Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

Appetite 58 (2012) 847–851

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Appetite
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/appet

Research report

Dealing with problematic eating behaviour. The effects of a mindfulness-based


intervention on eating behaviour, food cravings, dichotomous thinking and
body image concern
H.J.E.M. Alberts ⇑, R. Thewissen, L. Raes
Maastricht University, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Clinical and Psychological Science, P.O. Box 616, 6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study explored the efficacy of a mindfulness-based intervention for problematic eating behavior. A
Received 27 September 2011 non-clinical sample of 26 women with disordered eating behavior was randomly assigned to an 8-week
Received in revised form 28 November 2011 MBCT-based eating intervention or a waiting list control group. Data were collected at baseline and after
Accepted 5 January 2012
8 weeks. Compared to controls, participants in the mindfulness intervention showed significantly greater
Available online 10 January 2012
decreases in food cravings, dichotomous thinking, body image concern, emotional eating and external
eating. These findings suggest that mindfulness practice can be an effective way to reduce factors that
Keywords:
are associated with problematic eating behaviour.
Mindfulness
Food craving
Ó 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Dichotomous thinking
Body dissatisfaction
External eating

Introduction Eating behaviour

Mindfulness is the practice of focusing attention on the expe- Three dissimilar styles of eating behavior have been identified:
rience in the present moment in an accepting manner, without restrained, emotional and external eating (Van Strien, Frijters,
judgment or attachment to the way this experience should or Bergers, & Defares, 1986). Restrained eating involves restriction
should not be (Kabat-Zinn, 1990). Today, only a relatively small of food intake or dieting. Dieting has been found to play a role
number of studies have addressed the effectiveness of mindful- in the development of eating disorders (Stice, 1998) and pro-
ness in the domain of eating behavior. So far, the findings are motes unhealthy cycles of weight loss and gain (Lissner, Andres,
promising and suggest an inverse relationship between mindful- Muller, & Shimokata, 1990). Restrained eating can be driven by
ness and disordered eating behavior. Mindfulness practice has appearance related evaluative processes and cognitions, such as
been found to reduce BMI in overweight individuals (Tapper judgment of the self in terms of shape and weight (Spangler,
et al., 2009), decrease food cravings (Alberts, Mulkens, Smeets, 2002). Mindfulness cultivates acceptance and aims to reduce
& Thewissen, 2010) and reduce binge eating (Kristeller & Hallett, the impact of (self-related) judgmental processes by enhancing
1999). Moreover, high levels of mindfulness have been found to dis-identification from these judgments. Consequently, mindful-
be negatively associated with disordered eating-related cogni- ness is likely to reduce restrained eating that is driven by
tions (Masuda & Wendell, 2010). The goal of the present study negative self-evaluative processes.
was to extend this line of research and address the efficacy of a External eating is eating in response to external cues, not
mindfulness-based intervention on different important correlates considering internal states of hunger and satiety. Individuals who
of disordered eating behavior. More specifically, we explored the often engage in external eating are more likely to snack in stressful
impact of an 8 week mindfulness-based intervention on BMI, situations (Conner, Fitter, & Fletcher, 1999) and have feelings of
eating behavior, food cravings, dichotomous thinking and body low self-worth (Braet & Van Strien, 1997). To a large extent, mind-
image concern. fulness based practice includes exercises, such as the bodyscan,
that direct attention inward, to the experience of thoughts, feelings
and body related sensations (Kristeller & Hallett, 1999). In this
way, attention for internal cues is strengthened, which may atten-
⇑ Corresponding author. uate guidance of (eating) behaviour by external cues and thus
E-mail address: h.alberts@maastrichtuniversity.nl (H.J.E.M. Alberts). reduce external eating.

