Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Journal ofEnvimnmento1 Psychology (1996) 16,774X

(B 1996 Academic Press Limited

VIEWS TO NATURE: EFFECTS ON ATTENTION

CAROLYN M. TENNESSEN*~~AND BERNADINE CwmcHt


* Cornell University, Ithaca, New York and f University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, U.S.A.

Abstract

This study is based on a theoretical view which suggests that under increased demands for attention, individuals’
capacity to direct attention may become fatigued. Once fatigued, attentional restoration must occur in order
to return to an effectively functioning state. An attention-restoring experience can be as simple as looking at
nature. The purpose of this study was to explore whether university dormitory residents with more natural
views from their windows would score better than those with less natural views on tests of directed attention.
Views from dormitory windows of 72 undergraduate students were categorized into four groups ranging from all
natural to all built. The capacity to direct attention was measured using a battery of objective and subjective
measures. Natural views were associated with better performance on attentional measures, providing support
for -the proposed theoretical view.

Views to Nature: Effects on Attention there was a relationship between the degree of
naturalness in the view from university dormitory
Although few would argue about the importance of residents’ windows and their capacity to direct
having a window in any room that a person is going attention.
to spend time in, it has not been until recently that Directed attention is defined as the capacity to
researchers have begun to examine the importance of inhibit or block competing stimuli or distractions
windows. Early investigations established that during purposeful activity (Posner & Snyder, 1975;
people preferred rooms with windows to rooms Kaplan & Kaplan, 1982). This capacity is essential
lacking a window (Ne’eman & Hopkinson, 1970; for the effective performance of daily activities:
Ne’eman, 1974; Verderber, 1986). Not only do acquiring and using selected information; making
people prefer rooms with windows but it appears and carrying out plans; and self-regulation of
that those who have access to windows are better off responses and behavior to meet desired goals (Kaplan
in some ways than those who do not. For example, & Kaplan, 1982; Lezak, 1982). As first proposed
patients in intensive care units with windows were by William James (1890/1983) over 100 years ago,
found to maintain better mental functioning than directed or voluntary attention requires mental
those in units without windows (Wilson, 1972; Keep effort in order to sustain focus and prevent distrac-
et al., 1980). tions from interfering with intended activity. Intense
Further, the type of view from the window also or prolonged demands for directed attention can lead
may be important. In particular, a natural view as to attentional fatigue, manifested as a decreased
opposed to a view of the built environment has been capacity to inhibit competing stimuli along with
associated with positive physical and mental health reduced effectiveness in daily life (Kaplan & Kaplan,
effects when people are confined (Moore, 1981) or sick 1982).
(Ulrich, 1984; Verderber & Keuman, 1987). Although It has been theorized that attentional fatigue is a
views of nature appear to result in positive outcomes manifestation of overuse of the neural inhibitory
under demanding circumstances, the reasons for mechanism underlying the capacity to inhibit
this are not well understood. It has been suggested competing stimuli (Kaplan & Kaplan, 1982). When
that exposure to the natural environment may help attempting to carry out purposeful activity in the
maintain or restore the capacity to direct attention presence of distracting stimuli such as noise in the
i.e. to focus and concentrate, when this capacity is external environment or worries in the internal
overused or fatigued (Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989). The environment, individuals must expend greater
purpose of this study was to. determine whether inhibitory effort to suppress competing cognitive
77
78 C. M. Tennessen and B. Cimpich

