic/semantic distinction? [commentary]. Behav. Brain Sci. 7, 252–254. to semantic memory. Mem. Cogn. 31, 761–774.
Rosenbaum, R.S., Ziegler, M., Winocur, G., Grady, C.L., Moscovitch, M., 2004. “I have often Westmacott, R., Black, S.E., Freedman, M., Moscovitch, M., 2004. The contribution of
walked down this street before”: FMRI studies on the hippocampus and other structures autobiographical significance to semantic memory: evidence from Alzheimer’s
during mental navigation of an old environment. Hippocampus 14, 826–835. disease, semantic dementia, and amnesia. Neuropsychologia 42, 25–48.
Ryan, J.D., Cohen, N.J., 2004. Processing and short-term retention of relational Wiggs, C.L., Weisberg, J., Martin, A., 1999. Neural correlates of semantic and episodic
information in amnesia. Neuropsychologia 42, 497–511. memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia 37, 103–118.

You might also like