0195-6663/$ - see front matter Ó 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.appet.2012.01.009
848 H.J.E.M. Alberts et al. / Appetite 58 (2012) 847–851

Emotional eating has been defined as eating in response to neg- Food cravings
ative emotions (Van Strien et al., 1986) and has been identified as
an essential aspect of binge eating (e.g., Arnow, Kenardy, & Agras, Food cravings are defined as an intense desire or urge to eat
1995). Whereas emotional eating can be perceived as an escape specific food (Weingarten & Elston, 1991). Positive correlations
from experiencing negative emotions (avoidance coping; Cochra- have been observed between food cravings and the development
ne, Brewerton, Wilson, & Hodges, 1992), mindfulness promotes of obesity (Schlundt, Virts, Sbrocco, & Pope-Cordle, 1993) and eat-
willingness to approach and experience emotions, and is therefore ing disorders (Mitchell, Hatsukami, Eckert, & Pyle, 1985). Recent
likely to reduce avoidance based coping, such as emotional eating. findings suggest that mindfulness-based coping is effective in
reducing cravings. In a study by Alberts et al. (2010) it was found
Body image concern that overweight and obese participants who received a 7-week
mindfulness-based intervention reported significant reductions in
A negative perception of one’s physical appearance (body im- food cravings compared to control participants. Although prelimin-
age; Fisher, 1990) has been identified as an important factor con- ary, these findings imply that mindfulness can help to reduce crav-
tributing to vulnerability to, and maintenance of disordered ing for food.
eating behaviour (Cooley & Toray, 2001). Factors that have been
suggested to contribute to the development and maintenance of Method
body dissatisfaction include appearance ideals (Thompson, Hein-
berg, Altabe, & Tantleff Dunn, 1999), body checking and body Participants
avoidance (Shafran, Fairburn, Robinson, & Lask, 2004). Placing high
value on appearance ideals, such as the thin body ideal, can raise Patients were recruited through a newspaper advertisement
body dissatisfaction by increasing the awareness of the discrep- and flyers soliciting for individuals with problematic eating behav-
ancy between one’s current and ideal body. Body avoidance in- ior. The inclusion criterions were that participants were (1) be-
volves behavior that aims to prevent or avoid situations that tween 18 and 65 years and (2) experienced one or more of the
trigger concern about one’s physical appearance. Examples are following types of problematic eating: emotional eating, stress
wearing baggy clothes, not weighing or avoiding mirrors. Body related eating, eating without awareness and/or overeating. Exclu-
avoidance may prevent disconfirmation of irrational ideas about sion criteria were: (1) eating disorder (bulimia nervosa or anorexia
one’s body (Rosen, Srebnik, Saltzberg, & Wendt, 1991). In contrast, nervosa), (2) suicidality, (3) substance abuse and/or dependence,
body checking refers to a critical examination of one’s body, like for (4) severe mental disorder, and (5) other concurrent treatment. A
instance checking oneself repeatedly in the mirror or negatively total of 26 women (mean age = 48.5 years, SD = 7.90), participated
comparing oneself to others. in this study. The mean weight of the participants was 94.6 kg
Mindfulness is in sharp contrast with the above described pro- (SD = 16.41; range 68.0–123.0) and the mean body mass index