activity in order to maintain clarity of focus and views of water and vegetation tended to hold
effective functioning. The characteristic effect of subjects’ attention and interest more effectively
overuse and fatigue of such an inhibitory mechanism than urban scenes. Hartig et al. (1991) compared
would be distractibility and a reduced capacity to three groups of experienced backpackers: one went on
direct attention in subsequent activity. Following a wilderness trip; one went on an urban vacation;
conditions requiring increased use of directed and the third did not vacation. Improvement on
attention, healthy individuals have shown attentional proofreading scores were found only for the
fatigue effects including increased distractibility wilderness vacationers. In a second study, Hartig
and reduced performance on effortful tasks (Glass et al. (1991) had subjects complete 40 min of tasks
& Singer, 1972; Hartley, 1973; Cohen & Spacapan, designed to induce attentional fatigue followed by one
1978; Neuchterlein et al., 1983; Parasuraman, 1986) of three conditions: walking in a natural environ-
increased irritability and strain (Warm & Dember, ment, walking in an urban environment, or passive
19861, and more impulsive and hostile behavior relaxation. Those completing the natural experience
(Donnerstein & Wilson, 1976). scored highest on a proofreading task. Finally, in
University students are a group likely to be at an intervention study focused on restoring directed
increased risk of attentional fatigue. New students attention in women treated for breast cancer
are confronted with multiple situations in the (Cimprich, 1993a), participants were randomly
university environment which require increased assigned to an experimental or control group. The
use of directed attention. Students must adjust to a notion of restorative activities was explained to the
novel environment, including, in many instances, experimental group, who contracted to participate
dormitory living. A dormitory environment often regularly in selected restorative activities over the
contains multiple demands including new rules and 3 months following breast cancer surgery. Subjects
constraints on behavior. At the same time, students typically chose to participate in nature-based activities,
must meet a new level of academic requirements including looking at nature. The restorative concept
(Aycock, 1989). In addition, Grayson (1985) has was not explained to the control group until after
described a continuous series of changes and the study. The experimental group showed
adaptations that must be made each year in order significantly greater improvements in performance
to complete the college experience successfully. across various measures of directed attention over
Previous research has shown that after a demanding time than those in the control group.
examination, college students showed a decline If exposure to nature has a restorative effect on
on measures of directed attention as compared to the capacity to direct attention, then such an effect
baseline (Cimprich, 1989). Thus, the development may, at least in part, explain the beneficial effects of
of attentional fatigue is salient in the college windows and in particular windows with a view of
experience and could undermine a student’s ability nature. The specific objective of this study was to
to succeed at a university. determine whether the degree of naturalness in the
A new era of research focuses on finding ways view from college dormitory windows was associated
to maintain or restore directed attention under with residents’ levels of capacity to direct attention.
demanding circumstances. Four factors are deemed It was hypothesized that students who had more
important for an attention-restoring experience natural views from their dormitory windows would
(Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989): fascination, involving a show higher levels of performance on measures of
spontaneous, effortless kind of selective attention directed attention than those who had less natural
called involuntary attention; a sense of being away, window views.
which involves relief from daily concerns by an
escape to a different place (even if only conceptually);
sufficient extent or scope, allowing enough magnitude Method
for one to become immersed in it; and compatibility
with an individual’s inclinations or purposes, which Setting and participants
reduces the need for directed attention.
The natural environment appears to contain factors Three university-owned and operated dormitories
necessary for an attention-restoring experience at a large midwestern university were selected as
(Kaplan & Kaplan, 1989). Gazing at a natural view the study sites. The dormitories provided a variety
from a window in one’s living environment may be of views ranging from all natural (lake and trees) to
an easily accessible ‘micro-restorative’ activity. In all built (city streets, other buildings or a brick
fact, Ulrich (1981) showed that slides of natural wall), Windows were of a similar size across the
Views to Nature 79

dormitories, ranging from about 1.17 m2 to 1.39 m2. in substituting numbers for a random presentation
All three dormitories contained a mix of males and of 10 geometric symbols including three minor
females and of single and double rooms. Students image pairs, according to a specified key. The score
were assigned to specific rooms based upon is the correct number of substitutions in a ninety
seniority. second period based on standard scoring procedures
Healthy adult volunteers who were residents of (Smith, 1973). Normative data have been obtained
the selected university dormitories were recruited in healthy and clinical populations (Smith, 1973;
for the study by door to door solicitation. Potential Lezak, 1983).
subjects were excluded based upon the following The Necker Cube Pattern Control (NCPC) Test
criteria: insufficient command of the English language was designed to measure directly ability to inhibit
that would preclude testing; history of cognitive competing stimuli, i.e. the capacity to direct atten-
disorder, such as dyslexia, or severe head injury; tion (Cimprich, 1990, 1993a). The Necker cube is a
uncorrected visual or hearing impairments; or taking drawing of a three-dimensional wire cube that can
medication&hugs known to impair or enhance atten- be viewed from two different perspectives due to
tion (e.g. depressants, anti-depressants, ampheta- reversals of the foreground and background. When
mines, psychoactive drugs). Of the 115 students who viewing the cube, the availability of alternate
met the criteria for the study, 72 (63%) volunteered. patterns leads to a tendency to switch focus from
one perspective to the other (James, 1890/1983;
Measures Orbach et al. 1963). Thus, a decreased capacity to
direct attention is manifested by a decreased ability
All testing was done in the participants’ rooms with to inhibit pattern reversals. After providing a 30-s
only the researcher and the subject present. To baseline rate of spontaneous pattern reversals by
ensure privacy and no interruptions during the tapping on a recorder for each reversal, the subject
testing, a sign was posted on the door asking others is then instructed to hold each pattern (keep the
to be quiet and not to disturb, and the telephone pattern from reversing) for as long as possible during
was taken off the hook. All measures were adminis- a 30-s period. The score is the per cent reduction in
tered using standard formats and instructions. pattern reversals from baseline to holding conditions.
Capacity to direct attention was measured by In studies involving healthy adults (Cimprich, 19933)
performance, i.e. speed, accuracy or ability to sustain and breast cancer patients (Cimprich, 1993a),
activity, on measures requiring inhibition of competing baseline scores of spontaneous pattern reversals
or distracting stimuli. The battery included standard obtained in tandem and over time were highly
neurocognitive measures, i.e. Digit Span Forward and reliable.
Backward, and Symbol Digit Modalities Test, that The Attentional Function Index (AFI) was designed
have a known validity and reliability (Lezak, 1983). to measure perceived effectiveness in common
A newer measure of directed attention, the Necker cognitive activities requiring directed attention, i.e.
Cube Pattern Control Test, also was included. In planning, deciding, following a train of thought, and
addition, a subjective rating of attentional func- concentrating on details (Cimprich, 1992). The
tioning, the Attentional Function Index, was used. measure consists of 16 linear analogue scales (each
The Digit Span test is a standardized clinical 100 mm) labeled at each end with polar opposite
measure of attention (Lezak, 1983; Mesulam, 1985). phrases (e.g. ‘not at all’, ‘extremely well’). The
Both Digit Span Forward (DSF) and Backward subject is asked to place a mark on the horizontal
(DSB) were measured using standard testing line at whatever point best describes ‘how you are
procedures described by Wechsler (1955). Digit doing in each area at the present time’. Examples
Span Forward is the number of bits of information a of index items include, ‘Finishing things you’ve
person can attend to at one time, while Digit Span started’ and ‘keeping your mind on what others are
Backward requires sustained attentional effort to saying’. Items are scored from the low end (O-100)
mentally hold and recite digits in reverse sequence. and a single overall score is computed consisting of
Normative data have been obtained on large the average score on the 16 scales. The AFI has
numbers of healthy adults (Wechsler, 1955; Lezak, been shown to be a reliable measure of attentional
1983). functioning in studies involving 32 cancer patients
The Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) (Smith, (Cimprich, 1992, 1993a) and 112 healthy adults
1973) is another standardized test of directed atten- (Cimprich, 19933) with Cronbach alpha coefficients of
tion in a complex task (Lezak, 1983). The SDMT 0.87 and 0.84, respectively. The internal consistency
requires the participant to use directed attention coefficient in this sample was O-84.
C. M. Tennessen and B. Cimprich

FIGURE 1. m natural view. FIGURE 2. Mostly natural view.