cesses and behavior that are proposed to increase and maintain (BMI) was 32.7 (SD = 6.1; range 23.5–45.8).
body image concern. First, mindfulness is not primarily focused
on reaching a goal or ideal state, but fosters willingness to accept
Design
the present state. In other words, instead of attempting to reach
an appearance ideal, mindfulness promotes acceptance of the cur-
After diagnostic evaluation and intake assessment, participants
rent appearance, despite social pressures to do otherwise. Second,
were randomly assigned to the treatment group (n = 12) or wait-
mindfulness draws on the ability to stay in contact with an expe-
ing-list control group (n = 14). The waiting-list period lasted for
rience. Thus, in contrast to body avoidance, mindfulness requires
the duration of the treatment period (8 weeks), and the control
willingness to expose oneself to whatever arises. Importantly,
group entered active treatment after 10 weeks. Measures for both
mindfulness cultivates compassion and attention without judg-
groups were collected at baseline and at post-treatment.
ment. This is opposite to the principle of body checking, which is
a strongly judgmental and self-critical evaluative process. Follow-
Measures
ing this line of reasoning, increased levels of mindfulness are ex-
pected to be associated with less body image concern.
Weight
Weight (kg) was recorded at pre- and post-test. Participants
Dichotomous thinking
were weighed in street clothes, without shoes.
Dichotomous thinking entails a type of cognitive rigidity in
which reality is perceived in terms of polarities (e.g. food is either Kentucky Inventory Mindfulness Skills Extended (KIMS-E)
‘‘good’’ or ‘‘bad’’). This thinking style has been identified as an In order to test whether the current intervention successfully
important factor contributing to the maintenance of eating disor- increased levels of mindfulness, the KIMS-E (Baer, Smith, & Allen,
ders (Fairburn, Cooper, & Shafran, 2003). Dichotomous thinking en- 2004) was administered. This is a 46-item scale that measures
hances obsessive processing by stimulating feelings of guilt after mindfulness skills or sub skills. The scale consists of five subscales;
consumption of ‘‘forbidden’’ food (Dewberry & Ussher, 2001) and observe, describe, act with awareness, act without judgement and
by increasing the attractiveness of forbidden food (Mann & Ward, non-reactivity to inner experience (Crohnbach’s alpha = .94).
2001).
A core component of mindfulness is non-judgmental observa- Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEB-Q)
tion of internal and external stimuli. Instead of labeling reality in The DEB-Q (Van Strien et al., 1986) consists of 33 items and
dichotomous terms such as ‘‘good’’ or ‘‘bad’’, mindfulness promotes assesses external, restraint and emotional eating (Cronbach’s
willingness to accept and let things be just as they are the moment alpha = .85).
we become aware of them. Mindfulness practice can help to in-
crease awareness of critical and judgmental thoughts, without get- Body Shape Questionnaire (BSQ)
ting involved in these thoughts. This process of dis-identification The BSQ was originally developed by Cooper, Taylor, Cooper,
allows one to gain distance from evaluative thoughts and is there- and Fairburn (1987) to measure concern about body weight and
fore likely to decrease dichotomous thinking. shape experienced by individuals with eating disorders or related
H.J.E.M. Alberts et al. / Appetite 58 (2012) 847–851 849