Because attentional performance may be influenced 64 film with a 35 mm camera. The interior window
by a depressed mood state. i.e. decreased arousal frame served as the border for each picture, ensuring
level, the Profile of Mood States (POMS) was used that the whole view could be seen from as close a
to assess depressed mood state (McNair et al., 1981). perspective as possible. A group of six adults (with
The POMS is a 65item adjective checklist which knowledge of the subject matter ranging from very
measures six identifiable mood states including little to quite a lot) was asked to rate each slide with
depression-dejection. Subjects rate themselves on a one for ‘all natural’ to a four for ‘all built’ based on
how they have been feeling ‘over the past week the following guidelines. The all natural view con-
including today’ on each adjective using a &point sisted of trees, grass, bushes, and/or lakes and no
scale ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). evidence of human influence (see Fig. 1). The mostly
The test has been shown to be a valid and reliable natural view included evidence of human influence
instrument for the measure of mood state across such as paths, roads, or a parking lot along with a
numerous studies (McNair et al., 1981). The total mostly natural setting (see Fig. 2). If the majority of
POMS and depression-dejection subscale scores what could be seen was built, including buildings,
were used in the analysis. In the present study, the roads, or parking lots but with a natural component
internal consistency coefficient was 0.93 for the to the view such as a few trees, bushes or a lawn, it
total POMS and 0.91 for the depression-dejection was mostly built (see Fig. 3). The all built view,
subscale. which included views that were completely blocked
by another building as well as views of streets and
View categorization buildings, had no natural component (see Fig. 4).
The level of agreement across the raters revealed
In order to categorize the views, slides were taken an acceptable internal consistency reliability of
from each participant’s window using Kodachrome 0.85. The average of the six ratings for each slide
Views to Nature 81

FIGURE 3. Mostly built view. FIGURE 4. All built view.

was computed and rounded to the nearest whole TABLE 1


number. This number determined the category for Demographic characteristics of sample
each view, i.e. an average rating of two meant the
Demographic variable Data
view was put into the mostly natural category.
Sex
Data analysis Male 30 (41.7%)
Female 42 (58.3%)
Data were analysed using descriptive statistics, Age
Analysis of Variance (ANOVA), t-tests, and Pearson Mean f S.D. 20* 1
(Min, Max) (18,251
correlation coefficients. Given the exploratory nature
Year in school
of this study, the analysis was conducted using the Freshman 9 (12.5%)
four view groups, as well as combinations of the Sophomore 34 (47.2%)
groups, in order to determine possible differences Junior 20 (27.8%)
between those with natural views and those with Senior 8 (11.1%)
built views. Special 1 (1.4%)
Subjects by dormitory location
One 26 (36.1%)
TWO 22 (30.6%)
Results Three 24 (33.3%)
Subjects by view category
The sample consisted of 72 undergraduate students, All natural 10 (13.9%)
30 males and 42 females living in the three selected Mostly natural 10 (13.9%)
dormitories at a large midwestern university (see Mostly built 26 (36.1%)
All built 26 (36.1%)
Table 1). Their ages ranged from 18 to 25 (M = 20,
82 C. M. Tennessen and B. Cimprich

Attention and mood scores by view category Attention and mood scores by all natural and all other
view categories
Test View category
Test View category
AI1 Mostly Mostly AI1
natural natural built built AI1 natural AI1 other categories
(n= 10) (n = 10) (n=26) (n=26) (n = 10) (n = 62)

DSF DSF
Mean 750 7.40 7.38 6.96 Mean 7.50 7.20
(S.D.) (1.08) (1.35) (0.90) (1.11) (S.D.) (1.08) (1.07)
DSB DSB
Mean 5.60 5.11 5.04 5.00 Mean 5.60 5.03
(S.D.) (1.35) (1.62) (1.18) (1.13) (S.D.) (1.35) (1.21)
SDMT SDMT
Mean 74.00* 64.40 61.50 63.08 Mean 74.00** 62.63
(S.D.) (9.87) (10.76) (10.36) (8.74) (S.D.) (9.87) (9.67)
NCPC NCPC
Mean -60.53% -64.18% -35.38% -36.35% Mean -60.53%** -40.43%
(S.D.) (13.61) (22.48) (38.14) (39.78) (S.D.) (13.61) (37.81)
AFI AFI
Mean 71.97 61.03 66.09 63.17 Mean 71.97* 64.05
(S.D.) (10.26) (11.34) (11.80) (11.73) (S.D.) (10.26) (11.66)
POMS POMS
Mean 1.31 1.40 1.25 1.48 Mean 1.31 1.37
(S.D.) (0.32) (0.42) (0.36) (0.45) (S.D.) (0.32) (0.42)
Depression-Dejection Depression-Dejection
subscale of the POMS subscale of the POMS
Mean 0.65 0.95 0.57 0.94 Mean 0.65 0.78
(S.D.) (0.60) (0.77) (0.50) (0.68) (S.D.) (0.60) (0.64)

* p < 0.05: AI1 natural > Mostly built and AI1 built view * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
categories. Abbreviations: DSF = Digit Span Forward; DSB = Digit
Abbreviations: DSF = Digit Span Forward; DSB = Digit Span Backward; SDMT = Symbol Digit Modalities Test;
Span Backward; SDMT = Symbol Digit Modalities Test; NCPC = Necker Cube Pattern Control; AFI = Attentional
NCPC = Necker Cube Pattern Control; API = Attentional Function Index; and POMS = Profile of Mood States.
Function Index; and POMS = Profile of Mood States.
sleep per night over the past week as well as total
S.D. = 1). The greatest percentage (47.2%) were sopho- number of exams taken over the past week and
mores. The subjects were about evenly distributed anticipated in the coming week, and again, no
among the three dormitories. Of the four view differences were found across view groups.
categories, 13.9% had an all natural view, another There were no significant differences across the
13.9% had a mostly natural view, 36.1% had a four view groups based on the demographic variables
mostly built view, and 36.1% had an all built view. of age, sex, and year in school. With respect to mood
Subjects in two of the dormitories (dormitories state, ANOVA showed that the view groups did not
one and two) were about evenly split across all four significantly differ on mean scores for the total
view groups. The third dormitory (dormitory three), POMS or the depression-dejection subscale (see
which was surrounded by streets and buildings, Table 2).’ Also, no correlations were discovered
contained about an equal number of subjects with between mood state (either the total POMS or
mostly and all built views with none having all the depression-dejection subscale) and any of the
natural or mostly natural views. There were no objective measures of attention. However, scores on
significant demographic differences (i.e. age, sex, the total POMS as well as the depression-dejection
and year in school) among the subjects by dormitory subscale of the POMS were significantly inversely
location. There were no differences across view correlated with scores on the self-rated API (r(72) =
groups on subjects’ ratings of their rooms and -0.49, p < 0.001 and r(72) = -0.29, p c 0.05 respec-
dormitories on noise and crowding. In addition, tively). Thus, more negative mood states (higher
subjects reported their average number of hours of scores) were associated with lower self-ratings of
Views to Nature 83

TABLED mostly built and all built view groups were combined
Attention and mood scores by natural and built view into a single group. Table 3 shows a comparison of
categories the all-natural view group vs the combined group.
Test View category Subjects with the all natural view scored sig-
nificantly better on the SDMT, t(71) = 3.29,
AI1 & mostly Mostly & all p c 0.01, the NCPC (t(71) = 3.12, p c O-01, and the
natural built AFI t(72) = 2.02, p < 0.05.
(n = 20) (n = 52) Next, in order to determine the possible differ-
DSF ences between subjects who had a more natural
Mean 7.45 7.17 view and those who did not, view categories were
(S.D.) (1.19) (1.02) combined into two groups for analysis: more natural
DSB (combination of all natural and mostly natural) and
Mean 5.37 5.02 more built (combination of mostly and all built).
(S.D.) (1.46) (1.15) Table 4 shows that the combined natural view
SDMT group scored significantly higher on the SDMT
Mean 68.95* 62.29 t(71) = 2.49, p < 0.05 and had a greater percentage
(SD.) (11.20) (9.52)
reduction in pattern reversals on the NCPC t(71) =
NCPC
Mean -62.35%** -35.87% 2.94, p c 0.01 than the combined built view group.
(S.D.) (18.19) (38.59) Finally, to determine whether the dormitory that
API had only built (mostly and all) view categories
Mean 66.50 64.63 (dormitory three) was in some way different from
(SD.) (11.93) (11.74) the two dormitories that contained all four view
POMS categories (dormitories one and two>, the data
Mean 1.35 1.36 were analysed using only subjects residing in the
(S.D.) (0.36) (0.42) dormitories containing all four view groups (10 to 16
Depression-Dejection subjects in each group). This analysis revealed the
subscale of the POMS same pattern of significant results as those reported
Mean 0.79 0.75
(S.D.) (0.68) (0.62) using subjects from all three dormitories.