body image problems. We used the shortened 16 item version meetings. On average, participants indicated that they spend
(Evans & Dolan, 1993) (Cronbach’s alpha = .91). 29.38 min (SD = 17.41; range 10–60) per day on the exercises.

The Dichotomous Thinking Scale (DTS) Analyses


The DTS (Byrne, Cooper, & Fairburn, 2004) is a 16-item scale
that measures the extent to which individuals engage in dichoto- All of the following analyses were performed using repeated-
mous thinking. The scale consists of two sections; six items relate measures ANOVA with measurement time as a within subjects fac-
specifically to food, dieting and weight, and 10 items concern more tor (two levels; pre-test and post-test) and condition as a between
general forms of dichotomous thinking (Cronbach’s alpha = .90). subjects factor. In order to address within group comparisons,
paired samples t-tests were employed. All means are summarized
General Food Craving Questionnaire Trait (G-FCQ-T) in Table 1.
The G-FCQ-T is a reliable and valid 21-item self-report measure
of a general ‘desire for food’ or ‘desire to eat’ (Nijs, Franken, & BMI
Muris, 2007) consisting of the following four subscales (1) preoccu-
pation with food, (2) loss of control, (3) positive outcome expec- For BMI, only a significant main effect of measurement time
tancy, and (4) emotional craving (Cronbach’s alpha = .94). emerged, F(1, 24) = 8.65, p < .01, g2 = .27, indicating a decrease of
BMI at post-test, in general. Within group comparisons showed a
Participation check significant reduction in BMI for participants in the control condi-
After the intervention, participants in the experimental condi- tion, t(13) = 2.22, p < .05, d = .04 and a marginally significant de-
tion were asked to indicate how much time on average they spent crease for those in the experimental condition, t(12) = 2.0, p = .07,
per day on the exercises. In addition, session attendance was reg- d = .06.
istered by the trainer.
Mindfulness
Procedure
Scores on the KIMS-E were found to significantly interact with
All participants completed the questionnaires before onset of condition, F(1, 24) = 4.90, p = .04, g2 = .17. Participants in the con-
the intervention period and directly after the intervention period trol condition reported a non-significant increase in mindfulness,
at home using a website. They were told that the completion of t(13) = 1.93, p = .08, d = .19, while individuals who participated in
the questionnaires would be anonymous. For participants in the the training reported a significant increase in mindfulness,
treatment condition, height and weight (kg) were recorded at the t(12) = 3.31, p < .01, d = 1.15.
beginning of the first session and at the beginning of the last ses-
sion. Weight of participants in the control condition was measured Eating behaviour
at the same time intervals as the treatment condition, but at a dif-
ferent location. Restrained eating
Only a marginally significant main effect was observed for the
restrained eating subscale of the DEB-Q, F(1, 24) = 3.80, p = .06,
Treatment
g2 = .14, suggesting that both the experimental and control group
reported an increase in restrained eating. No significant interaction
A specially designed mindfulness-based eating program was
effect for this subscale was found, F(1, 24) = .21, p = .65.
delivered by the second author based on the MBCT protocol
developed by Segal, Williams, and Teasdale (2002). While largely
maintaining the overall structure and practices of the original External eating
MBCT-protocol, some adjustments were made in order to increase For the external eating subscale, a significant interaction effect
it’s relevance for eating behavior. The intervention consisted of five was found, F(1, 24) = 4.80, p = .04, g2 = .17. Within group compari-
core components: (1) mindful eating (awareness of sensations such sons revealed no significant difference in external eating score
as taste), (2) awareness of physical sensations (hunger, satiety, between the pre- and post-measurement of the control group,
craving and stress), (3) awareness of thoughts and feelings related t(13) < .001. In contrast, participants in the treatment condition
to eating (e.g., inner self-talk, beliefs, judgments, expectations, reported a significantly lower amount of external eating at the
(diet)-rules, fear, sadness, shame and guilt), (4) acceptance and post-measurement, t(11) = 2.52, p = .03, d = .60.
non-judgment of sensations, thoughts, feelings and body, (5)
awareness and step-by-step change of daily patterns and habits Emotional eating
of eating and physical activity. The intervention consisted of A significant interaction effect was observed for the emotional
8 weekly sessions of 2.5 h. Exercises and skills being taught in- eating subscale, F(1, 24) = 8.15, p < .01, g2 = .25. At the post-
cluded the bodyscan, sitting and walking meditation, mindful measurement, participants in the treatment condition reported
eating skills, acceptance of oneself and one’s body, and dealing with a significantly lower amount of emotional eating, t(11) = 1.08,
the paradox of control. Participants were invited to practice these p = .03, d = .53, compared to the control group t(13) = 1.08, p = .30.
exercises at home for approximately 45–60 min a day.
Body image concern
Results
A significant interaction between condition and scores on the
Participation check BSQ was observed, F(1, 24) = 9.64, p < .01, g2 = .29. Within group
comparisons revealed that participants in the treatment group
There were no drop-outs in the experimental condition. Of the showed significantly less body image concern at the post measure-
12 people in the treatment group, five failed to attend one meeting, ment, t(11) = 3.93, p < .01, d = .68, compared to the control group,
two failed to attend two meetings and one person missed three t(13) = .09, p = .93.
850 H.J.E.M. Alberts et al. / Appetite 58 (2012) 847–851

Table 1
Means and standard deviations of scores on all of the dependent measures.