* p < 0.05; ** p c 0.01.


Abbreviations: DSF = Digit Span Forward; DSB = Digit Discussion
Span Backward; SDMT = Symbol Digit Modalities Test;
NCPC = Necker Cube Pattern Control; API = Attentional
Function Index; and POMS = Profile of Mood States. Those who had natural views from their dormitory
windows were better able to direct attention than
those with less natural views on some of the
attentional functioning and vice versa. measures used to test the capacity to direct atten-
Overall, the subjects’ attention scores fell within tion in this study. When all four of the view cate-
normal ranges for healthy adults (Smith, 1973; gories were compared, those with all natural views
Lezak, 1983). A comparison of mean scores among scored significantly higher on the SDMT, an objective
the four view groups on the attention measures is measure of directed attention, than those with less
found in Table 2. To determine differences in mean natural or built views. When categories were
scores among the four groups, ANOVA using Scheffe’s collapsed to form two groups, the more natural
method for post hoc comparisons was performed. view group performed significantly better on both
Subjects in the all natural view category showed the SDMT and the NCPC than the more built
a significantly higher mean score on one test, view group. Finally, in addition to performing
SDMT, than subjects in the mostly and all built significantly better on the SDMT and NCPC, stu-
view categories (F(3, 67) = 3.78, p c O-05). However, dents who had an all natural view rated their atten-
examination of the pattern of the means of the tional functioning as more effective than those in all
four groups revealed that subjects in the all natural the other view groups combined. Overall, the find-
view category scored higher on nearly all attention ings support the hypothesis that dormitory resi-
measures than subjects in the other view groups, dents with more natural views from their windows
but not significantly so. . would have a stronger capacity to direct attention
Subjects in the all natural view category were than those with less natural or built views.
compared with all others, i.e. sthe mostly natural, Natural views did not have an influence on mood
a4 C. M. Tennessen and B. Cimprich

state in this study. Further, there was no relationship There are certain limitations to the present study
between scores on the objective measures of directed that should be taken into consideration in interpreting
attention and mood state, as might be expected if the findings. First, the results of this study are
depressed mood state interfered with the capacity to limited by the size and nature of the sample. In
direct attention, There was, however, a relationship particular, there were small numbers of subjects in
between self-ratings of attentional functioning and the natural view groups. Another limitation is that
mood state such that subjects across view categories the study could not provide information about the
who reported a more negative or depressed mood capacity to direct attention prior to moving into the
state also tended to rate themselves lower on atten- dormitory. Obtaining a baseline and repeated
tional functioning. Previous research suggests that measures would make it possible to examine changes
affective states may have a pronounced influence in the capacity to direct attention associated with
on thought (Isen, 1984). Thus, it is possible that the type of window view over time. A final limita-
subjects’ judgments of effectiveness in common tion involves the procedure of testing subjects in
activities requiring directed attention may have their dormitory rooms. Although steps were taken
been influenced by mood state. Future studies are to prevent noise and distraction, the environment
needed to explore the relationships among various could not be strictly controlled. Thus, it is unknown
dimensions of mood state, attentional functioning, to what extent distracting stimuli may have affected
and natural views. performance on the measures.
Previous research has suggested physical and Although this study provided initial evidence of a
psychological benefits of natural views in certain positive relationship between natural views from
populations (Moore, 1981; Ulrich, 1984; Verderber dormitory windows and the capacity to direct atten-
& Reuman, 1987) while the present study suggests tion, further research is needed to understand
that university students with natural dormitory the beneficial effects of a natural view on attention
views benefit in terms of increased capacity to better. Longitudinal studies involving repeated
direct attention. Thus, another possible benefit measures are needed to understand better the
may be that natural views allow for restoration of effects of the environment and a natural view on
directed attention. the capacity to direct attention over time.
In the present study, those dormitory residents Little is known about factors that may moderate or
with the more natural views automatically may have influence the attention-restoring effect of a natural
had an additional mode of restoration not available view. Thus, for example, some research suggests
to those without a natural view. This would suggest, that view preference may be an important variable in
then, with similar attentional demands, that those assessing reactions to views (Butler & Steuerwald,
with a more natural view have access to an addi- 1991). There is a need to understand better whether
tional mode of restoration of directed attention view preference influences attentional responses
above and beyond that of those who do not have this to varying window views. There is also a need to
view. If natural views from windows do have posi- explore other aspects of the environment that may
tive effects on students’ capacity to direct attention, influence attentional responses. For example, where
this would have considerable implications for the natural views are lacking, what role might other
placement and design of dormitories. Students natural objects or ‘surrogate views’.play (Heerwagen
require strong attention to master successfully the & Orians, 1986; Verderber, 1986)? Heerwagen and
multiple demands of education in the university Orians (1986) found that those in windowless office
setting (Grayson, 1985; Aycock, 1989) and the spaces put up a similar amount of such surrogate
provision of natural views from dormitory rooms views (pictures of landscape or cityscapes) as those
appears to be an easy way to allow them the oppor- in windowed offices but the content in the window-
tunity to restore their capacity to direct attention. less offices was more likely to include landscapes
The finding of a relationship, in this study, and natural scenes than cityscapes. The addition of
between natural views and the capacity to direct items such as pictures of natural scenes or plants
attention provides support for the theoretical could be systematically controlled and effects on the
link between the natural environment, attentional capacity to direct attention observed. In addition,
fatigue, and restoration of attention (Kaplan & it is important to examine other aspects of the
Kaplan, 1989). Further, the flndings suggest that environment (that the individual may or may not be
even a modest exposure to the natural environ- able to control) such as noise, space, or lighting, for
ment-a window view-may have some beneficial their effect on the capacity to direct attention.
effects on the capacity to direct attention. Finally, there is a need for similar research on the
Views to Nature 86