Measure Control group Experimental group


Pre measurement Post measurement Pre measurement Post measurement
1 1
BMI 31.46 (5.37) 31.23 (5.54) 34.23 (6.73) 33.85 (6.56)
Mindfulness 2.93 (.55) 3.03 (.49) 3.01 (.31) 1 3.35 (.30) 1
Restrained eating 2.71 (.74) 1 2.84 (.71) 1 3.09 (.39) 1 3.30 (.49) 1
External eating 3.35 (.41) 3.35 (.40) 3.32 (.49) 1 3.03 (.48) 1
Emotional eating 3.69 (.87) 3.78 (.72) 3.63 (.92) 1 3.19 (.70) 1
Body image concern 2.63 (.84) 2.64 (.95) 3.08 (.20) 1 2.42 (.82) 1
Dichotomous thinking 2.46 (.70) 2.53 (.20) 2.48 (.32) 1 2.10 (.57) 1
Food cravings 3.60 (.97) 3.70 (.20) 3.73 (.61) 1 3.19 (.20) 1

Note: Numbers represent mean scores on each of the measures. Standard deviations are given in parentheses. Means within a row with the same
superscript concern within group comparisons that differ significantly at p < .05.

Dichotomous thinking et al., 2010) the present study revealed that food cravings reduced
significantly in the mindfulness group. This finding suggests that
Scores on the Dichotomous Thinking Scale interacted signifi- mindfully dealing with food cravings ultimately reduced food crav-
cantly with condition, F(1, 24) = 7.09, p = .01, g2 = .24. After the ings, which can be regarded as successful self-regulation.
intervention period, participants in the treatment condition re- Interestingly, the findings revealed a significant decrease for the
ported significantly less dichotomous thinking, t(11) = 2.41, MBCT group relative to the waiting-list control group on all mea-
p = .03, d = .82, compared to the control participants, t(13) = 1.13, sures except for restrained eating and weight. With regard to re-
p = .28. strained eating behaviour, both the control and the treatment
group reported a significant increase. At this point, however, it is
Food cravings impossible to gain insight into the exact nature of the observed in-
crease in restraint. Restrained eating can be a form of dieting which
A significant interaction for scores on the G-FCQ-T was observed, is externally driven and may interfere with self-attunement. In
F(1, 24) = 9.49, p < .01, g2 = .29. Participants in the experimental contrast, mindfulness is internally-driven and promotes self-
group reported a significant decline in food cravings after the inter- attunement. In line with this, the present intervention aimed to in-
vention, t(11) = 2.72, p = .02, d = .98, whereas no difference in food crease awareness of physical sensations such as hunger, satiety,
craving was observed for the control group, t(13) = 1.09, p = .30. craving and stress. This is likely to reduce eating in the absence
of hunger. Indeed, research has demonstrated that teaching people
Discussion how to recognize and respond appropriately to hunger results in
healthier body weights (Ciampolini & Bianchi, 2006; Ciampolini,
The present results provide support for the efficacy of a mind- Lovell-Smith, & Sifone, 2010). In addition, dietary restraint has of-
fulness-based intervention for problematic eating behavior. Indi- ten been linked with negative self-evaluations and body dissatis-
viduals who participated in an 8-week MBCT-based eating faction. The observed decrease in body concern and dichotomous
intervention reported significantly lower levels of food cravings, thinking for the treatment group may suggest that the restraint
dichotomous thinking, body dissatisfaction, emotional eating and behaviour of this group is guided to lesser extent by negative
external eating after the intervention period, compared to a wait- self-evaluations compared to the control group. However, no defi-
ing list control group. In addition, the intervention group showed nite conclusions on this matter can be drawn.
a significantly stronger increase in trait mindfulness than the con- A marginally significant reduction in weight was observed for
trol group. These findings suggest that increasing mindful aware- individuals in the mindfulness group and a significant reduction
ness of internal experiences and automatic patterns related to for the control group. A possible explanation for the less pro-
eating, emotion regulation, and self-acceptance may help to reduce nounced reduction in BMI of the intervention group is the focus
problematic eating behavior. of the intervention. Instead of promoting weight loss, the present
The current findings are in line with previous studies demon- intervention aimed to increase awareness of both physical and
strating the beneficial effects of mindfulness on (problematic) eat- psychological determinants of eating in the absence of hunger.
ing behaviour and provide support for the notion that higher levels Increasing awareness involves changing automatic and habitual
of mindfulness affect different components of disrupted eating patterns and is likely to be a gradual process which continues to
behavior. First, mindfulness practice has been suggested to help develop and impact (eating) behaviour and weight. Follow-up
individuals ‘‘connecting’’ with their inner experiences (such as measurement could have provided valuable insights in the long-
hunger), thereby attenuating the sensitivity to external or emo- term effects of the intervention.
tional cues to eat (Kristeller & Wolever, 2011). Indeed, the results In sum, the present study was an exploratory attempt to ad-
show that both emotional and external eating diminished signifi- dress the effect of mindfulness on problematic eating behaviour.
cantly in the mindfulness group. On a cognitive level, mindfulness Although the current findings are consistent with previous re-
has been proposed to reduce identification with thoughts about search on mindfulness and eating, some limitations remain. First,
food, body and shape, thereby interrupting dysfunctional thinking we used a relatively small sample size. Second, the present inves-
patterns (Albers, 2011). In line with this, participants in the treat- tigation used questionnaires to measure changes in cognition.
ment condition engaged in less dichotomous thinking and reported Including measures that do not solely rely on self-reports, could
less concern about body weight and shape after the intervention have provided stronger evidence. Finally, adding a standard treat-
period. Finally, mindfulness has been linked with self-regulatory ment group to the design would have provided information on the
processes. Mindfulness involves willingness to stay in contact with effectiveness of the current intervention in relation to other inter-
uncomfortable and/or negative experiences, but without reacting ventions. By addressing these concerns, future research may help
upon them, thereby decreasing impulsivity (Fetterman, Robinson, to further unveil the impact of this relatively novel approach to dis-
Ode, & Gordon, 2010). Consistent with previous findings (Alberts rupted eating behavior.
H.J.E.M. Alberts et al. / Appetite 58 (2012) 847–851 851