attentional effects of environments in other popula- Heerwagen, J. H. & Orians, G. H. (1986). Adaptations to window-
tions. For example, would the capacity to direct lessness: a study of the use of visual decor in windowed and
attention in older adults be affected by access to windowless offices. Environment and Behavior, l&623-639.
Isen, A. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in
natural views? If so, access to a natural view would cognition. In R. Wyler L T. Skrull, eds., Handbook of Social
be an important consideration in the design of living Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 179-235.
environments for the older person. Also, would James, W. (1983). The Principles of Psychology, Cambridge, MA:
windows with a natural view have a similar effect Harvard University Press (Original work published in 1890).
on employees in office environments? It would seem Kaplan, R. & Kaplan, S. (19891. The Experience of Nature:A
psychological perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University
important to learn whether employees with natural
Press.
views or with natural interior landscaping were Kaplan, S. & Kaplan, R. (1982). Environment and Cognition.
more productive due to the attention-restoring New York: Praeger.
benefits of nature. Keep, P., James, J. & Inman, M. (1980). Windows in the inten-
sive therapy unit. Anaesthesia, 35257-262.
Lezak, M. D. (19831. Neuropsychological Assessment. New York:
Notes Oxford University Press.
McNair, D. M., Lorr, M. L Droppleman, L. F. (19811. EZTS Manual
(1) All subscales of the POMS were tabulated but are not for the Profile of Mood States. San Diego, CA: Educational and
presented. It csn be noted that there were no differences Industrial Testing Service.
on mean scores for any of the subscales across view Mesulam, M. (19851. Principles of Behavioral Neurology.
categories. Philadelphia: F. A. Davis.
(2) All correspondence should be addressed to C. M. Moore, E. 0. (1981). A prison environments effect on health care
Tennessen, IWSP, DEA, E213 MVR Hall, Cornell service demands. Journal of Environmental Systems, 11,17-34.
University, Ithaca, NY 14853, U.S.A. Ne’eman, E. (1974). Visual aspects of lighting in buildings. Lighting
Research and Technology, 6, 159-184.
Ne’eman, E. & Hopkinson, R. G. (1970). Critical minimum
References acceptable window size: a study of window design and provision
of a view. Lighting Research and Technology, 2, 17-27.
Aycock, D. (19891. Educating the whole person: a rationale for Neuchterlein, K., Parasuraman, R. & Jiang, Q. (1983). Visual
counselling services on Christian college campuses. Journal of sustained attention: image degradation produces rapid sensi-
Psychology and Christianity, 8,38-44. tivity decrement over time. Science, 220,327-329.
Butler, D. & Steuerwald, B. (19911. Effects of view and room size Orbach, J., Ehrlich, D. & Heath, H. A. (1963). Reversibility of the
on window size preferences made in models. Environment and Necker cube: 1. An examination of the concept of ‘satiation of
Behavior, 23, 334-358. orientation’. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 17,439458.
Cimprich, B. (19891. Attentional effort and mental fatigue effects Parasuraman, R. (19861. Vigilance, monitoring and search. In K.
in healthy adults. Unpublished manuscript, University of Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. P. Thomas, Eds., Handbook of Percep-
Michigan, Ann Arbor. tion and Human Performance. Vol. ZZ. Cognitive processes and
Cimprich, B. (19901. Attentional fatigue and restoration in performance. New York: Wiley.
individuals with cancer. Dissertation Abstracts International, Posner, M. I. & Snyder, C. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive con-
SIB, 1740. trol. In R. L. Solso, Ed., Information Processing and Cognition.
Cimprich, B. (19921. Attentional fatigue following breast cancer Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 55-85.
surgery. Research in Nursing and Health, 15, 199-207. Smith, A. (1973). Symbol Digit Modalities Test. California: Western
Cimprich, B. (1993a). Development of an intervention to restore Psychological Services.
attention in cancer patients. Cancer Nursing, 16,83-92. Ulrich, R. (1981). Natural versus urban scenes: some psycho-
Cimprich, B. (19936). Evaluating usefulness of tests of atten- physiological effects. Environment and Behavior, l&523656.
tional capacity in healthy adults. Unpublished raw data. Ulrich, R. (1984). View through a window may influence recovery
Cohen, S. & Spacapan, S. (19781. The aftereffects of stress: an from surgery. Science, 224,420-421.
attentional interpretation. Environmental Psychology and Verderber, S. (1986). Dimensions of person-window transactions
Nonverbal Behavior, 3,43-57. in the hospital environment. Environment and Behavior, 18,
Donnerstein, E. % Wilson, D. W. (1978). Effects of noise and per- 450-466.
ceived control of ongoing and subsequent aggressive behavior. Verderber, S. & Reuman, D. (19871. Windows, views, and health
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34,774-781. status in hospital therapeutic environments. The Journal of
Glass, D. C. & Singer, J. E. (1972). Urban Stress: Experiments in Architectural and Planning Research, 4, 120-133.
Noise and Social Stressors. New York: Academic Press. Warm, J. S. & Dember, W. N. (1986). Awake at the switch.
Grayson, P. A. (1985). College time: implications for student Psychology Today, April, 48-57.
mental health services. Journal of American College Health, Wechsler, D. (1965). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence
33,198-204. Scale. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Hartig, T., Mang, M. & Evans, G. (1991). Restorative effects of Wilson, L. M. (1972). Intensive care delirium: the effect of outside
natural environment experiences. Environment and Behavior, deprivation in a windowless unit. Archives of Internal
283-26. Medicine, 130,225-226.
Hartley, L. R. (1973). Effect of prior noise or prior performance
on serial reaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 101, Manuscript received 1 April 1994
255-261. Revised manuscript received 8 February 1995

You might also like