References Kristeller, J. L., & Hallett, B. (1999). Effects of a meditation-based intervention in the
treatment of binge eating. Journal of Health Psychology, 4, 357–363.
Kristeller, J. L., & Wolever, R. Q. (2011). Mindfulness-based eating awareness
Albers, S. (2011). Using mindful eating to treat food restriction. A case study. Eating
treatment (MB-EAT). Conceptual basis. Eating Disorders. The Journal of
Disorders, 19, 97–107.
Treatment & Prevention, 19, 49–61.
Alberts, H. J. E. M., Mulkens, S., Smeets, M., & Thewissen, R. (2010). Coping with food
Lissner, L., Andres, R., Muller, D. C., & Shimokata, H. (1990). Bodyweight variability
cravings. Investigating the potential of a mindfulness-based intervention.
in men. Metabolic rate, health and longevity. International Journal of Obesity, 14,
Appetite, 55, 160–163.
373–383.
Arnow, B., Kenardy, J., & Agras, W. S. (1995). The emotional eating scale. The
Mann, T., & Ward, A. (2001). Forbidden fruit. Does thinking about a prohibited
development of a measure to assess coping with a negative affect by eating.
food lead to its consumption? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 29,
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18, 79–90.
319–327.
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-
Masuda, A., & Wendell, J. W. (2010). The role of mindfulness on the relations
report. The Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills. Assessment, 11, 191–206.
between disordered eating-related cognition and psychological distress. Eating
Braet, C., & Van Strien, T. (1997). Assessment of emotional externally induced and
Behaviors, 11, 293–296.
restrained eating behaviour in nine to twelve-year-old obese and non-obese
Mitchell, J. E., Hatsukami, D., Eckert, E. D., & Pyle, R. L. (1985). Characteristics of 275
children. Behaviour Research, 35, 863–873.
patients with bulimia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 482–485.
Byrne, S. M., Cooper, Z., & Fairburn, C. G. (2004). Psychological predictors of weight
Nijs, I. M. T., Franken, I. H. A., & Muris, P. (2007). The modified Trait and State Food-
regain in obesity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1341–1356.
Cravings Questionnaires. Development and validation of a general index of food
Ciampolini, M., & Bianchi, R. (2006). Training to estimate blood glucose and to form
craving. Appetite, 49, 38–46.
associations with initial hunger. Nutrition and Metabolism, 3(42).
Rosen, J. C., Srebnik, D., Saltzberg, E., & Wendt, S. (1991). Development of a body
Ciampolini, M., Lovell-Smith, D., & Sifone, M. (2010). Sustained self-regulation of
image avoidance questionnaire. Psychological Assessment, 3, 32–37.
energy intake. Loss of weight in overweight subjects. Maintenance of weight in
Schlundt, D. G., Virts, K. L., Sbrocco, T., & Pope-Cordle, J. (1993). A sequential
normal-weight subjects. Nutrition and Metabolism, 7(4).
behavioural analysis of craving sweets in obese women. Addictive Behaviors, 18,
Cochrane, C. E., Brewerton, T. D., Wilson, D. B., & Hodges, E. L. (1992). Alexithymia in
67–80.
eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 14, 219–222.
Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindfulness-based cognitive
Conner, M., Fitter, M., & Fletcher, W. (1999). Stress and snacking. A diary study of
therapy for depression. New York: Guilford Press.
daily hassles and between-meal snacking. Psychology and Health, 14, 51–63.
Shafran, R., Fairburn, C. G., Robinson, P., & Lask, B. (2004). Body checking and it’s
Cooley, E., & Toray, T. (2001). Body image and personality predictors of eating
avoidance in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 35,
disorder symptoms during the college years. International Journal of Eating
93–101.
Disorders, 30, 28–36.
Spangler, D. L. (2002). Testing the cognitive model of eating disorders. The role of
Cooper, P. J., Taylor, M. J., Cooper, Z., & Fairburn, C. G. (1987). The development and
dysfunctional beliefs about appearance. Behavior Therapy, 33, 87–105.
validation of the Body Shape Questionnaire. International Journal of eating
Stice, E. (1998). Relations of restraint and negative affect to bulimic pathology. A
disorders, 6, 485–494.
longitudinal test of three competing models. International Journal of Eating
Dewberry, C., & Ussher, J. M. (2001). Restraint and perception of body weight among
Disorders, 23, 243–260.
British adults. The Journal of Social Psychology, 134, 609–619.
Tapper, K., Shaw, C., Ilsley, J., Hill, A. J., Bond, F. W., & Moore, L. (2009). Exploratory
Evans, C., & Dolan, B. (1993). Body Shape Questionnaire. Derivation of shortened
randomised controlled trial of a mindfulness based weight loss intervention for
‘alternate forms’. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 13, 315–321.
women. Appetite, 52, 396–404.
Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., & Shafran, R. (2003). Cognitive behavior therapy for
Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L. J., Altabe, M., & Tantleff Dunn, S. (1999). Exacting
eating disorders. A ‘transdiagnostic’ theory and treatment. Behavior Research
beauty. Theory, assessment, and treatment of body image disturbance.
and Therapy, 41, 509–528.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Fetterman, A. K., Robinson, M. D., Ode, S., & Gordon, K. H. (2010). Neuroticism as a
Van Strien, T., Frijters, J. E. R., Bergers, G. P. A., & Defares, P. B. (1986). The Dutch
risk factor for behavioral dysregulation. A mindfulness-mediation perspective.
Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29, 301–321.
and external eating behaviour. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 5,
Fisher, S. (1990). The evolution of psychological concepts about the body. In T. F.
747–755.
Cash & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body images. Development, deviance and change
Weingarten, H., & Elston, D. (1991). Food cravings in a college population. Appetite,
(pp. 3–20). New York: The Guilford Press.
17, 167–175.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your mind to face
stress, pain and illness. New York: Dell Publishing.

You might also like